ArticlePDF Available

The Effect of Noise on Human Performance: A Clinical Trial

Authors:

Abstract

Noise is defined as unwanted or meaningless sound that apart from auditory adverse health effects may distract attention from cues that are important for task performance. Human performance is influenced by many job-related factors and workplace conditions including noise level. To study the effect of noise on human performance. The participants included 40 healthy male university students. The experimental design consisted of 3 (sound pressure level) × 3 (noise schedule) × 2 (noise type) factors. To investigate occupational skill performance, some specific test batteries were used: 1) steadiness test, 2) Minnesota manual dexterity test, 3) hand tool dexterity test, and 4) two-arm coordination test. Time duration of test completion was measured as speed response; to determine error response, the time taken during committing an error by participants while performing a task was measured. Speed response obtained from the 4 tests in combined conditions of noise schedule, harmonic index, and sound pressure level was highest for (intermittent, treble, 95 dB), (continuous, treble, 95 dB), (continuous, treble, 85 dB) and (intermittent, treble, 95 dB), respectively. Treble noise was found significant in reducing human performance; also, intermittent noise, especially at high pressure levels, was responsible for worsening environmental conditions during performing a task.
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 2013 87
This work is licensed
under a Creative
Commons Attribution-
NonCommercial 3.0
Unported License.
To review this article online, scan this
QR code with your Smartphone
Original Article
The Effect of Noise on
Human Performance: A
Clinical Trial
P Nassiri1, M Monazam1, B Fouladi Dehaghi1,
L Ibrahimi Ghavam Abadi2, SA Zakerian1, K Azam3
1Department of Occu-
pational Health, School
of Public health, Tehran
University of Medical
Sciences, Tehran, Iran
2Department of Environ-
mental Management,
Islamic Azad University,
Science and Research
Branch, Khouzestan-
Iran
3Epidemiology and Bio-
statistics Department,
School of Public Health,
Tehran University of
Medical Sciences,
Tehran, Iran
Correspondence to
Behzad Fouladi De-
haghi, PhD, Depart-
ment of Occupational
Health, School of Public
Health, Tehran Uni-
versity of Medical Sci-
ences, Tehran, Iran
E-mail: bdehaghi@
gmail.com
Received: Dec 13, 2012
Accepted: Feb 25, 2013
Cite this article as: Nassiri P, Monazam M, Fouladi Dehaghi B, et al. The effect of noise on human perfor-
mance: A clinical trial. Int J Occup Environ Med 2013;4:87-95.
Abstract
Background: Noise is dened as unwanted or meaningless sound that apart from auditory
adverse health effects may distract attention from cues that are important for task perfor-
mance. Human performance is inuenced by many job-related factors and workplace condi-
tions including noise level.
Objective: To study the effect of noise on human performance.
Methods: The participants included 40 healthy male university students. The experimental
design consisted of 3 (sound pressure level) x 3 (noise schedule) x 2 (noise type) factors. To
investigate occupational skill performance, some specic test batteries were used: 1) steadi-
ness test, 2) Minnesota manual dexterity test, 3) hand tool dexterity test, and 4) two-arm
coordination test. Time duration of test completion was measured as speed response; to
determine error response, the time taken during committing an error by participants while
performing a task was measured.
Results: Speed response obtained from the 4 tests in combined conditions of noise sched-
ule, harmonic index, and sound pressure level was highest for (intermittent, treble, 95 dB),
(continuous, treble, 95 dB), (continuous, treble, 85 dB) and (intermittent, treble, 95 dB),
respectively.
Conclusion: Treble noise was found signicant in reducing human performance; also, inter-
mittent noise, especially at high pressure levels, was responsible for worsening environmen-
tal conditions during performing a task.
Keywords: Noise; Occupational employee performance appraisal; Psychomotor perfor-
mance; Environmental exposure
Introduction
Noise—generally defined as un-
wanted and unpleasant sound—
usually disrupts the activity or bal-
ance of human life. Nowadays, with fast
growth of industrial and technological ad-
vancements, humans face noise pollution
in their working and living environments
as a major problem. This growing concern
has proved causing health hazards apart
from hearing loss in exposed people.1-6
Research has demonstrated that the pres-
ence of industrial noise has serious conse-
quences on the performance and produc-
tivity of workers.7-11 Despite this barrier in
achieving maximum productivity and ef-
ficiency, industries try hard to cope with
such problems and achieve the highest
levels of profit.12 Between the measures of
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 201388
article
Noise and Human Performance
job performance, worker productivity is
an important one and severe adverse en-
vironmental conditions can restrict work-
er productivity to a large extent.12-16
Human performance is under the influ-
ence of many factors such as job type, job
complexity level, job context and working
conditions such as heat, light, humidity,
dust, pollution and noise.12,15
Constant exposure to these environ-
mental stressors in the workplace can
lead to adverse effects on human perfor-
mance as they would cause problems in
concentration, adverse health outcomes,
etc,11,12,15 which all affect workers' health,
comfort and performance. The latter is of
major concern as it affects productivity
and efficiency of the worker, hence lead-
ing to a loss of profit for any company or
industry.17
As it is stated by many studies, the ef-
fect of noise on human performance is
very complex.18-22 The effect of noise on
task performance depends on various fac-
tors such as external environment, task
complexity and other external stress-
ors.18,21 On the effect of noise on worker
performance, there are controversial
views between researchers.18,22-24 To in-
vestigate occupational skill performance,
some specific test batteries are used: 1)
steadiness test, 2) Minnesota manual
dexterity test, 3) hand tool dexterity test,
and 4) two-arm coordination test. This
battery of tests is basically used to test
the rehabilitation and return to work ca-
pabilities of humans and helps in testing
applicants for assembly work and various
other jobs.25 They cover the hand-eye co-
ordination, steadiness, mechanical skills,
arm dexterity, two-arm coordination, and
motor skills.
