ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Have the core discussion networks of Americans changed in the past two decades? In 1985, the General Social Survey (GSS) collected the first nationally representative data on the confidants with whom Americans discuss important matters. In the 2004 GSS the authors replicated those questions to assess social change in core network structures. Discussion networks are smaller in 2004 than in 1985. The number of people saying there is no one with whom they discuss important matters nearly tripled. The mean network size decreases by about a third (one confidant), from 2.94 in 1985 to 2.08 in 2004. The modal respondent now reports having no confidant; the modal respondent in 1985 had three confidants. Both kin and non-kin confidants were lost in the past two decades, but the greater decrease of non-kin ties leads to more confidant networks centered on spouses and parents, with fewer contacts through voluntary associations and neighborhoods. Most people have densely interconnected confidants similar to them. Some changes reflect the changing demographics of the U.S. population. Educational heterogeneity of social ties has decreased, racial heterogeneity has increased. The data may overestimate the number of social isolates, but these shrinking networks reflect an important social change in America.
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades
Miller McPherson
University of Arizona and Duke University
Lynn Smith-Lovin
Duke University
Matthew E. Brashears
University of Arizona
APPENDIX TABLE OF CONTENTS
Table A1. Influence of Proportion Kin on Network Density and Heterogeneity
Table A2. Basic Parameters of Discussion Networks: 1985, 1987 and 2004
Table A3. Number of Respondents That Had Various Relationships with at Least One Discussion Partner
Table A4. Respondents with Spouses Only, Non-Spouse Kin, and Non-Kin in the Network
Table A5. Differences by Age, Education, Sex and Race in Network Size and Kin/Nonkin Composition
Table A6. Cooperativeness as Rated by the GSS Interviewer in 1985 and 2004
Table A7. Negative Binomial Regression of Size of Discussion Network
Table A8. Logistic Regression Coefficients for Having Someone to Talk to About Important Matters
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
2
Table A1. Influence of Proportion Kin on Network Density and Heterogeneity (OLS Regression of Density and Heterogeneity on Proportion Kin)
Unstandardized Regression
Coefficient for Proportion Kin Predicted Value at Proportion Kin of R-squared
0 1
Dependent Variable 85 04 85 04 85 04 85 04
Density .36 .26 .42 .49 .78 .78 .18 .10
Age Heteregeneity (S.D. of alters’ ages) 5.48 7.53 7.30 6.56 13.54 13.88 .09 .15
Education Heterogeneity (S.D. of alters’
educations) .30 .20NS 1.61 1.38 1.92 1.58 .01 .001
Race Heterogeneity (IQV) -.08 -.12 .09 .15 .01 .03 .03 .04
Sex Heterogeneity (IQV) .27 .27 .54 .53 .79 .80 .06 .06
Note: All coefficients significant, p < .01, except where indicated.
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
3
Table A2. Basic Parameters of Discussion Networks: 1985, 1987 and 2004
1985 (N=1531a) 1987 (N=1800) 2004 (N=1467b)
Network Size
0 10.0% 4.51% 24.6%
1 15.0% 14.27% 19.0%
2 16.2% 19.02% 19.2%
3 20.3% 49.06% 16.9%
4 14.8% 6.78% 8.8%
5 18.2% 3.20% 6.5%
6+ 5.4% 3.12% 4.9%
Mean 2.94 2.63 2.08
Mode 3 3 0
S.D. 1.79 1.28 1.81
Kin Network Size
0 26.4% 22.25% 42.4%
1 29.6% 35.70% 29.0%
2 21.6% 25.67% 14.5%
3 12.6% 16.39% 9.1%
4 6.3% 0.00% 3.5%
5c3.3% 0.00% 1.4%
Mean 1.44 1.36 1.12
Mode 1 1 0
S.D. 1.41 1.00 1.38
Non-kin Network Size
0 36.4% 36.79% 54.1%
1 22.2% 26.99% 21.3%
2 18.9% 20.03% 14.0%
3 13.0% 11.14% 5.7%
4 6.3% 3.50% 3.3%
5 3.3% 1.54% 1.5%
Mean 1.42 1.22 .88
Mode 1 0 0
S.D. 1.57 1.22 1.40
Proportion Kin
0 19.2% 18.57% 21.0%
.01-.33 15.4% 4.44% 10.9%
.34-.66 20.7% 25.71% 19.4%
.67-.99 14.5% 17.48% 11.6%
1 30.2% 33.80% 37.1%
Mean .53 .57 .60
S.D. .44 .38 .53
a Proportion kin is defined only for those who have at least one alter. Therefore, the N = 1395 for the proportion kin
analysis.
