Content uploaded by Enrique H. Bucher
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Enrique H. Bucher
Content may be subject to copyright.
742
THE WILSON BULLETIN *
Vol.
107, No. 4,
December I995
Wilson Bull., 107(4), 1995, pp. 742-746
Effects of laying date, clutch size, and communal nest size on the reproductive success
of
Monk Parakeets.-Monk Parakeets
(Myiopsittu monachus)
build conspicuous, enclosed
stick nests, which are often large and which have several isolated chambers (Forshaw 1978,
Martella and Bucher 1993). These chambers are occupied throughout the year by breeding
pairs and non-breeding individuals (Martin 1989, Martella and Bucher 1993). The Monk
Parakeet is iteroparous, monogamous, and breeds in the spring and summer (October to
February) (Navarro et al. 1992). Although all members of a compound nest participate in
the building, maintenance, and vigilance of the nest, only the breeding pair cares for the
nestlings (Martella 1985, Martella and Bucher 1993). Few cases of allofeeding by members
of different nests have been observed, which could function for the development of a com-
munal breeding system (Martella 1985, Bucher et al. 1991).
Monk Parakeets have a variable clutch size (Navarro et al. 1992) the adaptive nature of
which has been the subject of extensive discussion (e.g., Lack 1954, Perrins and Jones
1974). However, except for the research of Beissinger and Waltman (1991) on the Green-
rumped Parrotlet (Forpus passen’nus), this topic has been little studied in parrots.
In this paper we examine how laying date, clutch size, and the size of communal nest
affect the reproductive success of Monk Parakeets.
Study areas and
methods.-This study was carried out in 1982 near the town of Arroyito
(31”25’S, 62”59’W) and from 1983 to 1988 close to the city of Jesus Maria (31”05’S,
64”l l’W), in Cordoba Province, Argentina. Overall, we recorded 205 breeding attempts.
Nest chambers were checked at seven to 12-day intervals from October to early March.
This interval was selected to avoid a reduction of the breeding success due to observers
disturbance (Navarro et al. 1992). In Arroyito, sampled nests were all grouped in
Eucalyptus
trees planted at both sides of the 1000-m long entrance to the ranch. In J. Maria, all nests
were in the 610.ha study area in native trees. We could not sample very large nests (with
more than ten individuals) because they were on inaccessible structures on powerline towers,
windmills and tall trees. However, large nests comprised only 2% of the nests in both areas
(Bucher et al. 1991).
Eggs found in each nest chamber were marked individually with indelible ink. Nestlings
were identified by clipping different toenails and later were banded with numbered alumi-
num rings. The fate of each egg was recorded as either lost before hatching, failed to hatch,
or hatched. Eggs that might have hatched but whose chicks disappeared before the following
visit were assumed not to have hatched. Nestlings that disappeared from the nest before
reaching fledging age (35 days; Navarro and Bucher 1990) were recorded as deaths. In
doubtful cases, the survival was checked through capture-release procedures carried out three
times a year during all the study (Navarro 1989; Martin and Bucher 1993). Initiation dates
for clutches were determined by back-dating each clutch based on a 2.1 -day interval between
successive eggs, a 24.day incubation period (Navarro 1989) and the calculated age of
nestlings (Navarro and Bucher 1990). Twenty-two clutches in which we could not estimate
the onset of laying were discarded. To compare dates among years, we used the distance in
days of a given date from the median date of first clutch for the population in that year,
assigning negative values to those dates earlier than the median. First clutches refer to the
first group of eggs laid in a given nest chamber in that season, replacement clutches are
those started after failure of the first clutch, and second clutches are those laid after a
successful first brood.
Results.-The
earliest clutch was laid on 1 October 1983 and the latest on 29 October
1985. At both areas, 90% of first clutches within the population were laid within a nine
week period (see also Navarro et al. 1992; Navarro and Bucher 1992). Clutch sizes ranged
SHORT COMMUNICATIONS
CLUTCH SIZE
FIG. 1. Clutch size frequency (bars) and fledging success (squares) in Monk Parakeets.
Open bars represent tirst clutches; shaded bars are replacement and second clutches. Vertical
lines indicate 95% Tukey’s HSD intervals above and below mean values.
from one to 11 eggs (mode = 6 eggs in first clutches); whereas replacement and second
clutches ranged from one to seven eggs (mode = 5 eggs) (Fig. 1). First-clutches were
significantly larger than second and replacement clutches (Mann-Whitney test; 2 = 3.27; P
= 0.001).
