ArticlePDF Available

NASA Faked the Moon Landing-Therefore, (Climate) Science Is a Hoax: An Anatomy of the Motivated Rejection of Science

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Although nearly all domain experts agree that carbon dioxide emissions are altering the world's climate, segments of the public remain unconvinced by the scientific evidence. Internet blogs have become a platform for denial of climate change, and bloggers have taken a prominent role in questioning climate science. We report a survey of climate-blog visitors to identify the variables underlying acceptance and rejection of climate science. Our findings parallel those of previous work and show that endorsement of free-market economics predicted rejection of climate science. Endorsement of free markets also predicted the rejection of other established scientific findings, such as the facts that HIV causes AIDS and that smoking causes lung cancer. We additionally show that, above and beyond endorsement of free markets, endorsement of a cluster of conspiracy theories (e.g., that the Federal Bureau of Investigation killed Martin Luther King, Jr.) predicted rejection of climate science as well as other scientific findings. Our results provide empirical support for previous suggestions that conspiratorial thinking contributes to the rejection of science. Acceptance of science, by contrast, was strongly associated with the perception of a consensus among scientists.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Science
24(5) 622 –633
© The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797612457686
pss.sagepub.com
Research Article
More than 90% of climate scientists agree that the global
climate is changing, largely because of carbon dioxide
emissions resulting from human activity (Anderegg, Prall,
Harold, & Schneider, 2010; Doran & Zimmerman, 2009).1
There are indications that the 2007 assessment of the
Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) was
conservative rather than “alarmist” (Allison et al., 2009;
Freudenburg & Muselli, 2010); however, the scientific
indicators of increasing actual risks are accompanied by
an apparent decrease in the public’s perception of those
risks in some countries (Brulle, Carmichael, & Jenkins,
2012; Hanson, 2009; Scruggs & Benegal, 2012).
The reasons for this declining public concern are man-
ifold. Researchers in history and sociology frequently cite
the “manufacture of doubt” by vested interests and politi-
cal groups as a factor (Jacques, Dunlap, & Freeman,
2008; McCright & Dunlap, 2003, 2010; Mooney, 2007;
Oreskes & Conway, 2010; Stocking & Holstein, 2009). For
example, more than 90% of books endorsing skepticism
toward environmentalism that have been published since
1972 have been sponsored by conservative think tanks
(Jacques et al., 2008). Oreskes and Conway (2010) ana-
lyzed the shared ideological underpinnings of organized
attempts over the past few decades to discredit well-
established scientific findings, such as the link between
smoking and lung cancer, the causal role of chlorofluoro-
carbons (CFCs) in eroding the ozone layer, and, most
recently, the findings of climate change. Oreskes and
Conway documented that a small number of organiza-
tions and individuals have been instrumental in those
457686PSSXXX10.1177/0956797612457686Lewandowsky et al.Motivated Climate Denial
research-article2013
Corresponding Author:
Stephan Lewandowsky, School of Psychology, University of Western
Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, Western Australia 6009,
Australia
E-mail: stephan.lewandowsky@uwa.edu.au
NASA Faked the Moon Landing—Therefore,
(Climate) Science Is a Hoax: An Anatomy
of the Motivated Rejection of Science
Stephan Lewandowsky1, Klaus Oberauer1,2, and
Gilles E. Gignac1
1University of Western Australia and 2University of Zurich
Abstract
Although nearly all domain experts agree that carbon dioxide emissions are altering the world’s climate, segments of
the public remain unconvinced by the scientific evidence. Internet blogs have become a platform for denial of climate
change, and bloggers have taken a prominent role in questioning climate science. We report a survey of climate-
blog visitors to identify the variables underlying acceptance and rejection of climate science. Our findings parallel
those of previous work and show that endorsement of free-market economics predicted rejection of climate science.
Endorsement of free markets also predicted the rejection of other established scientific findings, such as the facts that
HIV causes AIDS and that smoking causes lung cancer. We additionally show that, above and beyond endorsement of
free markets, endorsement of a cluster of conspiracy theories (e.g., that the Federal Bureau of Investigation killed Martin
Luther King, Jr.) predicted rejection of climate science as well as other scientific findings. Our results provide empirical
support for previous suggestions that conspiratorial thinking contributes to the rejection of science. Acceptance of
science, by contrast, was strongly associated with the perception of a consensus among scientists.
Keywords
scientific communication, policymaking, climate science
Received 5/22/12; Revision accepted 7/7/12
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 623
contrarian activities, arguably motivated by a laissez-faire
free-market ideology that views as threatening any scien-
tific finding with potential regulatory impact, such as
interference with the marketing of tobacco products,
bans on CFCs, or a price on carbon emissions (cf. Dunlap
& McCright, 2011).
These historical analyses complement empirical results
showing that people’s rejection of climate science is asso-
ciated with an embrace of laissez-faire free-market eco-
nomics (Heath & Gifford, 2006; Kahan, 2010). There is
little doubt that people’s personal ideology—also often
referred to as worldview or cultural cognition—is a major
predictor of the rejection of climate science (Dunlap &
McCright, 2008; Feygina, Jost, & Goldsmith, 2010;
Hamilton, 2011; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Kahan, 2010;
Kahan, Jenkins-Smith, & Braman, 2011; McCright &
Dunlap, 2011a, 2011b).
In the study reported here, we investigated predictors
of the rejection of climate science and investigated
whether they generalize across content domains. We
define the rejection of science as the dismissal of well-
established scientific results for reasons that are not sci-
entifically grounded (Diethelm & McKee, 2009; Jacques,
2012; McKee & Diethelm, 2010).2 Those reasons may
comprise the psychological factors that are of interest
here, but they may also include indecision arising from
inaccurate or misleading media coverage—for example,
the scientific consensus on climate change is often mis-
represented in the media (e.g., Boykoff, 2007). Rejection
of science must be distinguished from true skepticism,
which may prompt the revision of a scientific claim on
the basis of evidence and reasoned theorizing. Skepticism
not only is at the core of scientific reasoning, but also has
been shown to improve people’s discrimination between
true and false information (e.g., Lewandowsky, Stritzke,
Oberauer, & Morales, 2005, 2009).
In addition to a worldview that endorses free-market
economics, another variable that has been associated
with the rejection of science is conspiratorial thinking, or
conspiracist ideation, defined here as the attempt to
explain a significant political or social event as a secret
plot by powerful individuals or organizations (Sunstein &
Vermeule, 2009). The presumed conspirators are typically
perceived as virtually omnipotent (Bale, 2007). Thus,
internal documents of the tobacco industry referred to
scientists doing medical research on the health effects of
smoking as a “vertically integrated, highly concentrated,
oligopolistic cartel” that—in combination with “public
monopolies”—“manufactures alleged evidence, sugges-
tive inferences linking smoking to various diseases, and
publicity and dissemination and advertising of these so-
called findings” (Abt, 1983, p. 126). Likewise, rejection of
the link between HIV and AIDS has been associated with
the conspiratorial belief that HIV was created by the U.S.
government to eradicate Black people (e.g., Bogart &
Thorburn, 2005; Kalichman, Eaton, & Cherry, 2010).