b N = 1070 for the analysis of proportion kin.
c Information on kinship was collected only on the first five alters cited. Therefore, the sum of the kin and non-kin alters is
not equal to the overall network size distribution.
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
4
Table A3. Number of Respondents That Had Various Relationships with at Least One Discussion Partner (e.g. What
Percent of the Sample Mentioned A Spouse/Parent/etc. As A Person with Whom They Discussed
Important Matters?)
Type of Relationship to Respondentc1985 (N=1531) 1987a (N=1800) 2004b (N=1467)
Spouse 30.9% 43.21% 38.1%
Parent 23.6% 26.81% 21.1%
Sibling 21.1% 16.0% 14.11%
Child 17.9% 16.92% 10.19%
Other Family Member 18.2% 14.79% 11.8%
Co-worker 29.4% 28.44% 17.9%
Co-member of group 26.1% 21.48% 11.8%
Neighbor 18.5% 18.84% 7.8%
Friend 73.2% 77.98% 50.6%
Advisor 25.2% 45.03% 19.2%
Other 2.1% 4.50% 7.2%
a All differences between 1987 and 1985 are statistically significant at the p<.01 level except for Spouse, Parent,
Child, Friend, and Other, which are non-significant.
b All differences between 2004 and 1985 are statistically significant at the p<.01 level. Differences between 1987
and 2004 are statistically significant at the p<.01 level except for Siblings and Others, which are non-significant.
c Since more than one type of relationship can be mentioned for any given discussion partner (e.g., a co-worker can
also be a co-member of a group, an advisor and a friend), the percentages do not sum to 100.
Table A4. Respondents with Spouses Only, Non-Spouse Kin, and Non-Kin in the Network
1985 (N = 1531) 1987 (N=1800) 2004a (N = 1467)
Spouse ONLY Present 6.4% 5.5% 9.3%
At Least One Non-Spouse Kin 58.79% 60.19% 42.87%
At Least One Non-Kin 80.15% 83.79% 57.16%
a Significantly different from both 1987 and 1985 at the p < .01 level.
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
5
Table A5. Differences by Age, Education, Sex and Race in Network Size and Kin/Nonkin Compositiona (Unstandardized OLS Regression Coefficients of
Network Variables on Respondents’ Demographic Characteristics)
Dependent Variables
Network Size # of Kin # of Non-Kin Proportion Kin
Independent Variables 85 87 04 85 87 04 85 87 04 85 87 04
A. Age
Age .02NS .02 NS .02 NS -.02 NS .02 .01 NS .03 -.01 NS .00 NS -.01 .01 .00 NS
Age2-.00 -.00 -.00 NS .00 NS -.00 -.00 NS -.00 .00 NS -.00 NS .00 -.00 NS -.00 NS
Constant 3.17 2.47 1.68 2.06 .97 .85 NS 1.17 1.51 .88 NS .69 .40 .48 NS
R .27 .14 .03 NS .08 .09 .04 NS .26 .08 .01 NS .18 .07 NS .05 NS
B. Educa tion
Sex
Educ (yrs) .19 .10 .14 .02 .02 .05 .15 .08 .08 -.03 -.01 -.02
Constant .61 1.33 .08 1.15 1.17 .45 -.47 .20 NS -.28 .87 .75 .79
R .35 .27 .23 .06 .05 .11 .35 .21 .20 NS .23 .12 .10
C.