Clutch size decreased with the date of laying. As the breeding season advanced, first
clutches were smaller (Spearman Rank Correlation; r, = -0.290, N = 200, P < 0.001).
Hatching success was significantly correlated to laying date and clutch size. Hatching
success decreased throughout the nesting season when a partial correlation was used to hold
clutch size constant (Partial Correlation coefficient; R, = -0.225, N = 200, P < O.Ol), and,
in turn, increased with the clutch size when laying date was held constant (R, = 0.332, N
= 200, P < 0.01).
Fledging success was not correlated with either laying date (RP = -0.050, N = 200, P
> 0.05) or clutch size (& = 0.123, N = 200, P > 0.05) when clutch size and relative
laying date were held constant, respectively.
As a result of both the greater number of eggs and the higher hatching success of larger
clutches, the number of young fledging per pair increased with clutch size. The most pro-
ductive clutches were those of seven eggs, but they did not differ significantly from clutches
of six or 2 eight eggs (Fig. 1). Virtually none of the clutches of one to three eggs success-
fully fledged young (Fig. 1). The low frequency of these small clutches led us to think that
they could be the result of undetected layings and subsequent predation (between visits), or
due to the desertion or death of at least one member of the pairbond. Furthermore, their
low productivity is likely to be a consequence of any of these factors,
Laying date of first clutches did not affect wether females laid a replacement or second
clutch. Early and late breeders did not differ
in the frequency of starting replacement or
744
THE WILSON BULLETIN
l
Vol.
107, No. 4,
December 1995
second clutches (x2 = 0.155, df = 1, P = 0.7). N evertheless, the date of the onset of laying
had a significant effect on the chances of producing at least one young from the first clutch.
Early breeding pairs (those that began their first clutch before the respective median date of
each year) produced at least one young in 55% of the first clutches started compared to
only 39% for late breeders (x2 = 4.79, df = 1, P = 0.029).
Laying date of first clutches was significantly negatively correlated with the number of
parakeets inhabiting compound nests (Y, = -0.256, N = 184, P < 0.001). First clutches
were laid earlier in larger nests. Hatching success and fledging success of first clutches had
positive but non-significant relationships with the number of individuals occupying the com-
pound nest, when the date of onset of laying was held constant by partial correlation (Rr =
0.140 and R, = 0.083, N = 184; respectively).
The productivity of a nest chamber depended on the date of laying but not on the number
of individuals inhabiting the compound nest to which it belongs. The total number of young
fledged from a nest chamber throughout the season (considering all clutches that had been
laid) was significantly correlated with the laying date of first clutch when the number of
individuals in the compound nest was held constant (R = -0.262, N = 184, P < 0.01).
On the other hand, when date was held constant, the number of young produced was not
correlated with the number of individuals inhabiting the nest (RP = 0.075, N = 184).
Overall, the total number of young fledged from breeding chambers in compound nests
that were occupied by more than four birds (median value) was higher (but not significantly)
than fledging success of smaller nests (Z = 0.693; N = 43 and 162, respectively; P =
0.488). Also, the proportion of successful breeding pairs (first clutches only) in nests inhab-
ited by more than four birds (55.8%) was higher (but not significantly) than that of smaller
nests (42.6%) (x2 = 2.397; df = 1; P = 0.122). This difference was even lower when first
and non-first clutches were considered together.
Discussion.-Our results show that the date of laying was the most important correlate
of reproductive success in Monk Parakeets. Laying date correlates both with the clutch size
and hatching success, so on average early clutches are larger and produce more nestlings
and, consequently, more fledglings than late clutches (because the fledging success is in-
dependent of date and clutch size). This result coincides with the observations of Beissinger
and Waltman (1991) in Green-rumped Parrotlet, in which the number of young increased
with clutch size. In addition, in the Monk Parakeet early clutches are more likely to produce
at least one juvenile than late clutches.