Rejection of climate science has also long been infused
with notions of a conspiracy among scientists. As early as
1996, accusations of corruption in the IPCC were aired in
the Wall Street Journal (Lahsen, 1999; Oreskes & Conway,
2010). More recently, a book by a U.S. senator is called
The Greatest Hoax: How the Global Warming Conspiracy
Threatens Your Future (Inhofe, 2012).
The prominence of conspiracist ideation in people
who espouse science denial is not entirely surprising
because if an overwhelming scientific consensus cannot
be accepted as the result of researchers independently
converging on the same evidence-based view, then the
very existence of the consensus calls for an alternative
explanation. The ideation of a complex and secretive
conspiracy among researchers can provide that explana-
tion (Diethelm & McKee, 2009; McKee & Diethelm, 2010).
However, there is no empirical evidence about how
widespread such ideations are among people who reject
scientific evidence, in particular as it relates to climate
change. Moreover, to date, analyses of conspiracist ide-
ation in the rejection of science have exclusively focused
on conspiracy theories pertaining to the scientific issue
under consideration: Thus, denial of HIV’s connection
with AIDS has been linked to the belief that the U.S. gov-
ernment created HIV (Kalichman, 2009), members of the
tobacco industry viewed lung-cancer researchers as an
“oligopolistic cartel” (Abt, 1983, p. 127), and climate
deniers believe that communists, socialists, and a “global
elite” have manufactured global warming as the “biggest
scam in history” (Sussman, 2010, p. 215). In all these
cases, the conspiracy theory serves to explain away over-
whelming scientific evidence. Thus, the conspiracist ide-
ation may be an accoutrement of the denial of an
inconvenient scientific fact, rather than an independent
and potentially stable psychological variable that is asso-
ciated with the rejection of science more generally.
It is known that people’s propensity for conspiracist
ideation is not limited to one theory in isolation. Instead,
the belief that AIDS was created by the government is
likely accompanied by the conviction that the Federal
Bureau of Investigation (FBI) killed Martin Luther King,
Jr., or that the Air Force is hiding evidence of extrater-
restrial visitors (Goertzel, 1994; Swami, Chamorro-
Premuzic, & Furnham, 2009). Douglas and Sutton (2011),
in a study supporting the notion of a general disposition
toward conspiracist ideation, showed that people’s
endorsement of conspiracy theories was associated with
their willingness to engage in a conspiracy themselves
when they deemed it necessary. It is therefore possible
that this disposition predicts the rejection of science inde-
pendently of the scientific domain in question: If con-
spiracist ideation reflects a stable personality or cognitive
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
624 Lewandowsky et al.
characteristic, then people who believe that NASA faked
the moon landing may be likely to reject a range of other
scientific propositions, from medical facts on HIV-AIDS
to consensus on climate change.
Another attribute common to people who reject sci-
ence is reliance on the Internet (Diethelm & McKee,
2009; McKee & Diethelm, 2010). By definition, the peer-
reviewed literature does not promote denial; for exam-
ple, questioning the link between HIV and AIDS lost
intellectual respectability decades ago (Nattrass, 2010,
2011), and there are few peer-reviewed contrarian cli-
mate publications (Anderegg et al., 2010). The Internet,
by contrast, provides the opportunity for individuals who
reject a scientific consensus to feed “each other’s feelings
of persecution by a corrupt elite” (McKee & Diethelm,
2010, pp. 1310–1311). Accordingly, climate-“skeptic” blogs
have become a major staging post for denial, although
blogs are also used by supporters of climate science to
disseminate scientific evidence. The influence of blogs
should not be underestimated: For example, one skeptic
blogger (Steven McIntyre of the “Climate Audit” blog, at
climateaudit.org) has triggered several congressional
investigations, and one anonymous proscience blogger
(“Deep Climate”) uncovered a plagiarism scandal involv-
ing a report skeptical of climate change for Congress,
which ultimately led to the retraction of a peer-reviewed
article. Popular climate blogs can register upward of
700,000 monthly visitors, a self-selected audience that is
by definition highly engaged in the increasingly polar-
ized climate debate.
Climate-blog denizens therefore present a highly rele-
vant population for the study of variables underlying
endorsement or rejection of the scientific consensus on
climate. We surveyed blog denizens on (a) their views
on climate science and a range of other scientific proposi-
tions; (b) two constructs that we hypothesized to be
associated with rejection of science (endorsement of free-
market ideology and agreement with a range of conspir-
acy theories); (c) a construct targeting sensitivity to
environmental problems (e.g., whether previous concerns
about acid rain have been addressed); and (d) the per-
ceived consensus among scientists, which has been
repeatedly linked to acceptance of science (Ding, Maibach,
Zhao, Roser-Renouf, & Leiserowitz, 2011; Dunlap &
McCright, 2008; Kahan et al., 2011; Lewandowsky, Gignac,
& Vaughan, 2012).
Method
Participants
Visitors to climate blogs voluntarily completed an online
questionnaire between August and October 2010 (N =
1,377). Links were posted on eight blogs that have a pro-
science stance but a diverse audience (see the
Supplemental Material for more on audience composi-
tion); an additional five “skeptic” (or “skeptic”-leaning)
blogs were approached, but none posted the link.
Questionnaire
Table 1 lists items retained for analysis together with their
abbreviated variable names.3 The free-market items were
taken from Heath and Gifford (2006). Most of the con-
spiracy items were adapted from previous research (e.g.,
Swami et al., 2009). The conspiracies covered the politi-
cal spectrum, with fears of a “world government” being
most pronounced on the political right and a theory that
September 11 was an “inside job” being prevalent on the
left (Nyhan, 2010). The remaining items were designed
for this study. The table shows the ordering of items into
groups for one version of the questionnaire; in another
version, the conspiracist items were positioned between
the items querying free-market ideology and those focus-
ing on acceptance of climate science. The two versions
were arbitrarily assigned to blogs.
Results
Following standard recommendations (Gosling, Vazire,
Srivastava, & John, 2004), whenever more than one
response was submitted from the same Internet Protocol
address, we eliminated all those responses (n = 71). An
additional 161 responses were eliminated because the
respondent’s age was implausible (< 10 or > 95 years
old), values for the consensus items were outside the
range of the rating scale, or responses were incomplete.
This left 1,145 complete records for analysis. Items were
reverse-scored when necessary, such that larger scores
pointed to greater endorsement of the underlying con-
struct. Raw correlation matrices and summary statistics
are reported in the Supplemental Material available
online (Tables S3 and S4).
Analyses focused on the relations among the con-
structs of greatest interest: free-market ideology, accep-
tance of climate science and of other sciences, perceived
consensus among scientists, conspiracist ideation, and
the belief that earlier environmental problems have been
resolved. The overarching analysis used a structural
equation model (SEM), with the data preprocessed as
follows.