Our study was based on the investi-
gation of the effect of various factors of
industrial noise on participants' perfor-
mance and efficiency with the means of
these occupational skill assessment tests.
In other words, the objective of the pres-
ent study was to determine: 1) the effect
of noise harmonic index on human per-
formance, 2) the effect of noise pressure
level on human performance, 3) the effect
of noise schedule on human performance,
and 4) to reveal the interaction of com-
bined effects of the variables.
Materials and Methods
Study population
This study included 40 healthy male uni-
versity students. Ethical aspects were
considered. All the participants received
the information regarding the objective
of the study and enrolled in the study as
volunteers. Furthermore, they were free
to leave the experiments whenever de-
manded. The mean±SD age of partici-
pants was 24.5±3 years. Self-reporting
general health status questionnaire was
completed by all participants. They were
also trained so that they became familiar
with the equipment they had to work with.
Before the participants were asked to take
part in the experiments, their hearing and
vision were checked.
Experimental design
The experimental design consisted of
thee independent factors—3 (sound
pressure level) × 3 (noise schedule) × 2
(noise harmonic index) factors. The in-
dependent variables considered for the
experiments were continuous, fluctuat-
ing and intermittent noise, three differ-
TAKE-HOME MESSAGE
Intermittent noise, especially at high pressure levels, re-
duces human performance significantly.
Treble noise is more important in reducing human perfor-
mance.
For more information
on the effect of noise
on children education
see
www.theijoem.com/
ijoem/index.php/ijoem/
article/view/185
For more information
on noise-induced ves-
tibular symptoms see
www.theijoem.com/
ijoem/index.php/ijoem/
article/view/124
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 2013 89
B. Fouladi Dehaghi, P. Nassiri, et al
ent levels of noise (75, 85 and 95 dBA);
and two noise harmonic indices (posi-
tive and negative). The two internation-
ally standardized weighting networks for
different frequencies in common use are
the “A” and “C,” which have been built
to correlate to the frequency response of
the human ear for different sound levels
in different frequencies. The “A” network
modifies the frequency response to fol-
low approximately the equal loudness
curve of 40 phones, while the “C” network
approximately follows the equal loud-
ness curve of 100 phones, respectively.26
Thus, noise harmonic index is defined as
the difference between these two weight-
ing networks, i.e., dB(C) – dB(A). When
the result of this equation is positive, the
noise is in “bass” form; when it is nega-
tive, the noise is called “treble.”
The dependent variables of the study
were speed and time duration of com-
mitting errors by the participants while
performing the battery of tests. The speed
was measured as the amount of time tak-
en to perform the test.
Battery of tests
Steadiness test
In this test, the participant is asked to
hold a metal tipped stylus in various sized
holes. The participant is not supposed to
touch the sides of the holes. The steadi-
ness test is mainly used to test the psy-
chomotor phenomenon of steadiness in
various participants. The holes are pro-
gressively reduced in size and the partici-
pant is asked to insert the stylus inside
the holes first from the largest hole to
the smallest hole size and then from the
smallest hole to the largest hole size. The
speed response is the amount of time that
the participant takes to insert the stylus
in the holes and it is measured in seconds
(test completion time). The error time is
measured in terms of duration of time
for committing errors by the participants
while performing the experiment.
Minnesota manual dexterity test
In this experiment, the participant is test-
ed for hand-arm coordination and hand-
eye coordination. The participant's task is
to begin from the right side, pick up the
bottom disk and place it on top of the sec-
ond board. Next, the participant is sup-
posed to pick up the next disk on the right
and place it in the next hole of the second
board. The participant is supposed to
move from right to left and continue from
one column to the other until the entire
board is filled. The response that is re-
corded is speed. Speed is recorded as the
time taken by the participant to perform
the entire experiment.
Two-arm coordination test
The participants' task is to trace the star
pattern in clockwise as well as counter-
clockwise direction. The black star pat-
tern can be traced by spreading the han-
dles, bringing them together, or moving
them left or right so that the stylus traced
the star pattern. The participant is sup-
posed to maintain the stylus on the black
area of the star; if the stylus left the black
star pattern, an error time is recorded by
the recorder attached to the equipment.
The responses measured during this test
are participants' speed, which is the time
taken by him or her to perform the experi-
ment, and error time.
Hand tool dexterity test
The participant's task is to remove all the
bolts starting from left to right and from
top to bottom, completing each row at a
time. Then the participant had to mount
the bolts starting from right side and from
the bottom row moving up to the top. All
of the bolt heads are supposed to be facing
the inside and the bolts had to be removed
and mounted on the wooden frame with
the help of various tools which are pro-
article
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 201390
article
vided. In this experiment, the response
measured is the speed. Speed is calculat-
ed as the time taken by the participant to
remove the bolts from one upright side of
the wooden frame and mount them on the
other upright side of the frame.
