ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Deviance and Dissent in Groups

Authors:

Abstract

Traditionally, group research has focused more on the motivations that make people conform than on the motivations and conditions underpinning deviance and dissent. This has led to a literature that focuses on the value that groups place on uniformity and paints a relatively dark picture of dissent and deviance: as reflections of a lack of group loyalty, as signs of disengagement, or as delinquent behavior. An alternative point of view, which has gained momentum in recent years, focuses on deviance and dissent as normal and healthy aspects of group life. In this review, we focus on the motivations that group members have to deviate and dissent, and the functional as well as the dysfunctional effects of deviance and dissent. In doing so we aim for a balanced and complete account of deviance and dissent, highlighting when such behaviors will be encouraged as well as when they will be punished. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology Volume 65 is January 03, 2014. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Deviance and Dissent
in Groups
Jolanda Jetten and Matthew J. Hornsey
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Queensland 4072, Australia;
email: j.jetten@psy.uq.edu.au
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014. 65:461–85
First published online as a Review in Advance on
June 7, 2013
The Annual Review of Psychology is online at
http://psych.annualreviews.org
This article’s doi:
10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115151
Copyright c
2014 by Annual Reviews.
All rights reserved
Keywords
group processes, black sheep effect, intragroup dynamics, social identity
Abstract
Traditionally, group research has focused more on the motivations that make
people conform than on the motivations and conditions underpinning de-
viance and dissent. This has led to a literature that focuses on the value that
groups place on uniformity and paints a relatively dark picture of dissent and
deviance: as reflections of a lack of group loyalty, as signs of disengagement,
or as delinquent behavior. An alternative point of view, which has gained
momentum in recent years, focuses on deviance and dissent as normal and
healthy aspects of group life. In this review, we focus on the motivations that
group members have to deviate and dissent, and the functional as well as
the dysfunctional effects of deviance and dissent. In doing so we aim for a
balanced and complete account of deviance and dissent, highlighting when
such behaviors will be encouraged as well as when they will be punished.
461
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
Click here for quick links to
Annual Reviews content online,
including:
• Other articles in this volume
• Top cited articles
• Top downloaded articles
• Our comprehensive search
Further
ANNUAL
REVIEWS
Watch a video lecture online
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Deviance: the
violation of the norms
of a group
Dissent:
the expression of
disagreement with
group norms, group
action, or a group
decision
Contents
INTRODUCTIONAND HISTORICALOVERVIEW ........................... 462
DEFINITIONSOF DEVIANCEAND DISSENT................................ 463
REASONSFOR DEVIANCEAND DISSENT.................................... 464
MOTIVESFOR REJECTINGDEVIANCE ANDDISSENT ..................... 467
RestoringThreatened GroupPositivity.......................................... 467
RestoringThreatened GroupCohesion.......................................... 468
RestoringThreatened GroupDistinctiveness .................................... 469
RestoringThreatened GroupLocomotion....................................... 469
Restoringa ThreatenedSelf-Image.............................................. 470
THEVALUE OFDISSENT ANDDEVIANCE .................................. 470
TheValue ofDissent........................................................... 471
TheValue ofDeviance ......................................................... 472
TOLERANCEFOR DISSENTAND DEVIANCE................................ 473
Tolerancefor Dissent........................................................... 473
Tolerancefor Deviance......................................................... 474
FACTORS THAT AFFECT GROUP TOLERANCE
OFDEVIANCE ANDDISSENT.............................................. 474
Qualitiesof theDissenter/Deviant............................................... 474
Qualitiesof theGroup.......................................................... 475
GroupTreatment ofthe Deviantor Dissenter................................... 476
StrategicFactors................................................................ 476
AN APPRECIATION FOR THE IMPORTANCE OF DEVIANCE AND
DISSENTIN GROUPS....................................................... 477
INTRODUCTION AND HISTORICAL OVERVIEW
Classic studies in social psychology have focused on the pervasiveness of conformity and pressures
for unanimity. For example, the message of Asch’s (1951) line judgment studies is that conformity is
a powerful and prevalent force in groups and that nonconformists risk mockery. Social psychologi-
cal theorizing argues that group members will see deviance and dissent as dangerous for the group;
that they threaten people’s worldview, create uncertainty, and rob groups of momentum (Festinger
1950). Consistent with this, there is now considerable evidence that deviants are more likely to be
rejected than are group members who conform (e.g., Marques & Paez 1994, Tata et al. 1996).
This classic view sometimes obscures the fact that deviance and dissent are also prevalent
phenomena within groups (Haslam & Reicher 2012a,b; Jetten & Hornsey 2011, 2012; Reicher &
Haslam 2006; see also Moscovici 1976). Even in Asch’s original paradigm, only 12% of participants
conformed on all trials—a larger percentage (24% of participants) never conformed (Asch 1951).
Conformity is not the default in groups, nor is dissent and deviance the exception. Rather, con-
formity (like dissent and deviance) is observed only under some conditions and in some contexts.
What is more, even though deviants and dissenters are at times perceived as troublemakers,
these individuals can also be the most admired members in the group, the people who liven up
the group and who contribute most to the achievement of group goals. In sum, a growing body
of literature argues that deviance and dissent—although potentially stressful—are normal and
healthy aspects of group life and are often recognized as such by group members.
462 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Group norms: beliefs
about how group
members should act
within the group
Positive deviants:
individuals who
deviate by
contributing more to
the group than the
average group member
In this review, we start by defining our key terms and then elaborate on various motives that
group members might have for engaging in deviance and dissent. We then review work examining
reactions of other group members to deviance and dissent; focus on the value of and tolerance
for deviance and dissent for the group; and review the conditions under which deviance and
dissent are more or less likely to be punished by others. In our review, we focus mainly on work
that has been published in the past 20 years. We only refer to classic work when it is important to
understand the origin of ideas and when such background is needed to gain an appreciation of how
the field has developed. For reviews of earlier work, we refer the reader to Levine (1989), Levine
& Kerr (2007), Moscovici (1976), and Turner (1991). We also confine our review to deviance
or dissent in relation to relevant group norms. Therefore, we do not use the word “deviance”
to refer to criminality, delinquency, or other stigmatized behavior in society (an approach that
is quite common in sociology) and do not review the associated literature on this. Neither do
we review research on personal or interpersonal forms of negative and harmful behavior (e.g.,
bullying, aggression).
DEFINITIONS OF DEVIANCE AND DISSENT
We define deviance as the violation of the norms of a group. As such, the likelihood that behavior
will be labeled as deviant is always determined in relation to (a) the content of a group norm that
is salient and (b) the contexts in which deviance and dissent are expressed. This means that the
same behavior can be perceived both as deviant and normative depending on the group norm and
context. For example, for boxers, hitting someone will be normative during a boxing match but
not after the match has finished.
What is considered deviant may also change with time. Group norms are dynamic, responsive
to contextual changes, and in constant flux. Group members negotiate the content of norms that
should guide behavior and may contest their validity when they are no longer seen as appropriate for
the group. As a result, behavior that was previously perceived as deviant might become conformist
when the majority changes its stance and other behaviors become normative (Blanton & Christie
2003, Chan et al. 2010). In the words of the sociologist Merton: “...the rebel, revolutionary,
nonconformist, individualist, heretic and renegade of an earlier time is often the culture hero of
today” (1968, p. 237).
Having said that, in social psychological research certain behaviors are regularly interpreted as
deviant in group contexts, and these tend to form the basis for operationalizations of deviance in
the literature. For example, deviance has been operationalized as possessing qualities or attitudes
that differ from the prototype (Hutchison et al. 2011), poor performance in a group task (Marques
& Paez 1994), disloyalty (Branscombe et al. 1993), engaging in socially undesirable or antisocial
behavior (see, e.g., Marques et al. 2001) or arguing against group interests (see, e.g., Castano
et al. 2002).
When we define deviance as the violation of group norms, we need to be open to the possibility
that norms can be violated in two ways: Individuals can fail to live up to important group norms
or reject group norms (i.e., negative deviants), or individuals can deviate by contributing more to
the group than the average group member (i.e., positive deviants). The latter deviants stand out
from the group in an objectively positive way, for example by evincing exceptional performance
or exceptional integrity (Fielding et al. 2006). Interestingly, and as we outline in greater detail
below, it is not the case that acceptance of these positive deviants is necessarily greater than the
acceptance of negative deviants.
We define dissent as the expression of disagreement with group norms, group action, or a
group decision. In many cases dissent can be considered an example of deviance, which is why
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 463
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Table 1 Reasons for deviance and dissent in groups
Motivations for deviance and dissent
Motive 1: disengagement, disloyalty, or disrespect for the group
Motive 2: loyalty and concern for the group
Motive 3: moral rebellion—when personal moral convictions take precedence over group norms
Motive 4: desire to express difference, individuality, and uniqueness
Motive 5: tangible rewards and instrumental gain derived from dissent and deviance
deviance and dissent are often referred to in the same breath. But not all groups would see dissent
as synonymous with deviance. For example, in academia, being critical and challenging of the
status quo is collectively encouraged, and failure to engage in critical dissent might be seen as
deviant (Hornsey et al. 2006, McAuliffe et al. 2002). Out of respect for this conceptual difference
we avoid using the terms deviance and dissent interchangeably, although we acknowledge that
they are related.
REASONS FOR DEVIANCE AND DISSENT
What makes people conform, and what makes them defy group pressure? Traditionally, group
members are seen to conform for two reasons (Deutsch & Gerard 1955). First, in the face of
uncertainty, people look to others as a guide to reality and as a guide to the appropriate way
to behave (that is, they see others as a source of informational influence). Second, people may
conform so they fit in, obtain approval from others, or avoid punishment and social isolation
(normative influence).
The social identity approach (comprising social identity theory and self-categorization theory)
points to the importance of group identification in understanding conformity (Turner 1991).
Conformity to norms signifies inclusion in the group, makes members feel good about their
group membership, and is a way to express loyalty and commitment (Louis et al. 2005, Sassenberg
et al. 2011). In particular, those who are strongly identified with a group will be motivated to
conform to norms even when such conformity clashes with their own personal interests (Zdaniuk
& Levine 2001).
Because most research has focused on explaining conformity, far less is known about the motives
that might lead people to deviate or dissent from groups.1In Table 1 we identify five reasons why
people might seek to engage in deviance or dissent. Motive 1 captures the implicit assumption
of the majority of the literature: Because it is assumed that conformity is a sign of loyalty, then
deviance and dissent are seen to be motivated by disloyalty or disengagement (Blanton & Christie
2003). Indeed, those who are less committed to the group have been found to conform less to
salient group norms than those who are more committed (Spears et al. 1997). Packer (2008) has
argued that low identifiers’ behavior can best be described as passive nonconformity: Those who
are less committed to the group do not care about group performance or group functioning, and
their disinterest manifests itself as withdrawal, noncompliance, and indifference to group norms.
Others have focused on more sinister motives of deviants and dissenters and have associated their
1Of course not all deviance is motivated: Sometimes people deviate simply because they do not have the ability or awareness to
conform to normative requirements (Monin & O’Connor 2011). For example, newcomers may not be sufficiently socialized
yet to know the group norms or they may not have acquired the skills to conform. Because of space constraints, we do not
discuss these examples of deviance and dissent here.
464 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Constructive
deviance: deviance or
dissent motivated by a
desire to attempt to
change group norms
for the better
Moral rebels:
individuals who give
priority to following
personal convictions
over group norms
when individual and
group norms clash
actions with a motivation to harm or sabotage the group (Warren 2003). In the organizational
literature, it has been noted that employees may deviate from norms because they are dissatisfied
with the organization, angry about a perceived injustice, or because they enjoy the thrill associated
with rebellion (Christian & Ellis 2013, Ferris et al. 2012). In some of those cases, the deviant’s rule
violation is deliberate, aimed at undermining the group’s well-being and functioning (Bennett &
Robinson 2000). These motives are particularly relevant when explaining deviance and dissent
among low identifiers (Cronin & Smith 2011).