There is a slight trend by breeding pairs inhabiting large nests to start egg laying earlier
in the season. In larger nests, interactions among individuals such as social stimulation, self-
maintenance behavior (e.g., foraging) and nest construction and maintenance, are facilitated
(e.g., all members of a compound nest participate in bringing material to the nest and in
building and maintenance activities) (Bucher et al. 1991, Martella and Bucher 1993). These
interactions seem to play an important role in the Monk Parakeet breeding system (Martella
1985, Bucher et al. 1991), and could promote earlier laying attempts.
Despite the above mentioned trend, individuals inhabiting larger nests do not have, on
average, a significantly higher productivity nor chances of being successful with their first
clutch compared to individuals at smaller nests. Larger nests are frequently occupied si-
multaneously (in different chambers) by adults with breeding experience and inexperienced
breeders (two-year-olds; Martella 1985, Martin and Bucher 1993). Taking into account that
either experienced or novice breeders are physiologically capable of breeding, and that egg
laying could be not as costly to females as rearing the young, as proposed by Waltman and
Beissinger (1992) in Green-rumped Parrotlet, one can expect that all pairs should begin a
clutch. However, unskilled breeders could not always be “ready” (see below) for facing the
SHORT COMMUNICATIONS
745
effort of taking care of a large brood. Therefore, the lower success of inexperienced breeders
could be contributing to reduce the average productivity of large communal nests.
Given that the Monk Parakeet is a long lived species (about 5 years on average and 12
years maximum; Martin 1989), the reduction of its reproductive output could be explained
by the “restraint” hypothesis, the “constraint” hypothesis, or a combination of both (Curio
1983). If additional breeding effort required to rear a large brood under unfavorable con-
ditions could reduce the individual’s probability of subsequent survival, young Monk Par-
akeets should refrain from investing maximally in actual reproduction and should increase
their breeding effort as they become older to maximize lifetime reproductive success. Be-
sides, the less successful breeding attempts may allow Monk Parakeets to gain experience
and, therefore, to improve their breeding performance in following breeding seasons.
Lack’s (1954) suggested that the most common clutch size should be the most productive.
However, the Monk Parakeet’s modal clutch size (six eggs) was not more productive than
larger clutch sizes. This argues against Lack’s prediction, but agrees with other studies in
parrots (Smith and Saunders 1986, Beissinger and Waltman 1991) and other bird species in
which deviations from Lack’s principle have been ascribed either to differences in female
age, experience and condition, or to territory quality (e.g., Klomp 1970, Winkler and Walters
1983). Under the assumption that reproductive restraint or constraint hypotheses are oper-
ating, one could expect that the frequency distribution of clutch sizes of Monk Parakeets
would not fit to Lack’s (1954) predictions.
All above mentioned issues are closely related and, as consequence, this study is not
conclusive about the forces driving the reproductive success in the Monk Parakeet. Although
under field conditions it could be difficult to tease apart their effect, further research focused
on these topics is required.
Acknowledgments.-We
thank A. Gdmez Durti and the Romanutti family for the use of
their properties as study areas, and the Campos family for their kind hospitality throughout
the study. The Cooperativa ElCctrica of Arroyito provided logistical support. L. Martin, H.
Merlini, I? Wallace, and S. Miglietta gave us helpful aid during fieldwork. M. Nores made
valuable comments on an earlier draft of this paper. We are also indebted to S. R. Beissinger
and an anonymous referee for critically reading the manuscript. The Consejo National de
Investigaciones Cientificas y TCcnicas of Argentina (CONICET) provided funds through
post-graduate scholarships to J.L.N. and a research grant to E.H.B. (PID 3-908101/8.5).
LITERATURE CITED
BEISSINGER, S. R.
AND
J. R. WALTMAN. 1991. Extraordinary clutch size and hatching asyn-
chrony of a neotropical parrot. Auk 108:863-871.
BUCHER, E. H., L. E MARTIN, M. B. MARTELLA, AND J. L. NAVARRO. 1991. Social behaviour
and population dynamics of the Monk Parakeet. Acta XX Congressus Internationalis
Ornithologici:681-689.
CURIO, E. 1983. Why do young birds reproduce less well? Ibis 125:4OO-t03.
FORSHAW, J. 1978. Parrots of the world. Landsdowne Editions. Melbourne, Australia.
CLOMP,
H. 1970. The determination of clutch-size in birds. A review. Ardea 58: l-124.
LACK, D. 1954. The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon, Oxford, England.