Sex (f=1) -.04 NS .11 NS .20 NS .28 .17 .23 -.30 -.07 NS -.02 NS .07 .04 .01 NS
Constant 2.94 2.56 1.95 1.28 1.09 1.00 1.59 1.33 .89 .49 .50 .59
R .01 NS .04 NS .05 NS .10 .09 .03 .10 .03 NS .01 NS .10 .06 NS .01 NS
D. Race/ethnic (White is
reference category)
Black -.79 -.51 -.51 -.58 -.34 -.53 -.19 NS -.15 NS -.12 NS -.08 NS -.05 NS -.08 NS
Other -.43 NS -.06 NS -.64 -.42 -.03 NS -.49 .00 NS -.00 NS -.11 NS -.08 NS -.02 NS -.11
Constant 3.03 2.68 2.22 1.51 1.41 1.23 1.44 1.24 .91 .54 .58 .61
R .14 .14 .15 .14 .11 .17 .04 NS .04 NS .04 NS .07 NS .05 NS .10 NS
Note: All coefficients significant, p < .01, except where indicated.
a Marsden (1987) also analyzed differences in network size and kin composition by size of place, but this variable has not yet been coded for 2004 so comparable
analyses are not possible at this time. (The size of place variable is added to the data set after the data are collected, using the respondents’ addresses and current
Census tract information.)
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
6
Table A6. Cooperativeness as Rated by the GSS Interviewer in 1985 and 2004
1985 (N = 1531) 1987 (N=1800) 2004 (N = 1467)
Level of Cooperativeness
Interested/Friendly 79.27% 77.05% 82.18%
Cooperative 16.74% 18.78% 15.13%
Restless/Impatient 3.58% 3.65% 2.65%
Hostile .41% .52% .04%
Note: X2 with 6 df = 32.07, p < 0.00
Table A7. Negative Binomial Regression of Size of Discussion Network
Model
Independent Variable I II III
Constant 1.069 1.129 .554
Wave2 (1 =1987) -.108 -.084 -.10
Wave3 (1=2004) -.349 -.336 -.404
Cooperative – -.187 -.121
Restless/Impatient – -.501 -.431
Hostile (Compared to Friendly/Interested) -.785 -.618
Number Missing in Preceding Questions -.197 -.162
Education (in yrs) .047
Female – .069
Agea– – -.002
Currently Married .014NS
Black – -.210
Other Race (Compared to White) -.177
Number of Adults in Household .018NS
Number of Children Ever Born .008NS
Number of Siblings .001NS
Alpha (Heterogeneity Coef.) 1.01*10-7 7.50*10-8 8.95*10-9
F 66.84 50.77 47.21
Note: All coefficients significant at p <.01, unless otherwise indicated.
a The squared term for age was not significant.
ONLINE SUPPLEMENT
to article in
AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (JUNE:353–375)
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Brashears
“Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades”
7
Table A8. Logistic Regression Coefficients for Having Someone to Talk to About Important Matters (Dependent
Variable: No One to Talk to= 0, Someone to Talk to = 1)
Model
Independent Variable I II III IV
Constant 2.200 2.433 2.159 1.266
Wave2 (1 =1987) .851 .986 .897 1.273
Wave3 (1=2004) -1.080 -1.066 -1.392 -.247NS
Cooperative – -.364 -.159NS -.160NS
Restless/Impatient – -1.429 -1.293 -1.269
Hostile (Compared to Friendly/Interested) – -2.258 -1.968 -1.968
Number Missing in Previous Module -.549 -.473 -.469
Education (in yrs) .089 .152
Education*Wave2 – – – -.038NS
Education*Wave3 – – – -.100
Femaleb– – .254 .253
Agea– – -.018 -.016
Currently Married .315 .250NS
Black – -.903 -.863
Other Race -.284NS -.267NS
Number of Adults in Household .089NS
Number of Children Ever Born .018NS
Number of Siblings – – – -.013NS
F 118.56 50.78 37.28 27.12
Note: All coefficients significant at p < .01 unless otherwise indicated.
a The squared term for age was not significant.
b Significant at the p<.05 level.