Study equipment
A stopwatch was used to record the time
taken to perform the experiments. A
sound level meter (B&K model 2236)
was used for frequency analysis and to
determine sound pressure levels in test
room. Also, PULSE Multi-analyzer Sys-
tem (B&K Type 3560) was used to record
real noise in an industrial setting, and for
reproducing the recorded real noise in the
test room with the help of Goldwave soft-
ware (ver 5.1).
Test room layout
The test room consisted of a work desk
with 70 cm in height, where the experi-
mental apparatus was placed; a chair
where the participant was asked to sit
while performing the experiments, and
two speakers placed at a distance of 1.5
meters on the participant's left and right.
The test room conditions were kept at
comfort zone temperature and levels of
humidity, lighting, etc.
Data collection and statistical analyses
The effect of three different attributes of
noise on human performance was ana-
lyzed during this experimental study.
The error response was analyzed only in
the case of two-arm coordination and the
steadiness test. In the hand tool dexterity
test and Minnesota manual dexterity test,
it was difficult to quantify the errors, thus,
they were not reported. The participants
were given sufficient training and prac-
tice before conducting the actual experi-
ments. This helped to make them familiar
with the test equipment and equalizing
their skill level. The participants were
given sufficient rest after conducting each
experimental run.
For the statistical analysis of the exper-
imental data, Student's t test, Tukey test
and three-way ANOVA were used. The
data analyses were performed by SPSS®
ver 19 for Windows®.
Results
Speed response as test completion time
The speed response for the four tests un-
der the effect of three noise pressure lev-
els, two harmonic sound indices, and three
schedules of noise were measured (Table
1). The test completion time was highest
(mean±SD of 44.6±17 sec) in the steadi-
ness test for the combination of intermit-
tent noise, negative harmonic index (tre-
ble) at a pressure level of 95 dBA. For the
same test, the completion time was low-
est (mean±SD of 29.9±16.0 sec) for the
situation in which continuous noise, posi-
tive harmonic index (bass), and 75 dBA
noise pressure level were used. The speed
variable had a normal distribution (Kol-
mogorov-Smirnov test). ANOVA revealed
that the speed response was only affected
by noise harmonic index. To determine
the kind of effective harmonic index, Stu-
dent's t test was used that revealed that a
negative harmonic index was responsible
for reducing the test performance speed
in steadiness test (p<0.001).
In Minnesota manual dexterity test
results (Table 1), the test completion
time was highest (mean±SD of 32.2±14.0
sec) for the combination of continuous
noise, negative harmonic index (treble)
and a pressure level of 95 dBA. It was
lowest (mean±SD of 21.3±7.7 sec) when
continuous noise, a positive harmonic
index (bass), and a pressure level of 75
dBA were combined. ANOVA revealed
that the speed response in this test was
dependent on both the noise schedule
Noise and Human Performance
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 2013 91
(p<0.025) and harmonic index (p<0.041)
(the negative harmonic index [treble] was
the only effective one [p<0.038]). Tukey
test was performed to compare the three
noise schedules and revealed a significant
(p<0.04) difference between continuous
and intermittent noise schedules.
The results for the third test, two-arm
coordination test, showed that the test
completion time was highest (mean±SD
of 68.3±5.4 sec) for the combination of
continuous noise, negative harmonic in-
dex (treble) and pressure level of 85 dBA.
It was lowest (mean±SD of 61.9±7.0 sec)
when fluctuating noise, a positive har-
monic index (bass) and a pressure level of
75 dBA were combined. The noise pres-
sure level and harmonic index affected the
speed response significantly (p<0.023).
There was also a significant (p<0.04) dif-
ference between pressure level of 75 dBA
and other noise intensities used.
Speed responses (test completion time)
for the last test, hand tool dexterity test,
was highest (mean±SD of 436.0±106.1
sec) for the combination of intermittent
noise, negative harmonic index (treble)
and a pressure level of 95 dBA. It was
lowest (mean±SD of 244.7±51.6 sec)
when continuous noise, positive harmon-
ic index (bass) and a pressure level of 75
dBA were used. All the three independent
variables—noise schedule, noise pressure
level, and harmonic index—affected the
speed response significantly (p<0.005).
Negative harmonic index was associated
with reduction in the performance speed.
There was a significant (p<0.003) differ-
ence between the pressure level of 75 dBA
and 85dBA. The effect of continuous noise
was significantly (p<0.001) different from
that of other noise schedules used.
