ArticlePDF Available

Preferences for Facial and Vocal Masculinity in Homosexual Men: The Role of Relationship Status, Sexual Restrictiveness, and Self-Perceived Masculinity

Authors:

Abstract

Studies on mate preferences usually examine heterosexual attraction; comparatively little is known about preferences of individuals whose sexuality is aimed at the same sex. We examined preferences of two groups of androphilic individuals--homosexual men and heterosexual women--for male facial and vocal level of masculinity. Facial images of 58 men and vocal recordings of 30 men were rated by 51 heterosexual women and 33 homosexual men for their attractiveness and masculinity--femininity. In both groups of raters, ratings of vocal attractiveness and masculinity were positively correlated, but there was no overall preference for facial masculinity. After splitting raters according to their relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-rated masculinity, we found significant preferences for masculine voices only in single homosexual men and coupled heterosexual women, while a preference for feminine male faces was found in coupled homosexual men. Furthermore, homosexual men describing themselves as relatively masculine significantly preferred masculine voices but also more feminine male faces. Our results demonstrate that conditional mate preferences are not restricted to heterosexual interactions, and homosexual men prefer a mixture of masculine and feminine traits in their potential male partners.
Perception, 2013, volume 42, pages 187 197
doi:10.1068/p6909
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual
men: the role of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness,
and self-perceived masculinity
Jaroslava Valentová1, S Craig Roberts2, Jan Havlíček3
1 Center for Theoretical Study, Charles University and the Academy of Sciences of the Czech
Republic, Jilska 1, 11000 Prague, Czech Republic; e-mail: valentova@cts.cuni.cz; 2 School
ofNatural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland, United Kingdom; 3 Department of
Zoology, Facultyof Science, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
Received 18 January 2011, in revised form 16 January 2013
Abstract. Studies on mate preferences usually examine heterosexual attraction; comparatively little
is known about preferences of individuals whose sexuality is aimed at the same sex. We examined
preferences of two groups of androphilic individuals—homosexual men and heterosexual women—
for male facial and vocal level of masculinity. Facial images of 58 men and vocal recordings of 30 men
were rated by 51 heterosexual women and 33 homosexual men for their attractiveness and masculinity–
femininity. In both groups of raters, ratings of vocal attractiveness and masculinity were positively
correlated, but there was no overall preference for facial masculinity. After splitting raters according
to their relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-rated masculinity, we found signicant
preferences for masculine voices only in single homosexual men and coupled heterosexual women,
while a preference for feminine male faces was found in coupled homosexual men. Furthermore,
homosexual men describing themselves as relatively masculine signicantly preferred masculine
voices but also more feminine male faces. Our results demonstrate that conditional mate preferences
are not restricted to heterosexual interactions, and homosexual men prefer a mixture of masculine and
feminine traits in their potential male partners.
Keywords: attractiveness, face, voice, sexual orientation
1 Introduction
A large body of recent empirical research has focused on aspects of human physical
attractiveness and its social and evolutionary implications. Although it is only part of the
broader picture, physical appearance undoubtedly plays a key role in mate choice decisions,
with signicant sex differences in preferences for different traits which, more often than not, are
robust across different cultures (Andersson and Iwasa 1996). In addition to characteristics such
as symmetry, averageness, and adiposity, sex-typical physical traits are perceived as attractive
by individuals of the opposite sex (Perrett et al 1998; Rhodes et al 2000). Expression of these
physical sex-typical traits are thought to be mediated by sex hormone action during prenatal
and early childhood, and/or at pubertal development (Johnston et al 2001), and to reliably
indicate an individual’s health, genetic quality and immunocompetence (Folstad and Karter
1992; Gangestad and Thornhill 2003; Yeo et al 1994; review in Roberts and Little 2008).
1.1 The effect of male sex-typical vocal and facial traits in opposite-sex preferences
One of the sexually dimorphic traits which develops under the inuence of pubertal
testosterone is voice pitch (a perceptual correlate of fundamental frequency). On average,
adult men produce lower-pitched voices than women and children (Hollien et al 1994).
The development of preferences for low voice pitch is linked to pubertal changes and
only becomes fully developed as adulthood approaches and mate choice decisions become
relevant (Saxton et al 2006, 2009b, 2009c). Lower-pitched male voices are rated as
masculine and attractive by female raters (Berry 1992; Collins 2000; Feinberg et al 2005a,
188 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček
2005b; Hodges-Simeon et al 2010; Oguchi and Kikuchi 1997; Puts 2005; Riding et al 2006;
Zuckermann et al 1995), are associated with perceived maturity of their owners (Childers
and Wu 1991; Wu and Childers 1991), perceived muscularity, age, and weight (Collins
2000), actual body size (Evans et al 2006, 2008; Puts et al 2012, Zuckermann et al 1995),
and actual level of testosterone (Dabbs and Mallinger 1999). Low voice pitch is also linked
to self-reported number of sexual partners (Puts 2005), and self-reported reproductive
success (Apicella et al 2007). Moreover, attractiveness of male voices predicted sexual
behaviour in males, such as lower age of rst sexual intercourse and higher number of
extra-pair sexual relationships (Hughes et al 2004). These relationships may arise as a
result of selection pressures on male vocal display (Puts et al 2006, 2012), such that higher
reproductive success of men with lower-pitched voices reect female preferences for male
dominance (Wells et al 2009).
Another extensively studied domain of physical attractiveness is facial appearance. As
with voices, since human faces display sex-dimorphic morphological traits, such as larger
jaws and prominent eyebrow ridges in men, it has been suggested that male facial masculinity
is related to attractiveness and mating success (Johnston et al 2001), and sociosexuality in
men (Boothroyd et al 2008).
Although preferences for sex-typical traits occur across cultures and are thus robust,
there is nonetheless substantial individual variation in strength of preference for such traits,
which has been particularly well-described in the face literature. Positive relationships have
been found between rated facial masculinity in males and attractiveness (Perrett et al 1998),
their actual health (Rhodes et al 2003), symmetry (Little et al 2008), and level of salivary
testosterone (Penton-Voak and Chen 2004). Relative strength of women’s preferences for male
traits are context-dependent, ie being inuenced by perception of females’ own attractiveness
(Little et al 2001), phase of menstrual cycle (Jones et al 2008), relationship status (Little et al
2002), or sexual restrictiveness (Burt et al 2007). Overall, it has been shown that women
who perceive themselves as more attractive, coupled women, those who seek short-term
relationships, and those who are less sexually restricted, show stronger preferences for male
facial masculinity.
1.2 Effect of male masculinity–femininity in same-sex preferences
In contrast with the extensive investigations of heterosexual partner preferences (Barber 1995;
Enquist et al 2002; Penton-Voak and Perrett 2000; Rhodes 2006; Roberts and Little 2008),
there is very limited research on partner preferences of homosexual individuals. It has been
reported that homosexual men show male-typical mating psychology, including interest in
casual sex and visual sexually explicit material (Bailey et al 1994); they also report male-
typical mate retention behaviour (Vanderlaan and Vasey 2008), prefer potential partners
who are younger than themselves (Hayes 2001; Silverthorne and Quinsey 2000) and, again
similarly to heterosexual men, value physical attractiveness in their potential partners more
than heterosexual women (Howard et al 1987). On the other hand, homosexual men prefer
as partners men who describe themselves as rather masculine, and this is dependent on their
own level of masculinity (Bailey et al 1997; Muscarella 2002). This is further supported
by a recent study that found that homosexual men prefer digitally masculinised male facial
pictures over feminised pictures (Glassenberg et al 2010). Except for the last-mentioned study,
the previous research on partner preferences of homosexual men is methodologically limited
to questionnaire data or data based on personal advertisements. To investigate preferences for
masculinity–femininity of homosexual men in more detail, in this study, we use non-manipulated
male facial pictures and vocal recordings.