The conflating of conformity with loyalty—and of nonconformity with disloyalty—is so en-
trenched in the literature that it sometimes obscures the fact that deviance and dissent can be
multiply determined. Motive 2 describes the other side of the coin of Motive 1: that dissent and
deviance can be motivated by group loyalty. That is, group members may dissent because they care
for the group and are concerned about the course of action that other group members are taking.
Dissent is then motivated by an attempt to change group norms for the better, a phenomenon
that has been variously described as “constructive deviance” (e.g., Galperin 2012) or “constructive
patriotism” (Shatz et al. 1999).
The normative conflict model of dissent (Packer 2008, 2011) speaks to this motive. The model
focuses on how those with lower and higher levels of identification with the group respond to
normative conflict (i.e., a discrepancy between current group norms and a group member’s per-
ception of what is right or moral). This model suggests that both lower and higher identifiers
might be motivated to dissent, but for different reasons. Whereas those who are less committed
to the group dissent or deviate because they disengage from group goals (i.e., Motive 1), highly
identified group members dissent because they care about the group. In other words, their dissent
is in the service of the group, aimed at changing group norms that they perceive as detrimental.
Because dissent by low and high identifiers is motivated by different concerns, the way in
which dissent is expressed will also differ. Low identifiers are more likely to engage in actions
that undermine the group (e.g., they might withdraw or aim to exit the group), whereas high
identifiers’ dissent reflects group engagement and attempts to improve the group. As we elaborate
below, because dissent is expressed in such different ways and motivated by different concerns, it
is also likely to be responded to differently by other group members.
A growing body of work provides support for predictions derived from the normative con-
flict model of dissent (Crane & Platow 2010, Packer 2009, Packer & Chasteen 2010, Tauber &
Sassenberg 2012; for a review, see Packer 2011). These studies typically involve designs in which
group identification is measured and normative conflict is measured or manipulated. When nor-
mative conflict is measured, participants are asked to what extent they find a particular group norm
harmful for the group (Packer 2009). When normative conflict is manipulated, participants are
typically presented with contexts in which group members engage in behavior or hold attitudes
that are harmful for the group. For example, among students, normative conflict arises when they
reflect on the negative health consequences of binge drinking (Packer & Chasteen 2010). In other
conditions, participants are asked to consider the extent to which engagement in these negative
behaviors might harm them personally. Consistent with the model, dissent is higher among those
who are more strongly committed to the group when participants perceive harmful consequences
for the collective (but not when considering personal harm). In contrast, perceptions of collective
harm do not affect the responses of those lower in identification, and levels of dissent among these
members have been found to be relatively low.
Motive 3 describes the fact that people often have moral convictions that prevent them from
following and acting in line with group norms (“moral rebels,” see Monin et al. 2008). For example,
people might resile from expressing racist comments even though the group demands this, or
they might resist harming someone else because that would be inconsistent with personal moral
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 465
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
norms. By way of illustration, participants in Milgram’s obedience studies who refused to go to the
highest shock levels often referred to moral norms that prevented them from continuing: “I’d like
to continue, but can’t do that to a man” or “I can’t go on with this; no, this is not right” (Milgram
1963, p. 376). For these individuals, nonconformity was less a choice than a moral imperative.
Consistent with this, subsequent work shows that those with moral convictions are less likely
to be swayed by group pressure. For example, in two studies examining attitudes toward gay law
reform and toward a government apology to Aborigines for past wrongdoing, those who had a
strong moral basis for their attitudes were unaffected by the group norm, an effect that was in-
dependent of attitude intensity (Hornsey et al. 2003). Indeed, on public behavioral intentions,
those with a strong moral basis for their attitude were more likely to act in line with their personal
conviction when they felt they were in a minority than when they felt they were in a majority
(counter-conformity). Along similar lines, Shamir (1997) found that prevailing government opin-
ion did not affect Israelis’ willingness to express their opinion about the Palestinian territories.
Presumably this was because the issue was of such personal relevance that concerns about social
isolation or punishment for dissenting became irrelevant.
Motive 4 relates to the notion that group members might dissent and deviate because they want
to express individuality and uniqueness (Imhoff & Erb 2009). In this sense we agree with Codol
(1984, p. 317), who argued that “both conformity and resistance to conformity are fundamentally
linked to the image of oneself that one wishes to present to others (and undoubtedly also to
oneself).” In some ways, deviance and dissent are ideally suited to help define and clarify that
image. Blanton and colleagues (2001) found evidence for this in a study where participants were
presented with a social influence attempt (e.g., that it would be a good idea for all students on
campus to receive a flu shot). When the behavior was described as the right thing to do, students’
intentions to get a flu shot were higher when such behavior was counternormative (i.e., only a
minority of students on campus were expected to get a flu shot) than when such behavior was
perceived as normative for the group (i.e., it was anticipated that the majority of students on
campus would get a flu shot). Consistent with these findings, other research has found that people
can endorse a minority position (rather than a majority position) as a way of clarifying their self-
concept and communicating to others who they are (Rios Morrison & Wheeler 2010). Particularly
when publicly expressed, dissent and deviance might be good ways to enhance individuality and
uniqueness in group contexts (Blanton & Christie 2003).
The desire to stand out and be different may be especially pronounced when people attribute
the normative consensus to obedience. Conway & Schaller (2005) found that, when an authority
explicitly commanded group members to make a particular choice, this enhanced participants’
desire to deviate from that group norm (and especially when the authority was still present). The
principles of reactance—and the associated desire for control and authenticity of expression—can
drive a perverse desire for deviance in precisely the conditions when the explicit pressure for
conformity is greatest.
At times the desire to seek out distinctiveness leads an individual to adopt norms, values, and
lifestyles that run counter to the mainstream. For example, Jetten and colleagues (2001) found that
the motivation to shock others and to differentiate oneself from the mainstream was an important
reason for having body piercings. Interestingly, subcultures often emerge whereby all those who
want to be different express their difference from the mainstream in a similar way. That is, being
different and standing out with the aim of upsetting the mainstream may involve the development
of new subcultural norms strictly prescribing how one should rebel (Hornsey & Jetten 2004).
Finally, people might engage in deviance and dissent because norm violations are associated
with tangible rewards, making the costs of punishment less of a deterrent (Motive 5). The organi-
zational literature in particular has focused on this motive for deviance: Employees might engage
466 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Group locomotion:
the ability of a group
to achieve its goals
in fraud, steal from the company, or commit crimes because these behaviors are associated with
financial gain and success (Bennett & Robinson 2000, Warren 2003).
Another literature that has focused on instrumental gain as a motive for deviance is work on
impostorism (Hornsey & Jetten 2003, 2011; Warner et al. 2007). Impostorism is a particular form
of deviance and involves individuals breaking group norms by passing themselves off as genuine
group members even though they do not meet key criteria for group membership. Impostorism
allows people to cross what are normally impermeable group boundaries (e.g., gender, race, or
class) and is a way to obtain access to groups, professions, or classes that could not be legitimately
claimed otherwise. By resorting to impostorism, people are able to escape prejudice, can run away
from persecution, can gain social acceptance, or can pass themselves off as more successful than
they really are. For example, to avoid exclusion and prejudice, gays and lesbians may hide their
sexual orientation (Warner et al. 2007), and those who are HIV positive might not reveal their
illness (Molero et al. 2011).
MOTIVES FOR REJECTING DEVIANCE AND DISSENT
In this section we review literature that has examined the motives for rejecting deviants and
dissenters, both for individual group members and for the group as a whole. We identify five
distinct motives: rejection of deviance and dissent helps to restore (a) threatened group positivity,
(b) threatened group cohesion, (c) threatened group distinctiveness, (d) threatened group locomo-
tion, and (e) threatened self-image. We discuss these motivations and recent empirical support for
them in turn.
Restoring Threatened Group Positivity
The notion that deviance and dissent threaten the positivity of the group—and that rejection
of these individuals serves the function of restoring positivity—dominates recent empirical work
(e.g., Castano et al. 2002, Hutchison & Abrams 2003). Indeed, this reasoning is at the heart of
work on the so-called black sheep effect (Marques et al. 2001). This work centers on two interre-
lated observations. First, a favorable in-group member (e.g., someone who is likeable, qualified,
loyal, attractive) is rated more positively than an out-group member who engages in similarly
favorable behavior. Second, the opposite effect occurs when group members are confronted by an
unlikeable, incompetent, or disloyal in-group member, such that the deviant in-group member is
rated more harshly by fellow group members than is an out-group member who engages in the
same behavior. Heightened derogation of the in-group deviant is seen as a form of in-group en-
hancement, distancing the group from qualities that reflect poorly upon it (Hutchison & Abrams
2003, Marques & Paez 1994).
Two studies by Hutchison and colleagues (2008) provide direct evidence that devaluation serves
the function of identity maintenance and protection. Among highly identified group members,
descriptions of the in-group were more positive the more these members excluded an undesirable
target. Reinforcing the notion that rejection of black sheep serves an identity-enhancing function,
making an intergroup context salient increases the derogation of in-group deviants and dissenters
(Chekroun & Nugier 2011, Matheson et al. 2003). Similarly, people who criticized their groups
faced heightened censure if they made their comments in public or directly to an out-group
audience (Elder et al. 2005, Hornsey et al. 2005). It has also been found that deviants are more
likely to be derogated (a) when the deviant behavior relates to a dimension of comparison that
is directly relevant to in-group identity (Abrams et al. 2008b), (b) when the group identity is
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 467
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
threatened (Branscombe et al. 1993), or (c) when the group’s position is unstable or challenged
(Marques et al. 2001).
In addition to the protection of group identity, derogation of in-group members can also serve
the function of protecting the threatened identities of individual group members. Eidelman &
Biernat (2003) showed that group members lower their identification with the group after being
presented with an in-group deviant. In that way, they dissociate and distance themselves from
these deviants and limit the threat of being associatively miscast with the unfavorable in-group
member. Chekroun & Nugier (2011) showed that rejection of in-group deviants was positively
correlated with the level of shame and embarrassment the deviant invoked.
One might wonder whether the fear that deviants and dissenters damage the positive image of
the group is real or imagined. This question has not been examined in great depth, but there is
some evidence from the stereotyping literature that the more typical of the in-group the deviant
is perceived to be, the more the image of the in-group is negatively affected by the deviant’s
presence (Castano et al. 2002). There is also evidence that deviants in other groups are perceived
as harmful to those groups. For example, Kunda & Oleson (1997) found that an out-group with
an extreme deviant in its midst was seen as less favorable than a group that included only moderate
deviants.
Restoring Threatened Group Cohesion
The above research speaks to the fact that group members are motivated to see their group in a
positive light and so are especially keen to rid the group of members who behave in a dislikeable
or incompetent way. But group members are not just motivated to see their group as positive; they
also are motivated to see their group as tight, well defined, and cohesive. One reason for this is that
cohesive groups offer certainty and structure about what to think and how to behave (Festinger
1950). Thus, censuring of deviance and dissent can be a way of protecting the subjective validity
of one’s attitudes, beliefs, and worldviews. In extreme cases, divergent attitudes may threaten the
very definition of the group, leading to internecine conflict or schisms (Sani & Reicher 2000).
Another reason why cohesion is important is because it offers resilience in the intergroup
context. Classic work on minority influence makes the point that consistency and cohesion within
minority groups are important preconditions for converting the majority and changing the status
quo (Moscovici 1976). Minority groups that are fractured or riven with internal dissent are less
likely to challenge the majority’s worldview—or indeed to hold together. This might help explain
meta-analytical evidence that rejection of deviance increases as the proportionate size of the
group decreases (Tata et al. 1996).
Similarly, Janis (1972) makes the point that pressure for cohesion and unanimity is particularly
strong when the group faces threat from the outside (e.g., media scrutiny, budgetary crisis, or
armed conflict). Indeed, external pressures and excessive cohesion are two important ingredients
of the phenomenon of groupthink, one symptom of which is the active censuring of dissenters
by self-appointed “mindguards.” It is easy to recognize this phenomenon in historical events,
for example the normative pressure to silence dissent within the United States and its allies
following the two wars with Iraq (Kelman 1995). It can also be detected experimentally: Muslims
who criticized their religion in Indonesia faced significantly more personal censure from other
Muslims when inter-religious conflict was primed than when it was not (Ariyanto et al. 2010).