Error response (time duration of
committing errors)
The error response was analyzed only in
the case of two-arm coordination and the
Table 1:
Speed response (test completion time) in various
test conditions
Noise
Schedule
Noise pres-
sure level
dB(A)
Harmonic
index
Mean±SD
speed
(sec)
Test
Continuous 75 Positive*
29.9±16.0
S
21.3±7.7
M
66.1±7.1
H
244.7±51.6
T
Continuous 75 Negative
36.8±12.9
S
28.7±12.0 M
67.5±6.5
H
314.3±67.8
T
Fluctuating 75 Positive
38.4±16.0
S
22.1±8.8
M
61.9±7.0
H
343.4±55.3
T
Fluctuating 75 Negative
38.7±14.3
S
23.9±10.3
M
64.1±6.6
H
384.1±41.4
T
Intermittent 75 Positive
31.5±15.1
S
30.8±13.4
M
62.8±6.5
H
337.2±91.1
T
Intermittent 75 Negative
38.7±14.4
S
23.5±8.8
M
65.2±5.4
H
391.0±88.9
T
Continuous 85 Positive
34.6±18.4
S
21.4±7.5
M
65.7±7.1
H
323.4±66.4
T
Continuous 85 Negative
39.9±19.2
S
22.8±6.6
M
68.3±5.4
H
362.8±102.5
T
Fluctuating 85 Positive
36.5±20.7
S
20.8±8.0
M
62.2±7.3
H
412.8±86.1
T
Fluctuating 85 Negative
42.6±19.1 S
21.9±10.2
M
66.2±9.0
H
432.0±93.3 T
B. Fouladi Dehaghi, P. Nassiri, et al
article
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 201392
article
steadiness test with the help of an error
time recorder that recorded the time du-
ration of errors committed by the partici-
pant during the experiment.
The error response for the steadiness
test and two-arm coordination test un-
der the effect of three levels of noise, two
harmonic noise indices and three sched-
ules of noise were measured (Table 2). In
steadiness test, the error time duration
was highest (mean±SD of 6.3±3.7 sec)
for the combination of continuous noise,
a negative harmonic index (treble), and
a pressure level of 85 dBA. It was lowest
(mean±SD of 3.8±1.6 sec) when inter-
mittent noise, a positive harmonic index
(bass), and a 75 dBA noise pressure level
were used. The error response was only
affected by noise harmonic index. The
negative harmonic index (treble) was the
main cause of increasing error in perfor-
mance (p<0.001).
In two-arm coordination test, the error
time was highest (mean±SD of 1.4±1.8
sec) when a continuous noise, a negative
harmonic index (treble), and a pressure
level of 85 dBA were used. The error time
was lowest (mean±SD of 0.2±0.2 sec)
when a continuous noise, a positive har-
monic index (bass), and a 75 dBA noise
pressure level were combined. None of
the variables had a significant effect on
the error response.
Discussion
We found that speed responses (time
spent for completion of a task) for all four
occupational skill assessment tests were
mainly affected by the harmonic index.
Of all the three studied factors, a nega-
tive harmonic index was found to be the
most significant factor that affected the
speed performance of participants while
performing the four occupational skill as-
sessment tests.
The interaction between the three
Continued
Table 1: Speed response (test completion time) in various
test conditions
Noise
Schedule
Noise pres-
sure level
dB(A)
Harmonic
index
Mean±SD
speed
(sec)
Test
Intermittent 85 Positive
32.5±17.3 S
22.1±6.1 M
64.0±9.5
H
392.3±69.3
T
Intermittent 85 Negative
42.6±18.8 S
22.4±10.4 M
62.2±9.0 H
409.7±75.7 T
Continuous 95 Positive
32.3±16.3
S
22.5±11.1 M
65.8±9.1
H
277.5±61.2 T
Continuous 95 Negative
35.3±16.3 S
32.2±14.0
M
67.7±7.0 H
279.9±61.6 T
Fluctuating 95 Positive
34.8±19.9
S
22.9±13.7 M
62.3±6.8 H
395.1±79.7
T
Fluctuating 95 Negative
42.9±22.3 S
23.0±12.2 M
65.9±6.4 H
435.2±91.4 T
Intermittent 95 Positive
33.6±14.8 S
21.9±10.3 M
61.5±4.8 H
389.7±78.9
T
Intermittent 95 Negative
44.6±17.0 S
24.3±13.7 M
65.7±10.3 H
436.0±106.1 T
*Bass; Treble; S: Steadiness test; M: Minnesota manual dexter-
ity test; H: Hand tool dexterity test; T: Two-arm coordination test
Noise and Human Performance
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 2013 93
studied factors of noise was not the same
for all the tests: for steadiness test and
Minnesota manual dexterity test, there
was a significant difference between con-
tinuous and intermittent noise sched-
ules. As it is stated by many researchers,
of various noise characteristics studied,
noise schedule is of utmost importance;
intermittent noise is responsible for dis-
traction of attention away from the task
and leads to reduced performance par-
ticularly in performing complex cogni-
tive tasks. When an intermittent noise
occurs at high intensities, the performer
briefly diverts his or her attention to the
noise.23,26-27 Intermittent noise is reported
to be more disruptive than continuous
noise, however, change in the intensity
is also very important.30 Our findings
support this finding as the lowest perfor-
mances were observed with intermittent
noise in different tests. On the other hand,
in two-arm coordination test, the noise
intensity was responsible for variation in
participants' performance as it has been
stated by other researchers.11,29-31 Fluctu-
ating noise, caused no significant effect
probably because the designed tasks test-
ed were simple and this kind of noise was
something between intermittent and con-
tinuous noise. However, more research in
this area seems to be necessary.
In the hand tool dexterity test, it was
found that the speed was affected by all
the studied factors including noise har-
monic index, noise schedule, noise pres-
sure level and the interaction between
them. This could be due to the fact that
the task tested an individual for his or her
mechanic skills, and that the duration for
this task was much longer than the dura-
tion for any other tasks tested. Therefore,
participants received more exposure to
noise while they were performing this
task compared to other tasks.