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 189
1.3 Aims of the current study
The main aim of the current study was to investigate preferences for male facial and vocal
masculinity in androphilic individuals, ie men and women sexually attracted to men (thus,
we intentionally did not include ratings of heterosexual men and lesbian women, since they
are not sexually attracted to men), and to test effects of potentially modulating variables. In
line with the aforementioned ndings, we expect that lower-pitched male voices perceived
as masculine will in general be rated as attractive by both homosexual male and heterosexual
female raters, and because of individual condition-dependence, that there is no general
overall preference for facial masculinity. Based on ndings described above for heterosexual
preferences, we test for possible inuence of relationship status, sexual restrictiveness, and
self-perceived masculinity on preferences for both facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual
males and heterosexual females.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Participants
Facial photos and vocal recordings of 61 men (targets; mean age 23.3 years; SD 3.82 years;
range 18–35 years) were obtained for this study; 27 men were identied as homosexual, and
34 as heterosexual. There were no signicant differences between the two groups of target
men in age, type and level of education, and religious belief, but homosexual men reported
higher income (t55 = 5.37; p < 0.001). Except for two participants who were of Slovak origin
(both of them were homosexual and were excluded from the voice rating study), all of the
targets were Czech, Caucasian. The target sample was mainly recruited by distribution of
information yers in various faculties of Charles University in Prague, on gay web pages,
in bars, and by the snowball method; ie through social networks of the rst author who sent
an information e-mail about the research to her heterosexual and homosexual friends and
colleagues, and asking them to spread the information further. Data collection was performed
by the rst author during two summer months of 2006 (ie within one season), in order to
reduce possible seasonal effects (eg on skin colour due to tanning).
2.2 Sampling procedure
All data were collected under standard conditions at the Laboratory of Human Ethology and the
participants were each reimbursed by 300 CZK (approximately 17 USD) in compensation for
their time. All participants signed a consent form having been informed about the procedure,
and assured that data would be treated condentially and for scientic purposes only. They
further completed a battery of questionnaires collecting information on basic demographic
data, sexual orientation, and self-reported attractiveness.
2.3 Facial photographs
All targets were dressed in white T-shirts of appropriate size (which we provided) when
photos were taken, in order to standardise dress and reduce shadows in faces caused by
coloured clothes. Each participant used a black hairband to remove hair from the forehead.
Further, the participants were asked to remove earrings, facial jewellery, and to adopt a neutral
facial expression. The portraits were taken with a Canon 350D camera with the focus Canon
EF 50/1.8 II from a distance of 1.5 m. A light blue background was used to optimise white
balance, following digital adjustments. In order to eliminate possible inuences of hairstyle,
only faces with scalps covered were used for the ratings (procedure used in previous studies,
eg Pivonkova et al 2011). The scalps were covered using Photoshop 7.0 software, and faces
were placed on a black background.
190 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček
2.4 Vocal recordings
Vocal samples were recorded with a digital recorder Olympus WS310M with an external
Sennheiser E845-S microphone. Seated targets were asked to read aloud a standard paragraph
of an emotionally neutral text describing various concepts of a rainbow (Jacobs et al 2006)
which was translated into the Czech language. Each participant was familiarised with the
text before recordings were taken. To avoid potential effects of stress or fatigue, we extracted
an intermediate section (approximately 20 s out of 90 to 120 s) of the recording, similar to
the research of Jacobs and colleagues. SoundForge 8.0 software was used for the extraction
and the volume standardisation. All 59 voice samples (two individuals of Slovak origin were
excluded from the analyses and the voice of one participant was not recorded) were analysed
by Praat software (www.praat.org) for average fundamental frequency (F0) that ranged
from 86.4 to 191.8 Hz. Fundamental frequency was measured using Praat’s autocorrelation
algorithm with parameters set to a pitch oor of 75 Hz and a pitch ceiling of 300 Hz, with
all other values set to default. Fundamental frequencies were averaged across recordings for
each speaker.
2.5 Raters
The sample consisted of 50 heterosexual female raters (mean age 24.8 years, SD = 5.57 years,
range 17–42 years) and 33 homosexual male raters (mean age 28.7 years, SD = 6.08 years, range
19–48 years). Women were recruited in various public places in Prague (mostly open-air
cafés) with access to sit at the computer. Ratings took place during afternoons, and only
individuals who had not been drinking alcoholic beverages were recruited, since there is
some evidence that alcohol consumption may affect attractiveness ratings (Parker et al 2008).
Before rating, raters completed a short questionnaire on basic demographic data, including age,
use of hormonal contraception, and sexual orientation as assessed on a 7-point Kinsey scale
(0 = heterosexual; 6 = homosexual). Of 62 females who answered this question, 50 (81%)
rated themselves as heterosexual (ie scores of 0 and 1 on the Kinsey scale), and 12 (19%) as
bisexual (ie 2–4 on the Kinsey scale). For all analyses, we only included data from women
identifying as heterosexual. The average number of reported male sexual partners was 9.4
(with median 6). 6 women stated they had had at least one female sexual partner during their
lifetime. 20 women (39.2 %) reported being in a long-term relationship at the time of the
study and 29 female raters (56.9 %) were using hormonal contraception at the time of the study.
5 women stated they had at least one child.
Male raters were recruited in gay bars, by the snowball method, and by distributing
leaets in gay internet sites. 3 men did not complete the questionnaires. 33 men (89 %) rated
themselves as homosexual (5–6 on the Kinsey scale) and 4 (11 %) as bisexual (ie scores
of 3–5). All the analyses are based on male raters who identied themselves as homosexual.
The average number of reported male sexual partners was 68.1 (with median 20.0). 11 men
(29.7 %) reported having a long-term partner at the time of the study. 20 men (54.1 %) stated
they had had at least 1 female sexual partner during their lifetime. Raters also answered a
question on how masculine or feminine they would rate themselves (answers on a 7 point
scale, 1 = masculine, 7 = feminine).
Recruitment locations were chosen in order to minimise the between-location variability
(for factors such as lighting or surrounding noise). We also asked participants to complete
ratings while sitting at the researcher’s table in order to reduce possible distraction by their
friends. We note that differences across locations are more likely to reduce the chance of
nding signicant effects than to generate them. None of the raters was paid for his/her
participation.
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 191
2.6 Rating procedure
In this study we chose to use unmanipulated voices and faces to explore preferences instead
of experimental manipulation (eg manipulation of vocal fundamental frequency, or masculine
facial traits) of facial and vocal masculinity. By using this method, we can estimate the effect
of perceived masculinity in faces and voices with other acoustic or visual parameters that are
freely varying. It was shown that vocal fundamental frequency correlates with other acoustic
parameters (Hodges-Simeon et al 2010) but can be used as a proxy measure of overall
vocal masculinity (Puts 2006). Furthermore, we were not only interested in objectively
measured vocal or facial masculinity, which can be experimentally manipulated, but also in
the relationship between perceptions of attractiveness, and perceptions of facial and vocal
masculinity.