Furthermore, small-group experiments have found that intergroup conflict increases enforcement
of in-group norms, particularly when out-group participation in the conflict is high (Benard
2012).
468 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Restoring Threatened Group Distinctiveness
Deviants and dissenters not only threaten within-group cohesion, their actions may also undermine
between-group distinctiveness. That is, deviants’ norm breaking might mean that their behavior
is more representative of a rival out-group than of their own group, thus reducing the clarity of
in-group boundaries. Robbed of intergroup distinctiveness, groups can become fuzzy and poorly
defined, diluting their ability to provide meaning and self-definition for their members.
This principle is captured by the model of subjective group dynamics (Abrams et al. 2000),
which was established to help account for the black sheep effect described previously. According
to the subjective group dynamics model, people evaluate group members’ behavior as a function
of whether they validate or undermine prescriptive and descriptive group norms, and interactions
with deviants are shaped by the desire to enhance or maintain this subjective validity (Frings et al.
2012). Importantly, actions and attitudes are not evaluated in isolation but rather in the context
of a salient intergroup comparison. Consistent with this principle, there is now a large body of
work showing that deviants who exaggerate intergroup differences are liked more than deviants
who dilute them, even when the severity of their group norm violations is identical (e.g., Abrams
et al. 2000, 2002; Hichy et al. 2008).
Distinctiveness from relevant out-groups might be particularly important for minority groups.
One reason for this is that people use group solidarity as a buffer against the self-esteem conse-
quences of stigma. Thus, distinctiveness helps provide the kind of certainty and definition that can
nourish the self-concept and provide meaning. Another reason that distinctiveness might be im-
portant for minority groups is that it offers the psychological platform from which groups can work
to change the status quo. Work on impostorism in the gay community confirms the importance
of distinctiveness for minority groups. Warner and colleagues (2007) presented gay participants
with one of two targets who turned out to be impostors: a straight person trying to pass as gay,
and a gay person trying to pass as straight. Even though both impostors were disliked more than
control targets who had not resorted to impostorism, mediational analysis showed that they were
derogated for different reasons. Gay targets who tried to pass as straight were rated as damaging
because they were seen to be ashamed about their sexuality. In contrast, a straight person trying
to pass as gay was seen to be blurring the boundaries between the in-group and the out-group,
and this was associated with greater perceived damage to the group. Schoemann & Branscombe
(2011) also found that distinctiveness threat underpinned negative reactions to older people who
try to “pass” as young.
Restoring Threatened Group Locomotion
A fourth motive for derogating deviants and dissenters is that these individuals may cause emo-
tional, cognitive, and communicative stress, undermining the ability of group to achieve its goals
[what Festinger (1950) described as group locomotion]. Dealing with dissent and divergent opin-
ions can be emotionally and cognitively taxing for a group. For example, when confronted with
a deviant group member, those who strongly identified with the group showed reduced recall
on an unrelated memory task (Coull et al. 2001). Presumably, cognitive resources are needed to
cognitively “fence off” deviant members from the rest of the group.
When facing other stressors, groups can strive for premature closure whereby they seek con-
sensus simply to avoid being exhausted by dissent (Kruglanski & Webster 1996). Dissenters’ and
deviants’ actions are met with frustration when they slow the group down and prevent it from
achieving its goals. Derogation then serves the function of excluding those who stand in the way of
goal achievement. In line with this reasoning, Kruglanski & Webster (1991) found that dissenters
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 469
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
were evaluated more harshly when the group faced deadline pressure or when group members
were experiencing cognitive load (e.g., the discussion took place in a noisy environment).
Restoring a Threatened Self-Image
In the research on black sheep and subjective group dynamics, attitudinal deviance tended to be
operationalized on the basis of relatively neutral, nonmoral issues: Participants would be told, for
example, that participants were deviant in their attitudes about student accommodation (Abrams
et al. 2002) or their styles of imagination (Marques et al. 2001). But what might happen if the
attitudinal deviance has a clear moral flavor? This is what Monin would call a moral rebel: someone
who stands up and takes a conspicuous stance in opposition to a norm, expectation, or convention
that they perceive to be immoral. Images of the moral rebel are often heroic and noble: Nelson
Mandela’s resistance to apartheid; those who participated in the civil rights movement; and the
“unknown rebel” who stood in the path of the tanks during the Tiananmen Square massacre
(Wolf & Zuckerman 2012). Surely this subset of deviants would be received more positively by
rank-and-file group members.
Research by Monin and colleagues suggests otherwise (Monin & O’Connor 2011, Monin et al.
2008). As an example of the type of paradigm they used, participants were asked to take part in
a role-playing game that could be interpreted as racist. Somewhat dramatically, a confederate
refused to do the experiment on the grounds that it was offensive. Some of the participants were
third-party observers who witnessed the event but were doing an unrelated task in the experiment
and so were not personally implicated in the moral decision. These participants tended to reward
the moral rebel, describing him as more moral, likable, and respected than an obedient participant
who participated in the role-play. This effect, however, was reversed for those participants who
had previously engaged in the task themselves.
Monin and colleagues concluded that moral rebellion represents a threat to group members
on three fronts: (a) The rebel’s moral stance is seen as an implicit criticism of those who did not
take the stance, so group members anticipate condemnation from the rebel; (b)theactionsofthe
rebel make you question your own assumptions and attitudes, leading to a dissonance-like state;
and (c) the rebel strips those of us who conspire in immoral acts from the rationalization that we
had no choice. By reminding us of our freedom to act, the rebel makes us confront our own past
actions, leading to an existential crisis. In each case, the moral rebel arouses resentment, and a
solution to the threat posed by rebels is to derogate them and to deny that they are moral at all.
The moral rebel can be seen as an example of a broader category: the so-called positive deviant
(Fielding et al. 2006). Positive deviants can be constructive role models who trigger positive
change (e.g., Kraschnewski et al. 2011, Ma & Magnus 2012). However, in some contexts people
who perform exceptionally well can also face jealousy and resentment: They might be pressured to
reduce their performance, and their failures might elicit schadenfreude (Feather & Sherman 2002,
Shoenberger et al. 2012). Parks & Stone (2010) found that group members who were especially
unselfish in their behavior were seen by some as setting an undesirable behavioral standard and
were downgraded accordingly.
THE VALUE OF DISSENT AND DEVIANCE
Even though social psychologists traditionally have focused on the threatening and dysfunctional
aspects of dissent and deviance, there is now a growing body of theorizing and empirical work
elaborating how these individuals’ actions can leave a positive legacy. Here, we focus on evidence
that dissent increases the quality of group decision making and how it is essential for creativity,
470 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
innovation, and learning. We also discuss how deviance can sharpen the group’s understanding
of its values and how it can facilitate social change.
The Value of Dissent
The notion that dissent is essential for good group decision making is well developed. Research on
minority influence (De Dreu & West 2001, Nemeth 1995, Nemeth & Goncalo 2011; for a meta-
analytical overview, see Wood et al. 1994) has shown that dissenting minorities exert influence on
the group as a whole because they force the majority to think outside the box. It is also clear from
this body of work that minorities influence the group in a different way than do majorities. The
influence of minorities is indirect, delayed, and not always visible—when these group members
are influential, change is often not attributed to their influence. The value of opinion minorities
lies in their ability to guard the group against complacency, to challenge conventional wisdom,
and to keep the group sharp and on its toes. In that way, dissenting minorities can change norms or
behavioral conventions that have lost their utility. This resonates with classic work on groupthink
( Janis 1972), which demonstrated the dangers of having too much convergence and agreement in
the group, especially in the early stages of group decision making. The presence of dissent might
lead group members to tackle the faults of their group, and the dissenters’ critical stance protects
the group from suboptimal decision making (Packer 2009). Accordingly, there is evidence that
groups that include devil’s advocates make higher-quality decisions (Nemeth et al. 2001) and are
less prone to classic decision-making mistakes such as escalation of commitment in the face of
losses (Greitemeyer et al. 2009).
Dissenting minorities promote higher-quality decisions because they change the way groups
think and process information. Researchers working from within the minority influence literature
have shown that majorities instigate a narrowing of focus and convergent thinking (Nemeth
1995). This is because group members confronted with a majority position are mostly motivated
to corroborate existing views: They believe that because many people in a group say this, they must
be right (consensus implies correctness). However, minorities who express views that dissent from
the majority are more likely to stimulate divergent thinking, as group members strive to understand
why the minority is dissenting. Divergence of opinion also leads to dissonance in groups, and this
leads to greater attention to the message, the consideration of multiple perspectives, a reassessment
of what the issues are, and a motivation to reduce the negative tension by seeking a new group
consensus (Matz & Wood 2005). This work has led some to argue that groups would benefit from
adopting norms of critical dissent. For example, Postmes et al. (2001) experimentally induced
norms encouraging group members either to seek consensus or to engage critically with the
available information. Groups with critical norms were less likely to fall in the trap of focusing on
shared information at the expense of unshared information, and they were less likely to display
signs of groupthink than were groups with consensus norms. This, in turn, was associated with
improved decision quality.
Related to the impact of dissent on the quality of decision making, there is also evidence that
dissent—because it stimulates divergent thinking—enhances creativity and innovation in groups
(De Dreu & West 2001, Nemeth et al. 2001, Troyer & Youngreen 2009, Van Dyne & Saavedra
1996). For example, instructions encouraging criticism and debate within a group lead to more
creative ideas being generated than when groups are instructed to brainstorm (Nemeth et al.
2004), although this link may emerge only in groups where there is a high degree of participation
in team decision making (De Dreu & West 2001).
Van Dyne & Saavedra (1996) conducted a 10-week field experiment among natural work
groups. They also found evidence that groups including confederates who were instructed to
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 471
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
engage in minority influence developed more original products than did control groups. Interest-
ingly, they did not find any evidence that there was more conflict in the minority influence group
than in the control condition. This suggests that expressing divergent opinions or even dissent is
not necessarily associated with enhanced tension (see also Wood et al. 1994).
There is also evidence that dissent can be beneficial in educational contexts. When working on a
task, dissent with one or several others can promote quality of learning (Butera et al. 2011). This is
because dissent and exposure to disconfirming information and opposing arguments may stimulate
cognitive activity. This might be beneficial for learning because it forces people to critically reflect
on the validity of their views and because it enhances openness to other viewpoints.
However, dissent is not always beneficial. Darnon et al. (2007) found that only in contexts
where dissent is regulated in an epistemic manner (i.e., dissent and conflict relate to the learning
task) did conflict enhance learning outcomes. When conflict was introduced among learners that
related to their performance (i.e., the participant’s competence was questioned), self-threat was
enhanced and learning was reduced.
It is also important to keep in mind that dissent does not always lead to better performance
in groups. At times, the presence of dissenters in the group is associated with conflict, and this
undermines task focus and performance (Butera et al. 2011, De Dreu 2006). There is a fine balance
between dissent that can help a group to move forward and dissent that undermines harmony and
group functioning ( Jehn et al. 1999). For example, Dooley & Fryxell (1999) found that dissent was
associated with better decision making only when group members were committed to the group
and therefore wanted the group to do well.
The Value of Deviance
Although recent social psychological theorizing has made important contributions to understand-
ing the value of dissent (see previous section), it has very little to say on the value of deviance
within groups. In order to develop a better understanding of why and how group deviants can
be beneficial for the group, it is instructive to consider sociological work on deviance in groups
(Durkheim 1958, Erikson 1966; see also Ben-Yehuda 2010). At the origin of this theorizing lies
the work of Durkheim (1958), who argued for the normality and functionality of deviance.