... Negative reinforcement in individualistic cultures is more significantly associated with feelings of loneliness. Research has shown (McPherson et al., 2006) that although social media provides a wide range of social interaction opportunities, this interaction often lacks depth, leading individuals to feel lonelier. For example, research in the United States has found that the higher the frequency of social media use, the stronger the sense of loneliness individuals feel (McPherson et al., 2006). ...
... Research has shown (McPherson et al., 2006) that although social media provides a wide range of social interaction opportunities, this interaction often lacks depth, leading individuals to feel lonelier. For example, research in the United States has found that the higher the frequency of social media use, the stronger the sense of loneliness individuals feel (McPherson et al., 2006). Religious and ethical norms are also important considerations, such as in conservative cultures (such as the Middle East), where social media anonymity amplifies negative reinforcement use (Li, 2023) (evading the social constraints of reality). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social media addiction has become a global public health challenge, and understanding its mechanism’s complexity requires the integration of the transitional characteristics of addiction development stages and breaking through the traditional single-reinforcement-path explanatory framework. This study is based on the dual pathway of positive and negative emotional reinforcement, integrating multidisciplinary evidence from neuroscience, psychology, and computational behavioral science to propose an independent and dynamic interaction mechanism of positive reinforcement (driven by social rewards) and negative reinforcement (driven by emotional avoidance) in social media addiction. Through a review, it was found that early addiction is mediated by the midbrain limbic dopamine system due to immediate pleasurable experiences (such as liking), while late addiction is maintained by negative emotional cycles due to the dysfunction of the prefrontal limbic circuit. The transition from early addiction to late addiction is characterized by independence and interactivity. Based on this, a phased intervention strategy is proposed, which uses reward competition strategies (such as cognitive behavioral therapy and alternative rewards) to weaken dopamine sensitization in the positive reinforcement stage, enhances self-control by blocking emotional escape (such as through mindfulness training and algorithm innovation) in the negative reinforcement stage, and uses cross-pathway joint intervention in the interaction stage. This study provides a theoretical integration framework for interdisciplinary research on social media addiction from a dynamic perspective for the first time. It is recommended that emotional reinforcement variables are included in addiction diagnosis, opening up new paths for precise intervention in different stages of social media addiction development.
... Importantly, some scholars suggest that the mere existence of at least one meaningful social bond is more critical to happiness than the number of relationships (4). Conversely, a lack of close relationships is a key predictor of poor health, diminished wellbeing, and unhappiness (35)(36)(37)(38). ...
Article
Full-text available
In the contemporary United States, many individuals suffer from a lack of close relationships, negatively affecting their happiness. At the same time, many individuals do not feel a loving relationship with God/Spirit (i.e., religious love). In the 1950s, theorist Pitirim Sorokin posited that love, particularly religious love, acts as a transformative energy capable of fostering resilience in the modern rational world, and scholars hypothesized a link between religious love and happiness. However, the topic remains understudied. This study analyzes United States data from the Global Flourishing Study and presents results from linear regression with robust standard errors. The findings indicate that a central component of religious love, love by God, mitigates the adverse effects of lacking close relationships. This study calls on researchers to further investigate the significant yet understudied role of religious love in individuals’ lives.
... Network size is our first measure, for which we primarily draw on "important matters" networks measured in national surveys. This item asks respondents to name up to five people with whom they discuss things that are important to them. 1 Despite the heterogeneity in what counts as an "important matter" to different respondents (Bearman and Parigi 2004), it is widely agreed that this name-generator battery captures the core networks for most individuals in the United States (Burt 1984;McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Brashears 2006). Having someone to talk about important matters, however defined, can be thought as an important social resource in itself (Small 2017), and the size of these ego networks is associated with multiple benefits (Cornwell and Laumann 2015;McClurg 2003;Umberson and Montez 2010). ...