We randomly chose only half of the vocal recordings because of time constraints for raters
(the rating took approximately 20 min in total) and each rater rated only 1 parameter to avoid
carry-over effects. Each rater judged rstly the vocal recordings and then the whole sample
of facial images (each photo and vocal recording of a heterosexual individual was alternated
by a photo or vocal recording of a homosexual individual) for attractiveness or masculinity
on a 7 point scale (1 = attractive/masculine, 7 = not attractive/feminine). Of 50 female raters,
33 rated both facial and vocal stimuli and 17 rated only facial stimuli. All homosexual male
raters rated both facial and vocal stimuli. Image ratings were carried out on a laptop screen
with resolution of 1280 × 800 pixels using ImageRater 1.3 software specically developed
for our purposes. Vocal recordings were rated with the same laptop with Koss headphones.
2.7 Statistical analyses
Analyses were carried out with SPSS v.16.0. Initially, data normality was tested by visual
examination of histograms, and the assumptions of univariate normality for rating scores
was checked with Shapiro–Wilks’s W test. In several cases data violated the assumption of
normality, we have thus used nonparametric correlations to test for relationships between
the variables. Moreover, to compute possible differences between the two correlations, we
used Fisher’s Z. We then did some exploratory analyses to prepare for the planned inferential
testing.
First, using Mann–Whitney U tests, we tested for possible differences between homo-
sexual and heterosexual targets in judged attractiveness and masculinity. The ratings of
attractiveness and masculinity for both vocal and facial stimuli did not signicantly differ
between homosexual and heterosexual targets as rated either by male or female raters (all
p s > 0.12); therefore the ratings were further analysed for both groups of targets together.
Furthermore, both female and male raters showed high consistency in their facial attract-
iveness judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.965 and 0.940, respectively) and vocal attractiveness
judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.627 and 0.763, respectively); they also showed high consistency
in their facial masculinity judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.938 and 0.942, respectively) and
vocal masculinity judgments (Cronbach’s a = 0.897 and 0.620, respectively). Ratings of vocal
(r = 0.646; N = 30; p < 0.001) and facial (r = 0.752; N = 61; p < 0.001) attractiveness among
heterosexual females and homosexual males were highly correlated. Similarly, ratings of
vocal (r = 0.891; N = 30; p < 0.001) and facial (r = 0.775; N = 61; p < 0.001) masculinity
were highly correlated among heterosexual females and homosexual males.
The preferences for facial and vocal masculinity were analysed by Spearman’s
correlations, with mean ratings for each target as the unit of analysis. In order to test for a
possible effect of relationship status, we split the raters into two groups based on their self-
report. Similarly, to test for possible inuence of sexual restrictiveness, we split the raters
by the median of their number of reported sexual partners (we describe the group below the
median as restricted and those above the median as unrestricted). There was no signicant
192 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček
difference in age between coupled and single heterosexual women (Mann–Whitney U = 296;
N = 51; p = 0.786), nor in age or self-ascribed masculinity–femininity between coupled
and single homosexual men (Mann–Whitney U = 79.5; N = 32; p = 0.715; Mann–Whitney
U = 75; N = 32; p = 0.552, respectively). However, in both women and men, those with a
higher number of sexual partners were signicantly older than more restricted individuals
(Mann–Whitney U = 160.5; N = 51; p = 0.002; Mann–Whitney U = 38.5; N = 29; p = 0.004,
respectively).
Male raters were also categorised according to their self-rated masculinity (also using
a median split), with those below the median being described as feminine and those above
as masculine. To adjust for multiple comparisons, Bonferroni corrections were applied.
Finally, to test for the possible inuence of voice pitch on general vocal attractiveness and
masculinity ratings, we used simple linear regression with rated parameters (attractiveness
or masculinity) as dependent variables, and age and voice pitch as predictors.
3 Results
3.1 General preferences
To test our main hypothesis, we ran a series of non-parametric correlations between rated
masculinity–femininity and attractiveness. In homosexual male raters, rated vocal masculinity
was strongly and positively correlated with vocal attractiveness (Spearman’s t = 0.745; N = 30;
p < 0.001), and the same effect was found in heterosexual female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.536;
N = 30; p = 0.002). There was no signicant difference between the correlation coefcients
( p = 0.18). Concerning facial ratings, we found a non-signicant trend towards a negative
correlation between rated facial masculinity and attract iveness in homosexual male raters
(Spearman’s t = – 0.246; N = 61; p = 0.056), but no evidence of a signicant relationship
between these variables in heterosexual female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.064; N = 61; p = 0.625).
These correlations did not differ signicantly ( p = 0.08).
3.2 The effect of relationship status
We then split the raters according to their relationship status (for all subsequent analyses
we applied Bonferroni correction, α = 0.0125). The positive relationship between vocal
attractiveness and masculinity remained signicant only in single homosexual male raters
(Spearman’s t = 0.594; N = 30; p = 0.001), but no signicant results were found in coupled
homosexual males and heterosexual females ( p s > 0.06). There was a signicantly higher
correlation between vocal attractiveness and masculinity in single homosexual male raters
than in coupled homosexual males (Z = 2.49; p = 0.012), and in coupled heterosexual
females than in single females (Z = 2.36; p = 0.018).
Concerning facial ratings, we found a signicant negative relationship between rated facial
masculinity–femininity and attractiveness only in coupled homosexual men (Spearman’s
t = – 0.371; N = 61; p = 0.003), while it remained non-signicant in other groups of raters
( p s > 0.21). Fisher’s r-to-z test revealed that this relationship was signicantly stronger
in coupled than in single homosexual male raters (Z = 2.98; p = 0.002), but there was no
signicant difference in female ratings ( p = 0.36).
3.3 The effect of sexual restrictiveness
After dividing the sample according to the median number of sexual partners, signicant
positive relationships between vocal attractiveness and masculinity–femininity were found
in both restricted (Spearman’s t = 0.724; N = 30; p < 0.001) and unrestricted (Spearman’s
t = 0.603; N = 30; p < 0.001) homosexual male raters. There was also a trend (α = 0.0125)
in restricted (Spearman’s t = 0.404; N = 30; p = 0.027), and a signicant relationship in
unrestricted, female raters (Spearman’s t = 0.542; N = 30; p < 0.002). There was no signicant
difference between correlation coefcients among any of the groups of raters (all p s > 0.07).
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 193
In facial ratings, unrestricted homosexual men tended (α = 0.0125) to prefer less masculine
faces (Spearman’s t = – 0.294; N = 61; p = 0.022), while there was a trend towards a positive
correlation in women, with unrestricted women tending to prefer more masculine faces
(Spearman’s t = 0.284; N = 61; p = 0.027). The relationship was not signicant in the other
groups of raters ( p > 0.112). Sexually unrestricted homosexual men preferred signicantly
more feminine male faces than unrestricted heterosexual women (Z = 3.2; p = 0.001).
3.4 Effect of self-rated masculinity in male raters
Correlations between vocal masculinity and attractiveness were positive and signicant in
both masculine and feminine homosexual men (Spearman’s t = 0.750; N = 30; p < 0.001;
Spearman’s t = 0.496; N = 30; p = 0.005, respectively), and these coefcients did not differ
signicantly. Furthermore, rated facial masculinity was negatively associated with attractiveness
in homosexual raters who described themselves as masculine (Spearman’s t = – 0.364; N = 61;
p = 0.004), while this relationship remained non-signicant in feminine homosexual raters
(Spearman’s t = 0.067; N = 61; p = 0.608). Fisher’s r-to-z test revealed that this relationship
was signicantly different in feminine homosexual men than in masculine men (Z = 2.42;
p = 0.015).
