ArticlePDF Available
742
THE WILSON BULLETIN *
Vol.
107, No. 4,
December I995
Wilson Bull., 107(4), 1995, pp. 742-746
Effects of laying date, clutch size, and communal nest size on the reproductive success
of
Monk Parakeets.-Monk Parakeets
(Myiopsittu monachus)
build conspicuous, enclosed
stick nests, which are often large and which have several isolated chambers (Forshaw 1978,
Martella and Bucher 1993). These chambers are occupied throughout the year by breeding
pairs and non-breeding individuals (Martin 1989, Martella and Bucher 1993). The Monk
Parakeet is iteroparous, monogamous, and breeds in the spring and summer (October to
February) (Navarro et al. 1992). Although all members of a compound nest participate in
the building, maintenance, and vigilance of the nest, only the breeding pair cares for the
nestlings (Martella 1985, Martella and Bucher 1993). Few cases of allofeeding by members
of different nests have been observed, which could function for the development of a com-
munal breeding system (Martella 1985, Bucher et al. 1991).
Monk Parakeets have a variable clutch size (Navarro et al. 1992) the adaptive nature of
which has been the subject of extensive discussion (e.g., Lack 1954, Perrins and Jones
1974). However, except for the research of Beissinger and Waltman (1991) on the Green-
rumped Parrotlet (Forpus passennus), this topic has been little studied in parrots.
In this paper we examine how laying date, clutch size, and the size of communal nest
affect the reproductive success of Monk Parakeets.
Study areas and
methods.-This study was carried out in 1982 near the town of Arroyito
(31”25S, 62”59W) and from 1983 to 1988 close to the city of Jesus Maria (31”05S,
64”l lW), in Cordoba Province, Argentina. Overall, we recorded 205 breeding attempts.
Nest chambers were checked at seven to 12-day intervals from October to early March.
This interval was selected to avoid a reduction of the breeding success due to observers
disturbance (Navarro et al. 1992). In Arroyito, sampled nests were all grouped in
Eucalyptus
trees planted at both sides of the 1000-m long entrance to the ranch. In J. Maria, all nests
were in the 610.ha study area in native trees. We could not sample very large nests (with
more than ten individuals) because they were on inaccessible structures on powerline towers,
windmills and tall trees. However, large nests comprised only 2% of the nests in both areas
(Bucher et al. 1991).
Eggs found in each nest chamber were marked individually with indelible ink. Nestlings
were identified by clipping different toenails and later were banded with numbered alumi-
num rings. The fate of each egg was recorded as either lost before hatching, failed to hatch,
or hatched. Eggs that might have hatched but whose chicks disappeared before the following
visit were assumed not to have hatched. Nestlings that disappeared from the nest before
reaching fledging age (35 days; Navarro and Bucher 1990) were recorded as deaths. In
doubtful cases, the survival was checked through capture-release procedures carried out three
times a year during all the study (Navarro 1989; Martin and Bucher 1993). Initiation dates
for clutches were determined by back-dating each clutch based on a 2.1 -day interval between
successive eggs, a 24.day incubation period (Navarro 1989) and the calculated age of
nestlings (Navarro and Bucher 1990). Twenty-two clutches in which we could not estimate
the onset of laying were discarded. To compare dates among years, we used the distance in
days of a given date from the median date of first clutch for the population in that year,
assigning negative values to those dates earlier than the median. First clutches refer to the
first group of eggs laid in a given nest chamber in that season, replacement clutches are
those started after failure of the first clutch, and second clutches are those laid after a
successful first brood.
Results.-The
earliest clutch was laid on 1 October 1983 and the latest on 29 October
1985. At both areas, 90% of first clutches within the population were laid within a nine
week period (see also Navarro et al. 1992; Navarro and Bucher 1992). Clutch sizes ranged
SHORT COMMUNICATIONS
CLUTCH SIZE
FIG. 1. Clutch size frequency (bars) and fledging success (squares) in Monk Parakeets.
Open bars represent tirst clutches; shaded bars are replacement and second clutches. Vertical
lines indicate 95% Tukeys HSD intervals above and below mean values.
from one to 11 eggs (mode = 6 eggs in first clutches); whereas replacement and second
clutches ranged from one to seven eggs (mode = 5 eggs) (Fig. 1). First-clutches were
significantly larger than second and replacement clutches (Mann-Whitney test; 2 = 3.27; P
= 0.001).