In assessing the error time while per-
forming the task, we found that harmonic
Table 2: Error time in steadiness and two-arm coordination
tests
Noise
schedule
Noise pres-
sure level
dB(A)
Harmonic
index
Mean±SD
error time
(sec)
Test
Continuous 75 Positive*
4.1±1.9
0.2±0 .2 T
Continuous 75 Negative4.4±1.7 S
0.8±1.2 T
Fluctuating 75 Positive
4.2±1.8
0.6±0.9 T
Fluctuating 75 Negative
4.5±1.6 S
0.6±1.0 T
Intermittent 75 Positive
3.8±1.6 S
0.6±0.9 T
Intermittent 75 Negative 5.3±3.0 S
0.2±0.4 T
Continuous 85 Positive 4.7±2.4 S
0.3±0.4
Continuous 85 Negative 6.3±3.7 S
1.4±1.8
Fluctuating 85 Positive 4.0±2.4 S
0.3±0.5 T
Fluctuating 85 Negative
4.2±2.0 S
0.4±0.5 T
Intermittent 85 Positive 4.2±2.0 S
0.5±0.7
Intermittent 85 Negative 5.0±2.3 S
0.6±0.7
Continuous 95 Positive
4.4±2.3 S
0.3±0.3 T
Continuous 95 Negative
4.2±2.0 S
0.4±0.5 T
Fluctuating 95 Positive
4.5±2.7 S
0.8±1.3 T
Fluctuating 95 Negative 5.5±2.5 S
1.0±1.2
B. Fouladi Dehaghi, P. Nassiri, et al
article
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 201394
article
index mainly affected the error response
in steadiness test. Therefore, more dura-
tion of errors occurred due to the pres-
ence of negative harmonic index. In the
two-arm coordination test, apart from
negative harmonic index, no other signifi-
cant factors affected the error response of
human performance.
In many of the reported cases of this
study, higher noise pressure levels were
associated with more reduction in per-
formance.24 Moreover, it should be men-
tioned that in this study, real noise from
industrial settings was used in well-con-
trolled experiments. Most of previous
studies used artificially generated noises
in artificial settings with usually very
short exposure times. In an experiment,
it was shown that reducing noise levels
in a factory improved work performance
through reduction of the number of work
errors.33 According to Broadbent,33 now
classic in theoretical treatment of the ef-
fects of noise on performance, loud noise
leads to over-arousal, which narrows at-
tention and restricts one's focus to a lim-
ited range of cues. This inability to attend
to less salient cues would ultimately lead
to deterioration of performance. Our find-
ings supported these results.
Although there are a vast variety of
studies on the effects of noise on human
performance in different conditions and
designs, with of course many controver-
sial results, to the best of our knowledge,
none of them has so far focused on the
effect of noise harmonic index on hu-
man performance. We found that when
the noise harmonic index was negative
(treble noise) the performance was sig-
nificantly affected. The results of the ef-
fects of the two other noise characteristics
we studied, were in keeping with previous
reports. Further controlled studies are
needed to confirm the importance of the
noise characteristics in design of working
environments.
Acknowledgements
The authors appreciate the partici-
pants for their interest and enthusiasm
to participate in the study. We also ac-
knowledge Tehran University of Medical
Sciences and School of Public Health for
their invaluable help and support during
the research.
Conflicts of Interest: None
declared.
References
1. Fyhri A, Aasvang GM. Noise, sleep and poor
health: Modeling the relationship between road
traffic noise and cardiovascular problems. Sci Total
Environ 2010;408:4935-42.
2. Hohmann C, Grabenhenrich L, de Kluizenaar Y,
et al. Health effects of chronic noise exposure
in pregnancy and childhood: A systematic
review initiated by ENRIECO. Int J Hyg Envir Heal
2013;216:217-29.
3. Atmaca E, Peker I, Altin A. Industrial noise and
its effects on humans. Polish J Environ Studies
2005;14:721-6.
4. Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of
endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nat
Rev Neurosci 2009;10:397-409.
5. Ahmad Badayai AR. A Theoretical Framework and
Analytical Discussion on Uncongenial Physical
Workplace Environment and Job Performance
among Workers in Industrial Sectors. Procedia Soc
Behav Sci 2012;42:486-95.
6. Cohen S, Weinstein N. Nonauditory effects
Noise and Human Performance
Continued
Table 2: Error time in steadiness and two-arm coordination
tests
Noise
schedule
Noise pres-
sure level
dB(A)
Harmonic
index
Mean±SD
error time
(sec)
Test
Intermittent 95 Positive
4.6±3.4
0.3±0.3
Intermittent 95 Negative
5.9±4.3
1.3±1.7 T
*Bass; Treble; S: Steadiness test; T: Two-arm coordination test
www.theijoem.com Vol 4 Number 2; April, 2013 95
B. Fouladi Dehaghi, P. Nassiri, et al
of noise on behavoir and health. J Soc Issues
1981;37:36-70.
7. Sayli N, Khasawneh MT, Lu S, Koon T. Effect of
industrial noise on occupational skill perfor-
mance capability. UMI 1470346, ProQuest LLC
2009 Available from http://gradworks.umi.
com/14/70/1470346.html (Accessed May 23,
2012).