3.5 Voice pitch as a predictor of rated vocal attractiveness and masculinity
To test for an effect of voice pitch of the rated individuals on rated attractiveness and masculinity,
we ran linear regressions with voice pitch of the target individuals entered as independent
variable, and rated attractiveness and masculinity as separate dependent variables. Because
voice pitch was positively correlated with age, age of the target participants also entered as
another independent variable. Rated vocal attractiveness was signicantly predicted by voice
pitch in both male and female ratings (men: t = 2.623; b = 0.476; p = 0.014; women: t = 3.990;
b = 0.649; p < 0.001). Similarly, rated vocal masculinity was signicantly predicted by voice
pitch in both male and female ratings (t = 3.955; b = 0.650; p < 0.001; t = 5.078; b = 0.746;
p < 0.001).
3.6 Additional analyses
We also tested the relationship between ratings of attractiveness and masculinity between the
two studied modalities (ie vocal and visual). We found no signicant correlation between
facial and vocal stimuli either in ratings of attractiveness or masculinity, in either group of
raters (all p s > 0.3).
Finally, to test whether other-rated attractiveness from facial and vocal stimuli predicts
self-rated attractiveness of the targets, we ran a linear regression with self-rated attractiveness
as the dependent variable and facial and vocal attractiveness (as rated by both groups of
raters) entered as independent variables. This model explained 27% of the variance, and we
found that self-rated attractiveness was signicantly predicted only by facial attractiveness
as rated by homosexual male raters (t = 2.130; b = 0.597; p = 0.043).
4 Discussion
In this study, we aimed to test whether vocal and facial preferences of androphilic men
and women are linked to male sex-typicality in targets. As predicted, we found that
both homosexual men and heterosexual women prefer male voices which are perceived
masculine, and which are low in pitch. In contrast, there was no general preference for
masculinity in facial stimuli. Next, we investigated the possible inuences of relationship
status, sexual restrictiveness, and self-perceived masculinity on preference for masculinity.
We found a signicant effect of relationship status on preferences for masculinity in voices
which, however, showed opposite patterns in homosexual male and heterosexual female
raters. In facial ratings, we also found this opposing direction in preference among sexually
194 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček
unrestricted women and men. Finally, the results suggest that homosexual men describing
themselves as relatively masculine prefer masculine male voices but more feminine male faces.
Our results, in general, indicate that androphilic individuals, irrespective of their sex,
prefer sex-typical male voices over high-pitched, feminine voices. This result is in agreement
with previous studies reporting similar preferences in English-speaking female raters (Collins
2000; Feinberg et al 2005a, 2005b; Riding et al 2006), and suggests that this preference is not
specic to a particular language, sex or sexual orientation.
Nevertheless, this preference is modulated by the raters’ relationship status. It is suggested
that masculinity in men provides a cue to underlying genetic quality, and is thus preferred
by women. However, the faces of masculine men are associated with perceived traits that
indicate lower investment in potential offspring, and masculinity might be thus relatively
preferred by women who already have a long-term relationship with a man willing to invest
in their offspring. However, it should be noted that the evidence supporting this assumption is
mixed (eg Gangestad and Simpson 2000; Pawlowski and Zelazniewicz 2012). Furthermore,
this effect could also be due to an exposure effect, as coupled women are exposed on a daily
basis to their male partner, which might shift their preference towards more masculine faces
(eg Saxton et al 2009c).
In contrast, we found an opposite pattern in homosexual male raters: single homosexual
men preferred more masculine male voices and faces than coupled men, which would be
inconsistent with an exposure effect. Previous research has shown general preferences
in homosexual men for potential sexual partners who describe themselves as relatively
masculine (Bailey et al 1997) and also preferences for masculinised over feminised male
faces (Glassenberg et al 2010). However, none of the previous studies tested possible effects
of relationship status on these preferences, and we show that these preferences might be more
pronounced in single homosexual men.
Furthermore, we also found an effect of self-perceived masculinity on facial and vocal
preferences, showing that homosexual men who describe themselves as relatively masculine
prefer more masculine male voices, but also more feminine male faces. To our knowledge,
only one other study has investigated this, in personal advertisements, where more masculine
homosexual men preferred men describing themselves as masculine (Bailey et al 1997). Here
we show that self-perceived masculinity also inuences preferences of homosexual men, but
rather than seeking either sex-typical or atypical traits in their potential partners, it seems they
prefer a mosaic of masculine and feminine traits. This is consistent with our result showing
that there was no correlation between judged attractiveness across the two modalities. Several
previous studies have claimed that different cues in one individual signal the same underlying
quality, particularly in men (eg Feinberg et al 2008; Roberts et al 2011; Saxton et al 2006,
2009a). However, in our study, facial attractiveness was not associated with vocal attractiveness,
and facial masculinity did not correlate with vocal masculinity. Thus, our targets were rated
differently in the two modalities by both groups of raters. This nding is consistent with Collins
(2000), who found no correlations between male voice pitch and either measured or estimated
bodily characteristics. In her study, voices with low frequency were rated as attractive, and their
owners as heavier, older, and more likely to be hairy-chested. Nevertheless, these estimations
did not relate to the measured or self-reported qualities of the rated individuals. Thus, facial and
vocal masculinity–femininity in men might be associated with different qualities—while vocal
masculinity might cue to positively valued social dominance or maturity (Wells et al 2009),
masculine faces might be rather associated with aggressiveness or impulsivity (Perrett et al 1998).
It is also worth noting that our results show that, with increasing age, male faces were
rated as more masculine but not as more attractive, which is in agreement with a previous
study (Boothroyd et al 2005). Consequently, masculine physiognomy in faces (and not low
voice pitch) might evoke perceptions of higher age or dominance, rather than attractiveness.
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 195
The current study has also several limitations. For example, although raters were asked
about their relationship status, other details such as their relationship length, general and sexual
satisfaction within the relationship, and extra-pair sexual activities might have explained
additional variation in raters’ responses.
In summary, our results show that conditional mate preferences are not restricted to hetero-
sexual interactions. Circumstances such as relationship status or self-perceived masculinity
inuence preferences for male sex-typical traits in both females and homosexual males.
Acknowledgments. We would like to thank to all participants of this study and in particular to the gay
community in Prague who helped us with participants’ recruitment. We also wish to thank our colleagues
and friends, particularly to Martin SVU Cech, for their help with data collection. JV was supported
by the Czech Science Foundation (GACR 13-16959P), JH was supported by the Czech Science
Foundation grant (P407/10/1303) and both JV and JH were supported by Charles University Research
Center (UNCE 204004).