Clutch size decreased with the date of laying. As the breeding season advanced, first
clutches were smaller (Spearman Rank Correlation; r, = -0.290, N = 200, P < 0.001).
Hatching success was significantly correlated to laying date and clutch size. Hatching
success decreased throughout the nesting season when a partial correlation was used to hold
clutch size constant (Partial Correlation coefficient; R, = -0.225, N = 200, P < O.Ol), and,
in turn, increased with the clutch size when laying date was held constant (R, = 0.332, N
= 200, P < 0.01).
Fledging success was not correlated with either laying date (RP = -0.050, N = 200, P
> 0.05) or clutch size (& = 0.123, N = 200, P > 0.05) when clutch size and relative
laying date were held constant, respectively.
As a result of both the greater number of eggs and the higher hatching success of larger
clutches, the number of young fledging per pair increased with clutch size. The most pro-
ductive clutches were those of seven eggs, but they did not differ significantly from clutches
of six or 2 eight eggs (Fig. 1). Virtually none of the clutches of one to three eggs success-
fully fledged young (Fig. 1). The low frequency of these small clutches led us to think that
they could be the result of undetected layings and subsequent predation (between visits), or
due to the desertion or death of at least one member of the pairbond. Furthermore, their
low productivity is likely to be a consequence of any of these factors,
Laying date of first clutches did not affect wether females laid a replacement or second
clutch. Early and late breeders did not differ
in the frequency of starting replacement or
744
THE WILSON BULLETIN
l
Vol.
107, No. 4,
December 1995
second clutches (x2 = 0.155, df = 1, P = 0.7). N evertheless, the date of the onset of laying
had a significant effect on the chances of producing at least one young from the first clutch.
Early breeding pairs (those that began their first clutch before the respective median date of
each year) produced at least one young in 55% of the first clutches started compared to
only 39% for late breeders (x2 = 4.79, df = 1, P = 0.029).
Laying date of first clutches was significantly negatively correlated with the number of
parakeets inhabiting compound nests (Y, = -0.256, N = 184, P < 0.001). First clutches
were laid earlier in larger nests. Hatching success and fledging success of first clutches had
positive but non-significant relationships with the number of individuals occupying the com-
pound nest, when the date of onset of laying was held constant by partial correlation (Rr =
0.140 and R, = 0.083, N = 184; respectively).
The productivity of a nest chamber depended on the date of laying but not on the number
of individuals inhabiting the compound nest to which it belongs. The total number of young
fledged from a nest chamber throughout the season (considering all clutches that had been
laid) was significantly correlated with the laying date of first clutch when the number of
individuals in the compound nest was held constant (R = -0.262, N = 184, P < 0.01).
On the other hand, when date was held constant, the number of young produced was not
correlated with the number of individuals inhabiting the nest (RP = 0.075, N = 184).
Overall, the total number of young fledged from breeding chambers in compound nests
that were occupied by more than four birds (median value) was higher (but not significantly)
than fledging success of smaller nests (Z = 0.693; N = 43 and 162, respectively; P =
0.488). Also, the proportion of successful breeding pairs (first clutches only) in nests inhab-
ited by more than four birds (55.8%) was higher (but not significantly) than that of smaller
nests (42.6%) (x2 = 2.397; df = 1; P = 0.122). This difference was even lower when first
and non-first clutches were considered together.
Discussion.-Our results show that the date of laying was the most important correlate
of reproductive success in Monk Parakeets. Laying date correlates both with the clutch size
and hatching success, so on average early clutches are larger and produce more nestlings
and, consequently, more fledglings than late clutches (because the fledging success is in-
dependent of date and clutch size). This result coincides with the observations of Beissinger
and Waltman (1991) in Green-rumped Parrotlet, in which the number of young increased
with clutch size. In addition, in the Monk Parakeet early clutches are more likely to produce
at least one juvenile than late clutches.