8. Noweir MH. Noise exposure as related to pro-
ductivity, disciplinary actions, absenteeism, and
accidents among textile workers. J Safety Res
1984;15:163-74.
9. Chiovenda P, Pasqualetti P, Zappasodi F, et al. En-
vironmental noise-exposed workers: Event related
potentials, neuropsychological and mood assess-
ment. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;65:228-37.
10. Leather P, Beale D, Sullivan L. Noise, psychosocial
stress and their interaction in the workplace. J
Environ Psychol 2003,23:213-22.
11. Melamed S, Fried Y, Froom P. The joint effect of
noise exposure and job complexity on distress and
injury risk among men and women: the cardiovas-
cular occupational risk factors determination in Is-
rael study. J Occup Environ Med 2004;46:1023-32.
12. Kahya E. The effects of job characteristics and
working conditions on job performance. Int J
Indust Ergo 2007;37:515-23.
13. Kim TG, Tochihara Y, Fujita M, Hashiguchi N. Physi-
ological responses and performance of loading
work in a severely cold environment. Int J Indust
Ergo 2007;37:725-32.
14. Meerding WJ, Ijzelenberg W, Koopmanschap
MA, et al. Health problems lead to considerable
productivity loss at work among workers with high
physical load jobs. J Clin Epidemiol 2005;58:517-
23.
15. O’Neill MJ. Measuring Workplace Performance,
2nd ed. Taylor & Francis, New York, 2007.
16. Robertson MM, Huang YH. Effect of a workplace
design and training intervention on individual
performance, group effectiveness and collabora-
tion: the role of environmental control. Work
2006;27:3-12.
17. Parsons KC. Environmental ergonomics: A review
of principles, methods and models. Appl Ergo
2000;31:581-94.
18. Suter AH. The effects of noise on performance.
Final Report. Gallaudet University Washington DC,
1989.
19. Waye KP, Bengtsson J, Kjellberg A, Benton S. Low
frequency noise «pollution» interferes with per-
formance. Noise Health 2001;4:33-49.
20. McCoy JM, Evans G. Physical work environment.
In: Barling J, Kelloway EK, eds. Handbook of Work
Stress. Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage Publication,
2005:219-45.
21. Vischer J. The effects of the physical environment
on job performance: Towards a theoretical model
of workspace model. J Stress Health 2007;23:175-
84.
22. Melamed S, Fried Y, Froom P. The interactive effect
of chronic exposure to noise and job complexity
on changes in blood pressure and job satisfaction:
A longitudinal study of industrial employees. J
Occup Health Psychol 2001;6:182-95.
23. Szalma JL, Hancock PA. Noise effects on human
performance: a meta-analytic synthesis. Am Psy-
chol Assoc 2011;137:682-707.
24. Smith DG, Baranski JV, Thompson MM, Abel SM.
The effects of background noise on cognitive per-
formance during a 70 hour simulation of condi-
tions aboard the international space station. Noise
Health 2003;6:3-16.
25. Occupational skill assessment test battery.
Available from www.lafayetteinstrument.com (Ac-
cessed March 1, 2009).
26. Bies DA, Hansen CH. Engineering Noise Control.
3rd ed. Spon Press. Taylor & Francis, 2003:100-1.
27. Conway GE, Szalma JL, Hancock PA. A quantitative
meta-analytic examination of whole-body vibra-
tion effects on human performance. Ergonomics
2007;50:228-45.
28. Hancock PA, Ross JM, Szalma JL. A meta-analysis
of performance response under thermal stressors.
Hum Factors 2007;49:851-77.
29. Loeb M. Noise and human efficiency. Chichester,
England: Wiley, 1986.
30. Cohen S, Evans GW, Stokols D, Krantz DS. Behav-
ior, health and environmental stress. New York,
London: Plenum Press, 1986.
31. Baron RA. The physical environment of work set-
tings: effects on task performance, interpersonal
relations and job satisfaction. Res Organ Behav
1994;16:1-46.
32. Kroemer KHE, Grandjean E. Fitting the task to the
human. 5th ed. A textbook of occupational ergo-
nomics. London, Bristol, PA, Taylor and Francis,
1997.
33. Broadbent DE, Little EAJ. Effects of noise reduction
in a work situation. Occup Psychol 1960;34:133-
40.
article
... Noise has many adverse outcomes on human health which can be listed as temporary and permanent hearing loss, increase in blood pressure, circulatory system disorders, acceleration in breathing, slow heart rate, behavioral disorders, excessive irritability, increased stress, decreased work efficiency and concentration problems. Temporary or permanent hearing loss occurs in people exposed to prolonged, severe noise levels [4]. In the study by Basner et al. [5] it was determined that noise mostly causes such effects as hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, sleep disturbance, anger and stress. ...
... The questionnaire form was created by the researchers by reviewing the current relevant literature [4][5][6][7][8][11][12][13][14][15]. This consists of a total of 25 questions including an introductory information form (sociodemographic and study characteristics) and a noise-related symptom chart (physical, psychological and physiological symptoms and performance effect). ...