References
Andersson M, Iwasa Y, 1996 “Sexual selection” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11 53–58
Apicella C L, Feinberg D R, Marlowe F W, 2007 “Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male
hunter-gatherers” Biology Letters 3 682–684
Bailey J M, Gaulin S, Agyei Y, Gladue B A, 1994 “Effects of gender and sexual orientation on
evolutionarily relevant aspects of human mating psychology” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 66 1074–1080
Bailey J M, Kim P, Hills A, Linsenmeier J, 1997 “Butch, femme, or straight acting? Partner preferences
of gay men and lesbians” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 960–973
Barber N, 1995 “The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: sexual selection and human
morphology” Ethology and Sociobiology 16 395–424
Berry D S, 1992 “Vocal types and stereotypes: Joint effects of vocal attractiveness and vocal maturity
on person perception” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 16 41–54
Boothroyd L G, Jones B C, Burt D M, Cornwell R E, Little A C, Tiddeman B P, Perrett D I, 2005
“Facial masculinity is related to perceived age but not perceived health” Evolution and Human
Behavior 26 417–431
Boothroyd L G, Jones B C, Burt D M, DeBruine L M, Perrett D I, 2008 “Facial correlates of
sociosexuality” Evolution and Human Behavior 29 211–218
Burt D M, Kentridge R W, Good J M M, Perrett D I, Tiddeman B P, Boothroyd L G, 2007 “Q-cgi:
New techniques to assess variation in perception applied to facial attractiveness” Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London B 274 2779–2784
Childers D G, Wu K, 1991 “Gender recognition from speech. Part II: Fine analysis” Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 90 1841–1856
Collins S A, 2000 “Men’s voices and women’s choices” Animal Behaviour 60 773–780
Dabbs J M, Mallinger A, 1999 “High testosterone levels predict low voice pitch among men” Personality
and Individual Differences 27 801–804
Evans S, Neave N, Wakelin D, 2006 “Relationships between vocal characteristics and body size and
shape in human males: an evolutionary explanation for a deep male voice” Biological Psychology
72 160–163
Evans S, Neave N, Wakelin D, Hamilton C, 2008 “The relationship between testosterone and vocal
frequencies in human males” Physiological Behavior 93 783–788
Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Lundquist D, Wachtmeister C-A, 2002 “An ethological theory of attractiveness”,
in Facial Attractiveness: Evolutionary, Cognitive, and Social Perspectives. Advances in Visual
Cognition Eds G Rhodes, L Zebrowitz (Westport, CT: Ablex) pp 127–151
Feinberg D R, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Little A C, 2008 “Correlated preferences for men’s facial and
vocal masculinity” Evolution and Human Behavior 29 233–241
Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Burt D M, Perrett D I, 2005a “Manipulations of fundamental and formant
frequencies inuence the attractiveness of human male voice” Animal Behaviour 69 561–568
196 J Valentová, S C Roberts, J Havlíček
Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Smith M J L, Moore F R, DeBruine L, Cornwell R E, Hillier S G, Perrett D I,
2005b “Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice”
Hormones and Behavior 49 215–222
Folstad I, Karter A J, 1992 “Parasites, bright males and the immunocompetence handicap” The American
Naturalist 139 603–622
Gangestad S W, Simpson J A, 2000 “The evolution of human mating: trade-off and strategic pluralism”
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 573–644
Gangestad S W, Thornhill R, 2003 “Facial masculinity and uctuating asymmetry” Evolution and
Human Behavior 24 231–241
Glassenberg A N, Feinberg D R, Jones B C, Little A C, DeBruine L M, 2010 “Sex-dimorphic face
shape preference in heterosexual and homosexual men and women” Archives of Sexual Behavior
39 1289–1296
Hayes A F, 2001 “Age preferences for same- and opposite-sex partners” Journal of Social Psychology
135 125–133
Hodges-Simeon C R, Gaulin S J, Puts D A, 2010 “Different vocal parameters predict perceptions of
dominance and attractiveness” Human Nature 21 406–427
Hollien H, Green R, Massey K, 1994 “Longitudinal research on adolescent voice change in males”
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96 2646–2654
Howard J A, Blumstein P, Schwartz P, 1987 “Social or evolutionary theories? Some observations on
preferences in human mate selection” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53 194–200
Hughes S M, Dispenza F, Gallup G G, 2004 “Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior
and body conguration” Evolution and Human Behavior 25 295–304
Jacobs G, Smyth R, Rogers H, 2006 “Language and sexuality: Searching for the phonetic correlates of
gay- and straight-sounding male voices” Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 18 46–61
Johnston V S, Hagel R, Franklin M, Fink B, Grammer K, 2001 “Male facial attractiveness—Evidence
for hormone-mediated adaptive design” Evolution and Human Behavior 22 251–267
Jones B C, DeBruine L M, Perrett D I, Little A C, Feinberg D R, Law Smith M J, 2008 “Effects of
menstrual cycle phase on face preferences” Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 78–84
Little A C, Burt D M, Penton-Voak I S, Perrett D I, 2001 “Self-perceived attractiveness inuences
human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces” Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London B 268 39–44
Little A C, Jones B C, Penton-Voak I S, Burt D M, Perrett D I, 2002 “Partnership status and the
temporal context of relationships inuence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in
male face shape” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 269 1095–1100
Little A C, Jones B C, Waitt C, Tiddeman B P, Feinberg D R, Perrett D I, Apicella C L, Marlowe F W,
2008 “Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species” PLoS
ONE 3 e2106
Muscarella F, 2002 “Preferred partner characteristics in homosexual men in relation to speculated
patterns of brain differentiation” Neuroendocrinology Letters 23 299–302
Oguchi T, Kikuchi H, 1997 “Voice and interpersonal attraction” Japanese Psychological Research 39
56–61
Parker L L C, Penton-Voak I S, Attwood A S, Munafo M R, 2008 “Effects of acute alcohol consumption
on ratings of attractiveness of facial stimuli: Evidence of long-term encoding” Alcohol and
Alcoholism 43 636–640
Pawlowski B, Zelazniewicz A, 2012 “Newborn’s condition at birth does not depend on maternal
sexual strategy—evidence against the ‛hunting for good genes’ hypothesis” American Journal of
Human Biology 24 420–424
Penton-Voak I S, Chen J Y, 2004 “High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial
appearance in humans” Evolution and Human Behavior 25 229–241
Penton-Voak I S, Perrett D, 2000 “Consistency and individual differences in facial attractiveness
judgements: an evolutionary perspective” Social Research 67 219–244
Perrett D I, Lee K J, Penton-Voak I S, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt D M, Henzil S P, Castles D L,
Akamatsu S, 1998 “Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness” Nature 394 884–887
Preferences for facial and vocal masculinity in homosexual men 197
Pivonkova V, Rubesova A, Lindova J, Havlicek J, 2011 “Sexual dimorphism and personality attributions
in male faces” Archives of Sexual Behavior 40 1137–1143
Puts D A, 2005 “Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch”
Evolution and Human Behavior 26 388–397
Puts D A, 2006 “Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits: Potential hormonal
causes” Human Nature 17 114–127
Puts D A, Apicella C L, Cárdenas R A, 2012 “Masculine voices signal men’s threat potential in forager
and industrial societies” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 279 601–609
Puts D A, Gaulin S J C, Verdolini K, 2006 “Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in
human voice pitch” Evolution and Human Behavior 27 283–296
Rhodes G, 2006 “The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty” Annual Review of Psychology 57
199–226
Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz L A, Simmons L W, 2003 “Does sexual dimorphism in human faces
signal health?” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270 S93–S95
Rhodes G, Hickford C, Jeffrey L, 2000 “Sex-typicality and attractiveness: Are supermale and
superfemale faces super-attractive?” British Journal of Psychology 91 125–140
Riding D, Lonsdale D, Brown B, 2006 “The effects of average fundamental frequency and variance of
fundamental frequency on male vocal attractiveness to women” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
30 55–61
Roberts S C, Little A C, 2008 “Good genes, complementary genes and human mate preferences”
Genetica 134 31–43
Roberts S C, Ferdenzi C, Kralevich A, Little A C, Jones B C, DeBruine L M, Havlicek J, 2011 “Body odor
quality predicts behavioral attractiveness in humans” Archives of Sexual Behavior 40 1111–1117
Saxton T K, Burriss R P, Murray A K, Rowland H M, Roberts S C, 2009a “Face, body and speech cues
independently predict judgments of attractiveness” Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 7 23–35
Saxton T K, Caryl P G, Roberts S C, 2006 “Vocal and facial attractiveness judgments of children,
adolescents and adults: the ontogeny of mate choice” Ethology 112 1179–1185
Saxton T K, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Little A C, Roberts S C, 2009b “Face and voice attractiveness