Durkheim (1958) identified several ways in which groups benefit from having deviants in their
midst. First, deviants have an important role to play in affirming group values, clarifying norms,
and in helping group members to understand how they are different and distinct from other
groups (for a similar point in the organizational context, see Markova & Folger 2012). That is,
group members know what is the right thing to do by knowing what is normatively deviant and
unacceptable (to be “good” makes sense only in relation to being “bad”; Coser 1962, p. 174).
Interestingly, it is clear from this work that it is not only through rejection of the deviant that
norms and values become affirmed and strengthened. Indeed, deviance also serves this function
when the deviant is not excluded but rather is accepted by the group. That is, by tolerating a
deviant or dissenter, group members can show that they act in accordance with their beliefs and
values, and this strengthens the social fabric of the group (Coser 1962).
Second, deviance is functional and valuable for groups because it draws attention to alternative
forms of behavior and thereby allows for social change (Choi & Levine 2004, Ellemers & Jetten
2013, Hansen & Levine 2009, Prislin & Christensen 2005, Prislin & Filson 2009). For example,
Rosa Parks’s refusal to give up her bus seat to a white passenger was an important catalyst for the
civil rights movement, ultimately leading to the abolishment of segregation laws in the United
States. By drawing attention to different viewpoints, by exposing group norms through the process
of violating them, by challenging conventional wisdoms, and by creating conflict, tension and
472 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
debate, deviants are able to move a group forward (see also Moscovici 1976). As Durkheim explains:
“It would never have been possible to establish the freedom of thought we now enjoy if the
regulations prohibiting it had not been violated before being solemnly abrogated” (1958, p. 67).
TOLERANCE FOR DISSENT AND DEVIANCE
Given the positive contributions of dissenters and deviants to the group, it is not surprising
that groups are often remarkably tolerant of these individuals. Indeed, there are many examples
where group members treat dissenters and deviants with respect and encourage them to continue
to express their views because they contribute in these positive ways to the functioning of the
group (Ellemers & Jetten 2013). The importance of dissent is increasingly recognized among
management theorists (Amabile 1996), who have turned their attention to the microstrategies that
organizations can employ to encourage dissent and fearless expression. Mindful of the benefits of
dissent, some groups might create informal roles within their ranks that free people up to engage
in dissent. An example is the role of court jesters in the Middle Ages who, perhaps because of
their marginal and nonthreatening position, were the only ones who were licensed to openly raise
unpleasant truths in the presence of the king or queen. A modern equivalent is the role of the
devil’s advocate, whose purpose is to question the group’s functioning in order to understand the
organization’s weaknesses. In this section, we review research that has examined tolerance for
dissenters and deviants.
Tolerance for Dissent
Contrary to the assumed wisdom that dissenters face personal censure, there is a growing body of
work showing that groups can be surprisingly accepting of dissenters within their ranks (Esposo
et al. 2013, Hiew & Hornsey 2010, Hornsey et al. 2002, Rabinovich & Morton 2010), and at
times dissenters are even liked more than other group members (Van Dyne & Saavedra 1996). In
work on group-directed criticism, group members are presented with either an in-group member
or an outsider criticizing their group (e.g., Hornsey et al. 2002, Rabinovich & Morton 2010).
These studies consistently show that in-group members criticizing the group are downgraded less
strongly than outsiders who make exactly the same comments (the intergroup sensitivity effect).
A critical message also arouses less emotional sensitivity and is agreed with more strongly when it
is delivered by an in-group member. Importantly, the greater acceptance of the in-group message
over the out-group message does not emerge when people are presented with positive or neutral
statements about the group; the effect seems to be specific to criticisms. Furthermore, evaluations
of in-group critics are often comparable to evaluations of in-group members who make positive
or neutral comments about the group (Hornsey et al. 2002).
This relative tolerance toward internal critics appears to be rooted in the relatively generous
attributions that group members make about their motives. When in-group members criticize the
group, they are more likely to be seen to be doing so because they care for the group and want
to create constructive change. Darker motivations are assumed to drive criticisms by out-group
members—it is assumed they criticize because they intend to undermine, harm, and weaken the
group—and it is this attributional bias that mediates the intergroup sensitivity effect (Hornsey &
Imani 2004). The work on responses to in-group criticism makes clear that deviance and dissent
are not unwelcome by definition. The fact that group members are interested in what a dissenter
has to say, and that they consider at length why the dissenter would be saying it, goes against
traditional views that have portrayed groups as reflexively intolerant and dogmatic (for a similar
point, see Spears 2010).
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 473
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
However, even though there is some evidence that dissenters are tolerated and respected by
other group members, this does not necessarily mean that dissenters are always liked and admired
by others, even when they bring objective value to the group. Rink & Ellemers (2009) showed that
newcomers were more involved in the group task, contributed more unique information during a
decision-making task, and impacted more positively on group performance than did old-timers.
However, newcomers were also less accepted than old-timers, and their presence dampened group
identification levels for all group members. Sometimes the presence of a dissenter can lead to an
ironic discrepancy between subjective and objective reality: Nemeth & Ormiston (2007) found that
although observers report more innovation and better decision making in groups with dissenters,
group members themselves reported lower innovation, cohesion, and task satisfaction (see also
Rijnbout & McKimmie 2012a).
Tolerance for Deviance
Groups may also be remarkably tolerant of deviants. Sociologists have even suggested that a
group’s solidarity benefits from tolerating the deviant because it shows the strength of the group
to deal with a plurality of views and behaviors (Coser 1962, Erikson 1966). Rejection of the deviant
might actually harm solidarity because it demonstrates the group’s rigidity and inability to adapt.
Durkheim went as far as to argue that—to keep them vibrant—groups need deviants to such an
extent that groups would quickly want to find someone breaking the rules if there were not enough
deviant individuals in their midst. Durkheim (1958, pp. 68–69) states:
Imagine a society of saints, a perfect cloister of exemplary individuals. Crimes, properly so called, will
there be unknown; but faults which appear venial to the layman will create there the same scandal that
the ordinary defense does in ordinary consciousness. If, then, this society has the power to judge and
punish, it will define these acts as criminal and will treat them as such.
Durkheim’s reasoning makes clear that deviants and dissenters are an important part of group
life and that groups often accept and tolerate these individuals because they might be beneficial
for group functioning. Therefore, the emphasis in sociological work is quite different from the
focus in social psychological work: Sociology focuses on keeping deviance within bounds, whereas
social psychologists focus on how groups aim to eradicate deviance. This suggests that the two
disciplines start from two very different assumptions. Whereas sociologists perceive deviants as
part of a healthy group, social psychologists perceive deviants as separate from a healthy group.
In the latter view, healthy group life can exist only after the deviant has been removed ( Jetten &
Hornsey 2011).
FACTORS THAT AFFECT GROUP TOLERANCE
OF DEVIANCE AND DISSENT
In the review above it is clear that deviants and dissenters may at times be seen to contribute to
group functioning, and at other times their presence might be seen to be detrimental to the group.
In this section we review conditions and factors that moderate these responses.
Qualities of the Dissenter/Deviant
One factor that affects whether deviants are treated harshly is whether they have engaged in similar
deviant behavior in the past. Gollwitzer & Keller (2010) showed that when group members were
evaluating a member of their own group, repeat offenders received harsher punishment for their
474 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
actions than did a first-time offender. Evaluations of out-group members were not affected by
whether they had engaged in similar rule breaking before. The authors suggest that ongoing
deviance is more of a threat to the group identity when confronted with an in-group member than
an out-group member, and this explains the harsher punishment of the former compared to the
latter. Other work has shown that negative responses to in-group deviants are affected by the extent
to which there is certainty about the guilt of the deviant. People reacted more negatively to in-group
than out-group suspects when guilt was certain, but they reacted more negatively to out-group than
in-group suspects when guilt was uncertain (van Prooijen 2006). The deviant’s actions also matter:
Intention to punish diminishes considerably when suspects express remorse (e.g., Carlsmith et al.
2002) and when they are willing to abandon their deviant position (Chan et al. 2010).
Unsurprisingly, the severity of the deviant’s actions and the objective harm that is caused to the
group affects responses to the deviant. Typically, modest violations of norms are derogated most
by high identifiers, who by definition are especially sensitive to the integrity of the group’s norms
(Castano et al. 2002, Hornsey & Jetten 2003). However, there is evidence that group identification
does not affect evaluations when the actions of the deviant are more harmful to the image of the
group. For example, high and low identifiers are equally harsh on in-group deviants involved in
serious violations such as academic fraud or abuse of Iraqi prisoners (Iyer et al. 2012).
There is also evidence that responses to deviants and dissenters depend on who they are and
what their position is in the group. For example, powerful group members are given the freedom
to behave in a more idiosyncratic way than nonpowerful members, and even subtle inductions of
power can have the effect of increasing minor violations of social norms (Galinsky et al. 2008).
Furthermore, dissenters and divergent thinkers within groups are tolerated more when they are
prototypical members of the group (Rijnbout & McKimmie 2012b). For leaders, however, the
norms are more nuanced. Whereas future leaders appear to be given license to innovate and to
question established norms, current and ex-leaders are expected to show more loyalty to the status
quo (Abrams et al. 2008a).
There is also evidence that group members are more willing to embrace criticism when the
critic has been a member of the group for a long time than when the same criticisms come from
a newcomer to the group (Hornsey et al. 2007b). This is consistent with the classic notion of
“idiosyncrasy credits”: People who have proven their loyalty to their group in the past have the
most permission to violate rules and to challenge the group culture (Hollander 1958). The central
role of loyalty attributions is evident also in the work on group criticism: In-group members
who criticize the group are tolerated only when their message is intended to be constructive and
when it is clear that they have the best interests of the group at heart (e.g., Hornsey et al. 2004).
Those expressing criticism can signal that their intentions are constructive by using inclusive
language emphasizing their commitment to the group (“we have a problem” rather than “they
have a problem”) or by otherwise emphasizing their “groupy” credentials (Hornsey et al. 2004)
or shared group membership (Wirtz & Doosje 2013).
Qualities of the Group
There is some evidence that the stage of group life affects how group members respond to deviance
and dissent. There are particularly strong pressures on members to conform in the initial phases
when the group has just formed (Agazarian & Gantt 2003, Worchel 1998). In these early stages,
groups often adopt a dress code or uniform and design other symbols that identify the group and
mark people as being members. Members may be asked to demonstrate their loyalty to the group,
and in this phase dissent or independent thinking is more likely to be viewed with suspicion (Rink
& Ellemers 2009, Van Dyne & Saavedra 1996).
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 475
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
At other times, the group may strive for greater diversity and heterogeneity. Because dissenters
and deviants provide such diversity, they are more accepted by other group members (Hutchison
et al. 2011). Broader cultural norms also matter; for example, conformity is greater in collectivist
cultures than in individualist cultures (Bond & Smith 1996), presumably because dissent is punished
more harshly in groups with collectivist values (Hornsey et al. 2006). It has also been found
that group goals affect whether group members attend to dissent. Toma and colleagues (2013)
showed that in groups with cooperative goals, initial dissent among group members led to a
greater willingness to be influenced by others than when the group held competitive goals. This
suggests that disagreeing in a cooperative context can help group members to overcome and
address differences of opinion.
A final factor that has been found to be important in moderating evaluations is whether the
deviant/dissenter is the only person in the group who does not conform or whether he/she has social
support from others. Classic work by Asch (1951) demonstrates that groups are more dismissive
of a sole dissenter than of a group of dissenters. Although a sole dissenter can be ridiculed and
pressured to get back in line, a group of dissenters and deviants might not be overlooked that
easily, making social influence and social change more likely (Haslam & Reicher 2012b).
Group Treatment of the Deviant or Dissenter
The punishment meted out to deviants or dissenters can have ironic effects on how they are
subsequently regarded. Eidelman and colleagues (2006) asked Christian participants to evaluate
a prolife Christian (a deviant target) either before or after having the opportunity to exclude the
target from the group. Evaluations of the target were more positive after exclusion from the group
than before. This suggests that the need to further derogate deviants is diminished after exclusion,
presumably because they can no longer threaten the group identity. Similar findings were obtained
in a paradigm where, over time, a deviant was either reintegrated or remained marginalized and
excluded (Chan et al. 2009). Group members who were more strongly committed to the group
were most negative toward an excluded deviant who was later reintegrated. In contrast, there was
evidence that group members were quite accepting of the deviant who faced continued exclusion.