There is a slight trend by breeding pairs inhabiting large nests to start egg laying earlier
in the season. In larger nests, interactions among individuals such as social stimulation, self-
maintenance behavior (e.g., foraging) and nest construction and maintenance, are facilitated
(e.g., all members of a compound nest participate in bringing material to the nest and in
building and maintenance activities) (Bucher et al. 1991, Martella and Bucher 1993). These
interactions seem to play an important role in the Monk Parakeet breeding system (Martella
1985, Bucher et al. 1991), and could promote earlier laying attempts.
Despite the above mentioned trend, individuals inhabiting larger nests do not have, on
average, a significantly higher productivity nor chances of being successful with their first
clutch compared to individuals at smaller nests. Larger nests are frequently occupied si-
multaneously (in different chambers) by adults with breeding experience and inexperienced
breeders (two-year-olds; Martella 1985, Martin and Bucher 1993). Taking into account that
either experienced or novice breeders are physiologically capable of breeding, and that egg
laying could be not as costly to females as rearing the young, as proposed by Waltman and
Beissinger (1992) in Green-rumped Parrotlet, one can expect that all pairs should begin a
clutch. However, unskilled breeders could not always be “ready” (see below) for facing the
SHORT COMMUNICATIONS
745
effort of taking care of a large brood. Therefore, the lower success of inexperienced breeders
could be contributing to reduce the average productivity of large communal nests.
Given that the Monk Parakeet is a long lived species (about 5 years on average and 12
years maximum; Martin 1989), the reduction of its reproductive output could be explained
by the “restraint” hypothesis, the “constraint” hypothesis, or a combination of both (Curio
1983). If additional breeding effort required to rear a large brood under unfavorable con-
ditions could reduce the individuals probability of subsequent survival, young Monk Par-
akeets should refrain from investing maximally in actual reproduction and should increase
their breeding effort as they become older to maximize lifetime reproductive success. Be-
sides, the less successful breeding attempts may allow Monk Parakeets to gain experience
and, therefore, to improve their breeding performance in following breeding seasons.
Lacks (1954) suggested that the most common clutch size should be the most productive.
However, the Monk Parakeets modal clutch size (six eggs) was not more productive than
larger clutch sizes. This argues against Lacks prediction, but agrees with other studies in
parrots (Smith and Saunders 1986, Beissinger and Waltman 1991) and other bird species in
which deviations from Lacks principle have been ascribed either to differences in female
age, experience and condition, or to territory quality (e.g., Klomp 1970, Winkler and Walters
1983). Under the assumption that reproductive restraint or constraint hypotheses are oper-
ating, one could expect that the frequency distribution of clutch sizes of Monk Parakeets
would not fit to Lacks (1954) predictions.
All above mentioned issues are closely related and, as consequence, this study is not
conclusive about the forces driving the reproductive success in the Monk Parakeet. Although
under field conditions it could be difficult to tease apart their effect, further research focused
on these topics is required.
Acknowledgments.-We
thank A. Gdmez Durti and the Romanutti family for the use of
their properties as study areas, and the Campos family for their kind hospitality throughout
the study. The Cooperativa ElCctrica of Arroyito provided logistical support. L. Martin, H.
Merlini, I? Wallace, and S. Miglietta gave us helpful aid during fieldwork. M. Nores made
valuable comments on an earlier draft of this paper. We are also indebted to S. R. Beissinger
and an anonymous referee for critically reading the manuscript. The Consejo National de
Investigaciones Cientificas y TCcnicas of Argentina (CONICET) provided funds through
post-graduate scholarships to J.L.N. and a research grant to E.H.B. (PID 3-908101/8.5).
LITERATURE CITED
BEISSINGER, S. R.
AND
J. R. WALTMAN. 1991. Extraordinary clutch size and hatching asyn-
chrony of a neotropical parrot. Auk 108:863-871.
BUCHER, E. H., L. E MARTIN, M. B. MARTELLA, AND J. L. NAVARRO. 1991. Social behaviour
and population dynamics of the Monk Parakeet. Acta XX Congressus Internationalis
Ornithologici:681-689.
CURIO, E. 1983. Why do young birds reproduce less well? Ibis 125:4OO-t03.
FORSHAW, J. 1978. Parrots of the world. Landsdowne Editions. Melbourne, Australia.
