ArticlePDF Available

Gratitude influences sleep through the mechanism of pre-sleep cognitions

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

To test whether individual differences in gratitude are related to sleep after controlling for neuroticism and other traits. To test whether pre-sleep cognitions are the mechanism underlying this relationship. A cross-sectional questionnaire study was conducted with a large (186 males, 215 females) community sample (ages=18-68 years, mean=24.89, S.D.=9.02), including 161 people (40%) scoring above 5 on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, indicating clinically impaired sleep. Measures included gratitude, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), self-statement test of pre-sleep cognitions, the Mini-IPIP scales of Big Five personality traits, and the Social Desirability Scale. Gratitude predicted greater subjective sleep quality and sleep duration, and less sleep latency and daytime dysfunction. The relationship between gratitude and each of the sleep variables was mediated by more positive pre-sleep cognitions and less negative pre-sleep cognitions. All of the results were independent of the effect of the Big Five personality traits (including neuroticism) and social desirability. This is the first study to show that a positive trait is related to good sleep quality above the effect of other personality traits, and to test whether pre-sleep cognitions are the mechanism underlying the relationship between any personality trait and sleep. The study is also the first to show that trait gratitude is related to sleep and to explain why this occurs, suggesting future directions for research, and novel clinical implications.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Gratitude influences sleep through the mechanism of pre-sleep cognitions
Alex M. Wood
a,
, Stephen Joseph
b
, Joanna Lloyd
c,1
, Samuel Atkins
c,1
a
School of Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, England, UK
b
University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK
c
University of Warwick, Warwick, UK
Received 7 May 2008; received in revised form 4 August 2008; accepted 2 September 2008
Abstract
Objectives: To test whether individual differences in gratitude
are related to sleep after controlling for neuroticism and other
traits. To test whether pre-sleep cognitions are the mechanism
underlying this relationship. Method: A cross-sectional ques-
tionnaire study was conducted with a large (186 males, 215
females) community sample (ages=1868 years, mean=24.89, S.
D.=9.02), including 161 people (40%) scoring above 5 on the
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, indicating clinically impaired
sleep. Measures included gratitude, the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index (PSQI), self-statement test of pre-sleep cognitions, the Mini-
IPIP scales of Big Five personality traits, and the Social
Desirability Scale. Results: Gratitude predicted greater subjective
sleep quality and sleep duration, and less sleep latency and
daytime dysfunction. The relationship between gratitude and each
of the sleep variables was mediated by more positive pre-sleep
cognitions and less negative pre-sleep cognitions. All of the
results were independent of the effect of the Big Five personality
traits (including neuroticism) and social desirability. Conclusion:
This is the first study to show that a positive trait is related to
good sleep quality above the effect of other personality traits, and
to test whether pre-sleep cognitions are the mechanism underlying
the relationship between any personality trait and sleep. The study
is also the first to show that trait gratitude is related to sleep and to
explain why this occurs, suggesting future directions for research,
and novel clinical implications.
© 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Gratitude; Personality; Sleep; Pre-sleep cognitions; Positive psychology
Introduction
Personality is an important predictor of sleep quality and
quantity [1]. Most previous research has focused on which
personality traits are related to impaired sleeping. Neuroti-
cism has emerged as a robust predictor of sleep quality, with
people who are dispositionally stressed, depressed, anxious,
and angry being shown to suffer from poor sleep [27]. This
research has been influential both in developing models of
the etiology of insomnia and in developing psychological
treatments of sleep disorders [1]. However, the positive
psychology movement has emphasized the importance of
studying positive aspects of life in addition to the more
traditional psychological focus on psychopathology [810].
Positive well-being involves more than the absence of
mental illness [11], and positive traits have been shown to
explain additional outcomes in health and well-being
variables after controlling for neuroticism [1214]. Recently,
positive psychological well-being has been linked to
improved quality of sleep [15]. It is not clear, however,
whether positive psychological traits can explain sleep
quality above the effect of other more traditionally studied
personality traits, including the negative traits represented
within the construct of neuroticism. Additionally, there is no
indication of the mechanisms which explain why positive
psychological traits are related to sleep. This study examines
these questions with regard to the specific positive
psychological trait of gratitude.
Gratitude is perhaps the quintessential positive psycho-
logy trait, involving a life orientation towards the positive in
the world [16]. In addition to feeling more grateful affect
Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
Corresponding author. School of Psychology, University of Manchester,
Manchester, England, UK. Tel.: +44 7790 816407.
E-mail address: alex.wood@manchester.ac.uk (A.M. Wood).
1
These authors made equal scientific contributions.
0022-3999/09/$ see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.002
[17], grateful people focus on the positive in their
environment and have greater appreciation of their posses-
sions and social relationships [18]. As a trait, gratitude is
related to positive coping [19], social functioning [20], and
has a unique and causal effect on positive well-being and
social relationships [14,21,22]. The aim is to investigate
whether individual differences in gratitude are related to
sleep, after controlling for neuroticism and other personality
traits, and to test for the mediational mechanisms underling
this relationship.
Only one previous study has indicated that trait gratitude
may be related to sleep, and no previous study has
suggested mechanisms which may underlie this relation-
ship. Patients with neuromuscular disorders listed three
things for which they were grateful each night for 21
nights. Subsequently, their physical and mental well-being
was compared with a control group which simply
completed outcome measures [21]. The gratitude group
self-reported increased hours of sleep and an improved
sense of refreshment upon waking. Gratitude was selected
as the focus of this study due to this indication that trait
gratitude may be related to sleep, and due to the centrality
of gratitude as a positive trait [23]. The current study will
extend this work by testing which aspects of sleep are
related to gratitude, whether this relationship is independent
of the effects of other personality traits, and by investigat-
ing the mediational mechanisms that may explain the
relationship between gratitude and sleep quality.