Separate exploratory factor analyses were conducted
for the free-market, climate-change, and conspiracist-
ideation items. For free-market items, a single factor com-
prising five items accounted for 56.5% of the variance;
the remaining item (FMNotEnvQual) loaded on a second
factor (17.7% of the variance) by itself and was therefore
eliminated. The four climate-change items (excluding
CauseCO2) loaded on a common factor that explained
87% of the variance; all were retained. For conspiracist
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 625
ideation, two factors were identified; the 2 items involv-
ing space aliens (CYArea51 and CYRoswell) loaded on
the second factor, which accounted for 9.6% of the vari-
ance, and the remaining 10 loaded on the first one, which
accounted for 42.0% of the variance (CYAIDS and
CYClimChange were not considered for the reasons
stated in Table 1). Responses to items loading on each
conspiracist-ideation factor were summed, so that two
composite manifest variables were created. The two com-
posites thus estimated a conspiracist-ideation construct
without any conceptual relation to the scientific issues
under investigation.
Table 1.Questionnaire Items, Variable Names, and Factor Loadings
Variable name Item Loadinga
Free-market ideology
FMUnresBest An economic system based on free markets unrestrained
by government interference automatically works best
to meet human needs.
.802
FMNotEnvQual I support the free-market system but not at the expense
of environmental quality. (R)
(omitted from analysis)
FMLimitSocial The free-market system may be efficient for resource
allocation, but it is limited in its capacity to promote
social justice. (R)
.624
FMMoreImp The preservation of the free-market system is more
important than localized environmental concerns.
.827
FMThreatEnv Free and unregulated markets pose important threats to
sustainable development. (R)
.887
FMUnsustain The free-market system is likely to promote
unsustainable consumption. (R)
.892
Acceptance of climate science
CO2TempUp I believe that burning fossil fuels increases atmospheric
temperature to some measurable degree.
.941
CO2AtmosUp I believe that the burning of fossil fuels on the scale
observed over the last 50 years has increased
atmospheric temperature to an appreciable degree.
.969
CO2WillNegChange I believe that the burning of fossil fuels on the scale
observed over the last 50 years will cause serious
negative changes to the planet’s climate unless there
is a substantial switch to non-CO2-emitting energy
sources.
.982
CO2HasNegChange I believe that the burning of fossil fuels on the scale
observed over the last 50 years has caused serious
negative changes to the planet’s climate.
.921
Perception that problems have been resolved
CFCNowOK The problem of chlorofluorocarbons (CFCs) is no longer
a serious threat to the ozone layer.
.801
AcidRainNowOK The problem of acid rain is no longer a serious threat to
the global ecosystem.
.927
Conspiracist ideationb
CYNewWorldOrder A powerful and secretive group known as the New
World Order is planning to eventually rule the world
through an autonomous world government that
would replace sovereign governments.
.742
CYSARS SARS (Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome) was
produced under laboratory conditions as a biological
weapon.
.742
CYPearlHarbor The U.S. government had foreknowledge about the
Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor but allowed the
attack to take place so as to be able to enter the
Second World War.
.742
(Continued)
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
626 Lewandowsky et al.
Variable name Item Loadinga
CYMLK The assassination of Martin Luther King Jr. was the
result of an organized conspiracy by U.S. government
agencies such as the FBI and CIA.
.742
CYMoon The Apollo moon landings never happened and were
staged in a Hollywood film studio.
.742
CYJFK The assassination of John F. Kennedy was not
committed by the lone gunman Lee Harvey Oswald
but was rather a detailed organized conspiracy to kill
the president.
.742
CY911 The U.S. government allowed the 9/11 attacks to take
place so that it would have an excuse to achieve
foreign (e.g., wars in Afghanistan and Iraq) and
domestic (e.g., attacks on civil liberties) goals that
had been determined prior to the attacks.
.742
CYDiana Princess Diana’s death was not an accident but rather
an organized assassination by members of the British
royal family who disliked her.
.742
CYOkla The Oklahoma City bombers Timothy McVeigh and
Terry Nichols did not act alone but rather received
assistance from neo-Nazi groups.
.742
CYCoke The Coca-Cola company intentionally changed to an
inferior formula with the intent of driving up demand
for their classic product, later reintroducing it for their
financial gain.
.742
CYRoswell In July 1947, the U.S. military recovered the wreckage
of an alien spacecraft from Roswell, NM, and covered
up the fact.
.891
CYArea51 Area 51 in Nevada is a secretive military base that
contains hidden alien spacecraft and/or alien bodies.
.891
CYClimChangecThe claim that the climate is changing due to emissions
from fossil fuels is a hoax perpetrated by corrupt
scientists who want to spend more taxpayer money
on climate research.
CYAIDScU.S. agencies intentionally created the AIDS epidemic
and administered it to Black and gay men in the
1970s.
Acceptance of other sciences
CauseHIV The HIV virus causes AIDS. .894
CauseSmoke Smoking causes lung cancer. .845
CauseCO2 Human CO2 emissions cause climate change
Consensus items
ConsensHIV Out of 100 medical scientists, how many do you think
believe that the HIV virus causes AIDS?
ConsensSmoke Out of 100 medical scientists, how many do you think
believe that smoking causes lung cancer?
ConsensCO2 Out of 100 climate scientists, how many do you think
believe that CO2 emissions resulting from human
activities cause climate change?
Note: All items used a 4-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (4), with the exception of the consensus items,
which used a scale from 0 to 100. R = reverse-scored. The section headings inside the table correspond to the latent variable names in
Figure 2.
aStandardized loadings of manifest variables on their corresponding latent variables are as shown in Figure 2. All loadings are significant.
bThere were two composite manifest variables for conspiracist ideation. The same loading is shown for all items that entered into a given
composite variable. cThese items were not entered as manifest variables to estimate the conspiracist-ideation latent variable because they
referred to conspiracies relevant to the scientific proposition being queried. People might therefore have endorsed these items because they
represented a convenient way to justify a rejection of science actually motivated by other variables.
Table 1. (Continued)
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 627
Acceptance of science and consensus
Pairs of acceptance-of-science and consensus items (e.g.,
CauseHIV-ConsensHIV) were entered into an SEM with
two correlated latent variables, one capturing the com-
mon variance of all acceptance questions and the other
representing all consensus questions (see Fig. 1). Pairwise
correlations between the residuals for each belief-con-
sensus pair represented content-specific covariance. All
SEMs in this article were performed with Mplus (http://
www.statmodel.com) using ordinal coding of the mani-
fest variables, with the consensus responses binned into
nine categories with approximately equal numbers.
The model fit reasonably well, χ2(5) = 53.71, p < .0001,
confirmatory fit index (CFI) = .989, root-mean-square
error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.092, 90% confidence
interval (CI) = [0.071, 0.115]. People’s content-general
inclination to accept science was associated with their
content-general perception of scientific consensus, r =
.43, Z = 12.76, p < .0001, over and above the content-
specific links represented by the pairwise correlations.
The fact that acceptance of climate science (CauseCO2)
and perceived consensus among climate scientists
(ConsensCO2) loaded onto their respective latent vari-
ables together with items concerning other very different
scientific propositions suggests that respondents did not
gauge consensus among climate scientists and evaluate
climate science independently of their views of other,
unrelated domains of scientific inquiry. Rather, their per-
ception of consensus and their endorsement of scientific
findings about the climate reflected in part a content-
independent disposition to perceive scientific consensus
and a correlated disposition to accept scientifically well-
established propositions. This finding replicated the fac-
tor structure reported in Lewandowsky, Gignac, and
Vaughan (2012).