CLOMP,
H. 1970. The determination of clutch-size in birds. A review. Ardea 58: l-124.
LACK, D. 1954. The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon, Oxford, England.
MARTELLA, M. B. 1985. Observaciones sobre el comportamiento de la cotorra
Myiopsittu
monachus
con especial tnfasis en la comunicacidn Sonora. Ph.D. diss. Univ. Nat. de
Cbrdoba, C&doba, Argentina.
AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1993. Estructura de1 nido y comportamiento de nidificacibn de
la cotorra
Myiopsittu monachus.
Boletin de la Sot. Zool. de1 Uruguay 8:21 l-217.
MARTIN, L. E 1989. Caracteristicas de1 sistema social cooperative de la cotorra
Myiopsittu
monachus.
Ph.D. diss. Univ. Nat. de Cdrdoba, Argentina.
746
THE WILSON BULLETIN *
Vol.
107, No. 4.
December 1995
- AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1993. Natal dispersal and first breeding age in Monk Parakeets.
Auk 110:930-933.
NAVARRO, J. L. 1989. Din&mica poblacional de la cotorra (Myiopsittu monachus). Ph.D.
diss. Univ. Nat. de Cbrdoba, Argentina.
-AND E. H. BUCHER. 1990. Growth of Monk Parakeets. Wilson Bull. 102:520-525.
AND -.
1992. Annual variation in the timing of breeding of the Monk Parakeet
in relation to climatic factors. Wilson Bull. 104545-549.
-, M. B. MARTELLA,
AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1992. Breeding season and productivity of
Monk Parakeets in C&doba, Argentina. Wilson Bull. 104:413424.
PERRINS, C. M.
AND P.
J. JONES. 1974. The inheritance of clutch size in the Great Tit.
Condor 76:225-229.
SMITH, G. T
AND
D. A. SAUNDERS. 1986. Clutch size and productivity in three sympatric
species of cockatoo (Psittaciformes) in the South-West of Western Australia. Aust.
Wildl. Res. 13:275-285.
WALTMAN, J. R. AND S. R. BEISSINGER. 1992. Breeding behavior of the Green-rumped
Parrotlet. Wilson Bull. 104:65-84.
WINKLER, D. W.
AND
J. R. WALTERS. 1983. The determination of clutch size in precocial
birds. Pp. 33-68 in Current Ornithology Vol. 1 (R. E Johnston, ed.). Plenum Press,
New York, New York.
JOAQU~N L. NAVARRO,
MONICA B. MARTELLA, AND
ENRIQUE H. BUCHER, Centro
de Zoologia
Aplicada, Univ. National de Cdrdoba, Casilla de correo 122, 5000 Cdrdoba, Argentina.
Received II Oct. 1994, accepted 15 April 1995.
Wilson Bull., 107(4),
1995, pp. 746-752
The influence of Yellow-bellied Sapsuckers on local insect community structure.-
Natural communities consist of irregular patchworks of organisms whose relative abun-
dances both reflect and influence environmental heterogeneity (e.g., Diamond and Case
1986, Cornell and Lawton 1992). Within a given habitat, any species that augments envi-
ronmental patchiness is likely to influence the abundances of sympatric species. For instance,
through their unusual foraging strategy, Yellow-bellied Sapsuckers
(Sphyrapicus varius)
cre-
ate rich resource patches in the form of sapwells, and may thereby affect population densities
and spatial distributions of many co-occurring species.
The Yellow-bellied Sapsucker feeds primarily on phloem and sap obtained through the
excavation of sapwells. A sapwell consists of horizontal rows of shallow holes drilled into
bark, into which sap flows (Tate 1973). Sapsuckers drill newer rows directly above older
wells, producing a two dimensional array of shallow holes. While the volume and quality
of sap flow depend on both the tree species and the daily rates of transpiration and photo-
synthesis (Foster and Tate 1966), flow can often be considerable and sap often contains 20-
30% sucrose (Tate 1973). Insects comprising seven orders and 20 families have been found
associated with sapwells (Foster and Tate 1966). Preferential orientation of insects toward
rich resource patches may be manifest as increased abundance and/or diversity of nectivo-
rous insect communities in close proximity to sapwells. Therefore, by serving as a focal
point for insect aggregation, sapwells may influence the local community structure (i.e.,
absolute and relative abundances) of the nectivorous insect guild. The purpose of this study
was to determine (1) whether sapsucker foraging influences local insect diversity and species
... Introduced in more than 20 countries, it is the only member of the parrot family able to build its own nest [7,8]. Thus, as they thrive in introduced urban colonies, so they do their nests; these are many-chambered structures that confer refuge and breeding substrate to colonies of many individuals [7,[9][10][11][12][13]. Given that nests provide shelter to several species, it has been recently classified as an ecosystem engineer due to its potential to shape the ecology of sympatric species [14,15]. ...