It is suggested that pre-sleep cognitions may underlie the
relationship between gratitude and sleep quality. Pre-sleep
cognitions are the thoughts that people have just before
sleep. A large literature has linked pre-sleep cognitions
with sleep quality [2429], and experimental evidence has
suggested that increased pre-sleep cognitions cause
impaired sleep [3033]. Negative pre-sleep cognitions
have attracted particular empirical attention, as precursors
of impaired sleep. More recently, positive pre-sleep
cognitions are beginning to be studied [24], and there is
indication that positive cognitions are related to good sleep
quality [34,35]. As there is evidence that gratitude causes a
variety of positive cognitions, including life evaluations
[21], perceptions of social support [22] and social situations
[20], it may be that gratitude is one determinant of pre-
sleep cognitions.
Specifically, we suggest that grateful people will have less
negative pre-sleep cognitions (which impair sleep) and more
positive pre-sleep cognitions (which promote sleep). This is
the first empirical study to link trait gratitude to sleep or to
suggest mechanisms explaining why this relationship may
occur. With gratitude interventions increasingly being
promoted in clinical settings [3638],establishingthe
mechanisms linking gratitude and sleep will indicate the
potential of gratitude interventions for treatment of insomnia.
This is also the first study to investigate the potential for
positive traits to increase our understanding of sleep above
the effects of neuroticism. More generally, this is the first
study to investigate pre-sleep cognitions as a mechanism
linking any personality trait to sleep.
Method
Participants and procedure
Participants were 401 (186 male, 215 female) people
recruited from the local community by one of two research
assistants. Ages ranged from 18 to 68 years (mean=24.89,
S.D.=9.02), with 80.3% aged under 30, and 90.1% aged
under 40. Participants were either recruited from adminis-
trative workers in the head office of a large multinational
company in London (n=200) or approached at a large chain
of coffee shops in the South West of England, a community
center in East Midlands, or a local Arts Center in the East
Midlands (n=201). Participation was voluntary and without
payment, and 75% of those approached agreed to participate.
Prior to completing the questionnaire the participants read a
sheet providing details of the study and ethical information,
and participants signed a declaration of informed consent.
All measures were completed in English with pencil and
paper. Prior to data collection the study was approved
according to the institutional review procedures of the
University of Warwick, and the research followed the ethical
guidelines of the American Psychological Association.
Measures
Gratitude was measured with the GQ-6 [13]. Six items
assess the frequency and intensity with which grateful affect
is experienced, and the range of events which elicit gratitude
[e.g., I have so much in life to feel thankful for(Item 1), I
am grateful to a wide variety of people(Item 4), and When
I look at the world, I don't see much to be grateful for(Item
3, reverse coded)]. Items are rated on a 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree) scale. The GQ6 has a robust one-factor
structure, convergent validity with peer reports [13], and
high testretest reliability [22].
Pre-sleep cognitions were measured with the self-
statement test [24]. Participants rate how frequently they
experience 60 different thoughts, in general, when trying to
initially fall asleep or get back to sleep. Items are rated on a 0
(never or hardly ever)to4(very often) scale. Items were
developed on the basis of a qualitative study of the thoughts
people experience whilst trying to fall asleep and include
both positive (e.g., enjoyable things I did during the last few
days) and negative pre-sleep cognitions (e.g., bad things
happening in the world). The self-statement test discrimi-
nates between insomniac and normal populations (known
group validity) and positive and negative thoughts load on
different factors [24].
Sleep was measured with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality
Index (PSQI) [39]. Referring to the past month, 19
questions ask about sleep quantity (e.g., when have you
44 A.M. Wood et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
usually gone to bed at nights?;when have you usually
gotten up in the morning?;how many actual hours of
sleep did you get at night?) and sleep quality (e.g., how
would you rate your sleep quality overall?). Responses are
made in a variety of formats; all responses are recoded on a
03 scale based on the ranges specified by the scale
development paper (the recoding also has the effect of
removing the effect of extreme outliers). A total sleep score
is formed, in addition to seven subscales representing sub-
jective sleep quality,sleep latency (how long it takes to get
asleep), sleep duration,sleep efficiency (hours spent asleep
divided by number of hours in bed), sleep disturbances,use
of sleep medicine, and daytime dysfunction (e.g., difficulty
staying awake during the day). The total sleep score and all
subscales are scored so that higher scores indicate more
impaired sleep and higher sleep dysfunction. The PSQI can
be used as a continuous measure of the continuum of
impaired sleep. Alternatively, using a total score N5 has
been shown to discriminate between groups of healthy
volunteers and groups referred to sleep disorder clinics by
physicians for sleep disorders (sensitivity and specificity of
N85%) [39]. The scale is one of the most widely used
measures of sleep in clinical research and practice.
The Big Five personality traits were measured with the
Mini-IPIP scales [40]. The personality factors of neuroti-
cism, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and
openness are assessed with 20 items (four per factor,
including a mixture of positively and negatively worded
items). These five traits have been shown to represent all
of personality at the broadest level of abstraction [41,42].
The Mini-IPIP shows very good testretest reliability,
convergent, discriminant, and criterion-related validity,
which is comparable to the NEO and other measures of
the Big Five [40].