... Némileg leegyszerűsítve az "erős" kötéseknek a családi, a "gyenge" kapcsolatoknak pedig a családon kívülről mozgósítható nexusokat tekintjük. Másodszor: a társadalmi tőke kapcsolati/strukturális dimenziójában még egy további árnyalást is beviszünk: elsősorban Mérei (1998) A társadalmi tőke strukturális összetevőjét, a kapcsolatokat -a szakirodalomban "névgenerátor" módszerként is ismert -hálózati kérdéssoron keresztül mértük, amely arra kérdezett rá, hogy az alanyok a különböző élethelyzetekben számíthatnak-e más emberek segítségére, illetve vannak-e körülöttük olyan személyek, akikkel barátkozni szoktak, társas tevékenységben közösen részt vesznek (Mardsen, 2005;McPherson et al., 2006;Smith et al., 2014, erdélyi alkalmazásban Veres, 2005. Összesen négy hipotetikus helyzetben kérdeztünk rá arra, hogy igénybe szokta-e venni más emberek segítségét, szokott-e bizalmas dolgokról beszélni, illetve társaság-ban összejárni másokkal. ...
Article
Full-text available
HU Ebben az elemzésben a társadalmi tőke kisebbségi nézőpo ntú konceptualizálására, operacionalizálá-sára és mérésére tettünk kísérletet – annak strukturális (kapcsolati) és kognitív (bizalom) dimenzióin keresztül. Egy átfogó kérdőíves kutatás eredményei alapján kijelenthető, hogy a Magyarországhatárain kívül élő magyarok körében az etnikai határokat átmetsző, “polgári” integráció korlátozottabban valósul meg, ehelyett inkább a párhuzamos, etnikailag és nyelvileg szegmentált társadalmak képe rajzolódik ki. Az etnikumközi kapcsolatok ritkábbak, ez abban is megmutatkozik, hogy magasabb bizalmatlanság a többségi etnikum tagjaival szemben. Erőforrás-szemléleti logikában kijelenthető tehát, hogy a kisebbségi magyarok által lakott régiók szociális és rendszerintegrációja szempontjából az összekötő (bridging) társadalmi tőke szerepe a strukturális és kognitív dimenziókban egyaránt korlátozott. EN In this analysis we attempt to conceptualise, operationalise and measure social capital from a minority perspective – through its structural (relational) and cognitive (trust) dimensions. Survey results suggest that minority Hungarians in the Charpatian Basin experience limited cross-ethnic “civic”integration; instead, images of parallel, ethnically and linguistically segmented societies emerges. Terelative lack of interethnic relationships is refl ected in higher levels of distrust toward members ofmajority ethnic groups. From a resource-oriented perspective, bridging social capital plays a limitedrole in both structural and cognitive dimensions in fostering the social and systemic integration ofregions inhabited by minority Hungarians.
Chapter
Healthy relationships are interpersonal connections that are mutually beneficial, supportive, and respectful, with an emphasis on open communication and trust. Social isolation and loneliness can negatively impact the development and quality of healthy relationships and are associated with poor physical and mental health outcomes. Social isolation and loneliness are influenced by various risk factors, such as disability, bereavement, family structure, urbanisation, and technology use, and affect different groups of people differently. Interventions to enhance healthy relationships can include improving social skills, enhancing social support, increasing opportunities for social contact, addressing maladaptive social cognition, and facilitating community engagement and volunteering. The relationship between clinician and patient is also a key factor for health outcomes, and can be improved by continuity of care, empathy, and trust. Clinicians can apply a Lifestyle Medicine approach to identify and address the impact of relationships on health, and support people to develop and maintain healthy relationships.