... For Monk parakeets, Tasmanian blue gum is important for nest construction in its native range in Argentina, contributing to their population expansion toward Patagonia [23]. This apparent preference of Monk Parakeets to build their nests in blue gum trees versus smaller native trees would relate to blue gum's height and, thus, a higher productivity of reproductive parakeet couples that choose them [11,12,70]. The Tasmanian blue gum tree and other genera such as Pinus sp. and Phoenix sp. were also chosen by invasive Monk Parakeets to construct their nests in Barcelona, Spain [9]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Monk Parakeets are considered one of the most invasive bird species given its unique capacity among psittacines to build their own communal nests. Originally introduced as pets in houses from where they escaped or were released, they are currently considered invasive in more than 20 countries worldwide. This is the case in Chile, where Monk Parakeets were introduced during the 1970s. Between 2016 and 2019 we searched Monk Parakeets’ nests structures in the Santiago metropolis region. We identified 1458 Monk Parakeets’ communal nests on 546 trees belonging to 34 tree species. Ninety-one percent of the occupied trees were also introduced. Paraná pine and cedar of Lebanon were the tree species with highest abundance of nests, averaging more than four nests/tree/species, with 23 and 18 maximum number of nests, respectively. Tasmanian blue gum and black locust were selected by parakeets more often than expected, based on availability. From all trees, 24.6% denoted health problems and 47.3% were pruned. The average nest height was 14.2 m and nests were observed mainly in secondary branches (59.3%). The occupancy rate was 89.7% and was associated to nest height and type of branch. During two reproductive seasons we quantified eggs and nestlings in chambers averaging 4.5 and 4.2, respectively. We provide a rough population size estimate and the characteristics of Monk Parakeets nest and tree selectivity, aiming to characterize several decades of a neglected urban invasion to warrant strategies for improved management measures.
... These nests may weigh up more than 1000 kg [10,50]. As non-migrants, they remain in their nests for nesting and roosting year-round [51]. It is estimated that in their native distribution, 30 to 60% of chambers will be occupied for breeding [44,52]. ...
... These complex structures, which parakeets build communally, include several isolated chambers and may weigh hundreds of kilograms [10,50,62]. The nests are occupied throughout the year and may be used by several members of a colony, as reproductive couples, non-reproductive helpers, or parakeets in developing stages [46,[49][50][51]. The nest also provides protection for offspring against predators and microclimate control [63]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The monk parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus) is considered to be one of the most invasive bird species because its unique ability among parrots to build their own communal nests. Currently, they are considered an invasive species in 19 countries and a pest—even in their native distribution—because of economic losses derived from their impacts. During the reproductive seasons of 2017 and 2018, we registered interactions between invasive monk parakeets and resident bird species in Santiago, Chile. We observed agonistic and affiliative interactions, and further, we described monk parakeets’ nest occupancy by nine bird species, two invasive and seven native. For this reason, we consider that the monk parakeet is an allogenic ecosystem engineer with the potential to shape distribution and richness of sympatric species in urban environments. Our results contribute to an assessment of the implications of the monk parakeet’s ecological invasion to other synanthropic species, and raise concern of other potential impacts, such as pathogen transmission derived from these interactions.
... Pairs have been reported to be socially monogamous (Navarro et al. 1995), but the duration of pair bonds is unknown. Nestlings fledge approximately 40 days after hatching (Bucher et al. 1991) and remain with their parents for about three months (Martin and Bucher 1993). ...