Social desirability was measured with the Social Desir-
ability Scale-17 [43]. Sixteen items (seven reverse coded)
provide statements with which most people would like to
agree but are probably unable to do so truthfully (e.g., ‘‘I
always accept other's opinions, even when they don't agree
with my own). (The measure originally had 17 items, but
one item was deleted in the scale validation process; we used
the revised 16-item version as recommended by the authors
[43].) Participants respond trueor falseto each item, and
higher scores represent socially desirable responding. The
measure was developed due to concerns that the items in
older measures were no longer socially desirable. The
measure shows sensitivity to socially desirable situations
(e.g., job applications), good convergent validity, and all
items have recently been rated as socially desirable [43].
Results
The total PSQI score was approximately normally dis-
tributed. The mean total PSQI score was 5.44 (S.D.=2.57),
slightly higher than representative population surveys
(mean=4.55, S.D.=3.71, t[1390]=4.45, d=.28) [44].At
the standard cut-off point of 5, 161 people (40%) could
be classified as poor sleepers (compared to 32.1% in
population surveys).
In the total sample, higher scores on the GQ-6 were
positively associated with superior sleep quality (indicated
by lower scores on the PSQI), with gratitude negatively
correlating with six measures of impaired sleep quality:
total sleep quality (r=.29, Pb.001), subjective sleep
quality (r=.25, Pb.001), sleep latency (r=.20,
Pb.001), sleep duration (r=.14, Pb.01), habitual sleep
efficiency (r=.11, Pb.05), and daytime dysfunction (r=
.27, Pb.001). Gratitude was also positively correlated
with positive pre-sleep cognitions (r=.21, Pb.001) and
negatively correlated with negative pre-sleep cognitions (r=
.11, Pb.001). A series of multiple regressions were
conducted to test whether the relationship between
gratitude and sleep was mediated by positive and negative
pre-sleep cognitions, using Baron and Kenny's [45] three
mediational steps and the Sobel test [46]. As gratitude was
positively correlated with extraversion, agreeableness, and
conscientiousness, and negatively correlated with neuroti-
cism (rranged from |.23| to |.35|, all Pb.001), the Big Five
traits and social desirability were also included as predictor
variables in each of the regressions in order to control for
the effects of these traits at each stage of the analysis.
Baron and Kenny's [45] first step requires the predictor to
be related to the outcome. With the effects of the Big Five
and social desirability controlled, gratitude still significantly
predicted total sleep quality (β=.24, Pb.001), subjective
sleep quality (β=.25, Pb.001), sleep latency (β=.17,
P=.002), sleep duration (β=.15, P=.006), and daytime
dysfunction (β=.16, P=.003). Gratitude was not uniquely
related to sleep efficiency (β=.08 P=.144), failing Baron
and Kenny's first step; this variable was not included in any
further analysis.
The second step was conducted separately for each of the
five sleep variables with which gratitude showed a unique
relationship (these are presented as path diagrams in Fig. 1,
where each of the path values shown is independent of the
effects of social desirability and the Big Five). This step
requires the mediators (pre-sleep cognitions) to be signifi-
cantly related to the outcome (the sleep measure) after
controlling for the predictor (gratitude). As shown in Fig. 1,
this step was met in each case.
In the third step, mediation is demonstrated if controlling
for the mediator significantly reduces the relationship
between the predictor (gratitude) and the outcome (sleep)
relative to the zero-order correlation. Sobel's ztests whether
the decrease in βis statistically significant and is mathema-
tically equivalent to testing the significance of the mediated
pathway [46].Fig. 1 shows Sobel's zfor each mediated
pathway and demonstrates that either positive or negative
pre-sleep cognitions significantly mediated the relationship
between gratitude and each of the five measures of sleep
(fulfilling the third step). Positive pre-sleep cognitions
45A.M. Wood et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
mediated the relationship between gratitude and subjective
sleep quality, sleep duration, sleep latency, and sleep
efficiency. Negative pre-sleep cognitions mediated the
relationship between gratitude and subjective sleep quality,
sleep latency, sleep efficiency, and daytime dysfunction.
Discussion
Gratitude was uniquely related to total sleep quality,
subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, and
daytime dysfunction, after controlling for the effects of the
Big Five and social desirability. The results of the current
study also provide evidence that pre-sleep cognitions
mediate the relationship between trait gratitude and sleep
quality. When falling asleep, grateful people are less likely to
think negative and worrying thoughts, and more likely to
think positive thoughts. It appears that negative pre-sleep
cognitions impair sleep, and gratitude reduces the likelihood
of such thoughts, protecting sleep quality. Equally, it appears
that positive pre-sleep cognitions have a positive effect on
sleep, and that gratitude facilitates these thoughts, leading to
superior sleep quality.
It is notable that gratitude was related to sleep and pre-
sleep cognitions after removing the effects of the Big Five
personality traits. This suggests that gratitude is not simply
linked to sleep due to the third variable effects of broad
personality traits or dispositional affect. Increasingly, there is
Fig. 1. Tests of mediation between gratitude and five sleep measures, with standardized β's and Sobel's z. All values are after covarying the effects of social
desirability and the Big Five. ⁎⁎⁎Pb.001, ⁎⁎Pb.01, Pb.05.
46 A.M. Wood et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
evidence that gratitude can predict well-being outcomes
above the effects of other personality traits [13,17,20,22],
including social desirability [18], coping [19], and the 30
facets of the Big Five [14]. The present results suggest that
gratitude may also uniquely be related to sleep, at least above
the broad effects of the Big Five. As positive and negative
affect are respectively represented in the Big Five traits of
extroversion and neuroticism [47], the results suggest that
the relationship between gratitude and sleep is not simply a
reflection of the relationships between gratitude and positive
affect (or happiness) or negative functioning (e.g., stress,
depression, anxiety, and negative affect). The Big Five traits
can, however, achieve broad coverage of the personality
domain at the expense of specificity [48]. Future research
should measure positive and negative affect specifically to
test whether the mechanism linking gratitude and sleep is
purely cognitive (as indicated by the current demonstration
of mediation via pre-sleep cognitions) or whether daily affect
forms a second mediational pathway.