judgments change during adolescence” Evolution and Human Behavior 30 398–408
Saxton T K, Little A C, DeBruine L M, Jones B C, Roberts S C, 2009c “Adolescents’ preferences for
sexual dimorphism are inuenced by relative exposure to male and female faces” Personality and
Individual Differences 47 864–868
Silverthorne Z A, Quinsey V L, 2000 “Sexual partner age preferences of homosexual and heterosexual
men and women” Archives of Sexual Behavior 29 67–76
Vanderlaan D, Vasey P, 2008 “Mate retention behavior of men and women in heterosexual and
homosexual relationships” Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 572–585
Wells T J, Dunn A K, Sergeant M J T, Davies M N O, 2009 “Multiple signals in human mate selection:
a review and framework for integrating facial and vocal signals” Journal of Evolutionary Psychology
7 111–139
Wu K, Childers D G, 1991 “Gender recognition from speech. Part I: Coarse analysis” Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America 90 1828–1840
Yeo R A, Thornhill R, Gangestad S W, 1994 “Facial attractiveness, developmental stability, and
uctuating asymmetry” Ethology and Sociobiology 15 73–85
Zuckermann M, Miyake K, Elkin C S, 1995 “Effects of attractiveness and maturity of face and voice
on interpersonal impressions” Journal of Research in Personality 29 253–272
... Focusing firstly on women's preferences for masculinity in men's faces, the evidence that women are attracted to masculinity is very mixed, using both real (e.g., Boothroyd et al., 2013;Ferdenzi et al., 2011;Fiala et al., 2021;Foo et al., 2017;Geniole & McCormick, 2013;Hönekopp, 2013;Koehler et al., 2004;Kočnar et al., 2019;Neave et al., 2003;O'Toole et al., 1998;Peters et al., 2008;Rhodes et al., 2003;Scheib et al., 1999;Scott et al., 2010;Stephen et al., 2012;Valentová et al., 2013;Waynforth et al., 2005) and computer-manipulated facial images (e.g., Alharbi et al., 2020;Alharbi et al., 2021;Boothroyd et al., 2009;Ciocca et al., 2014;Clarkson et al., 2020;de Lurdes Carrito et al., 2016;DeBruine et al., 2006DeBruine et al., , 2010aDeBruine et al., , 2010cDeBruine et al., , 2019Dixson et al., 2018;Docherty et al., 2020;Escasa-Dorne et al., 2017;Feinberg et al., 2008;Garza & Byrd-Craven, 2023;Geniole & MCCormick, 2013;Holzleitner & Perrett, 2017;Johnston et al., 2001;Jones et al., 2011Jones et al., , 2018Limoncin et al., 2015;Little et al., 2001Little et al., , 2007Little & Hancock, 2002;Marcinkowska et al., 2016Marcinkowska et al., , 2018bMarcinkowska et al., , 2018cMarcinkowska et al., , 2019aMarcinkowska et al., , 2021Muñoz-Reyes et al., 2014;Penton-Voak & Chen, 2004;Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000;Perrett et al., 1998;Rhodes et al., 2000;Rennels et al., 2008;Roney et al., 2011;Watkins et al., 2012;Welling et al., 2007Welling et al., , 2008Yang et al., 2015;Zietsch et al., 2015). Rather, women's preferences appear to be multifaceted and subject to individual differences. ...
... While the attraction literature shows a strong heterosexual bias in that individuals who identify as LGBT+ are severely undersampled in comparison to those who identify as heterosexual, emerging research indicate that sexual orientation predicts mate preferences. Namely, gay men generally appear to show preferences for masculinity in men's faces (Ciocca et al., 2014;Glassenberg et al., 2010;Zheng, 2019a;Zheng & Zhang, 2021; but see also Valentová et al., 2013), although this has been studied considerably less than women's preferences for men's traits. ...
... "Versatiles" (those who variously take a penetrating and receiving sexual role) show overall preferences for masculinity, but weaker than bottoms do (Zheng, 2021;Zheng & Zhang, 2021). In addition, mixed findings have been reported as to whether masculinity preferences differ between partnered and single gay men (Cassar et al., 2020;Valentová et al., 2013;Zheng, 2019b). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
... Although some studies suggest that homosexual men may prefer masculine facial characteristics (Glassenberg et al., 2010;Zhang et al., 2018;Zheng, 2019b), others suggest no preferences among homosexual men for either masculine or feminine facial features when evaluating both manipulated and non-manipulated images (Valentová et al., 2013;Welling et al., 2013). Thus, further examination of homosexual men's preferences for sexually dimorphic facial characteristics is needed . ...
... More feminine females were perceived to be more likely to be unfaithful, pursue short-term relationships, and engage in cuckoldry (Boothroyd et al., 2007;Little et al., 2014;Varella et al., 2014). Regarding homosexual men, preferences for masculine faces have been reported (Valentová et al., 2013;Zhang et al., 2018). Although preference for facial masculinity in homosexual and bisexual men cannot be attributed to health-related expressions of masculine features in terms of offspring and fertility, both heterosexual and homosexual men may need to balance between preferences for more attractive partners and involvement with more faithful partners (Little et al., 2014;Zheng, 2019b). ...
... Therefore, in this study, life circumstances and place of residence were treated as factors that might influence males' facial dimorphic preference. Relationship status has been found to relate to not only how women judge men's attractiveness (Conway et al., 2010;Holzleitner & Perrett, 2017;Lyons et al., 2016) but also homosexual men's preference for their potential partners' faces and tone of voice (Valentová et al., 2013;Zheng, 2019b). A higher preference for male facial masculinity was found in women with partners than in women without partners (Conway et al., 2010;Little et al., 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Frequent consumption of sexually explicit material (SEM) on the internet may influence attitudes toward sex (e.g., sociosexuality), and sociosexuality may influence people's preferences for potential partners' facial dimorphism. However, few studies have focused on the association between dimorphism preference and pornography consumption, and fewer have examined it in heterosexual‐identifying and homosexual‐identifying men. A total of 234 heterosexual‐identifying and 133 homosexual‐identifying men were asked to respond to questions regarding their preferences toward facial dimorphism, sociosexual orientation, and frequency of viewing SEM. Frequency of SEM consumption was related to sociosexuality and preference for potential partners' sex‐typical facial features, sociosexuality worked as a full mediator in this relationship, and similar mediating associations were found among heterosexual‐identifying and homosexual‐identifying men. The results identified associations between consumption of pornographic material and sexual dimorphic preference for the first time. Findings suggest not only that homosexual‐identifying men prefer masculinity in same‐sex partners but also that the sexual script theory that pornographic material consumption may influence men's short‐term relationship orientation among sexual minorities and under a conservative sexual culture. This study contributes to the understanding of the preference for facial dimorphism from a perspective on social learning and mate choice.
... Alternatively, Grindr users' preferences may have motivated bisexual speakers to emphasize the masculinity of their voices. Multiple lab and field studies demonstrate a demand bias for masculine over feminine partners amongst men seeking men (Bailey et al., 1997;Cascalheira & Smith, 2020;Reynolds, 2015;Sánchez & Vilain, 2012;Sarson, 2020;Zheng & Zheng, 2016) and gay men deem voices they consider more masculine to also be more attractive (Valentová et al., 2013). That said, any motivation to appear more masculine and less feminine would apply equally to our gay speakers who were judged the most feminine sounding. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has identified a range of perceptual voice and speech features that differ between gay and straight men, enabling listeners to determine if a man is gay or straight at a rate better than chance from his voice alone. To date, no published studies have examined if bisexual men's voices differ from gay and straight men's voices with regard to perceived masculinity-femininity - nor whether listeners can identify a bisexual man based only on his voice. In the present study, we examined if listeners could identify bisexual men's sexual identities from voice recordings. Seventy participants (N= 70) rated 60 voice recordings of a sample of 20 gay, 20 bisexual, and 20 straight Australian men on perceived sexual orientation and degree of masculinity-femininity. Participants could correctly categorize the sexual orientations of the gay and straight speakers at rates greater than chance, but bisexual men were only identified at chance. Bisexual voices were consistently misperceived as being the most exclusively female attracted, and, contrary to expectations, were perceived as the most masculine sounding of all the speakers. Together, these findings suggest that while the voices of bisexual men in our sample were perceived as more masculine and female attracted, listeners do not associate this impression with bisexuality, and thus cannot identify bisexual men from their voices. Consequently, while bisexual men appear to be at lower risk of facing voice-based identification and discrimination than gay men, they may be often misperceived as being straight.