... Work by Bucher and his collaborators (e.g., Bucher et al. 1991;Martin and Bucher 1993;Navarro et al. 1992Navarro et al. , 1995 in Cordoba Province, Argentina, has yielded a great deal of information about the basic and population biology of these birds. Here I present the results of a detailed behavioral study of breeding Monk Parakeets. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Monk Parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus) is unique among parrots because it constructs stick nests rather than nesting in holes. This study provides a detailed description of the species' breeding biology and provides evidence that this species might breed cooperatively. Although many parakeet pairs were observed roosting in solitary nests, breeding occurred only in nests within colonies or chambers within compound nests housing other parakeets. The male was responsible for all or most of the nest construction and maintenance. He fed the female during the incubation and early nestling periods, but later in the nestling period both the male and female fed the nestlings. Most breeding attempts involved a male-female pair, but three separate breeding attempts were made by trios (two trios included a female and two males, and the third trio was composed of a male and two females). In the trios, one of the auxiliary bird contributed less to the breeding effort than the primary male and female. The observations of trios support the idea that Monk Parakeets are similar to cooperative breeders, but the lack of cooperation in nest building indicates that colonial nesting may be a result of other benefits of group living, such as improved predator detection.
... These nests are entwined with twigs and branches by a parakeet couple or several family members, to construct chambers that may be aggregated over several seasons, thus enlarging these nests (Spreyer & Bucher, 1998). Monk parakeets use their nests for breeding and roosting year-round (Navarro et al., 1995) and are considered to be highly philopatric (Martin & Bucher, 1993). This bird species is deemed to be synanthropic (Briceño et al., 2019) and thus is mainly found in urban environments. ...
Article
Full-text available
Myiopsitta monachus is an invasive psittacine with wide distribution due to the pet trade. Its large communal nests and synanthropic nature contribute to its successful colonization of cities, from where it seems to be expanding in range and numbers. This is relevant with regard to pathogens that invasive species may harbor, especially when host populations thrive. We aimed to identify an abundant mite found in invasive monk parakeet chicks that had been collected in Santiago during 2017 and 2018. Through morphological and molecular identification of the 18S ribosomal RNA gene, we confirmed the presence of Ornithonyssus bursa. This was the first report of this mite in Chile. This mite is common in native and invasive monk parakeet populations and may affect other birds, including domestic fowl. Further, this mite bites people and can be a potential vector of pathogens such as bacteria or viruses. We conclude that this parasite was likely introduced with the parakeet and discuss possible ecological, health and economic consequences of this new potential pest.
... These nests are entwined with twigs and branches by a parakeet couple or several family members, to construct chambers that may be aggregated over several seasons, thus enlarging these nests (Spreyer & Bucher, 1998). Monk parakeets use their nests for breeding and roosting year-round (Navarro et al., 1995) and are considered to be highly philopatric (Martin & Bucher, 1993). This bird species is deemed to be synanthropic (Briceño et al., 2019) and thus is mainly found in urban environments. ...
Article
Full-text available
Myiopsitta monachus is an invasive psittacine with wide distribution due to the pet trade. Its large communal nests and synanthropic nature contribute to its successful colonization of cities, from where it seems to be expanding in range and numbers. This is relevant with regard to pathogens that invasive species may harbor, especially when host populations thrive. We aimed to identify an abundant mite found in invasive monk parakeet chicks that had been collected in Santiago during 2017 and 2018. Through morphological and molecular identification of the 18S ribosomal RNA gene, we confirmed the presence of Ornithonyssus bursa. This was the first report of this mite in Chile. This mite is common in native and invasive monk parakeet populations and may affect other birds, including domestic fowl. Further, this mite bites people and can be a potential vector of pathogens such as bacteria or viruses. We conclude that this parasite was likely introduced with the parakeet and discuss possible ecological, health and economic consequences of this new potential pest.