Ideology, conspiratorial thinking, and
acceptance of science
We next examined the interplay among the five con-
structs of greatest interest: acceptance of climate science,
acceptance of other scientific propositions, free-market
ideology, the belief that previous environmental prob-
lems have been resolved, and conspiracist ideation. For
this analysis, climate science was considered separately
from the other scientific propositions. Constructs were
measured using the manifest variables identified in
the earlier factor analyses, and acceptance of other
scientific propositions was measured by CauseHIV and
CauseSmoke. For the climate-science factor, pairwise cor-
relations were estimated between CO2TempUp and
CO2AtmosUp, and between CO2WillNegChange and
CO2HasNegChange, as this improved model fit consider-
ably. An exploratory model that estimated correlations
among all five latent variables fit well, χ2(78) = 261.0, p <
.0001, CFI = .997, RMSEA = 0.045, 90% CI = [0.039, 0.051];
the pairwise correlations between the latent variables are
given in Table 2.
ConsensHIV
ConsensSmoke
ConsensCO2
CauseCO2
CauseSmoke
CauseHIV
e1
e2
e3
e4
e5
e6
Consensus
Science
0.84
0.79
0.55
0.72
0.89
0.86
0.43
0.67 0.62 0.88
Fig. 1.Latent variable model for the relationship between perceived consensus among scientists
and acceptance of scientific propositions, as related to three scientific issues. All correlations (dou-
ble-headed arrows) and factor loadings (single-headed arrows) are significant and standardized.
See Table 1 for an explanation of the names of the manifest variables. e = residuals.
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
628 Lewandowsky et al.
We next sought to predict acceptance of climate sci-
ence and other sciences from the remaining three latent
variables while simultaneously simplifying the model by
removing nonsignificant correlations and regression
weights. This final model fit very well, χ2(82) = 182.1, p <
.0001, CFI = .999, RMSEA = 0.033, 90% CI = [0.026, 0.039],
and its fit did not differ from that of the unconstrained
model that included all correlations, Δχ2(4) = 3.525,
p > .10. Figure 2 shows the model at the level of latent
variables, displaying only weights and correlations that
were statistically significant, p < .05.
Several aspects of the model are noteworthy: First,
endorsement of free markets was highly predictive of
rejection of climate science, β = 0.77. Second, free-
market ideology also predicted the rejection of other
scientific propositions, although the magnitude of that
Table 2.Pairwise Correlations Between the Five Latent Variables in the Unconstrained Model
Variable
Acceptance of
climate science
Acceptance of
other sciences
Perception that problems
have been resolved Free-market ideology
Acceptance of climate
science
Acceptance of other
sciences
.563 —
Perception that problems
have been resolved
.586 .263 —
Free-market ideology .866 .464 .498
Conspiracist ideation .197 .538 .032 .021
0.680.41
Acceptance
of Climate
Science
Acceptance
of Other
Sciences
Free-Market
Ideology
Conspiracist
Ideation
Problems
Resolved
D1 D2
r2 = .83 r2 = .54
–0.49
–0.55
–0.20
–0.77
.50
–0.21
Fig. 2.Latent variable model predicting acceptance of climate science and acceptance
of other scientific propositions on the basis of free-market ideology, the perception
that earlier environmental problems have been resolved, and conspiracist ideation.
All regression weights (single-headed arrows) and the correlation (double-headed
arrow) are significant and standardized. Weights and correlations that are not shown
were set to zero (e.g., the correlation between the residuals of acceptance of climate sci-
ence and acceptance of other sciences). Manifest variables for each latent variable and
their loadings are provided in Table 1. See the text for further explanation.
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 629
link was smaller, β = 0.49. Third, conspiracist ideation
was negatively associated with acceptance of climate sci-
ence and other scientific propositions, β = 0.21 and β =
0.55, respectively. Finally, the perception that previous
environmental problems have been resolved was nega-
tively associated with acceptance of climate science, β =
0.20, but was unrelated to acceptance of other sciences
(β set to 0).
The three latent predictors accounted for much of the
variance in acceptance of climate science and for about
half the variance in acceptance of other sciences. Notably,
the three predictors explained the entire association
between the two latent criterion variables (initially r =
.563; see Table 2) because there was no remaining unex-
plained correlation (r between the residuals of accep-
tance of climate science and acceptance of other sciences
was set to 0 without loss of fit). Conspiracist ideation did
not correlate with the other two predictors.
Discussion
Rejection of climate science was strongly associated with
endorsement of a laissez-faire view of unregulated free
markets. This finding replicates previous work (e.g.,
Heath & Gifford, 2006), although the strength of associa-
tion found here (r ; .80) exceeds that reported in any
extant study. At least in part, this may reflect the use of
structural equation modeling, which enables measure-
ment of the associations between constructs in a way that
is free of measurement error (Fan, 2003).
Another variable that was associated with rejection of
climate science and other scientific propositions was
conspiracist ideation. This relationship emerged even
though conspiracies related to the queried scientific
propositions (HIV-AIDS, climate change) did not contrib-
ute to the conspiracist construct. By implication, the role
of conspiracist ideation in the rejection of science did not
simply reflect “convenience” theories that provided spe-
cific alternative “explanations” for a scientific consensus.
Instead, this finding suggests that a general propensity to
endorse any of a number of conspiracy theories predis-
poses people to reject entirely unrelated scientific facts.
The relative importance of free-market ideology and
conspiracist ideation differed between climate science
and the other scientific propositions. We suggest that
free-market ideology had a larger effect on rejection of
climate science than did conspiratorial thinking (β =
0.77 vs. β = 0.21) for two reasons: First, climate science
has arguably become more politicized than other sci-
ences (Hamilton, 2011; McCright & Dunlap, 2011a), and
second, given the fundamental importance of fossil fuels
(and hence carbon dioxide emissions) to contemporary
economies, climate science presents a far greater threat
to laissez-faire economics than do the medical facts that
HIV causes AIDS and that smoking causes lung cancer.
For the same reasons but in reverse, conspiracist ideation
had a larger effect on denial of facts about AIDS and lung
cancer (β = 0.55), compared with the effect of free-
market ideology on denial of these facts (β = 0.49).
The third predictor, the perception that previous envi-
ronmental problems have been resolved, predicted rejec-
tion of climate science but not of the other sciences. We
suggest that this construct reflects a predisposition to dis-
miss environmental concerns (or consider them resolved)
that is prevalent in particular among adherents of the free
market (as evidenced by the correlation relating percep-
tion that problems have been resolved with free-market
ideology, r = .50).
Finally, we replicated the finding that perceived scien-
tific consensus is associated with acceptance of science
(Ding et al., 2011; Dunlap & McCright, 2008; Kahan et al.,
2011; Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Vaughan, 2012). Although
the direction of causality cannot be ascertained from
these data, it has been shown that providing consensus
information can significantly enhance people’s accep-
tance of climate science (Lewandowsky, Gignac, &
Vaughan, 2012).