More research is needed into whether other positive
traits can explain sleep and sleep disorder, in order to
develop more complete models of sleep functioning.
Research is also needed into whether increasing positive
traits improves sleep. Emmons et al. show that therapeu-
tically increasing gratitude can increase sleep quality [21].
Further work is needed to explore the efficacy of this
approach in insomnia patients to test whether including a
focus on gratitude can improve existing sleep therapies and
whether pre-sleep cognitions are the mechanism through
which the therapy operates.
The study had some limitations, including the reliance on
self-report measures. This was partially mitigated by the use
of the PSQI measure of sleep (which discriminates between
groups of healthy people and physician-referred insomniacs
with sensitivity and specificity of N85%) [39] and through
showing that the results were not confounded with socially
desirable responding. Self-reports of sleep are also important
in themselves, given that most people self-refer to sleep
clinics. There is also evidence that mood and objective daily
performance is more strongly related to the self-report of
sleep quality than to sleep quality measured through
neuropsychological assessment [49]. However, further
research is needed into whether gratitude and other positive
traits are related to objective sleep assessment measured by
EEG (actual sleep measured through brain waves) or
actigraphy (actual sleep measured via wrist movements).
The current study also used a community sample, rather than
a sample of people clinically diagnosed with insomnia. The
community sample had the advantage of a wide range of
both personality and sleep quality, whereas a slanted clinical
sample would probably have exhibited range restrictions,
producing biased statistical analysis. However, future
research is needed into whether the findings extend to
clinical samples, perhaps using the experimental methodo-
logy discussed above. Finally, as the study was cross-
sectional it is not possible to demonstrate causality, and the
mediational analysis relies on the variables being arranged in
the correct order, which is normally inferred on the basis of
previous research [45]. However, given that gratitude has
been previously shown to cause better sleep [13], pre-sleep
cognitions have been shown to causally influence sleep [30
33], and gratitude causally influences cognitions [2022],it
seems likely the causal chain was correctly arranged in the
mediational analysis.
The results present the first indication that individual
differences in gratitude are related to sleep quality and
suggest that this is due to the mechanism of pre-sleep
cognitions. More research is needed into the role of
gratitude and other positive traits in relation to quality of
sleep in both healthy and clinical populations. Such a
positive psychology approach to sleep could potentially
provide better understanding of the etiology of sleep
disorders, explanation of positive sleep quality, and lead to
new therapies to treat sleep disorder and promote optimal
sleeping amongst the general population.
References
[1] Espie CA. Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development,
persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annu Rev
Psychol 2002;53:21543.
[2] Pilcher JJ, Ginter DR, Sadowsky B. Sleep quality versus sleep
quantity: Relationships between sleep and measures of health, well-
being and sleepiness in college students. J Psychosom Res 1997;42:
58396.
[3] Gray EK, Watson D. General and specific traits of personality and their
relation to sleep and academic performance. J Pers 2002;70:177206.
[4] Brummett BH, Babyak MA, Siegler IC, et al. Associations among
perceptions of social support, negative affect, and quality of sleep in
caregivers and noncaregivers. Health Psychol 2006;25:2205.
[5] Carmichael CL, Reis HT. Attachment, sleep quality, and depressed
affect. Health Psychol 2005;24:52631.
[6] Fortunato VJ, Harsh J. Stress and sleep quality: The moderating role of
negative affectivity. Pers Individ Differ 2006;41:82536.
[7] Pilcher JJ, Huffcutt AI. Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: A
meta-analysis. Sleep 1996;19:31826.
[8] Linley PA, Joseph S, Harrington S, Wood AM. Positive psychology:
Past, present, and (possible) future. J Posit Psychol 2006;1:316.
[9] Seligman MEP, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology: An
introduction. Am Psychol 2000;55:514.
[10] Duckworth AL, Steen TA, Seligman MEP. Positive psychology in
clinical practice. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2005;1:62951.
[11] Ryff CD, Love GD, Urry HL, et al. Psychological well-being and ill-
being: Do they have distinct or mirrored biological correlates?
Psychother Psychosom 2006;75:8595.
[12] Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW. Distinguishing optimism from
neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A
reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol 1994;
67:106378.
[13] McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang JA. The grateful disposition: A
conceptual and empirical topography. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;82:
11227.
[14] Wood AM, Joseph S, Maltby J. Gratitude uniquely predicts satisfaction
with life: Incremental validity above the domains and facets of the Five
Factor Model. Pers Individ Differ 2008;45:4954.
[15] Steptoe A, O'Donnell K, Marmot M, Wardle J. Positive affect,
psychological well-being, and good sleep. J Psychosom Res 2008;64:
40915.
47A.M. Wood et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
[16] Wood AM, Joseph S, Linley PA. Gratitude: The parent of all virtues.
Psychologist 2007;20:1821.
[17] McCullough ME, Tsang J-A, Emmons RA. Gratitude in intermediate
affective terrain: Links of grateful moods to individual differences and
daily emotional experience. J Pers Soc Psychol 2004;86:295309.
[18] Wood AM, Maltby J, Stewart N, Joseph S. Conceptualizing gratitude
and appreciation as a unitary personality trait. Pers Individ Differ 2008;
44:61930.