Article
Full-text available
The friendship paradox, initially discussed by Scott Feld in 1991, highlights a counterintuitive social phenomenon where individuals tend to have fewer friends than their friends do on average. The sociological implications of this paradox are profound, as it can create a distorted understanding of social norms and consequently influence beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors, particularly when highly connected individuals present a skewed representation of those norms. In essence, it can lead individuals to misjudge what is typical or desirable within their social circles. This study investigates the temporal dynamics of the friendship paradox using smartphone communication data from over 600 incoming freshmen at the University of Notre Dame participating in the NetHealth project. By tracking the friendship index– the ratio of an individual’s friends’ average number of friends to their own number of friends– over 119 days during the Fall semester of 2015, we examine how the paradox evolves over time. Our findings reveal that the friendship index stabilizes more rapidly than both the individuals’ own degree and the variation among their friends’ degrees. Results from the latent growth-curve model (LGCM) confirm that while the friendship index continues to increase, its growth rate declines over time. Moreover, the LGCM identifies individual degrees, ethnic backgrounds, and personality traits as influential factors shaping the manifestation and development of the friendship paradox. By exploring the mechanisms underlying this paradox in a dynamic communication network, this study enhances our understanding of the structural factors influencing the evolution of the friendship paradox in digitally mediated interactions. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s41109-025-00710-1.
Article
The formation of positive and negative relations between individuals in social networks can be described by different approaches. Two prominent mechanisms are structural balance and status hierarchies. Balance motivates stability among friends and enemies in triads (e.g., an enemy of my friend is my enemy). Status considers respect and disregard originating from social hierarchy (e.g., positive relations towards those we respect). We demonstrate that integrating the two mechanisms through the concept of ego dynamics is key to understanding observable patterns in many social groups. We propose an agent-based model where dynamical changes result from agents aiming to resolve inconsistencies with structural balance and status. In contrast to previous models, our approach employs the ego perspective. Agents have limited, local knowledge and can only change their own relations. By fitting the model to real-world networks, we successfully replicated the observed over- and under-representations of certain triads in 36 empirical signed networks. This close matching to empirical data is achievable only by taking the ego perspective and not assuming global knowledge. Additionally, the model reveals that, when the status mechanism dominates, people in real networks tend to strive for the top of the hierarchy. Finally, our numerical simulations and analytic solutions demonstrate that a previously thought as continuous phase transition towards the paradise state (all links positive) can become discontinuous when the status mechanism is involved. This discontinuity indicates that desirable social configurations may, in fact, be quite fragile.
Article
Full-text available
This paper examines social network size in contemporary Western society based on the exchange of Christmas cards. Maximum network size averaged 153.5 individuals, with a mean network size of 124.9 for those individuals explicitly contacted; these values are remarkably close to the group size of 150 predicted for humans on the basis of the size of their neocortex. Age, household type, and the relationship to the individual influence network structure, although the proportion of kin remained relatively constant at around 21%. Frequency of contact between network members was primarily determined by two classes of variable: passive factors (distance, work colleague, overseas) and active factors (emotional closeness, genetic relatedness). Controlling for the influence of passive factors on contact rates allowed the hierarchical structure of human social groups to be delimited. These findings suggest that there may be cognitive constraints on network size.
Article
Full-text available
Same-sex partners' familial expectations, including the division of household labor, are not well-understood in the existing research—research that largely references the experiences of white, middle- and upper-income lesbians who develop relationships with egalitarian goals. This article uses qualitative and survey research methods to evaluate the common view that two elements of feminist egalitarian ideology—economic independence and the equal distribution of housework and childrearing—are the defining features of lesbian-headed households. Analyses of 32 black women in lesbian stepfamilies suggest that partners share the providing role but biological mothers undertake significantly more household chores. More chore responsibility is used as a trade-off for greater authority over other aspects of household organization, including family finances and childrearing. The biological mother's control in a family is largely a function of her legal tie to a child and greater perceived responsibility for the child's well-being. Notably, this pattern persists even when she earns less than her mate. Without the gender structure of male privilege or the material advantage of high income, these families associate control over household labor with greater relationship power. These findings, while derived from analyses of a unique population, hold implications for broader conceptions of gender and power within families, the bases of interactional power, and the internal manifestations of power across various family and couple types.