Potential objections
Our respondents were self-selected denizens of climate
blogs. Therefore, one potential objection to our results
might be the selected nature of our sample. We acknowl-
edge that our sample is self-selected and that the results
may therefore not generalize to the population at large.
However, this fact has no bearing on the importance of
our results: We designed the study to investigate what
motivates the rejection of science in individuals who
choose to get involved in the ongoing debate about one
scientific topic, climate change. As noted earlier, this
group of people has a demonstrable impact on society,
and understanding their motivations and reasoning is
therefore of importance.
Another objection that might be raised is the possibil-
ity that our respondents willfully accentuated their replies
to subvert our presumed intentions. As in most behav-
ioral research, this possibility cannot be ruled out.
However, unless a substantial subset of the more than
1,000 respondents conspired to coordinate their
responses, any individual accentuation or provocation
would only have injected more noise into our data. This
seems unlikely because subsets of our items have been
used in previous laboratory research, and for those sub-
sets, our data did not differ in a meaningful way from
published precedent. For example, responses to the
Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, &
Griffin, 1985) replicated previous research involving the
population at large (see Table S2 in the Supplemental
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
630 Lewandowsky et al.
Material), and the model in Figure 1 exactly replicated
the factor structure reported by Lewandowsky, Gignac,
and Vaughan (2012) using a sample of pedestrians in a
large city. The Supplemental Material also shows that the
results are robust to the removal of potential outliers.
A final concern might be that respondents who
rejected science did so on the basis of a general critical
stance or predisposition to reject any proposition put
before them for potential endorsement. We find this
highly unlikely because respondents who rejected scien-
tific propositions were quite likely to endorse other items,
such as various conspiracy theories or the idea that “an
economic system based on free markets unrestrained by
government interference automatically works best to
meet human needs” (FMUnresBest in Table 1).
Theoretical implications
The pivotal role of personal ideology in the rejection of
climate science has been repeatedly demonstrated
(Dunlap & McCright, 2008; Feygina et al., 2010; Hamilton,
2011; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Kahan, 2010; Kahan et al.,
2011; McCright & Dunlap, 2011a, 2011b). We highlighted
the magnitude of this effect among climate-science blog
denizens, who have a strong interest in the issue, and we
additionally showed that endorsement of the free market
also predicted the rejection of two other well-established
scientific facts. This novel result is particularly intriguing
because only one of those facts, the link between tobacco
smoke and lung cancer, has regulatory implications and
has a history of organized ideologically motivated denial
(e.g., Michaels & Monforton, 2005; Oreskes & Conway,
2010). The fact that HIV causes AIDS, by contrast, seems
of little relevance to one’s views on the free market at
first glance. However, the association between ideology
and rejection of the link between HIV and AIDS is in
good agreement with our finding that perceived consen-
sus and acceptance of science were associated via gen-
eral factors that transcended pairwise correlations (Fig.
1). If acceptance of science is a coherent construct, then
it is not altogether surprising that rejection of established
facts is also consistently associated with free-market ide-
ology and conspiracist ideation.
Our results identify conspiracist ideation as a personal-
ity factor or cognitive style, as numerous conspiracy theo-
ries are captured by a single latent construct (cf. Goertzel,
1994; Swami et al., 2009; Swami et al., 2011). The negative
association between conspiracist ideation and acceptance
of well-established science confirms previous conceptual
analyses (Diethelm & McKee, 2009; Goertzel, 2010; McKee
& Diethelm, 2010). However, to our knowledge, our
results are the first to provide empirical evidence for the
correlation between a general construct of conspiracist
ideation and a general tendency to reject well-founded
science. This association is particularly notable because it
persisted after “convenience theories” were removed,
thus reliably linking broad-based rejection of science to
ideations that appear quite unrelated at first glance, such
as the notion that the U.S. government had advance
knowledge about the September 11th attacks or that the
FBI assassinated Martin Luther King, Jr.
We suggest that the study and analysis of conspiracist
ideation is of increasing importance: First, the spread of
conspiracy theories about the alleged risks from vaccina-
tions has been linked to reduced vaccination rates, with
consequent adverse public-health impacts (Goertzel,
2010). In the climate arena, the conspiracist ideation that
all of the world’s scientific academies have conspired
together to create a hoax known as global warming has
found traction in American mainstream politics (Inhofe,
2012). Second, there is evidence that conspiracy theories
are capable of influencing people even when they explic-
itly attempt to discount them. Douglas and Sutton (2008)
showed that after exposure to conspiracy theories about
the death of Princess Diana, participants were demon-
strably affected by those theories even when they tried to
dismiss their influence. Third, belief in conspiracy theo-
ries appears to be inducible. Swami et al. (2011) were
able to induce belief in an entirely fictitious conspiracy
theory involving a popular soft drink (e.g., that the drink
“raises dopamine levels”), especially among participants
who already held other conspiratorial views. That study
is arguably a laboratory equivalent of the real-life “exper-
iment” conducted by vested interests and political groups
with respect to climate science (cf. Oreskes & Conway,
2010).
In closing, we consider briefly what countermeasures
might be available to reduce the influence and spread of
conspiracy theories. Conspiracist ideation is, by defini-
tion, difficult to correct because any evidence contrary to
the conspiracy is itself considered evidence of its exis-
tence (Bale, 2007; Sunstein & Vermeule, 2009). Thus,
increasing global temperatures are reinterpreted as being
the result of government agencies selectively removing
thermometers that show a cooling trend and retaining
only those that show the “desired” warming trend.
Sunstein and Vermeule (2009) discussed several poten-
tial countermeasures that are at the disposal of govern-
ment officials, several of which agree well with our
finding. For example, Sunstein and Vermeule suggested
that instead of rebutting single conspiracy theories, scien-
tists and policymakers should try to rebut many at the
same time. This conforms with our finding that conspira-
cist ideation tends to be quite broad. Multiple rebuttals
also raise the complexity of possible conspiracist
responses (not only must there be a conspiracy to remove
thermometers, but there must also be a conspiracy to
launch a false “decoy” theory about the absence of a
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 631
plane hitting the Pentagon on September 11 in order to
detract from the real conspiracy, which was to destroy
the Twin Towers, and so on). Sunstein and Vermeule
noted the possibility of addressing the “demand” rather
than the “supply” of conspiracy theories; that is, rather
than being directed at changing the minds of actual
believers, communication should be directed at inoculat-
ing potential consumers of conspiracy theories against
accepting them.
Similarly, Lewandowsky, Ecker, Seifert, Schwarz, and
Cook (2012) offered a broad review of “debiasing” tech-
niques that are directly applicable to the rebuttal of con-
spiracy theories and include suggestions about how to
avoid various backfire effects that can arise when peo-
ple’s worldviews are challenged by corrective informa-
tion. Some of those suggestions, such as reaffirmation of
a subset of beliefs among consumers of conspiracy theo-
ries, were echoed by Sunstein and Vermeule (2009).