[19] Wood AM, Joseph S, Linley PA. Coping style as a psychological
resource of grateful people. J Soc Clin Psychol 2007;26:110825.
[20] Wood AM, Maltby J, Stewart N, Linley PA, Joseph S. A social-
cognitive model of trait and state levels of gratitude. Emotion 2008;8:
28190.
[21] Emmons RA, McCullough ME. Counting blessings versus burdens:
An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in
daily life. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003;84:37789.
[22] Wood AM, Maltby J, Gillett R, Linley PA, Joseph S. The role of
gratitude in the development of social support, stress, and depression:
Two longitudinal studies. J Res Pers 2008;42:85471.
[23] Emmons RA, McCullough ME. The psychology of gratitude. New
York: Oxford University Press, 2004.
[24] Fichten CS, Libman E, Creti L, Amsel R, Tagalakis V, Brender W.
Thoughts during awake times in older good and poor sleepers The
self-statement test: 60+. Cogn Ther Res 1998;22:120.
[25] Harvey AG. Pre-sleep cognitive activity: A comparison of sleep-onset
insomniacs and good sleepers. Br J Clin Psychol 2000;39:27586.
[26] Wicklow A, Espie CA. Intrusive thoughts and their relationship to
actigraphic measurement of sleep: towards a cognitive model of
insomnia. Behav Res Ther 2000;38:67993.
[27] Libman E, Creti L, Amsel R, Brender W, Fichten CS. What do older
good and poor sleepers do during periods of nocturnal wakefulness? The
Sleep Behaviors Scale: 60+. Psychology and Aging 1997;12:17082.
[28] Nicassio PM, Mendlowitz DR, Fussell JJ, Petras L. The phenome-
nology of the pre-sleep state the development of the Pre-Sleep
Arousal Scale. Behav Res Ther 1985;23:26371.
[29] Harvey KJ, Espie CA. Development and preliminary validation of the
Glasgow Content of Thoughts Inventory (GCTI): A new measure for
the assessment of pre-sleep cognitive activity. Br J Clin Psychol 2004;
43:40920.
[30] Gross RT, Borkovec TD. Effects of a cognitive intrusion manipulation on
the sleep-onset latency of good sleepers. Behav Ther 1982;13:1126.
[31] Ansfield ME, Wegner DM, Bowser R. Ironic effects of sleep urgency.
Behav Res Ther 1996;34:52331.
[32] Levey AB, Aldaz JA, Watts FN, Coyle K. Articulatory suppression and
the treatment of insomnia. Behav Res Ther 1991;29:859.
[33] Haynes SN, Adams A, Franzen M. The effects of pre-sleep stress on
sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol 1981;90:6016.
[34] Nelson J, Harvey AG. An exploration of pre-sleep cognitive activity in
insomnia: Imagery and verbal thought. Br J Clin Psychol 2003;42:
27188.
[35] Nelson J, Harvey AG. Pre-sleep imagery under the microscope: a
comparison of patients with insomnia and good sleepers. Behav Res
Ther 2003;41:27384.
[36] Seligman MEP, Rashid T, Parks AC. Positive psychotherapy. Am
Psychol 2006;61:77488.
[37] Bono G, Emmons RA, McCullough ME. Gratitude in practice and the
practice of gratitude. In: Linley PA, Joseph S, editors. Positive
psychology in practice. Hoboken (NJ): John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2004.
pp. 46481.
[38] Seligman MEP, Steen TA, Park N, Peterson C. Positive psychology
progress: Empirical validation of interventions. Am Psychol 2005;60:
41021.
[39] Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index a new instrument for psychiatric
practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989;28:193213.
[40] Donnellan MB, Oswald FL, Baird BM, Lucas RE. The Mini-IPIP
scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the big five factors of
personality. Psychol Assess 2006;18:192203.
[41] Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality-traits. Am
Psychol 1993;48:2634.
[42] McCrae RR, Costa PT. Validation of the 5-factor model of personality
across instruments and observers. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;52:8190.
[43] Stöber J. The Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17): convergent
validity, discriminant validity, and relationship with age. Eur J Psychol
Assess 2001;17:22232.
[44] Zeitlhofer J, Schmeiser-Rieder A, Tribl G, et al. Sleep and quality of
life in the Austrian population. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;102:24957.
[45] Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:117382.
[46] MacKinnon DP, Dwyer JH. Estimating mediated effects in prevention
studies. Evaluation Review 1993;17:14858.
[47] Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their
relevance to psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 1994;103:1831.
[48] Schimmack U, Oishi S, Furr RM, Funder DC. Personality and life
satisfaction: A facet-level analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2004;30:
106275.
[49] Orff HJ, Drummond SPA, Nowakowski S, Perlis ML. Discrepancy
between subjective symptomatology and objective neuropsychological
performance in insomnia. Sleep 2007;30:120511.