Presumably, this is because actions of the deviant who was evicted from the group no longer
represented a threat to the group, with the consequence that group members could open their
minds to the deviant and his/her message. In a similar vein, Curseu et al. (2012) found that groups
that harbored a dissenter who subsequently left showed greater cognitive complexity than did
groups that still had the dissenter within their ranks.
There is some evidence that unless the group changes its position or officially reintegrates
the deviant, a rejected deviant will stay rejected even after being subsequently vindicated. Chan
and colleagues (2010) manipulated historical facts about Galileo, who was excommunicated and
rejected in the seventeenth century for his view—later verified—that the Earth revolves around
the Sun. Catholic participants’ negativity toward Galileo remained relatively high when they were
led to believe that the Church had refused to formally reinstate him. This emphasizes that it is
the stance of the group that determines how deviance is perceived and responded to as much as it
is the actions of the individual deviant.
Strategic Factors
Previously we made the case that just as there are pressures to show conformity and respect
for group norms, there are also countervailing motives to engage in deviance and dissent. Peo-
ple may not be prepared to take the risk of breaking group rules themselves but may be qui-
etly relieved when others do. But what happens when a group member expresses a deviant or
476 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
group-threatening attitude that you privately agree with? If you actively support the dissenter,
you risk facing censure yourself. But if you fail to support the dissenter, you are violating your
principles and potentially missing an opportunity to steer the group in a better direction. Manag-
ing these competing pressures requires close attention to the possibilities afforded by the context.
Group members weigh up a number of strategic considerations: Who is watching? What can be
gained? What can be lost?
One well-established principle is that the willingness to confront deviance changes as a function
of the status of the audience. For example, in-group critics and other nonnormative members are
rated more negatively when evaluations are public than when participants evaluate them privately,
a tendency that gets more pronounced when the audience has high status (Hornsey et al. 2007a).
Group members tend to be especially strategic in their judgments when they are low identifiers and
so have not necessarily internalized the sanctity of the group’s norms. For example, low identifiers
and newcomers tend to express conformity and to confront rule violations in others only when
they are being monitored by a high-status audience ( Jetten et al. 2006, 2010). High identifiers
and old-timers, in contrast, tend to defend the group norms irrespective of audience.
It is not the case, however, that highly identified group members are never strategic when they
consider how to respond to deviance and dissent within their group. Packer (2013) found that
highly identified group members were quite sensitive to the intergroup context when expressing
dissent. Because highly identified group members are more concerned about the reputation of the
group than are low identifiers, they were less likely to “air the group’s dirty laundry” when doing
so would make the group vulnerable (either because they were communicating to an out-group
audience or because intergroup competition was particularly salient). Another recent paper also
provides a fascinating twist on the intuitive position that those who are least secure in the group
will be most likely to shy away from endorsing deviance and dissent in others. Across four studies,
Rios and colleagues (2012) showed that the people who are most likely to express minority
attitudes—and to endorse minority attitudes expressed by others—are those people who tradi-
tionally are associated with insecurity, vulnerability, and social defensiveness: those with high
self-uncertainty and those low in implicit self-esteem. The authors argue that minority opinions
can represent strivings for self-definition and defensive attempts to compensate for threats to the
self-concept.
Strategic factors also play a role in the evaluation of positive deviants. Positive deviance is
more likely to be embraced and celebrated if it helps enhance the reputation of the group; for
example, in an intergroup context (Schmitt et al. 2000) or when the positive deviant attributes
his/her success to the group (Fielding et al. 2006). Other work has shown that deviance and dissent
are tolerated when they fit with the broader goals of the group (Teixeira et al. 2011, 2013). For
example, Morton and colleagues (2007) found that partisans’ evaluations of an in-group political
candidate who expressed normative or deviant opinions depended on the implications of deviance
for achieving group goals. High identifiers expressed stronger intentions to vote for a normative
candidate over a deviant when public opinion was with the group. But when public opinion was
against the group (or when the deviant was attracting high public support), high identifiers gave
equal support to normative and deviant candidates. These findings suggest that backing the deviant
might be a strategic choice for high identifiers when the deviant has the potential to lift the group’s
standing in the eyes of relevant third parties.
AN APPRECIATION FOR THE IMPORTANCE OF DEVIANCE AND
DISSENT IN GROUPS
In this review we revisited the assumption that deviance and dissent are necessarily counterproduc-
tive forces for the group and therefore are perceived as problematic and unwelcome. By focusing
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 477
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
on the myriad motivations of deviants and dissenters—and by focusing on the functional as well
as the dysfunctional effects of deviance and dissent—we hope to have provided a balanced and
complete view of these phenomena.
Unavoidably, by focusing on the functional as well as the dysfunctional impact of deviance and
dissent, the picture that emerges is more complex than traditionally assumed: Groups are at times
hostile toward deviants and dissenters, but at times they embrace them and are even protective
of them. However, to conclude that the picture is more complex than previously assumed will
not be satisfying or useful for researchers in the field who want guidance on how this review
can inform their research. Therefore, instead of highlighting the complexity of the messages that
emerge from recent work, it might be more useful to consider how the research focus needs to
be redirected and broadened to enable a greater appreciation of the value of deviance and dissent.
Three pointers in particular emerge from this review that might be useful to keep in mind when
embarking on further research in this field.
First, the research reviewed here points to the normality and ordinariness of deviance and
dissent—as acts that all group members engage in from time to time. The observation that dissent
and deviance are not extraordinary behaviors that only a small number of individuals engage in
for some unknown or sinister reasons demands a change in the way that we study group processes:
The behaviors of deviants and dissenters should be seen and studied as part of group life rather
than as separate and distinct from it ( Jetten & Hornsey 2011).
Second, as we have outlined, the study of deviance and dissent should be broadened to
encompass the diverse motivations that underpin them. Deviants and dissenters at times are
troublemakers: Their actions are harmful to the group, and it is understandable that groups
will respond negatively to them. At other times, however, dissenters and deviants are indis-
tinguishable from any other group member who has the group’s interests at heart. This is
because they often engage in such behaviors for constructive reasons: because they wish to
improve the group, campaign to change suboptimal group cultures, or simply avoid collective
mistakes. In fact, very often the dissenter or deviant and the most loyal group member are
the same person. By assuming that deviants and dissenters are acting against the group rather
than for the group, we risk ignoring a rich and important ingredient of group life (Packer
2008).
Third, groups do often recognize the value of deviance and dissent. Engaging with the functions
and dysfunctions of dissent and deviance teaches us that group life is more than pressures for
conformity and striving for sameness. By assuming that groups are mainly motivated to exclude
deviants and punish dissent, we fail to acknowledge that deviants and dissenters help keep groups
healthy and vibrant and are often appreciated as such.
More than 30 years ago Moscovici (1976) complained that the final goal of researchers
always appears to be “the reclamation of deviants.” In particular, he argues that researchers
in the field have focused on only one set of forces that drive group responses to deviance:
“making everybody alike, blurring the particularity and individuality of persons or subgroups”
(Moscovici 1976, p. 17; see also Levine 1989). Similar sentiments have been voiced more
recently, and it has been argued that an understanding of basic group processes is necessarily
impoverished if theorizing does not take account of the group members that conform to
group norms as well as those members that challenge them (Ellemers & Jetten 2013; Haslam
& Reicher 2012a; Jetten & Hornsey 2011, 2012; Packer 2008). It is only when we integrate
research on deviants and dissent with theorizing on group processes more generally that we
can develop a more complete, balanced, and refined understanding of deviants and dissenters in
groups.
478 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
SUMMARY POINTS
1. Within groups, deviance and dissent are common and are often recognized as beneficial
to group functioning.
2. Healthy groups have deviants and dissenters in their midst.
3. What is considered deviant and dissenting is contextually determined and may change
with time.
4. There are a number of reasons why group members engage in deviance and dissent within
groups. Among others, deviance and dissent may be motivated by (a) disengagement,
disloyalty, or disrespect for the group, (b) group loyalty, (c) moral reasons, (d)adesire
to express difference, individuality, and uniqueness, and (e) a wish to obtain the tangible
rewards that deviance and dissent provide.
5. Rejection of deviance and dissent helps to restore threatened group positivity, threatened
group cohesion, threatened group distinctiveness, threatened group locomotion, and
threatened self-image.
6. Deviants and dissenters can damage the group but can also contribute in positive ways to
group functioning. In particular, under some conditions, dissent increases the quality of
group decision making and enhances group creativity, innovation, and learning. Deviance
can sharpen the group’s understanding of its values and can facilitate social change.
7. Other group members often recognize the value of deviants and dissenters and are there-
fore often remarkably tolerant of these individuals.
FUTURE ISSUES
1. Whereas classic research examined responses to deviants and dissenters in groups where
members interacted over a period of time [e.g., Schachter’s (1951) research on rejection,
deviance, and communication], recent work has examined responses to deviants and
dissenters using scenarios and contexts devoid of interaction. To understand fully the
trajectory of responses to deviants and dissenters, more research needs to be conducted
examining responses to deviants and dissenters over time in real interacting groups and
in different group contexts (e.g., different group goals or different group cultures).
2. We know little about the micro strategies for how group members manage dissent and
deviance in live, interactive groups. This would require a more descriptive communica-
tion approach to the question of deviance and dissent.
3. There are many different disciplines within the social sciences that examine deviance and
dissent. There is a need for a theoretical integration of work on dissent, minority influ-
ence, interpersonal deviance, interpersonal aggression, bullying, and criminal behavior.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that
might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 479
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Alex Haslam, John Levine, and Dominic Packer for insightful comments on earlier
drafts of this article. This review was supported by a Discovery grant from the Australian Research
Council (DP1094034).
LITERATURE CITED
Abrams D, Marques JM, Bown N, Dougill M. 2002. Anti-norm and pro-norm deviance in the bank and on
the campus: two experiments on subjective group dynamics. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 5:163–82
Abrams D, Marques JM, Bown N, Henson M. 2000. Pro-norm and anti-norm deviance within in-groups and
out-groups. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 78:906–12
Abrams D, Randsley de Moura G, Marques JM, Hutchison P. 2008a. Innovation credit: When can leaders
oppose their group’s norms? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 95(3):662–78
Abrams D, Rutland A, Ferrell JM, Pelletier J. 2008b. Children’s judgments of disloyal and immoral peer
behavior: subjective group dynamics in minimal intergroup contexts. Child Dev. 79(2):444–61
Agazarian Y, Gantt S. 2003. Phases of group development: systems-centered hypotheses and their implications
for research and practice. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 7:238–52
Amabile TM. 1996. Creativity in Context. Boulder, CO: Westview
Ariyanto A, Hornsey MJ, Gallois C. 2010. United we stand: Intergroup conflict moderates the intergroup
sensitivity effect. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40:169–77
A classic text showing
the prevalence of
dissent despite
tremendous conformity
pressures.
Asch SE. 1951. Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments. In Groups,
Leadership and Men: Research in Human Relations, ed. H Guetzkow, pp. 177–90. Pittsburgh, PA:
Carnegie Press
Benard S. 2012. Cohesion from conflict: Does intergroup conflict motivate intragroup norm enforcement and
support for centralized leadership? Soc. Psychol. Q. 75:107–30
Bennett RJ, Robinson SL. 2000. Development of a measure of workplace deviance. J. Appl. Psychol. 85:349–60
Ben-Yehuda N. 2010. Positive and negative deviance: more fuel for a controversy. Deviant Behav. 11:221–43
Blanton H, Christie C. 2003. Deviance regulation: a theory of action and identity. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 7:115–49
Blanton H, Stuart AE, Van den Eijnden RJJM. 2001. An introduction to deviance regulation theory: the effect
of behavioral norms on message framing. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27(7):848–58
Bond R, Smith PB. 1996. Culture and conformity: a meta-analysis of studies using Asch’s (1952b, 1956) line
judgment task. Psychol. Bull. 119:111–37
Branscombe NR, Wann DL, Noel JG, Coleman J. 1993. In-group or out-group extremity: importance of the
threatened social identity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19:381–88
Butera F, Darnon C, Mugny G. 2011. Learning from conflict. See Jetten & Hornsey 2011, pp. 36–53
Carlsmith KM, Darley JM, Robinson PH. 2002. Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives
for punishment. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 83:284–99
Castano E, Paladino M, Coull A, Yzerbyt VY. 2002. Protecting the ingroup stereotype: ingroup identification
and the management of deviant ingroup members. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.41:365–85
Chan MKH, Louis WR, Hornsey MJ. 2009. The effects of exclusion and reintegration on evaluation of deviant
opinion holders. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35(12):1619–31
Chan MKH, Louis WR, Jetten J. 2010. When groups are wrong and deviants are right. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol.