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
Preparation of this article was facilitated by a Discovery Grant
from the Australian Research Council and an Australian
Professorial Fellowship to S. L.
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss
.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
Notes
1. The existence of a scientific consensus about core principles
of climate change does not imply the absence of uncertainty
or the absence of legitimate scientific debate surrounding as-
yet unresolved issues. The core aspects of climate science for
which the consensus is beyond doubt are that the climate is
changing, that greenhouse gases are responsible, and that the
world is beginning to witness predicted changes in climate
patterns (Somerville, 2011). See the Supplemental Material
for further evidence about the pervasive scientific consensus
regarding climate science.
2. We prefer “rejection of science” to the term “denial,” which
in current scholarly usage typically pertains to an active pub-
lic denial of scientific facts by various means, such as the use
of rhetoric to create the appearance of debate where there
is none (Diethelm & McKee, 2009; Jacques, 2012; McKee &
Diethelm, 2010). Thus, whereas investigations of denial focus
on the techniques by which organizations or individuals seek
to undermine scientific findings in the public’s eye, our research
on the rejection of science focuses on the factors that predis-
pose people to be susceptible to organized denial. We thus use
“denial” to refer to public activities connected to the rejection
of science and use “rejection” when discussing individuals’ atti-
tudes toward science.
3. The survey included several additional items (e.g., perceived
income rank, well-being) that were not relevant to the con-
structs of interest. The data are available at www.cogsciwa.com.
References
Abt, C. C. (1983, September). The anti-smoking industry.
Retrieved from http://legacy.library.ucsf.edu/tid/vob81f00
Allison, I., Bindoff, N. L., Bindschadler, R. A., Cox, P. M., de
Noblet, N., England, M. H., . . . Weaver, A. J. (2009). The
Copenhagen diagnosis, 2009: Updating the world on the
latest climate science. Sydney, Australia: University of New
South Wales Climate Change Research Center.
Anderegg, W. R. L., Prall, J. W., Harold, J., & Schneider, S. H.
(2010). Expert credibility in climate change. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107, 12107–12109.
Bale, J. M. (2007). Political paranoia v. political realism: On
distinguishing between bogus conspiracy theories and
genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns of Prejudice, 41,
45–60.
Bogart, L. M., & Thorburn, S. (2005). Are HIV/AIDS conspiracy
beliefs a barrier to HIV prevention among African Americans?
Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 38,
213–218.
Boykoff, M. T. (2007). Flogging a dead norm? Newspaper cov-
erage of anthropogenic climate change in the United States
and United Kingdom from 2003 to 2006. Area, 39, 470–
481.
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting pub-
lic opinion on climate change: An empirical assessment of
factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S.,
2002–2010. Climatic Change, 114, 169–188.
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985).
The Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 49, 71–75.
Diethelm, P., & McKee, M. (2009). Denialism: What is it and
how should scientists respond? European Journal of Public
Health, 19, 2–4.
Ding, D., Maibach, E., Zhao, X., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz,
A. (2011). Support for climate policy and societal action are
linked to perceptions about scientific agreement. Nature
Climate Change, 1, 462–466.
Doran, P. T., & Zimmerman, M. K. (2009). Examining the
scientific consensus on climate change. Eos, 90(3), 21–
22.
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of
conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of the-
ories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal
of Social Psychology, 148, 210–221.
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to
know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influ-
enced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal
of Social Psychology, 50, 544–552.
Dunlap, R. E., & McCright, A. M. (2008). A widening gap:
Republican and Democratic views on climate change.
Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Develop-
ment, 50(5), 26–35.
Dunlap, R. E., & McCright, A. M. (2011). Organized climate
change denial. In J. S. Dryzek, R. B. Norgaard, & D.
Schlosberg (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of climate change
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
632 Lewandowsky et al.
and society (pp. 144–160). Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
Fan, X. (2003). Two approaches for correcting correlation attenu-
ation caused by measurement error: Implications for research
practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63,
915–930.
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System jus-
tification, the denial of global warming, and the possibil-
ity of “system-sanctioned change.” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 36, 326–338.
Freudenburg, W. R., & Muselli, V. (2010). Global warming
estimates, media expectations, and the asymmetry of sci-
entific challenge. Global Environmental Change, 20, 483–
491.
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political
Psychology, 15, 731–742.
Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO
Reports, 11, 493–499.
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004).
Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative anal-
ysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires.
American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.
Hamilton, L. C. (2011). Education, politics and opinions about
climate change evidence for interaction effects. Climatic
Change, 104, 231–242.
Hanson, F. (2009). Australia and the world: Public opinion
and foreign policy (Tech. Rep.). Sydney, New South Wales,
Australia: Lowy Institute.
Heath, Y., & Gifford, R. (2006). Free-market ideology and envi-
ronmental degradation: The case of belief in global climate
change. Environment & Behavior, 38, 48–71.
Inhofe, J. (2012). The greatest hoax: How the global warming
conspiracy threatens your future. Washington, DC: WND
Books.
Jacques, P. J. (2012). A general theory of climate denial. Global
Environmental Politics, 12, 9–17.
Jacques, P. J., Dunlap, R. E., & Freeman, M. (2008). The organ-
isation of denial: Conservative think tanks and environmen-
tal scepticism. Environmental Politics, 17, 349–385.
Kahan, D. M. (2010). Fixing the communications failure. Nature,
463, 296–297.
Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural
cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research,
14, 147–174.
Kalichman, S. C. (2009). Denying AIDS: Conspiracy theo-
ries, pseudoscience, and human tragedy. New York, NY:
Springer.
Kalichman, S. C., Eaton, L., & Cherry, C. (2010). “There is
no proof that HIV causes AIDS”: AIDS denialism beliefs
among people living with HIV/AIDS. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 33, 432–440.
Lahsen, M. (1999). The detection and attribution of con-
spiracies: The controversy over Chapter 8. In G. Marcus
(Ed.), Paranoia within reason: A casebook on conspiracy
as explanation (pp. 111–136). Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K. H., Seifert, C., Schwarz, N., &
Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: Continued
influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science
in the Public Interest, 13, 106–131.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2012). The
pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance
of science. Nature Climate Change. Advance online publi-
cation. doi:10.1038/nclimate1720
Lewandowsky, S., Stritzke, W. G. K., Oberauer, K., & Morales,
M. (2005). Memory for fact, fiction, and misinformation:
The Iraq War 2003. Psychological Science, 16, 190–195.
Lewandowsky, S., Stritzke, W. G. K., Oberauer, K., & Morales,
M. (2009). Misinformation and the “War on Terror”: When
memory turns fiction into fact. In W. G. K. Stritzke, S.
Lewandowsky, D. Denemark, J. Clare, & F. Morgan (Eds.),
Terrorism and torture: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp.
179–203). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2003). Defeating Kyoto: The
conservative movement’s impact on U.S. climate change
policy. Social Problems, 50, 348–373.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2010). Anti-reflexivity: The
American conservative movement’s success in undermin-
ing climate science and policy. Theory, Culture & Society,
27, 100–133.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011a). Cool dudes: The
denial of climate change among conservative white males
in the United States. Global Environmental Change, 21,
1163–1172.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011b). The politicization
of climate change and polarization in the American pub-
lic’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. The Sociological
Quarterly, 52, 155–194.