48 A.M. Wood et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (2009) 4348
... Higher levels of gratitude are associated with enhanced wellbeing and prosocial behavior. Cross-sectional studies have found that gratitude is positively associated with self-reported physical health (26), psychological health (26)(27)(28), hope (29,30), sleep quality and quantity (31,32), and engagement in health behaviors (26). A few studies have assessed gratitude and related outcomes in healthcare education stakeholders. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to diminished sleep and increased stress, anxiety, and burnout for many health professionals and health professions students. One simple approach that may be effective for bolstering personal well-being is consciously cultivating gratitude. Gratitude is positively associated with physical health, psychological health, hope, sleep, and health behavior engagement; and randomized studies indicate that gratitude interventions can improve psychological well-being and sleep. The primary aim of this study was to assess the impact of practicing Tiny Habits® on self-reported gratitude, as measured by the 6-Item Gratitude Questionnaire (GQ-6). In January 2021, 154 adult participants with GQ-6 <35/42 were randomized to one of 3 groups: Tiny Habits for Gratitude (n = 50), Tiny Habits Control (n = 52), and Inactive Control (n = 52). Both Tiny Habits groups chose 3 Tiny Habits Recipes to practice daily and participated in the free, email-based 5-Day Program with automated daily check-in emails and personalized feedback from a Certified Tiny Habits Coach. The Recipes for the Tiny Habits for Gratitude group focused on cultivating gratitude, while those for the Tiny Habits Control group did not. Post-intervention, the mean change in GQ-6 scores in the Tiny Habits for Gratitude (Δ = ↑6.9 ± 5.6; n = 37/50, 74%; p< 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.85) and Tiny Habits Control (Δ = ↑5.6 ± 4.1; n = 31/52, 60%; p = 0.009, Cohen's d = 0.71) groups were greater than that of the Inactive Control group (Δ = ↑2.5 ± 4.4; N = 42/52, 81%). At 1 month, the mean change in GQ-6 scores in the Tiny Habits for Gratitude group (Δ = ↑7.0 ± 5.3; N = 28/50, 56%) was greater than that of the Inactive Control group (Δ = ↑2.9 ± 5.4; N = 39/52, 72%; p = 0.002, Cohen's d = 0.78). These findings suggest that practicing Tiny Habits Recipes and participating in the 5-Day Program can significantly increase gratitude in the short term and focusing specifically on gratitude during this process can sustain the increase in gratitude for up to 1 month. Implementation is quick, simple, and free. This has significant implications for healthcare education stakeholders.
... The broadened thought-action repertories then motivate people to broaden attention and thinking, to strengthen social bonds, and to continue progression towards goals (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). Studies have empirically supported the association between gratitude and well-being, including personal growth, positive relationships, optimism, and sleep quality (Emmons & McCullough, 2003;Wood, Joseph, et al., 2009;Wood, Froh, et al., 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
While the ensuing COVID-19 pandemic has significantly altered individuals’ lives worldwide, it has been perhaps especially disruptive to the lives of sojourners as many have been unable to return home and are absent from their families, a familiar culture, and normal social support systems. While it is important to ask how such individuals can successfully survive in such a crisis, we were interested in extending our knowledge and understanding by asking “how can such individuals move beyond mere surviving to a state of thriving?” In answering this question, we utilized a positive psychology framework to develop a theoretical model wherein we expected higher/lower levels of perceived social support from host country people (i.e., host country support) to result in higher/lower levels of perceived gratitude, which would then result in higher/lower levels of thriving, and ethnocentrism moderated this indirect effect. To test our model, we utilized a sample of sojourners who responded to a survey measuring ethnocentrism (February 2020). We then administered daily surveys measuring perceived host country support, gratitude, and thriving over a nine-day period during the COVID-19 crisis (March 26–April 3, 2020). Results supported the indirect effect of host country support on thriving via gratitude. Further, we found that sojourners with lower levels of ethnocentrism exhibited stronger host country support- gratitude link, hence stronger indirect effect of host country support on thriving via gratitude. We close by offering implications for the existing literature, future research, and organizational practices.
... One study has also examined the sleep-related outcomes of trait gratitude. In a cross-sectional survey study of adults, trait gratitude was found to predict subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, and daytime dysfunction related to lack of sleep (Wood et al., 2009a), mediated by pre-sleep cognitions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Though gratitude research in organizational behavior (OB) is nascent, this emotion has a rich history in the social sciences. Research has shown gratitude to promote prosocial behaviors, encourage personal well-being, and foster interpersonal relationships. However, gratitude research has been siloed among these three outcomes of gratitude (moral, wellness, and relational). Similarly, past reviews of gratitude have focused on only one group of outcomes, one of its forms (trait, state, or expressed), or empirical findings without emphasis on the theoretical underpinnings. In contrast, this review recognizes that each type of gratitude, its functions, and outcomes are part of a single process model of gratitude. As such, in the current review we provide a comprehensive assessment of gratitude in the social sciences by distilling and organizing the literature per our process model of episodic gratitude. Then, we translate the insights for management scholars, highlighting possible differences and synergies between extant research and workplace gratitude thereby helping advance “gratitude science” in the workplace. In all, this review (a) examines definitions and operationalizations of gratitude and provides recommendations for organizational research; (b) proposes a process model of episodic workplace gratitude as a conceptual map to guide future OB research on gratitude; (c) reviews empirical gratitude research through the lens of our process model; and (d) discusses the current state of the literature, important differences for workplace gratitude, and future directions for organizational scholars.
... Grateful people report better physical health, and they are more likely to engage in physical activity (Hill et al., 2013). The experience of gratitude leads to better sleep, less fatigue and less cellular inflammation (Mills et al., 2015;Wood et al., 2009). Individuals who show high levels of gratitude show more resilience and vitality . ...