... This questionnaire was created by researchers by examining the related literature on occupational diseases and personal ear protection of employees [4,6,7,13,15]. After creating 25 questions, the hearing health information questionnaire was prepared in accordance with the content of the training, and expert opinion was obtained from seven academics who were specialist in their fields. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives. This study aimed to determine the effect of occupational training applied to workers working in noisy parts of a glass factory on the use of ear protectors. Methods. The study was a semi-experimental pre-test and post-test design. It was carried out with 45 workers determined by purposeful sampling and working in a glass production factory who accepted to participate in the study. The data were collected with an information form and noise-related symptom chart, hearing health information questionnaire and observation. In the analysis of the study data, the number/percentage, χ 2 test, Cohen κ test and analysis of variance were used. Results. It was determined that workers pre training did not use ear protectors (100%) and there were some noise-related health complaints. There was an increase in using ear protectors (57.8%) and a decrease in their health complaints post training. It was also found that the workers' pre-training hearing health information questionnaire mean score was 11.46 ± 3.20, and post training was 13.77 ± 3.42 in the first month and 15.77 ± 2.88 in the fourth month. Conclusions. Occupational training provided to the workers increased the workers' awareness level and their rate of wearing ear protectors, and health problems caused by work-related noise started to recover.
... Other cognitive abilities were also assessed in the research on noise effects. For example, Nassiri et al. [32] reported that noise significantly affected speed response, but did not affect error response. Ke et al. [33] revealed the negative correlation between noise and the task execution of 27 subjects. ...
... Sometimes, sound can be stimulating or cause a positive mood change, which might in turn result in an increase in performance [64]. This may be because low-level noise supports a state of wakefulness and mental activation, without which people can become sleepy and inefficient [28,32]. Thus, a certain amount of discomfort, such as distracting noise, may be occasionally introduced to avoid a state of sensory deprivation [32]. ...
... This may be because low-level noise supports a state of wakefulness and mental activation, without which people can become sleepy and inefficient [28,32]. Thus, a certain amount of discomfort, such as distracting noise, may be occasionally introduced to avoid a state of sensory deprivation [32]. The inclusion of a moderate degree of environmental stress was even viewed by Craig [65] as a basic engineering principle. ...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals exposed to elevated noise levels experience heightened emotional intensity, leading to increased cognitive disruption and a higher likelihood of accidents. This study seeks to investigate the impact of noise exposure on human cognitive performance, and the moderating role of emotion. Twelve healthy male college-age students underwent exposure to three noise conditions, each characterized by different sound pressure levels and sharpness. Each condition included an initial acoustic/thermal adaption period lasting approximately 40 min, followed by intermittent questionnaire tests and a battery of computerized cognitive tests. Statistical analysis revealed that reducing noise levels proved advantageous, enhancing perceived sound quality, positive emotions, and auditory perception abilities, while concurrently reducing false alerts and accelerating execution speed. Many of these effects were found to be counteracted by elevated sharpness. Correlation analyses and partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) results suggested that human emotions mediate the relationship between noise exposure and cognitive performance. The potential underlying mechanism suggests that negative feelings towards noise contribute to poor emotional states, subsequently influencing cognitive processes and impairing executive function. The outcomes of this study provide valuable insights into the mechanism of noise exposure and its effects on human cognition and subjective perceptions.
... According to Nassiri,et al, (2013) Noise as unwanted or meaningless sound that apart from auditory, adverse health effect and may distract attention from cues that are important for task performance. The excess sound makes employees to perform their tasks ineffectively. ...
... Moreover, due to higher noise levels in the workshops, increased in human errors results in the increase in work-related accidents and finally leads to a decrease in productivity (Nassiri,et al, 2013). Main goal of a safety at the workplace is to improve occupational safety by reducing frequency of occupational accidents and preventing high severity events and to create conducive work environment. ...
... Generally, 66% of the respondents agreed that noise in AMSMI has impact on their productivity, 19% neutral and 15% disagree that impact of noise on their productivity is not sensible and has no great impact on their performance. Moreover, due to higher noise levels in the workshops, increased in human errors results in the increase in work-related accidents and finally leads to a decrease in productivity (Nassiri,et al, 2013). The same case in the industry there is no device that records the sound level of the machines and another sound that exists in the industry. ...
... The effect of noise on mental workload and worker performance has been proven in several studies [1] [2]. Nassiry et al. proved that intermittent sound with high intensity could significantly reduce worker performance [3], while other studies conducted by Eakins show that sound affects the mental workload of workers [4]. ...
... Additionally, auditory characteristics of noise, such as level, tone, frequency, impulsivity, and exposure time, have been investigated [4]. In general, although noise level is a critical factor and studies point to an inversely correlated link between performance and noise levels [5], moderate noise is often overlooked [6]. ...
Article
Background Elevated ambient noise levels during trauma resuscitation can disrupt team member communication, yet limited research has examined interventions to mitigate these noise levels. Objective This study aims to measure the effect of trauma bay curtains on noise levels and the number of staff present during trauma resuscitations. Methods This single-center prospective randomized study was conducted at a U.S. Western Pacific State Level I trauma center from April 2019 through September 2023. The population inclusion was adult trauma patients meeting alert or activation criteria. The designated trauma bay secondary outer curtain was closed at the end of 2 min in the intervention group and at the end of 4 min in the control group. A t -test was used to compare control and intervention groups. The outcome variables were decibel levels, and the number of people in the designated area was recorded at the end of each minute for 6 min. Results A total of n = 45 participants were included in this study. The control group ( n = 19) had a mean of 9.89 decibels (SD = 8.42) compared to the intervention group ( n = 24) with a mean of −1.50 decibels (SD = 5.29). These two groups had a statistically significant difference in average decibels and the total number of people between the first and fourth minute. Conclusions We found that visual interruption using a secondary curtain effectively reduced the number of people and noise levels in the periphery during resuscitation.