... Most existing research nds that gay men prefer relatively masculinized male faces such as those with lower and more prominent brow ridges, wider jaws, and larger chins (Cassar et al., 2020;Glassenberg et al., 2010;Shiramizu et al., 2020) or are roughly equally likely to prefer feminized male faces and masculinized male faces (Welling et al., 2013). One study, however, found that gay men who described themselves as more masculine preferred relatively more feminine male faces (Valentova et al., 2013). Gay men were also more likely to prefer facial hair (and body hair) than heterosexual women . ...
Chapter
The Oxford Handbook of Human Mating covers the contributions and up-to-date theories and empirical evidence from scientists regarding human mating strategies. The scientific studies of human mating have only recently risen, revealing fresh discoveries about mate attraction, mate choice, marital satisfaction, and other topics. Darwin’s sexual selection theory primarily guides most of the research in the scientific study of mating strategies. Indeed, research on the complexities of human mate competition and mate choice has centred around Darwin’s classic book. This book discusses theories of human mating; mate selection and mate attraction; mate competition; sexual conflict in mating; human pair bonding; the endocrinology of mating; and mating in the modern world.
... Thus, men's preferences for women's voices and same-sex competition between women may have also contributed to sexual dimorphism in the human voice, albeit to a lesser extent than did women's preferences for men's traits and male-male contests (Hughes & Puts, 2021;Puts et al., 2012 for reviews). Human sexual relationships also span a variety of nontraditional and nonheterosexual forms that have been largely neglected in research on human mate preferences (but see Valentová et al., 2013;Zhang et al., 2018). Mate preferences furthermore vary, sometimes considerably, across human populations as a function of variable ecological conditions and sociocultural norms that impose differential cost-benet trade-offs in mate choice decisions, highlighting an urgent need to study voice preferences across diverse cultures (Bryant, 2021;Pisanski & Feinberg, 2013 for reviews). ...
Chapter
Acoustic features of the voice, including fundamental and formant frequencies, communicate qualities of the vocalizer. The preferences of potential mates may have shaped these vocal parameters in both sexes over human evolution, contributing not only to large sexual dimorphisms but also to patterns of vocal modulation that leverage mate preferences.
Chapter
Face preferences and facial attractiveness influence broad and varied aspects of social behavior, including partner choices, hiring decisions, and voting behavior. Past research has identified several visual parameters that influence the attractiveness of faces. This chapter outlines prominent theories of attraction: the perceptual bias account (which proposes that attractiveness judgments are a functionless by-product of the visual recognition system) and the evolutionary advantage account (which proposes that attractiveness judgments are psychological adaptations that identify high quality potential mates). Next, this chapter summarizes the literature surrounding the relationship between expression, self-resemblance, apparent health, youthfulness, averageness, symmetry, and sexual dimorphism (i.e., masculinity and femininity) and perceived attractiveness. In line with the evolutionary advantage view, the literature supports the proposal that facial attractiveness is a potentially adaptive indicator of mate quality in men and women. This chapter concludes with discussion on the relevance of this work to transgender and gender diverse people seeking gender-affirming surgeries and emphasizes the need for additional research using gender and sexual minority participants.
Article
Body image among sexual minority men (SMM) has received increasing attention. However, the current literature has primarily focused on white SMM and body image and physical appearance concerns among SMM. In response to a call for more nuance in understanding how SMM perceive and make sense of their body image in a broader sociocultural context, we conducted a scoping review to examine the extent and nature of body image research among SMM in the Mekong region. We searched CINAHL, MEDLINE, Scopus, PsycInfo-1806 (Ovid), ProQuest Central, Social Science Premium Collection, and Web of Science and identified 25 studies that met the inclusion criteria. Overall, the findings suggest that body image research in the Mekong region is limited in the range of topics, study populations, and methods, with quantitative studies on preferences for masculine physical traits among young and well-educated Chinese SMM dominating. Furthermore, the current literature is primarily influenced by Western scholarship, which predominantly uses Western measurement and theories. We encourage scholars from other countries in the region, including Vietnam, Laos, Cambodia, Myanmar, and Thailand, to engage sociocultural approaches in research on the body image of SMM.
Chapter
This chapter primarily covers the nonverbal expression of sexual interest in cisgender, heterosexual individuals, from their libidinal urges to their post-sex behaviors, with a major focus on flirtation. The focus on flirtation necessitates consideration of the theoretical frameworks and methodological issues that might elucidate, cloud, or distort a true understanding of this domain of scientific inquiry. In part 1, a new framework for defining and discussing gender differences in the human courtship sequence (e.g., expressions of sexual interest or flirting) is offered. In part 2, historical and contemporary research pertaining to how sexual interest is nonverbally expressed and gender differences in the same are discussed in the following areas: sex drive; flirtation; sexual arousal; consent to sex; sex activity; the orgasm gap; and post-copulation. Findings pertaining to non-heterosexual individuals are integrated within each of the covered areas, unless they were extensive enough to warrant separate treatment, which was the case with flirtation and sexual consent.
Article
Full-text available
Gender role attitudes refer to attitudes toward the appropriate roles, rights, and responsibilities of men and women in society. Evidence indicates that individuals with traditional gender role attitudes tend to prefer mates with sex-typical opposite-sex characteristics in heterosexual men and women. This study examined whether gender role attitudes were associated with vocal masculinity preference in gay men in China. Five hundred and sixty-seven participants aged between 16 and 49 years completed the vocal masculinity preference (voice pitch and vocal tract length; VTLs) and gender role attitudes scale. The results indicated that gay men generally preferred masculine voices (lower voice pitch and longer VTLs) and gender role attitudes were positively correlated with preferences for masculine cues in the voices of men. While individuals indicating an affinity with traditional gender roles exhibited stronger preferences for feminine voices, which were inconsistent with the present hypotheses. The results help us understand the role of traditional gender beliefs in the mate preferences of gay men in China. Furthermore, based on the results, understanding one's gender-role attitudes can help cultivate more diversified criteria for mate selection and facilitate gay men in better choosing suitable mates. Future longitudinal studies should examine the relationship between gender role attitudes and masculine preference changes over time. Whether this relationship differs in the different sexual roles of gay men should also be explored.
Chapter
Evolutionary social science is having a renaissance. This volume showcases the empirical and theoretical advancements produced by the evolutionary study of romantic relationships. The editors assembled an international collection of contributors to trace how evolved psychological mechanisms shape strategic computation and behavior across the life span of a romantic partnership. Each chapter provides an overview of historic and contemporary research on the psychological mechanisms and processes underlying the initiation, maintenance, and dissolution of romantic relationships. Contributors discuss popular and cutting-edge methods for data analysis and theory development, critically analyze the state of evolutionary relationship science, and provide discerning recommendations for future research. The handbook integrates a broad range of topics (e.g., partner preference and selection, competition and conflict, jealousy and mate guarding, parenting, partner loss and divorce, and post-relationship affiliation) that are discussed alongside major sources of strategic variation in mating behavior, such as sex and gender diversity, developmental life history, neuroendocrine processes, technological advancement, and culture. Its content promises to enrich students’ and established researchers’ views on the current state of the discipline and should challenge a diverse cross-section of relationship scholars and clinicians to incorporate evolutionary theorizing into their professional work.