Chapter
Models and Methods in Social Network Analysis, first published in 2005, presents the most important developments in quantitative models and methods for analyzing social network data that have appeared during the 1990s. Intended as a complement to Wasserman and Faust's Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications, it is a collection of articles by leading methodologists reviewing advances in their particular areas of network methods. Reviewed are advances in network measurement, network sampling, the analysis of centrality, positional analysis or blockmodelling, the analysis of diffusion through networks, the analysis of affiliation or 'two-mode' networks, the theory of random graphs, dependence graphs, exponential families of random graphs, the analysis of longitudinal network data, graphical techniques for exploring network data, and software for the analysis of social networks.
Article
In this essay I consider the nature of neighbourhoods in modern society, attempting to separate fact from fiction, and policy relevant theory from ideology. I discuss in particular the promises and perils of neighbourhood governance, with a special focus on public safety and community well being. My position is that neighbourhood and community do matter, but not for everything. I present a theory of collective efficacy and its implications for neighbourhood-based governance - what appears to work, what doesn't, and what might with proper guidance. Before I begin a little truth in advertising is in order. My background means that much of what I say is based on research from the United States. I fear this might be a handicap for a British audience, but then again it may provide a fresh perspective that has generalizable import. I also plead guilty to radical condensation and make no claims to be comprehensive. I specifically focus my attention on policy-related ideas that are (a) tied mainly to crime and public safety, and (b) do not rely excessively on formal mechanisms of control (for example mass incarceration) that may ultimately erode the foundations of social capital and lead to the de-legitimation of government institutions in disadvantaged communities.
Article
Social resources research has linked activated ties to outcomes-but not to the core networks from which the ties came. This study shifts the focus to the question of how networks allocate resources. The activation of core network ties is analyzed in a nonroutine situation-a hurricane-to determine how core network structure affects the degree to which individuals activate core network ties to gain one type of social resource-informal support. Results show that the structures of individuals' core networks affect the degree to which individuals activate ties from those networks to gain informal support. Individuals embedded in higher-density core networks (i.e., alters are connected to one another), core networks with more gender diversity (i.e., a mix of men and women), and networks that contain higher proportions of men, kin, and younger individuals, activated core network ties for informal support to a greater degree than did individuals embedded in core networks lacking these characteristics. The conclusions consider the study's implications for understanding resource activation in the contexts of social support and job searches.
Article
This paper examines bias toward status-similarity in adult friendships in Detroit and a West German city. Principles of meeting and “mating,” by which strangers are converted to acquaintances and acquaintances to friends, are stated. One of these, the proximity principle, claims that the more similar people are, the more likely they will meet and become friends. This principle is tested in matrices of friendship choice for twelve social characteristics. Two statistical measures of bias are used (odds ratio and marginal ratio) and their properties discussed. Compared to a random-choice model, adult friendships show strong bias toward status similarity for all social characteristics. Bias is strongest for “edge” categories of ranked statuses and for “best” friends. The less similar two people are in social characteristics, the less likely they are to be close friends. Demographic characteristics tend to show more bias toward homogeneous choices than other characteristics. These findings are explained and further analyses of adult friendship structure and dynamics are discussed.