Article
Purpose The aim of the current study was to examine the previously unexplored relationship between positive reframing as a mediator between gratitude and technostress in Indian students. By examining this relationship, the authors aim to expand the theoretical domain of gratitude research by examining its potential influence on technology-induced stress. Design/methodology/approach A cross-sectional survey was used to collect and analyze data from 552 Indian college students who participated in graduate and postgraduate programs across various educational institutions in India. Regression and mediation analyses were performed with both IBM SPSS 25 and AMOS. Findings This study’s data suggest that positive reframing plays an important mediating role between gratitude and technostress. Gratitude also encourages positive reframing, which reduced technostress among the students. Taken together, our data showed that gratitude induces positive reframing, which in turn reduces techno-stress among Indian students in the current study. Research limitations/implications The sample size in this study is relatively small in relation to the student population in India. The current study relied primarily on quantitative data and analysis and further research could use a mixed-method approach to better understand the underlying mechanisms between positive reframing, gratitude and technostress. The results are derived under an extreme coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic situation; therefore, the results cannot be generalized to normal times. Practical implications The paper includes implications for teachers, academic leaders, parents and civil society. Originality/value Overall, the relationship between positive reframing, gratitude and technostress has not been thoroughly explored. To the best of the authors' understanding, this is the first study to examine the influence of gratitude on technology-induced stress and the role of reframing.
... Research on the topic of gratitude has increased exponentially over the last two decades, spurred by the various psychosocial benefits that gratitude confers. For example, gratitude is positively related to life satisfaction , adaptive coping (Wood et al., 2007), improved sleep patterns (Wood et al., 2009), better physical health (Hill et al., 2013), positive social relationships and affiliation (Algoe, 2012;Algoe et al., 2008;Bartlett et al., 2012), wellbeing at work (Waters, 2012;Waters & Stokes, 2015) and prosocial behaviors (Ma et al., 2017). More recently, attention in this field has questioned the universality of gratitude experiences and moved towards examining gratitude in relation to the sociocultural context in which it is being studied (see, for example, Merçon-Vargas et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Examinations of the influence of culture on how gratitude is experienced are sparse, as are studies that simultaneously explore developmental differences in understandings of gratitude. This paper presents three studies that examine whether perceptions and experiences of gratitude differ across children, adolescents and adults in two individualistic, WEIRD and Commonwealth cultures—Australia and the UK. Studies 1a ( N = 88, ages 17–39) and 1b ( N = 77, ages 17–25) provide initial insights into “features of gratitude” in Australia through two stages of a prototype analysis. These features are compared to a previous prototype study of gratitude in the UK, alongside a further comparison to the US. Study 2 employs vignettes to examine how perceptions of the benefactor, benefit and mixed emotions influence the degree of gratitude experienced across adolescents and adults in Australia ( N = 1937, ages 11–85), with a comparison to the UK ( N = 398, ages 12–65). In Study 3, factors examined in Study 2 are adapted into accessible story workbooks for younger children (Australia N=135, ages 9–11; UK N=62, ages 9–11). Results across these studies demonstrate similarities and differences in understandings and experiences of gratitude across cultures. While adults across Australia and the UK responded similarly to gratitude scenarios, cross-cultural differences are observed between children and adolescents in these two countries. Developmental differences are noted in relation to more sophisticated reasoning around gratitude, such as recognition of ulterior motives. These findings highlight the need for gratitude research and interventions to be cross-culturally, and developmentally, responsive.
Article
Most measures of past parenting patterns have a restricted range of about two to three negative parenting constructs. The Young Parenting Inventory (YPI-R2) provides a more nuanced framework that measures a fuller spectrum of these negative parenting patterns and, therefore, holds the potential of being a more useful guide to parents and caretakers. The YPI-R2 is made up of six validated subscales. An additional four were identified but were not sufficiently robust to be included. The purpose of this study is to determine if these four scales can be strengthened through the development of additional items and be empirically validated. Using non-clinical, English-speaking community samples from Singapore (n = 592, 628) and Malaysia (n = 222, 229), these revised scales were tested using multiple exploratory factor analyses with fathers and mothers rated separately. After further scale refinement, the final model, which consisted of 10 subscales and 41 items, was then subjected to confirmatory factor analysis using 4 other non-clinical international samples with separate ratings for fathers and mothers—USA (n = 259, 281), South Africa (n = 318, 372), Nigeria (n = 328, 344) and India (n = 277, 289). The results show that the YPI-R3 with 10 subscales is a robust and cross-culturally acceptable model. Correlations and hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that the YPI-R3 has good convergent validity and predictive capabilities with measures of psychopathology, personality traits, emotional distress, negative schemas and other distal measures of functioning in everyday life—gratitude, humor and satisfaction with life.