... Of the 12 species with some form of geographic variation in their vocalisations, 11 species are cavity nesters that defend nesting territories. The exceptions are Monk Parakeets, which craft their stick nests and sometimes nest communally in nests with multiple chambers (Navarro et al. 1995). Thick-billed Parrots, which have invariant calls, are cavity nesters that will sometimes nest communally in trees that contain more than one suitable cavity and are not agonistic to their neighbours during the breeding season (Monterrubio-Rico et al. 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Vocal dialects have fascinated biologists for over 50 years. This mosaic pattern of geographic variation in learned vocalisations was first described in a songbird, and since that time most studies investigating dialects have focused on songbird species. Here we examine patterns of geographic variation in the calls of a different group of vocal learning birds, the parrots (order Psittaciformes). We summarise the growing literature on vocal variation in parrots, and complement this review with a survey of variation in the genus Amazona using calls from sound libraries. We find strikingly similar patterns to those previously found in songbirds. Over 90% of parrots examined in the literature, and 69% of Amazona species surveyed, showed geographic variation consistent with a propensity to share local call types. This trait is evolutionarily labile and widespread; within Amazona most clades contained species with and without geographic variation, and most major lineages of parrots include representatives with dialects. We found little support for the long-standing hypothesis that dialects isolate populations and thus generate genetic differences among populations. Instead, most studies support the idea that dialects are maintained by social benefits of matching local call types, a finding that has implications for the management of captive and endangered populations. Considerable scope remains for studies that experimentally test hypotheses for the exact nature of these benefits, as well as studies that employ comparisons among species, to understand how the interplay between ecology, social dynamics and vocal learning capacities produces different patterns of variation across the parrots.
... The monk parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus) is a medium-sized sexually monomorphic parrot originally distributed in Paraguay, Uruguay, Bolivia, southern Brazil and northern and central Argentina (EBERHARD, 1998). As non-migrants, they remain in their nests both for nesting and for roosting year-round (NAVARRO et al., 1995). It is the only parrot, among over 350 species, that is able to build its own communal nests, thus making them independent of the need for tree or cliff cavities (MARTIN & BUCHER, 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
Central Chile has been identified as a unique ecosystem with high conservation priority because of its high levels of endemism and intensive anthropic pressure. Over a period of almost four decades, the monk parakeet has been successful in establishing and dispersing in urban Santiago, although little is known about its potential impact. Furthermore, nothing is known about its epidemiological risks towards animals or even humans. For this reason, we conducted the first parasitic survey of monk parakeets in Chile through capture, necropsy and thorough external and internal inspection of 92 adult individuals. Among these, 45.7% presented lice that were identified as Paragoniocotes fulvofasciatum, 1.1% had mesostigmatid acari and 8.9% had free-ranging acari. Among 89 parakeets, 19.1% had structures identified as Cryptosporidium sp. This study provides the first description of Cryptosporidium sp. in monk parakeets. Along with the presence of a mesostigmatid acarus in one parakeet, this serves as a public health warning, given that both of these parasites have zoonotic potential.
... In the native range, monk parakeet only breed once per season (with often only one clutch), exhibit low fledging rates (1-2 per nest), a high variance in breeding performance across individuals, and delayed first reproduction (Navarro et al. 1995;Eberhard 1998). In addition, natal dispersal seems to be restricted to relatively short distances of 2 km or less (Martín and Bucher 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
Invasive species are a global threat to biodiversity, economy and human wellbeing. To mitigate these threats, identifying and halting the introduction of potentially invasive species is crucial. Although progress has been made in elucidating mechanisms underlying invasion success, the role of species behavioral strategies has only received scant attention. Here, we use the invasion of monk parakeets in Santa Catarina state, southern Brazil to study whether behavioral strategies such as nest thermoregulation and the ability to self-medicate against pathogens contribute to the establishment success of invading species. We relate data on monk parakeet reproductive success to ambient temperatures in- and outside nesting chambers and test the bactericidal potential of plants transported to the nest by breeding monk parakeets. Compared to breeding data from other invaded ranges and parts of the species’ native range, our results suggest both thermoregulation and the use of bactericidal plants could potentially influence monk parakeet reproductive success. Thermoregulation maintains stable temperatures of incubator chambers compared to large fluctuations (especially hotter extremes) outside the nest. At least one of the plants brought to the nest effectively inhibited growth of pathogenic bacteria. The union of these two factors could increase reproductive rates and may consequently aid the expansion of the species in new non-native environments.