40:1103–9
Chekroun P, Nugier A. 2011. “I’m ashamed because of you, so please, don’t do that!” Reactions to deviance
as a protection against a threat to social image. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41:479–88
Choi HS, Levine JM. 2004. Minority influence in work teams: the impact of newcomers. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
40:273–80
Christian JS, Ellis APJ. 2013. The crucial role of turnover intentions in transforming moral disengagement
into deviant behavior at work. J. Bus. Ethics. In press
Codol J-P. 1984. Social differentiation and non-differentiation. In The Social Dimension: European Developments
in Social Psychology, ed. H Tajfel, pp. 314–37. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
480 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Conway LG, Schaller M. 2005. When authorities’ commands backfire: attributions about consensus and effects
on deviant decision making. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 89:311–26
Coser L. 1962. Some functions of deviant behavior and normative flexibility. Am. J. Sociol. 68:172–81
Coull A, Yzerbyt VY, Castano E, Paladino MP, Leemans V. 2001. Protecting the ingroup: motivated allocation
of cognitive resources in the presence of threatening ingroup members. Group Process. Intergroup Relat.
4:327–39
Crane MF, Platow MJ. 2010. Deviance as adherence to injunctive group norms: the overlooked role of social
identification in deviance. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 49:827–47
Cronin T, Smith H. 2011. Protest, exit, or deviance: adjunct university faculty reactions to occupational
rank-based mistreatment. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 41:2352–73
Curseu PL, Schruijer SGL, Boros S. 2012. Socially rejected while cognitively successful: the impact of minority
dissent on groups’ cognitive complexity. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 51:570–82
Darnon C, Doll S, Butera F. 2007. Dealing with a disagreeing partner: relational and epistemic conflict
elaboration. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 22:227–42
De Dreu CKW. 2006. When too much and too little hurts: evidence for a curvilinear relationship between
task conflict and innovation in teams. J. Manag. 32:83–107
De Dreu CKW, West MA. 2001. Minority dissent and team innovation: the importance of participation in
decision making. J. Appl. Psychol. 86:1191–201
Deutsch M, Gerard H. 1955. A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judg-
ment. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 51:629–36
Dooley RS, Fryxell GE. 1999. Attaining decision quality and commitment from dissent: the moderating effects
of loyalty and competence in strategic decision-making teams. Acad. Manag. J. 42:389–402
Durkheim E. 1958 [1895]. The Rules of Sociological Method. Glencoe: Free Press
Eidelman S, Biernat M. 2003. Derogating black sheep: individual or group protection? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
39:602–9
Eidelman S, Silvia PJ, Biernat M. 2006. Responding to deviance: target exclusion and differential devaluation.
Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32:1153–64
Elder TJ, Sutton RM, Douglas KM. 2005. Keeping it to ourselves: effects of audience size and composition
on reactions to criticism of the ingroup. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 8:231–44
Ellemers N, Jetten J. 2013. The many ways to be marginal in a group. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 17:3–21
A classic text
emphasizing the
functionality of
deviance for group life.
Erikson KT. 1966. Wayward Puritans: A Study in the Sociology of Deviance. New York: Wiley
Esposo SR Hornsey MJ, Spoor JR. 2013. Shooting the messenger: Outsiders critical of your group are rejected
regardless of argument quality. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 52:386–95
Feather NT, Sherman R. 2002. Envy, resentment, schadenfreude, and sympathy: reactions to deserved and
undeserved achievement and subsequent failure. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28:953–61
Ferris DL, Spence JR, Brown DJ, Heller D. 2012. Interpersonal injustice and workplace deviance: the role of
esteem threat. J. Manag. 38:1788–811
Festinger L. 1950. Informal social communication. Psychol. Rev. 57(5):271–82
Fielding KS, Hogg MA, Annandale N. 2006. Reactions to positive deviance: social identity and attributional
dimensions. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 9:199–218
Frings D, Hurst J, Cleveland C, Blascovich J, Abrams D. 2012. Challenge, threat, and subjective group
dynamics: reactions to normative and deviant group members. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 16:105–21
Galinsky AD, Magee JC, Gruenfeld DH, Whitson J, Liljenquist KA. 2008. Social power reduces the strength of
the situation: implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 95:1450–66
Galperin BL. 2012. Exploring the nomological network of workplace deviance: developing and validating a
measure of constructive deviance. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 42:2988–3025
Gollwitzer M, Keller L. 2010. What you did only matters if you are one of us: Offenders’ group membership
moderates the effect of criminal history on punishment severity. Soc. Psychol. 41:20–26
Greitemeyer T, Schulz-Hardt S, Frey D. 2009. The effects of authentic and contrived dissent on escalation
of commitment in group decision making. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 39:639–47
Hansen T, Levine JM. 2009. Newcomers as change agents: effects of newcomers’ behavioral style and teams’
performance optimism. Soc. Influ. 4:46–61
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 481
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Haslam SA, Reicher SD. 2012a. When prisoners take over the prison: a social psychology of resistance. Personal.
Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16:154–79
Haslam SA, Reicher SD. 2012b. Contesting the “nature” of conformity: what Milgram and Zimbardo’s studies
really show. PLoS Biol. 10(11):e1001426
Hichy Z, Mari S, Capozza D. 2008. Pro-norm and anti-norm deviants: a test of the subjective group dynamics
model. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 148:641–44
Hiew DN, Hornsey MJ. 2010. Does time reduce resistance to out-group critics? An investigation of the
persistence of the intergroup sensitivity effect over time. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.49:569–81
Hollander EP. 1958. Conformity, status and idiosyncrasy credit. Psychol. Rev. 65:117–27
Hornsey MJ, de Bruijn P, Creed J, Allen J, Ariyanto A, Svensson A. 2005. Keeping it in-house: how audience
affects responses to group criticism. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 35:291–312
Hornsey MJ, Frederiks E, Smith JR, Ford L. 2007a. Strategic defensiveness: public and private responses to
group criticism. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.46:697–716
Hornsey MJ, Grice T, Jetten J, Paulsen N, Callan V. 2007b. Group-directed criticisms and recommendations
for change: why newcomers arouse more resistance than old-timers. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32:1620–32
Hornsey MJ, Imani A. 2004. Criticizing groups from the inside and the outside: an identity perspective on the
intergroup sensitivity effect. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30:365–83
Hornsey MJ, Jetten J. 2003. Not being what you claim to be: impostors as sources of group threat. Eur. J. Soc.
Psychol. 33:639–57
Hornsey MJ, Jetten J. 2004. The individual within the group: balancing the need to belong with the need to
be different. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8:248–64
This chapter outlines
the different reasons
why people may engage
in impostorism.
Hornsey MJ, Jetten J. 2011. Impostors within groups: the psychology of claiming to be something
you are not. See Jetten & Hornsey 2011, pp. 158–78
Hornsey MJ, Jetten J, McAuliffe B, Hogg MA. 2006. The impact of individualist and collectivist group norms
on evaluations of dissenting group members. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42:57–68
Hornsey MJ, Majkut L, Terry DJ, McKimmie BM. 2003. On being loud and proud: non-conformity and
counter-conformity to group norms. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 42:319–35
Hornsey MJ, Oppes T, Svensson A. 2002. “It’s OK if we say it, but you can’t”: responses to intergroup and
intragroup criticism. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 32:293–307
Hornsey MJ, Trembath M, Gunthorpe S. 2004. “You can criticize because you care”: identity attachment,
constructiveness, and the intergroup sensitivity effect. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 34:499–518
Hutchison P, Abrams D. 2003. Ingroup identification moderates stereotype change in reaction to ingroup
deviance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 33:497–506
Hutchison P, Abrams D, Gutierrez R, Viki GT. 2008. Getting rid of the bad ones: the relationship between
group identification, deviant derogation, and stereotype maintenance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44:874–81
Hutchison P, Jetten J, Gutierrez R. 2011. Deviant but desirable: perceived group variability and reactions to
atypical group members. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47:1155–61
Imhoff R, Erb H-P. 2009. What motivates nonconformity? Uniqueness seeking blocks majority influence.
Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35:309–20
Iyer A, Jetten J, Haslam SA. 2012. Sugaring o’er the devil: Moral superiority and group identification help
individuals downplay the implications of ingroup rule-breaking. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42:141–49
Janis IL. 1972. Victims of Groupthink. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
Jehn K, Northcraft G, Neale M. 1999. Why differences make a difference: a field study of diversity, conflict,
and performance in workgroups. Adm. Sci. Q. 44:741–63
Jetten J, Branscombe NR, Schmitt MT, Spears R. 2001. Rebels with a cause: group identification as a response
to perceived discrimination from the mainstream. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27:1204–13
This edited book
includes chapters that
show that dissent,
difference, deviance,
and defiance are just as
much a part of group
life and as essential for
good group functioning
as is conformity.
Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, eds. 2011. Rebels in Groups: Dissent, Deviance, Difference and Defiance.
Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell
Jetten J, Hornsey MJ. 2012. Conformity: beyond the Asch line judgment studies. In Refreshing Social Psychology:
Beyond the Classic Studies, ed. J Smith, SA Haslam, pp. 76–90. London: Sage
Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, Adarves-Yorno I. 2006. When group members admit to being conformist: the role of
relative intragroup status in conformity self-reports. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32:162–73
482 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, Spears R, Haslam SA, Cowell E. 2010. Rule transgressions in groups: the conditional
nature of newcomers’ willingness to confront deviance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40:338–48
Kelman HC. 1995. Decision making and public discourse in the Gulf War: an assessment of underlying
psychological and moral assumptions. Peace Confl. J. Peace Psychol. 1:117–30
Kraschnewski JL, Stuckey HL, Roviniak LS, Lehman EB, Reddy M, et al. 2011. Efficacy of a weight-loss
website based on positive deviance: a randomized trial. Am. J. Prev. Med. 41:610–14
Kruglanski AW, Webster DM. 1991. Group members’ reactions to opinion deviates and conformists at varying
degrees of proximity to decision deadline and of environmental noise. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 61:212–25
Kruglanski AW, Webster DM. 1996. Motivated closing of the mind: “seizing” and “freezing.” Psychol. Rev.
103:263–83
Kunda Z, Oleson KC. 1997. When exceptions prove the rule: how extremity of deviance determines the impact
of deviant examples on stereotypes. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 72:965–79
Levine JM. 1989. Reaction to opinion deviance in small groups. In Psychology of Group Influence, ed. PB Paulus,
pp. 187–231. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Levine JM, Kerr NL. 2007. Inclusion and exclusion: implications for group processes. In Social Psychology:
Handbook of Basic Principles, ed. AE Kruglanski, ET Higgins, pp. 759–84. New York: Guilford. 2nd ed.
Louis WR, Taylor DM, Douglas RL. 2005. Normative influence and rational conflict decisions: group norms
and cost-benefit analyses for intergroup behaviour. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 8:355–74
Ma P, Magnus JH. 2012. Exploring the concept of positive deviance related to breastfeeding initiation in black
and white WIC enrolled first time mothers. Matern. Child Health J. 16:1583–93
Markova G, Folger R. 2012. Every cloud has a silver lining: positive effects of deviant coworkers. Eur. J. Soc.