McKee, M., & Diethelm, P. (2010). Christmas 2010: Reading
between the lines how the growth of denialism undermines
public health. British Medical Journal, 341, 1309–1311.
Michaels, D., & Monforton, C. (2005). Manufacturing uncer-
tainty: Contested science and the protection of the pub-
lic’s health and environment. American Journal of Public
Health, 95, S39–S48.
Mooney, C. (2007). An inconvenient assessment. Bulletin of the
Atomic Scientists, 63(6), 40–47.
Nattrass, N. (2010). Still crazy after all these years: The chal-
lenge of AIDS denialism for science. AIDS and Behavior,
14, 248–251.
Nattrass, N. (2011). Defending the boundaries of science: AIDS
denialism, peer review and the medical hypotheses saga.
Sociology of Health & Illness, 33, 507–521.
Nyhan, B. (2010). Why the “death panel” myth wouldn’t die:
Misinformation in the health care reform debate. The Forum,
8(1), Article 5. Retrieved from http://www.dartmouth
.edu/~nyhan/health-care-misinformation.pdf
Oreskes, N., & Conway, E. M. (2010). Merchants of doubt.
London, England: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Scruggs, L., & Benegal, S. (2012). Declining public concern
about climate change: Can we blame the great recession?
Global Environmental Change, 22, 505–515.
Somerville, R. C. J. (2011). How much should the public know
about climate science? Climatic Change, 104, 509–514.
Stocking, S. H., & Holstein, L. W. (2009). Manufacturing doubt:
Journalists’ roles and the construction of ignorance in a
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Motivated Climate Denial 633
scientific controversy. Public Understanding of Science, 18,
23–42.
Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories:
Causes and cures. Journal of Political Philosophy, 17, 202–
227.
Sussman, B. (2010). Climategate: A veteran meteorologist exposes
the global warming scam. Washington, DC: WND Books.
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2009).
Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of
personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11
conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–
761.
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, A., Furnham, J.,
Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in
Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief sys-
tem and associations between individual psychological dif-
ferences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories.
British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443–463.
at UZH Hauptbibliothek / Zentralbibliothek Zuerich on February 14, 2014pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Conspiracy theories can be defined as beliefs about important events that reject well-evidenced or parsimonious explanations in favor of implausible, illogical, or fantastical ones involving secret plots by preternaturally powerful and malevolent group. 1 The denial of science is at the heart of many of these beliefs, from the specific rejection of Anthropogenic Climate Change and the efficacy of vaccination to a general rejection of "experts," There is also a strong and consistent association between conspiracy theory ideation and science denialism (Lewandowsky et al. 2013;Rutjens et al. 2018). Scientific findings are perhaps uniquely placed to be the subject of conspiracy theory beliefs: findings from the scientific method often lack intuitive and emotional appeal (Clearly et al. 2025) and can feel distant from day-to-day life (Rutjens and Hornsey Forthcoming). ...
... Conspiracy theory beliefs are harmful to society because they have consequences for their targets: institutions, ideas, groups, and individuals that enjoy or are perceived to enjoy power and admiration in wider society (Barnes et al. 2018;Jolley et al. 2022). Conspiracy theories negatively affect individuals in targeted groups (Jolley et al. 2024), weaken trust in public bodies and civic society (Einstein and Glick 2015), and undermine initiatives that tackle critical problems such as climate change (Lewandowsky et al. 2013) or global health emergencies (Grebe and Nattrass 2012;Romer and Jamieson 2020). For those who feel disadvantaged, accepting and propagating conspiracy theories can and does undermine the status of professionals and institutions, while at the same time maintaining the theorists' own ego, identity, and autonomy when these might otherwise feel challenged (Barnes et al. 2018;Douglas et al. 2019;Lantian et al. 2017;van Prooijen 2020). ...
... Conspiracy theories are at the heart of many instances of science denial or anti-science beliefs (Lewandowsky et al. 2013;Rutjens et al. 2018). Within this literature there are three broad, wellestablished predictors of conspiracy theory beliefs (EPBs): epistemic motives, existential motives, and social motives . ...
Article
Full-text available
Science denialism is at the heart of many conspiracy theory beliefs. We propose that such beliefs are manifestations of a distal social process: spite. In three pre‐registered studies, we test the hypothesis that established predictors of these beliefs (epistemic, existential, and social motives) are specific cues of competitive disadvantage that provoke a common facultative “spiteful” psychological response, making a person more open to believing in conspiracy theories. Study 1 ( N = 301; UK representative Prolific sample), found that spite mediated the relationship between realistic threat and in‐group narcissism (social motives), political powerlessness (existential motive), and intolerance for uncertainty (epistemic motive), and conspiracy theory belief and COVID‐19 conspiracies. This pattern was replicated in Study 2 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample). In Study 3 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample), we found that those who engaged in a spite‐inducing task reported higher levels of spite which indirectly resulted in stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories. The overall pattern of results provides initial evidence that spite may play a role in why people engage with false information. Research and policy implications of these findings are discussed.
... US Republicans, for instance, are more likely to deny the climate change science consensus, while US Democrats tend to accept it (Dunlap and McCright 2008;Hamilton 2011;Hamilton et al. 2015). Closely related, liberals and left-wing voters display less skepticism toward the scientific consensus on climate change compared to those with a right-wing authoritarian (RWA), conservative, or free-market ideology (Clarke et al. 2019;Heath and Gifford 2006;Lewandowsky et al. 2013a;Lewandowsky and Oberauer 2016;Pew Research Centre 2016;Rutjens et al. 2018;Stanley and Wilson 2019). Adding more nuance to the debate around ideology, Campbell and Kay (2014) argue that individuals are more likely to deny climate change when related policy solutions to climate change contradict their ideological beliefs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite widespread scientific consensus regarding the evidence for climate change and its effects, some members of the general public remain skeptical (i.e., believe that climate change is exaggerated, due to natural variation in the climate, etc.). While much research has considered the demographic and political correlates of climate skepticism, less work has considered the psychological roots, including individual differences in personality. Drawing on a large sample of voting-aged Canadians (N = 1725), we explore the relationship between personality (both general and antagonistic traits) and climate skepticism. Even when accounting for a variety of known correlates, our results reveal a consistent role for personality: antagonistic traits such as narcissism and psychopathy are related to increased levels of skepticism, whereas prosocial traits such as agreeableness and honesty-humility are related to lower levels of skepticism. In fact, the results of a dominance analysis reveal that the trait of openness is the second most predictor in the entire model, trailing left–right ideology in its explanatory power. By highlighting the psychological roots, our results provide insight into why climate skepticism is so persistent. The results further suggest that combatting climate skepticism will be a difficult task that requires strategies that take individual differences in personality into account.