Article
Full-text available
The Monk Parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus) is considered among the most damaging bird pest species for crops in Argentina. Historically, lethal control has been preferred as the most effective method to decrease damage. However, objections to this method are increasing. We summarize problems involving parakeets and crop production, including crop damage characterization as well as ecological and human aspects involved in the conflicts. Additionally, we summarize available management alternatives to decrease crop damage. Although certain ecological characteristics of the Monk Parakeet would favour crop damage in some situations, human factors, such as damage overestimation and tendency to use population control methods (lethal and reproductive), probably contribute to a magnification of the dimensions of the conflict. This also will be favoured by the practically inexistent damage estimations on crops, making an objective evaluation of conflicts difficult. In order to develop a more sustainable agriculture, we urge the application of strategic management principles, with the development of multidisciplinary research to clearly identify conflicts, quantify the magnitude of damage, apply management alternatives appropriate for each situation and, finally, objectively evaluate the results in terms of costs and benefits.
Article
Full-text available
RESUMEN.— La Cotorra (Myiopsitta monachus) está considerada entre las principales especies de aves que causan daños en cultivos agrícolas en Argentina. Históricamente, se ha favorecido el control letal como método más efectivo para disminuir los daños. No obstante, existen cada vez más cuestionamientos a esta práctica. En este trabajo se resumen los problemas que involucran a la Cotorra y las actividades agrícolas, incluyendo características de los daños y aspectos ecológicos y humanos que favorecerían los conflictos originados por los daños en los cultivos. Además, se presenta un compendio de las alternativas de manejo actualmente disponibles para disminuir los daños. Si bien existen características ecológicas de las cotorras que favorecerían los daños en determinadas situaciones, hay factores humanos, incluyendo la sobreestimación de los daños y la tendencia a usar métodos de control poblacional (letal y reproductivo), que probablemente contribuyan a generar un nivel de conflicto mayor del que realmente existe. Esto sería favorecido por la casi inexistencia de estimaciones independientes de las pérdidas ocasionadas por las coto-rras, lo que dificulta una evaluación objetiva del problema. A fin de contribuir a una agricultura más sustentable, se considera perentorio explorar enfoques de manejo estratégico, desarrollando investigaciones multidisciplinarias tendientes a identificar claramente los conflictos, cuantificar la magnitud de los daños, aplicar alternativas de manejo adecuadas a cada situación y, finalmente, evaluar objetivamente los resultados, en términos de costo–beneficio. ABSTRACT. CONSIDERATIONS FOR REDUCING CONFLICTS AROUND DAMAGE OF AGRICULTURAL CROPS BY MONK PARAKEET (MYIOPSITTAMONACHUS).— The Monk Parakeet (Myiopsitta monachus) is considered among the most damaging bird pest species for crops in Argentina. Historically, lethal control has been preferred as the most effective method to decrease damage. However, objections to this method are increasing. We summarize problems involving parakeets and crop production, including crop damage characterization as well as ecological and human aspects involved in the conflicts. Additionally, we summarize available management alternatives to decrease crop damage. Although certain ecological characteristics of the Monk Parakeet would favour crop damage in some situations, human factors, such as damage overestimation and tendency to use population control methods (lethal and reproductive), probably contribute to a magnification of the dimensions of the conflict. This also will be favoured by the practically inexistent damage estimations on crops, making an objective evaluation of conflicts difficult. In order to develop a more sustainable agriculture, we urge the application of strategic management principles, with the development of multidisciplinary research to clearly identify conflicts, quantify the magnitude of damage, apply management alternatives appropriate for each situation and, finally, objectively evaluate the results in terms of costs and benefits.
Article
The deficiency of young parent birds to mate and reproduce successfully has been conventionally explained with 'reproductive constraint', eg lack of experience. Both life history theory and the demonstration of increased mortality with age suggests that 'reproductive restraint' is equally tenable. Young breeders with high future prospects of breeding refrain from expending maximal parental effort to forestall the associated risk of dying and, hence, future reproductive loss. -P.J.Jarvis
Extraordinary clutch size and hatching asyn-chrony of a neotropical parrot
  • S R Beissinger
BEISSINGER, S. R. AND J. R. WALTMAN. 1991. Extraordinary clutch size and hatching asyn-chrony of a neotropical parrot. Auk 108:863-871