Psychol. 152:586–612
Marques JM, Abrams D, Ser ˆ
odio RG. 2001. Being better by being right: subjective group dynamics and
derogation of ingroup deviants when generic norms are undermined. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 81:436–47
Marques JM, Paez D. 1994. The “black sheep effect”: social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates, and
perception of group variability. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 5:37–68
Matheson K, Cole B, Majka K. 2003. Dissidence from within: examining intragroup reactions to attitudinal
opposition. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 39:161–69
Matz DC, Wood W. 2005. Cognitive dissonance in groups: the consequences of disagreement. J. Personal.
Soc. Psychol. 88:22–37
McAuliffe BJ, Jetten J, Hornsey MJ, Hogg MA. 2002. Individualist and collectivist norms: when it’s ok to go
your own way. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 33:57–70
Merton RK. 1968. Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: Free Press
Milgram S. 1963. Behavioral study of obedience. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 67:371–78
Molero F, Fuster MJ, Jetten J, Moriano JA. 2011. Living with HIV/AIDS: a psychosocial perspective on
coping with prejudice and discrimination. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 41:609–26
Monin B, O’Connor K. 2011. Reactions to defiant deviants: deliverance or defensiveness? See Jetten & Hornsey
2011, pp. 261–79
Monin B, Sawyer PJ, Marquez MJ. 2008. The rejection of moral rebels: resenting those who do the right
thing. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 95(1):76–93
Morton TA, Postmes T, Jetten J. 2007. Playing the game: strategic considerations and responses to normative
and deviant group members. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37:599–616
This book has become a
classic in the field and
reveals Moscovici’s
frustration with the
one-sided interest of
social psychologists in
conformity.
Moscovici S. 1976. Social Influence and Social Change. New York: Academic
Nemeth C. 1995. Dissent as driving cognition, attitudes and judgments. Soc. Cogn. 13:273–91
Nemeth C, Brown K, Rogers J. 2001. Devil’s advocate versus authentic dissent: stimulating quantity and
quality. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 31:707–20
Nemeth C, Goncalo JA. 2011. Rogues and heroes: finding value in dissent. See Jetten & Hornsey 2011,
pp. 17–35
Nemeth C, Ormiston M. 2007. Creative idea generation: harmony versus stimulation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
37:524–35
Nemeth C, Personnaz B, Personnaz M, Goncalo JA. 2004. The liberating role of conflict in group creativity:
a study in two countries. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 34:365–74
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 483
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
A conceptual paper
developing the
normative conflict
model outlining how
deviance and dissent can
be motivated by group
loyalty.
Packer DJ. 2008. On being with us and against us: a normative conflict model of dissent in social
groups. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12:50–72
Packer DJ. 2009. Avoiding groupthink: Whereas weakly identified members remain silent, strongly identified
members dissent about collective problems. Psychol. Sci. 20:546–48
Packer DJ. 2011. The dissenter’s dilemma, and a social identity solution. See Jetten & Hornsey 2011, pp. 281–
301
Packer DJ. 2013. On not airing our dirty laundry: Intergroup contexts suppress ingroup criticism among
strongly identified group members. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.In press
Packer DJ, Chasteen AL. 2010. Loyal deviance: testing the normative conflict model of dissent in social groups.
Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36:5–18
Parks G, Stone AB. 2010. The desire to expel unselfish members from the group. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
99:303–10
Postmes T, Spears R, Cihangir S. 2001. Quality of decision making and group norms. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
80:918–30
Prislin R, Christensen PN. 2005. Social change in the aftermath of successful minority influence. Eur. Rev.
Soc. Psychol. 16:43–73
Prislin R, Filson J. 2009. Seeking conversion versus advocating tolerance in the pursuit of social change.
J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 97:811–22
Rabinovich A, Morton T A. 2010. Who says we are bad people? The impact of criticism source and attributional
content on responses to group-based criticism. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36:524–36
Reicher SD, Haslam SA. 2006. Rethinking the psychology of tyranny: the BBC Prison Study. Br. J. Soc. Psychol.
45:1–40
Rijnbout JS, McKimmie BM. 2012a. Deviance in organizational group decision-making: the role of informa-
tion processing, confidence, and elaboration. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 15:813–28
Rijnbout JS, McKimmie BM. 2012b. Deviance in group decision making: group-member centrality alleviates
negative consequences for the group. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42:915–23
Rink F, Ellemers N. 2009. Temporary versus permanent group membership: how the future prospects of
newcomers affect newcomer acceptance and newcomer influence. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35:764–76
Rios K, Wheeler SC, Miller DT. 2012. Compensatory nonconformity: self-uncertainty and low implicit
self-esteem increase adoption and expression of minority opinions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48:1300–09
Rios Morrison K, Wheeler SC. 2010. Nonconformity defines the self: the role of minority opinion status in
self-concept clarity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36:297–308
Sani F, Reicher S. 2000. Contested identities and schisms in groups: opposing the ordination of women as
priests in the Church of England. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.39:95–112
Sassenberg K, Matschke C, Scholl A. 2011. The impact of discrepancies from ingroup norms on group
members’ well-being and motivation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41:886–97
Schachter S. 1951. Deviation, rejection, and communication. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 46:190–207
Schmitt MT, Silvia PJ, Branscombe NR. 2000. The intersection of self-evaluation maintenance and social
identity theories: intragroup judgment in interpersonal and intergroup contexts. Personal. Soc. Psychol.
Bull. 26:1598–606
Schoemann AM, Branscombe NR. 2011. Looking young for your age: perceptions of anti-aging actions.
Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 41:86–95
Shamir J. 1997. Speaking up and silencing out in face of a changing climate of opinion. J. Mass Commun. Q.
74:602–14
Shatz RT, Staub E, Lavine H. 1999. On the varieties of national attachment: blind versus constructive patri-
otism. Polit. Psychol. 20:151–74
Shoenberger N, Heckert A, Heckert D. 2012. Techniques of neutralization theory and positive deviance.
Deviant Behav. 33:774–91
Spears R. 2010. Group rationale, collective sense: beyond intergroup bias. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 49:1–20
Spears R, Doosje B, Ellemers N. 1997. Self-stereotyping in the face of threats to group status and distinctive-
ness: the role of group identification. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23:538–53
Tata J, Anthony T, Lin H, Newman B, Tang S, et al. 1996. Proportionate group size and rejection of the
deviate: a meta-analytic integration. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 11(4):739–52
484 Jetten ·Hornsey
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65CH17-Jetten ARI 31 October 2013 14:21
Tauber S, Sassenberg K. 2012. The impact of identification on adherence to group norms in team sports:
Who is going the extra mile? Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 16:231–40
Teixeira CP, Demoulin S, Yzerbyt VY. 2011. Choosing the best means to an end: the influence of ingroup
goals on the selection of representatives in intergroup negotiations. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47:228–34
Teixeira CP, Demoulin S, Yzerbyt VY. 2013. Playing with deviance: typicality assessments of ingroup members
as a strategy of outgroup approach. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43:32–39
Toma C, Giles I, Butera F. 2013. Strategic use of preference confirmation in group decision making: the role
of competition and dissent. Br.J.Soc.Psychol.32:44–63
Troyer L, Youngreen R. 2009. Conflict and creativity in groups. J. Soc. Issues 65:409–27
Turner JC. 1991. Social Influence. Milton-Keynes, UK: Open Univ. Press
Van Dyne L, Saavedra R. 1996. A naturalistic minority influence experiment: effects of divergent thinking,
conflict and originality in work groups. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 35:151–68
Van Prooijen J-W. 2006. Retributive reactions to suspected offenders: the importance of social categorizations
and guilt probability. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32:715–26
Warner R, Hornsey MJ, Jetten J. 2007. Why minority members resent impostors. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37:1–18
Warren DE. 2003. Constructive and destructive deviance in organizations. Acad. Manag. Rev. 28(4):622–32
Wirtz C, Doosje B. 2013. Reactions to threatening critical messages from minority group members with
shared or distinct group identities. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43:50–61
Wolf B, Zuckerman P. 2012. Deviant heroes: nonconformists as agents of justice and social change. Deviant
Behav. 33:639–54
Wood W, Lundgren S, Ouellette JA, Busceme S, Blackstone T. 1994. Processes of minority influence: influence
effectiveness and source perception. Psychol. Bull. 115:323–45
Worchel S. 1998. A developmental view of the search for group identity. In Social Identity: International
Perspectives, ed. S Worchel, JF Morales, D Paez, J-C Deschamps, pp. 53–74. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Zdaniuk B, Levine JM. 2001. Group loyalty: impact of members’ identification and contributions. J. Exp. Soc.
Psychol. 37:502–9
www.annualreviews.org Deviance and Dissent 485
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65-FrontMatter ARI 13 November 2013 20:27
Annual Review of
Psychology
Volume 65, 2014
Contents
Prefatory
I Study What I Stink At: Lessons Learned from a Career in Psychology
Robert J. Sternberg pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp1
Stress and Neuroendocrinology
Oxytocin Pathways and the Evolution of Human Behavior
C. Sue Carter ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp17
Genetics of Behavior
Gene-Environment Interaction
Stephen B. Manuck and Jeanne M. McCaffery ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp41
Cognitive Neuroscience
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Insight
John Kounios and Mark Beeman pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp71
Color Perception
Color Psychology: Effects of Perceiving Color on Psychological
Functioning in Humans
Andrew J. Elliot and Markus A. Maier ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp95
Infancy
Human Infancy. . . and the Rest of the Lifespan
Marc H. Bornstein pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp121
Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood
Bullying in Schools: The Power of Bullies and the Plight of Victims
Jaana Juvonen and Sandra Graham pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp159
Is Adolescence a Sensitive Period for Sociocultural Processing?
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore and Kathryn L. Mills ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp187
Adulthood and Aging
Psychological Research on Retirement
Mo Wang and Junqi Shi ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp209
Development in the Family
Adoption: Biological and Social Processes Linked to Adaptation
Harold D. Grotevant and Jennifer M. McDermott pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp235
vi
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65-FrontMatter ARI 13 November 2013 20:27
Individual Treatment
Combination Psychotherapy and Antidepressant Medication Treatment
for Depression: For Whom, When, and How
W. Edward Craighead and Boadie W. Dunlop ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp267
Adult Clinical Neuropsychology
Sport and Nonsport Etiologies of Mild Traumatic Brain Injury:
Similarities and Differences
Amanda R. Rabinowitz, Xiaoqi Li, and Harvey S. Levin pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp301
Self and Identity
The Psychology of Change: Self-Affirmation and Social
Psychological Intervention
Geoffrey L. Cohen and David K. Sherman pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp333
Gender
Gender Similarities and Differences
Janet Shibley Hyde ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp373
Altruism and Aggression
Dehumanization and Infrahumanization
Nick Haslam and Steve Loughnan pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp399
The Sociocultural Appraisals, Values, and Emotions (SAVE) Framework
of Prosociality: Core Processes from Gene to Meme
Dacher Keltner, Aleksandr Kogan, Paul K. Piff, and Sarina R. Saturn pppppppppppppppp425
Small Groups
Deviance and Dissent in Groups
Jolanda Jetten and Matthew J. Hornsey pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp461
Social Neuroscience
Cultural Neuroscience: Biology of the Mind in Cultural Contexts
Heejung S. Kim and Joni Y. Sasaki ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp487
Genes and Personality
A Phenotypic Null Hypothesis for the Genetics of Personality
Eric Turkheimer, Erik Pettersson, and Erin E. Horn pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp515
Environmental Psychology
Environmental Psychology Matters
Robert Gifford ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp541
Contents vii
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
PS65-FrontMatter ARI 13 November 2013 20:27
Community Psychology
Socioecological Psychology
Shigehiro Oishi ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp581
Subcultures Within Countries
Social Class Culture Cycles: How Three Gateway Contexts Shape Selves
and Fuel Inequality
Nicole M. Stephens Hazel Rose Markus, and L. Taylor Phillips ppppppppppppppppppppppppp611
Organizational Climate/Culture
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Organizations
Linda Klebe Trevi˜no, Niki A. den Nieuwenboer, and Jennifer J. Kish-Gephart ppppppp635
Job/Work Design
Beyond Motivation: Job and Work Design for Development, Health,
Ambidexterity, and More
Sharon K. Parker ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp661
Selection and Placement
A Century of Selection
Ann Marie Ryan and Robert E. Ployhart pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp693
Personality and Coping Styles
Personality, Well-Being, and Health
Howard S. Friedman and Margaret L. Kern pppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp719
Timely Topics
Properties of the Internal Clock: First- and Second-Order Principles of
Subjective Time
Melissa J. Allman, Sundeep Teki, Timothy D. Griffiths, and Warren H. Meck pppppppp743
Indexes
Cumulative Index of Contributing Authors, Volumes 55–65 ppppppppppppppppppppppppppp773
Cumulative Index of Article Titles, Volumes 55–65 ppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppppp778
Errata
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Psychology articles may be found at
http://psych.AnnualReviews.org/errata.shtml
viii Contents
Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014.65:461-485. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
by 145.100.224.175 on 01/09/14. For personal use only.