Article
Journal of Democracy 6.1 (1995) 65-78 As featured on National Public Radio, The New York Times, and in other major media, we offer this sold-out, much-discussed Journal of Democracy article by Robert Putnam, "Bowling Alone." You can also find information at DemocracyNet about the Journal of Democracy and its sponsor, the National Endowment for Democracy. Many students of the new democracies that have emerged over the past decade and a half have emphasized the importance of a strong and active civil society to the consolidation of democracy. Especially with regard to the postcommunist countries, scholars and democratic activists alike have lamented the absence or obliteration of traditions of independent civic engagement and a widespread tendency toward passive reliance on the state. To those concerned with the weakness of civil societies in the developing or postcommunist world, the advanced Western democracies and above all the United States have typically been taken as models to be emulated. There is striking evidence, however, that the vibrancy of American civil society has notably declined over the past several decades. Ever since the publication of Alexis de Tocqueville's Democracy in America, the United States has played a central role in systematic studies of the links between democracy and civil society. Although this is in part because trends in American life are often regarded as harbingers of social modernization, it is also because America has traditionally been considered unusually "civic" (a reputation that, as we shall later see, has not been entirely unjustified). When Tocqueville visited the United States in the 1830s, it was the Americans' propensity for civic association that most impressed him as the key to their unprecedented ability to make democracy work. "Americans of all ages, all stations in life, and all types of disposition," he observed, "are forever forming associations. There are not only commercial and industrial associations in which all take part, but others of a thousand different types -- religious, moral, serious, futile, very general and very limited, immensely large and very minute. . . . Nothing, in my view, deserves more attention than the intellectual and moral associations in America." Recently, American social scientists of a neo-Tocquevillean bent have unearthed a wide range of empirical evidence that the quality of public life and the performance of social institutions (and not only in America) are indeed powerfully influenced by norms and networks of civic engagement. Researchers in such fields as education, urban poverty, unemployment, the control of crime and drug abuse, and even health have discovered that successful outcomes are more likely in civically engaged communities. Similarly, research on the varying economic attainments of different ethnic groups in the United States has demonstrated the importance of social bonds within each group. These results are consistent with research in a wide range of settings that demonstrates the vital importance of social networks for job placement and many other economic outcomes. Meanwhile, a seemingly unrelated body of research on the sociology of economic development has also focused attention on the role of social networks. Some of this work is situated in the developing countries, and some of it elucidates the peculiarly successful "network capitalism" of East Asia. Even in less exotic Western economies, however, researchers have discovered highly efficient, highly flexible "industrial districts" based on networks of collaboration among workers and small entrepreneurs. Far from being paleoindustrial anachronisms, these dense interpersonal and interorganizational networks undergird ultramodern industries, from the high tech of Silicon Valley to the high fashion of Benetton. The norms and networks of civic engagement also powerfully affect the performance of representative government. That, at least, was the central conclusion of my own 20-year, quasi-experimental study of subnational governments in different regions of Italy. Although all these regional governments seemed identical on paper, their levels of effectiveness varied dramatically. Systematic inquiry showed that the quality of governance was determined by longstanding traditions of civic engagement (or its absence). Voter turnout, newspaper readership, membership in choral societies and football clubs -- these were the hallmarks of a successful region. In fact, historical analysis suggested that these networks of organized reciprocity and civic solidarity...
Article
This article develops and tests an evolutionary model of the growth, decline, and demographic dynamics of voluntary organizations. The model demonstrates a strong analogy between the adaptive landscape of Sewall Wright (1931) and the exploitation surfaces generated by a model of member selection and retention for voluntary associations. The article connects the processes of membership recruitment and loss to the social networks connecting individuals. The model generates dynamic hypotheses about the time path of organizations in sociodemographic dimensions. A key idea in this model is that membership selection processes at the individual level produce adaptation in communities of organizations. The article concludes with an empirical example and some discussion of the implications of the model for a variety of research literatures.
Article
This paper summarizes and discusses three studies of patterns in the recall of persons in socially bounded communities. Individual sin three different communities (a graduate academic program, a religious fellowship, and a department in a formal organization) free-listed the names of persons in their respective communities. Results indicate that the individuals in each community share a common cognitive structure of community members that is based on the community's social structure. These studies, combined with the results of other research, strongly suggest that persons are organized in memory according to social structural principles and that affiliation and dominance are the principal dimensions of social cognition. Suggestions are offered for future research to test the generality of these findings.