Article
Full-text available
In this research it is aimed to test the effectiveness of junior high school students writing gratitude journals on gratitude. The research group constitutes students who attend secondary school in a public school and volunteer to participate in the research. The research was carried out on two groups, one experimental group and one control group. There were 23 participants in the experimental group and 24 participants in the control group. In the scope of the research, Gratitude Scale and Subjective Well-Being Scale for Adolescents were used. Data were collected three times in the research process; pre-test, post-test, and follow-up test. After the pre-test was applied, a training seminar on gratitude was given to the experimental group. In this seminar, students were told about what they can be grateful for and how they can be expressed. After the training seminar, the experimental group was asked to write daily in gratitude diaries created by the researchers based on the balance model of positive psychotherapy for 21 days. At the end of 21 days, the last time was collected, and the final test data were collected. Finally, the follow up data were collected 10 weeks after the end of the study and the study was terminated. The data were analyzed using the Mann Whitney U Test and the Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test and the Friedman Test. Examination of the test results for the experimental and control groups revealed that the experimental group differed significantly from the control group in terms of gratitude and subjective well-being. In addition to these findings, according to the Wilcoxon Test results, the individuals in the experimental group got significantly higher scores in terms of gratitude and subjective well-being in the posttests than in the pretest scores. In the control group, there was no difference between the pretest and posttest scores. According to the Friedman test results used for the follow-up test analysis, the positive effect in the experimental group continues in the follow-up test. It was observed that individuals in the control group got significantly lower scores in the follow-up tests compared to gratitude and subjective well-being scores. According to the results of the follow-up test, it was understood that the effectiveness of the writing exercise continued. According to these findings, it was determined that the practice of writing gratitude diaries increased the gratitude and subjective well-being levels of adolescents
Article
Full-text available
In this research it is aimed to test the effectiveness of junior high school students writing gratitude journals on gratitude. The research group constitutes students who attend secondary school in a public school and volunteer to participate in the research. The research was carried out on two groups, one experimental group and one control group. There were 23 participants in the experimental group and 24 participants in the control group. In the scope of the research, Gratitude Scale and Subjective Well-Being Scale for Adolescents were used. Data were collected three times in the research process; pre-test, post-test, and follow-up test. After the pre-test was applied, a training seminar on gratitude was given to the experimental group. In this seminar, students were told about what they can be grateful for and how they can be expressed. After the training seminar, the experimental group was asked to write daily in gratitude diaries created by the researchers based on the balance model of positive psychotherapy for 21 days. At the end of 21 days, the last time was collected, and the final test data were collected. Finally, the follow up data were collected 10 weeks after the end of the study and the study was terminated. The data were analyzed using the Mann Whitney U Test and the Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test and the Friedman Test. Examination of the test results for the experimental and control groups revealed that the experimental group differed significantly from the control group in terms of gratitude and subjective well-being. In addition to these findings, according to the Wilcoxon Test results, the individuals in the experimental group got significantly higher scores in terms of gratitude and subjective well-being in the posttests than in the pretest scores. In the control group, there was no difference between the pretest and posttest scores. According to the Friedman test results used for the follow-up test analysis, the positive effect in the experimental group continues in the follow-up test. It was observed that individuals in the control group got significantly lower scores in the follow-up tests compared to gratitude and subjective well-being scores. According to the results of the follow-up test, it was understood that the effectiveness of the writing exercise continued. According to these findings, it was determined that the practice of writing gratitude diaries increased the gratitude and subjective well-being levels of adolescents.
Article
Gratitude has been linked with normal human functioning and well-being yet, its association with happiness and life satisfaction remains understudied among non clinical samples in collectivist cultures. Most studies on gratitude are focused on clinical settings and in individualist cultures. This study investigates the predictive strength of gratitude and purpose in life on life satisfaction among university undergraduates in Nigeria. Using a cross sectional research design, 390 university students were selected from 2 (public and private) universities. A questionnaire on socio-demographic profile, gratitude scale(r=0.84), purpose in life scale(r=0.96) and life satisfaction scale (r=0.90) was administered to participants. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics, correlation analysis and regression analysis at 0.05 level of significance. Three hypotheses were tested. The results revealed that gratitude and purpose in life jointly and independently predicted life satisfaction (R 2 = .24; F = 62.56; p<.05). Being grateful and having purpose are crucial for a comprehensive examination of life satisfaction.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The effect of a grateful outlook on psychological and physical well-being was examined. In Studies 1 and 2, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 3 experimental conditions (hassles, gratitude listing, and either neutral life events or social comparison); they then kept weekly (Study 1) or daily (Study 2) records of their moods, coping behaviors, health behaviors, physical symptoms, and overall life appraisals. In a 3rd study, persons with neuromuscular disease were randomly assigned to either the gratitude condition or to a control condition. The gratitude-outlook groups exhibited heightened well-being across several, though not all, of the outcome measures across the 3 studies, relative to the comparison groups. The effect on positive affect appeared to be the most robust finding. Results suggest that a conscious focus on blessings may have emotional and interpersonal benefits.
Article
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Article
This personal historical article traces the development of the Big-Five factor structure, whose growing acceptance by personality researchers has profoundly influenced the scientific study of individual differences. The roots of this taxonomy lie in the lexical hypothesis and the insights of Sir Francis Galton, the prescience of L. L. Thurstone, the legacy of Raymond B. Cattell, and the seminal analyses of Tupes and Christal. Paradoxically, the present popularity of this model owes much to its many critics, each of whom tried to replace it, but failed. In reaction, there have been a number of attempts to assimilate other models into the five-factor structure. Lately, some practical implications of the emerging consensus can be seen in such contexts as personnel selection and classification.
Article
Gratitude, like other positive emotions, has inspired many theological and philosophical writings, but it has inspired very little vigorous, empirical research. In an effort to remedy this oversight, this book brings together prominent scientists from various disciplines to examine what has become known as the most-neglected emotion. The volume begins with the historical, philosophical, and theoretical foundations of gratitude, and then presents the current research perspectives from social, personality, and developmental psychology, as well as from primatology, anthropology, and biology. The volume also includes a comprehensive, annotated bibliography of research on gratitude. This work contributes a great deal to the growing positive psychology initiative and to the scientific investigation of positive human emotions. It will be an invaluable resource for researchers and students in social, personality, developmental, clinical, and health psychology, as well as to sociologists and cultural anthropologists.
Article
Trait concepts are used extensively in psychopathology research, but much of this research has failed to consider recent advances in the dimensional structure of personality. Many investigators have discounted the importance of this structural research, arguing that (a) little progress has been made in this area, (b) structural models have little direct relevance for psychopathology research, and (c) the principal methodological tool of structural research--factor analysis--is too subjective to yield psychologically meaningful results. We dispute each of these objections. Specifically, we offer an integrative hierarchical model--composed of four higher order traits--that is congruent with each of the major structural subtraditions within personality. We also discuss the implications of this integrative scheme for basic trait research, for the conceptualization and assessment of psychopathology, and for the etiology of disorder.