... While social norms are inherently a group feature, they exist psychologically only through the individual's mental representation of the group, and they can be contested and challenged through competing representations of what the group's norms are, how the norms are changing, or what they should be. In this review we will focus on the often seemingly unpredictable ways that social norms may influence behavior, shaped by complex influences of social and moral identities (see also Jetten & Hornsey 2014, Spears 2021; issues of how social norms emerge, persist, or change have been reviewed by Gelfand et al. (2024). As we will discuss, norms can be powerful devices for behavior change or management, but they also have their pitfalls (see Figure 1 for an overview). ...
... Indeed, a further indication of the power of norms stems from the fact that alternative injunctive norms can be recruited to challenge group norms. Even when individuals self-categorize and identify with a given group, they may nevertheless question its norms-and they may do so exactly because they are committed to the group ( Jetten & Hornsey 2014 with the group but see its norms as damaging to the group's collective goals, they may not conform but rather speak out against those norms. Normative conflict can also arise from the group's practices or common behaviors (its descriptive norms) conflicting with the principles the group supposedly stands for (its injunctive norms) (Crane & Platow 2010). ...
... In yet other situations, an individual may act counter to the norms of a given group, even if they identify with it, because doing so grants them a personal benefit ( Jetten & Hornsey 2014). Flouting group norms can grant the individual an air of power and assertiveness, thus increasing their dominance; or doing so can gain them admiration for the skill shown in circumventing the norms, their risk taking, or their prosocial motives, thus increasing their prestige. ...
Article
Social norms have long been considered across the social and behavioral sciences for their effects on human behavior. This review focuses on norms that explicitly or implicitly convey socially shared views about what people should or should not do. Such injunctive norms can be powerful influences on behavior through internalization or through formal or informal enforcement. They can underpin interventions to shape people's behavior through conformity but can also be drawn upon by individuals to question social practices. However, norms also have pitfalls and may fail to affect behavior as intended: They can pose a threat to the individual's self (autonomy, morality, values), compromise motives and social meanings (intrinsic motivation, trust, sincerity), or be egocentrically reconstrued (through biased perceptions and selective social/temporal referents). Depending on one's stance on an issue, such pitfalls may also be turned into powers to inoculate against undesirable norms or to advance social change.
... Rule breaking is typically considered as one form of deviant behavior-a broader category constituted by actions that violate or contravene norms, expectations, or mandates of one's managers, organization, or other form of authority (Dahling & Gutworth, 2017;Hollinger, 1986;Jetten & Hornsey, 2014;Mishra, Ghosh, & Sharma, 2021;Vadera et al., 2013). Existing scholarship on rule breaking has focused largely on why individuals break rules. ...
... Punishing rule breakers reconfirms a group's existing values (Okimoto & Wenzel, 2009) and discourages subsequent misbehavior (Apel, 2013;Carlsmith, Darley, & Robinson, 2002). A second perspective focuses on how, in certain contexts, groups tolerate those rule breakers (Coser, 1962;Dentler & Erikson, 1959;Jetten & Hornsey, 2014;Jetten, Hornsey, Spears, Haslam, & Cowell, 2010), particularly if doing so is perceived to contribute to group goals (Fielding, Hogg, & Annandale, 2006;Morrison, 2006). ...
... Existing work in this area has largely focused on how rule breaking is punished (Podsakoff, Bommer, Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 2006;Dahling et al., 2012) or tolerated (Coser, 1962;Dentler & Erikson, 1959;Jetten & Hornsey, 2014;Jetten et al., 2010) by other group members. ...
... In other words, widespread adoption and promotion can lead to a quicker decline in enthusiasm and a faster rate of disillusionment. We call this tendency interest saturation (see Jetten and Hornsey, 2014 for a survey of the literature in social psychology that describe deviant behaviors with respect to conformism). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In the dynamic landscape of contemporary society, the popularity of ideas, opinions, and interests fluctuates rapidly. Traditional dynamical models in social sciences often fail to capture this inherent volatility, attributing changes to exogenous shocks rather than intrinsic features of the system. This paper introduces a novel, tractable model that simulates the natural rise and fall of ideas' popularity, offering a more accurate representation of real-world dynamics. Building upon the SIRS (Susceptible, Infectious, Recovered, Susceptible) epidemiological model, we incorporate a feedback mechanism that allows the recovery rate to vary dynamically based on the current state of the system. This modification reflects the cyclical nature of idea adoption and abandonment, driven by social saturation and renewed interest. Our model successfully captures the rapid and recurrent shifts in popularity, providing valuable insights into the mechanisms behind these fluctuations. This approach offers a robust framework for studying the diffusion dynamics of popular ideas, with potential applications across various fields such as marketing, technology adoption, and political movements.
... SIT cannot easily explain how group members may deviate from their group (e.g., criticize it). Recent advances address this discrepancy by explicating conditions under which deviance may be motivated by a high concern for the group [e.g., Ref. [15]; overview by Ref. [16]]. ...
... Our second hypothesis is that the fraternizer's perceived ideological position would elicit negative attitudes toward this person. This prediction is consistent with a large body of literature indicating that ingroup members who deviate from group consensus on attitudinal issues often threaten important goals for other members and are disliked as a result (Hornsey, 2016;Jetten & Hornsey, 2014;Levine, 2017). In the present context, an ingroup member who fraternizes with an outgroup member is likely to threaten several such goals. ...
Article
Extended contact (i.e., knowledge that an ingroup member has a close relationship with an outgroup member) often improves relations between groups. In the current research, we argue that such contact can also undermine relations within groups. Specifically, we propose a “fraternizing with the enemy” effect in which the fraternizer is viewed negatively by other ingroup members. In five preregistered experiments ( N = 2,035), we tested this effect in the context of political conflict using both real-world (Study 1) and fictitious (Studies 2–5) ingroup and outgroup members. Our results indicated that the fraternizer (vs. nonfraternizer) was viewed as less ideologically aligned with the ingroup, which in turn led to perceptions of this person as ambiguous and disloyal to the ingroup, and thereby elicited negative attitudes toward the fraternizer. We also found that, besides producing these negative effects, fraternizing with the enemy also produced the positive intergroup effects typically elicited by extended contact. We discuss the potential implications of our results for the effectiveness of extended contact in bridging political divides.
Article
Asian Americans (AsAms) carry unique group identifications that likely impact how they navigate workplace racial discrimination. Yet, extant workplace discrimination research has not thoroughly considered the implications associated with such unique group identifications, especially given the context of American society’s increasingly polarized views of AsAms as outsiders versus insiders. To gain insights into these aspects, we conducted three studies using qualitative and quantitative methods. Our qualitative interviews (Study 1) with AsAm employees during COVID-19 reveal that AsAms have internalized society’s polarization of their American and Asian group identifications and navigate their workplace discrimination accordingly. Integrating these findings with group identification research, we develop a dual-serial-mediation navigation process model, whereby AsAms with strong American group identification intend to leave their organization via blaming and then not forgiving their offenders (i.e., “suffering path”), whereas those with strong Asian group identification intend to stay in the organization via perspective taking and then forgiving their offenders (i.e., “protected path”). In a different sample of AsAms who faced workplace discrimination, we found support for our model (Study 2). Finally, we largely replicated these results in a third sample of AsAms who faced workplace discrimination and found that such navigation processes were largely unique to AsAms versus other racial-minority groups (Study 3). Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Article
Research into social identity and morality judgments typically focuses on how the former influences the latter. We approach this theme from the opposite direction, establishing the influence of morality on perceptions of social identity. In three studies, conducted in two cultures, we show that in-group members acting immorally are excluded from the group. Extending this investigation to the overlooked study of out-group-on-out-group behavior, in Studies 2 and 3, we compare perceptions of social inclusion for in-group members following (im)moral behavior toward the in-group with perceptions of out-group members following (im)moral behavior toward the out-group. We show that people treat in-group and out-group members alike with respect to binding morals, which concern preservation and protection of the group, but not individualizing morals, which concern preservation and protection of individual rights. Finally, in Study 3, we confirm the underlying motivating mechanism of reasserting in-group superiority by affirming the positive distinctiveness of one’s own group.
Article
Full-text available
One popular justification for punishment is the just deserts rationale: A person deserves punishment proportionate to the moral wrong committed. A competing justification is the deterrence rationale: Punishing an offender reduces the frequency and likelihood of future offenses. The authors examined the motivation underlying laypeople's use of punishment for prototypical wrongs. Study 1 (N = 336) revealed high sensitivity to factors uniquely associated with the just deserts perspective (e.g., offense seriousness, moral trespass) and insensitivity to factors associated with deterrence (e.g., likelihood of detection, offense frequency). Study 2 (N = 329) confirmed the proposed model through structural equation modeling (SEM). Study 3 (N = 351) revealed that despite strongly stated preferences for deterrence theory, individual sentencing decisions seemed driven exclusively by just deserts concerns.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this research was to develop broad, theoretically derived measure(s) of deviant behavior in the workplace. Two scales were developed: a 12-item scale of organizational deviance (deviant behaviors directly harmful to the organization) and a 7-item scale of interpersonal deviance (deviant behaviors directly harmful to other individuals within the organization). These scales were found to have internal reliabilities of .81 and .78, respectively. Confirmatory factor analysis verified that a 2-factor structure had acceptable fit. Preliminary evidence of construct validity is also provided. The implications of this instrument for future empirical research on workplace deviance are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
A group member voicing an opinion that differs from the modal group opinion may be considered a deviate and rejected by the group. We conducted a meta-analytic integration of 23 studies to examine the influence of proportionate group size on rejection of the deviate. The results suggest a strong tendency for rejection of the deviate to increase as the proportionate size of the deviate's subgroup decreases. These effects are moderated by the deviate's direction of movement (that is, whether the deviate consistently maintains one opinion, or changes opinions). Deviates consistently maintaining an opinion different from the modal group opinion were rejected more as the proportionate size of their subgroup decreased (r = -.23). In contrast, deviates changing opinion by sliding toward the modal group opinion were rejected less as the proportionate size of their subgroup decreased (r = .42).
Book
With contributions from leading scholars in the field, Rebels in Groups brings together the latest research which, contrary to traditional views, considers dissent, deviance, difference and defiance to be a normal and healthy aspect of group life. Brings together the latest research on the role of dissent, deviance, difference and defiance within groups Presents a new approach which considers dissent, deviance, difference and defiance to be a normal and healthy aspect of group life Examines a broad range of groups, such as political groups, task groups, and teams in organizations Considers diverse fields of psychology, including social, organizational, and developmental psychology Contributors are among the leading scholars in their areas of psychology.
Article
The Dissenter's DilemmaA Social Identity SolutionThe Normative Conflict ModelEmpirical EvidenceFinal ThoughtsPostscriptReferences
Chapter
Objective ImpostorsEye-of-the-Beholder ImpostorsWhat Does Social Psychology have to Say About Impostors?SummaryReferences