Article
Full-text available
On average, gay men are somewhat feminine and lesbians somewhat masculine, but there is variation within each group. The authors examined the consequences of this variation for gay men's and lesbians’ desirability as romantic partners. In 2 studies the authors analyzed personal advertisements. Homosexual people were more likely than heterosexual people to mention traits related to sex typicality and more likely to request sex-typical than sex-atypical partners. In 2 studies the authors assessed partner preferences directly. On average, gay men preferred men who described themselves as masculine rather than feminine, but this preference was weaker among men who rated themselves as relatively feminine. Lesbians preferred women who described themselves as feminine looking but did not discriminate against women calling themselves masculine acting. The authors discuss implications of the results for theories of sexual orientation and the adjustment of sex-atypical homosexual people.
Article
Full-text available
Evolutionary adaptation in variable environments is likely to give rise to several signals that can be used to identify a suitable mate in multisensory organisms. The presence of multiple signals for sexual selection could be advantageous, limiting the chance of mating with a suboptimal partner and avoiding the costs of inferior progeny. Despite extensive research into isolated signals of attractiveness, the amalgamation of multiple signals in sexual selection is poorly understood, particularly in humans. Inferences regarding both the function and importance of such signals are therefore tentative unless the effects are considered together. Here, the literature regarding two evolved signals of attraction (cf. faces and voices) is reviewed in relation to a framework ( Candolin 2003) for signal integration. It is argued that the functional nature of signals of attractiveness would be better studied through manipulation and experimentation with both single and multiple signals. Considering the prevalence of traits in relation to their combined effects may well provide a more fruitful and informative approach to human mate selection.
Article
Full-text available
Research on human attraction frequently makes use of single-modality stimuli such as neutral-expression facial photographs as proxy indicators of an individual's attractiveness. How- ever, we know little about how judgments of these single-modality stimuli correspond to judg- ments of stimuli that incorporate multi-modal cues of face, body and speech. In the present study, ratings of attractiveness judged from videos of participants introducing themselves were inde- pendently predicted by judgments of the participant's facial attractiveness (a neutral-expression facial photograph masked to conceal the hairstyle), body attractiveness (a photograph of the upper body), and speech attractiveness (the soundtrack to the video). We also found that ratings of the face, body and speech were positively related to each other. Our results support the assumption that the single-modality stimuli used in much attractiveness research are valid proxy indicators of overall attractiveness in ecologically valid contexts, and complement literature showing cross- modality concordance of trait attractiveness, but also recommend that research relying on assess- ments of individual attractiveness take account of both visual and vocal attractiveness where pos- sible.
Chapter
Full-text available
We explore the extent to which learning and memory mechanisms can explain variation in facial attractiveness. We suggest that two general mechanisms are in operation, and work together in determining attractiveness. Both can be understood in terms of general principles of learning, memory and generalization. One mechanism is related to the problem of many different stimuli requiring the same response. For instance, faces of babies are all different but all should be recognized as babies. The way the nervous system solves this problem leads to preference for average faces. The second mechanism favors extremes. For example, the discrimination task of telling female and male faces apart may result in extreme male or female faces appearing more attractive (i.e. supernormality, peak-shift, overgeneralization or receiver bias). Adding more aspects to the face discrimination task (e.g. telling individuals of different age apart) may account for the full range of variations in attractiveness. We also introduce an ontogenetic model inspired by ethological theories of imprinting. This model can potentially explain why cultural innovations such as spectacles, clothing and haircuts influence attractiveness. Our conclusion is that we do not need to invoke evolutionary theories linking genetic quality with appearance to explain observed patterns of attractiveness.
Article
Psychological evidence suggests that sex differences in morphology have been modified by sexual selection so as to attract mates (intersexual selection) or intimidate rivals (intrasexual selection). Women compete with each other for high quality husbands by advertising reproductive value in terms of the distribution of fat reserves and by exaggerating morphological indicators of youthfulness such as a small nose and small feet and pale, hairless skin. Men's physical appearance tends to communicate social dominance, which has the combined effects of intimidating reproductive rivals and attracting mates. In addition to their attractiveness and intimidatory effects, human secondary sexual characters also provide cues to hormonal status and phenotypic quality consistent with the good genes model of sexual selection (which includes parasite resistance). Low waist-hip ratio is sexually attractive in women and indicates a high estrogen/testosterone ratio (which favors reproductive function). Facial attractiveness provides honest cues to health and mate value. The permanently enlarged female breast appears to have evolved under the influence of both the good genes and the runaway selection mechanisms. The male beard is not obviously related to phenotypic quality and may have evolved through a process of runaway intersexual selection.
Article
Although many studies of male facial attractiveness assume that facial masculinity is related to circulating testosterone levels in adult males, there is little empirical evidence in support of this assumption. Here, we used salivary testosterone assays to investigate the relationship between circulating testosterone and both masculinity and attractiveness of facial appearance by (1) constructing digital composites from the faces of men with high and low testosterone, which were presented using a forced-choice task to subjects and (2) using a forced-choice task in which participants judged the masculinity of pairs of original photographs. Composites from high-testosterone men were judged to be more masculine than those from low-testosterone men. Evidence that high-testosterone composites are considered more attractive than low-testosterone composites was equivocal. The forced-choice task using the original face images indicated that participants identified faces associated with relatively high circulating testosterone as being more masculine than faces of men with lower circulating testosterone. This effect was more pronounced when the faces in the pair were from men who differed greatly in testosterone levels. These preliminary findings provide support for the underlying assumptions of much attractiveness research, particularly studies that have identified systematic variation in female preferences for masculine faces.
Article
Previous studies have reported variation in women's preferences for masculinity in men's faces and voices. Women show consistent preferences for vocal masculinity, but highly variable preferences for facial masculinity. Within individuals, men with attractive voices tend to have attractive faces, suggesting common information may be conveyed by these cues. Here we tested whether men and women with particularly strong preferences for male vocal masculinity also have stronger preferences for male facial masculinity. We found that masculinity preferences were positively correlated across modalities. We also investigated potential influences on these relationships between face and voice preferences. Women using oral contraceptives showed weaker facial and vocal masculinity preferences and weaker associations between masculinity preferences across modalities than women not using oral contraceptives. Collectively, these results suggest that men's faces and voices may reveal common information about the masculinity of the sender, and that these multiple quality cues could be used in conjunction by the perceiver in order to determine the overall quality of individuals.
Article
Recently, women have been found to prefer the scent of symmetrical men and relatively masculine male faces more during the fertile (late follicular and ovulatory) phases of their menstrual cycles than during their infertile (e.g., luteal) phases. These findings make most theoretical sense if men's symmetry is associated with the masculinity of their faces and, therefore, men's symmetry and facial masculinity tap a shared underlying quality. This study examined associations between masculine facial features and nonfacial body symmetry as well as facial symmetry in samples of 141 men and 154 women. As predicted, a component of facial features that discriminates the sexes and reflects masculinization of the face significantly covaried with symmetry in men. No significant correlation was observed for women. These findings suggest that men's facial masculinity partly advertises underlying developmental stability.