MARTELLA, M. B. 1985. Observaciones sobre el comportamiento de la cotorra
Myiopsittu
monachus
con especial tnfasis en la comunicacidn Sonora. Ph.D. diss. Univ. Nat. de
Cbrdoba, C&doba, Argentina.
AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1993. Estructura de1 nido y comportamiento de nidificacibn de
la cotorra
Myiopsittu monachus.
Boletin de la Sot. Zool. de1 Uruguay 8:21 l-217.
MARTIN, L. E 1989. Caracteristicas de1 sistema social cooperative de la cotorra
Myiopsittu
monachus.
Ph.D. diss. Univ. Nat. de Cdrdoba, Argentina.
746
THE WILSON BULLETIN *
Vol.
107, No. 4.
December 1995
- AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1993. Natal dispersal and first breeding age in Monk Parakeets.
Auk 110:930-933.
NAVARRO, J. L. 1989. Din&mica poblacional de la cotorra (Myiopsittu monachus). Ph.D.
diss. Univ. Nat. de Cbrdoba, Argentina.
-AND E. H. BUCHER. 1990. Growth of Monk Parakeets. Wilson Bull. 102:520-525.
AND -.
1992. Annual variation in the timing of breeding of the Monk Parakeet
in relation to climatic factors. Wilson Bull. 104545-549.
-, M. B. MARTELLA,
AND
E. H. BUCHER. 1992. Breeding season and productivity of
Monk Parakeets in C&doba, Argentina. Wilson Bull. 104:413424.
PERRINS, C. M.
AND P.
J. JONES. 1974. The inheritance of clutch size in the Great Tit.
Condor 76:225-229.
SMITH, G. T
AND
D. A. SAUNDERS. 1986. Clutch size and productivity in three sympatric
species of cockatoo (Psittaciformes) in the South-West of Western Australia. Aust.
Wildl. Res. 13:275-285.
WALTMAN, J. R. AND S. R. BEISSINGER. 1992. Breeding behavior of the Green-rumped
Parrotlet. Wilson Bull. 104:65-84.
WINKLER, D. W.
AND
J. R. WALTERS. 1983. The determination of clutch size in precocial
birds. Pp. 33-68 in Current Ornithology Vol. 1 (R. E Johnston, ed.). Plenum Press,
New York, New York.
JOAQU~N L. NAVARRO,
MONICA B. MARTELLA, AND
ENRIQUE H. BUCHER, Centro
de Zoologia
Aplicada, Univ. National de Cdrdoba, Casilla de correo 122, 5000 Cdrdoba, Argentina.
Received II Oct. 1994, accepted 15 April 1995.
Wilson Bull., 107(4),
1995, pp. 746-752
The influence of Yellow-bellied Sapsuckers on local insect community structure.-
Natural communities consist of irregular patchworks of organisms whose relative abun-
dances both reflect and influence environmental heterogeneity (e.g., Diamond and Case
1986, Cornell and Lawton 1992). Within a given habitat, any species that augments envi-
ronmental patchiness is likely to influence the abundances of sympatric species. For instance,
through their unusual foraging strategy, Yellow-bellied Sapsuckers
(Sphyrapicus varius)
cre-
ate rich resource patches in the form of sapwells, and may thereby affect population densities
and spatial distributions of many co-occurring species.
The Yellow-bellied Sapsucker feeds primarily on phloem and sap obtained through the
excavation of sapwells. A sapwell consists of horizontal rows of shallow holes drilled into
bark, into which sap flows (Tate 1973). Sapsuckers drill newer rows directly above older
wells, producing a two dimensional array of shallow holes. While the volume and quality
of sap flow depend on both the tree species and the daily rates of transpiration and photo-
synthesis (Foster and Tate 1966), flow can often be considerable and sap often contains 20-
30% sucrose (Tate 1973). Insects comprising seven orders and 20 families have been found
associated with sapwells (Foster and Tate 1966). Preferential orientation of insects toward
rich resource patches may be manifest as increased abundance and/or diversity of nectivo-
rous insect communities in close proximity to sapwells. Therefore, by serving as a focal
point for insect aggregation, sapwells may influence the local community structure (i.e.,
absolute and relative abundances) of the nectivorous insect guild. The purpose of this study
was to determine (1) whether sapsucker foraging influences local insect diversity and species