Article
Full-text available
A pervasive environmental stressor is one that damages mental and physical health as well as cognitive abilities by producing noise at a specific frequency and level. Current noise pollution levels pose a significant threat to public health, potentially leading to impaired cognitive function, increased stress, and other negative health consequences. This study aims to investigate the relationship between noise exposure and human cognitive abilities using a comprehensive analysis of power spectrum density (PSD) derived from EEG signals. Twenty-four participants completed the experiment to identify the effect of exposure to different noise levels (55 dB, 65 dB, 70 dB, 75 dB, 80 dB, and 85 dB) and two types of continuous and intermittent noise. The Stroop Color–Word Test and the Emotive Epoch EEG are cognitive task instruments used during experiments. Behavioral performance (accuracy and response time) and power spectrum electroencephalographic density were collected and analyzed. The methodology involved collecting EEG data from participants exposed to controlled noise stimuli and a subsequent PSD analysis to uncover frequency-specific patterns associated with cognitive processes. Attention levels were measured by examining beta wave activity, while stress responses were evaluated through an alpha wave analysis. Additionally, mental workload was assessed by considering the overall distribution of PSD through the theta-to-alpha ratio. The results revealed a significant relationship between the exposure to noise types and levels and human cognitive ability. The analysis of the power spectrum density on the cognitive aspects of attention and stress yielded results indicating that participants were in the best attention condition and in a relaxed or unstressed state when exposed to noise levels of 65 dB in both continuous and intermittent noise types. For the mental workload aspect, participants exposed to both continuous and intermittent noise types at a noise level of 70 dB began to indicate the presence of mental workload. These findings supported the importance of considering the impact of environmental noise on human cognitive well-being and demonstrated the potential of EEG monitoring as an objective tool for assessing the impact of noise on cognitive performance.
Chapter
The in-flight environment is a potentially hostile one that exposes patients and medical transport teams to a number of significant physiological changes and environmental stressors. It is important to understand the implications of these on the human body, so as to develop strategies to minimize their potential for related adverse clinical consequences during flight. This chapter reviews the physics of the atmosphere with a particular emphasis on the behavior of gases. The effects of altitude on the human body are considered demonstrating the clinical significance of Boyle’s, Dalton’s, and Henry’s Law. The impact of changes in humidity, ambient temperature, and exposure to G forces is discussed. The potential effects of exposure to noise and vibration are reviewed, with special consideration to how these can impair human performance. Finally, there is a brief discussion on how cabin atmosphere may impact on the spread of respiratory pathogens.KeywordsPathophysiologyFlightPhysicsAltitude
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this research is to explore the theoretical framework of the physical workplace environment and its effects on job performance. Early researchers have classified five factors of the workplace environment that can affect job performances: sound, temperature, air, light and colour, and space. These findings though largely still contradictory; still play a major role in the determinants of workers' job performance. Therefore, a recommendation of a better and congenial physical environmental design has been suggested by describing a new kind of work centre, acoustic privacy and general kind of work setting.
Article
Full-text available
The problem of noise in the industries around Sivas has been examined in this study; and noise measurement and survey studies have been carried out at concrete traverse, cement, iron and steel and textile factories located in this region. A questionnaire was completed by 256 workers during this study in order to determine the physical, physiological, and psycho-social impacts of the noise on humans and to specify what kind of measurements have been taken both by the employers and workers for protection from the effects of noise. It has been specified, during the surveys, that the noise levels detected in all the industries are much above the 80 dBA that is specified in the regulations: 73.83% of the workers in these industries are disturbed from the noise in their workplaces, 60.96% of them have complaints about their nervous situations, 30.96% of these workers are suffering hearing problems although they had not had any periodical hearing tests and they are not using ear protection equipment.
Chapter
Full-text available
Only 2 chapters are available as full-text: 1. Stress Processes and the Cost of Coping. 2. Correlational Field Methodology in the Study of Stress
Article
Scitation is the online home of leading journals and conference proceedings from AIP Publishing and AIP Member Societies
Article
Noise and Human Efficiency is the sixth volume in this Wiley series on "Studies in Human Performance". It is an account of the effects of noise on human performance. After delineating the physics of sound and noise, and presenting the essentials on the physiology of the ear with its associated brain mechanisms, the book explains the consequences of noise for auditory perception and the comprehension of speech. It also describes the after effects of persistent noise, in the form of various kinds of temporary or permanent deafness. With respect to performance of non-auditory tasks, the book reviews the experiments on annoyance and social disruption, in addition to those on various industrial and laboratory tasks. It brings the record up to date by showing that the effects of noise are not always detrimental. Noise has complex consequences, interacting with such factors as arousal, time-of-day, sex and personality. Although the appropriate psychological theories are reviewed, the author's main aim has been to pick his way through the experimental and methodological complexities in such a way as to show exactly what has been firmly established.