... Undermining trust in experts and science using unfounded beliefs is particularly dangerous, especially in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, as it had far-reaching public health consequences in the form of non-compliance with pandemic measures (Banai et al., 2021(Banai et al., , 2022Wirawan et al., 2021) and vaccine hesitancy (Ullah et al., 2021). Research on these aspects was also carried out before the pandemic (Jolley & Douglas, 2014b, 2014aLewandowsky et al., 2013). While it can be true that experts sometimes fail in their decisions, there are usually more corrective processes in play than in political institutions. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, we test two competing but mutually complementary hypotheses about the relationship between endorsement of unfounded beliefs during the COVID-19 pandemic (i.e., conspiracy and pseudoscientific beliefs) and institutional trust. To overcome the correlational nature, we used a 3-wave longitudinal design to examine whether low institutional trust predicts unfounded beliefs or vice versa. The final sample consists of 929 participants with full data sets from all three waves (49.80% women). We used the cross-lagged panel model (CLPM), which is well suited to examine the temporal directionality of the relationship between two variables. The results showed a consistent pattern in terms of trust in experts: the effects of conspiracy and pseudoscientific beliefs predicting distrust in experts were stronger than the reverse pattern. For trust in government, the results showed support for both hypotheses. The study contributed to explaining possible causal relationship between unfounded beliefs and institutional trust, i.e. conspiracy and pseudoscientific beliefs may have directly predicted lower trust in experts and scientific institutions.
Article
Across diverse policy domains, there is broad concern about whether trust in science and expertise has eroded during the past decade. Using quota-based surveys with over 7,500 respondents across five countries and preregistered vignette experiments, we investigate what persuades populists and non-populists to accept expert advice. We find first that populism is associated with less willingness to accept expert advice, yet with variation between countries and topics. Second, we find both populists and non-populists are similarly impacted by stronger arguments. Finally, we show that populists are more likely to judge advice as poorly reasoned and perceive it as politically biased. A mediation analysis showed that the relationship between populism and advice acceptance was nearly completely mediated by these judgments. Our study indicates that populists not only listen to expertise but also respond to the same qualities of expert advice as others, even if their skepticism is higher.
Article
Full-text available
Plain Language Summary How do people think about big issues in science and society?: A survey study comparing vaccine and climate change beliefs People’s opinions and beliefs are shaped by many things, including their social circles, the information they consume, who they trust and respect, their education, occupation, religion, political views, as well as personality traits and life experiences. These opinions and beliefs contribute to how people respond to challenging issues that involve science and society, such as COVID-19 vaccination behaviours and climate change. Our research was interested in understanding peoples’ beliefs on these issues, and how peoples’ beliefs in one issue (COVID-19 vaccination behaviours) related to the other (climate change). We conducted an online survey open to adults, who were able to answer the survey questions at their leisure. Our survey asked participants a variety of different questions, including (but not limited to) their beliefs on vaccines, COVID-19, climate change, and politics. Over 1,500 people completed the survey. We then categorised people into groups based on their beliefs about climate change and vaccines, and examined factors that could be influencing these beliefs. We identified two categories that helped us understand how people thinking about science and society issues. Both these categories were driven largely by political beliefs. These categories reflected distinctly different views on the economy, trust, conspiracy-theories, and other questions are survey explored. The relationship between COVID-19 vaccination behaviours and climate change was largely shaped by these political categories, rather than age, income, or other individual beliefs alone. Our hope is that this research will help inform science communication and public health interventions. However, while this study provides useful information, it is just a starting point. More research is needed to understand these patterns to find ways to use this information to promote positive social, political, and health related behaviours.
Article
The monological belief system model suggests that—for at least a subset of people—developing a belief in one conspiracy theory will cause them to be more likely to believe in others. This model has been influential in the literature, but its core causal hypothesis has never been credibly tested. We therefore tested it in two longitudinal studies. Study 1 used a sample from New Zealand and Australia ( N = 498), with 7 monthly waves. Study 2 (preregistered) used a sample from New Zealand, Australia and the United Kingdom ( N = 978), with 13 monthly waves. We applied random intercept cross‐lagged panel models, permitting a credible causal identification strategy, albeit we cannot rule out time‐varying confounds. We find that increased belief in a conspiracy theory at one wave did (on average) predict increased belief in other conspiracies at the next wave, although the estimated coefficients were small.
Article
Full-text available
Conspiracy theories and beliefs against LGBTQ+ people are a recurrent theme in the political agenda, depicting them as evil actors in a larger plot, seeking to undermine soci- etal norms, institutions, and traditional values. Lessening LGBTQ+ conspiracy beliefs is crucial to reaching more social equality, and intergroup contact might represent a useful strategy. Study 1 (N = 253) investigated the associa- tions of the quantity of direct contact with LGBTQ+ peo- ple, the quality of such contacts, and their interactive role with LGBTQ+ conspiracy beliefs. Taking a step forward, Studies 2 (N = 512) and 3 (N = 529) investigated, correla- tionally and experimentally, respectively, the relationship between the quality of contact with LGBTQ+ individuals and LGBTQ+ conspiracy beliefs, exploring the mediating associations of intergroup knowledge, empathy, and anxiety. Results consistently suggested that a higher quantity of di- rect contacts with LGBTQ+ people is negatively associated with LGBTQ+ conspiracy beliefs. Furthermore, positive contact was associated with lower conspiracy beliefs against LGBTQ+ people, with these associations being either par- tially (Study 2) or fully (Study 3) mediated by intergroup empathy. Overall, these findings highlight the importance of fostering positive intergroup interactions and enhancing empathy as strategies to combat harmful conspiracy beliefs about marginalized groups.
Article
Full-text available
Although the central principles of evolution by natural selection can feel both abstruse and culturally fraught for learners of all ages, evolutionary logic is at the core of biological science: once students have a solid understanding of evolution, they can better understand everything else in biology. We present here a hands-on experience, coupled with intentional questioning strategies, that inspires students to use high-level evolutionary thinking and to begin asking excellent questions about what we know and how we know it. This activity leverages skull replicas of humans, chimps, and hominins to tap into young people’s natural curiosity about where we come from and what it means to be a person—fundamental topics of inquiry for young people just coming into their own identities. Along the way, students are also exposed to important data and create arguments about what the archaeological record can currently tell us about the story of how people like us came to be.
Preprint
Full-text available
Amid historically low societal trust in science, four cross-sectional studies (N = 3,856) reveal a link between generic science denial and national narcissism. The findings support the pre-registered hypotheses that (1) national narcissism (a desire for national recognition) and ingroup satisfaction (pride in national value) have opposite unique associations with science denial (rejection of scientific consensus and generalized suspicion towards scientific experts) and (2) opposite indirect associations with specific outcomes of science denial during the COVID19 pandemic and beyond (climate change denial, anti-vaccination attitude, and support for unregulated ‘alternative’ medicine). After their common variance is controlled, national narcissism is positively associated with generic science denial and its outcomes, while national ingroup satisfaction is associated negatively. National narcissism was the strongest predictor of science denial, surpassing other established predictors such as low education and political conservatism. Studies 1 and 2 showed additionally, that vulnerable narcissistic personality was the second strongest predictor of generic science denial, demonstrating for the first time, that the narcissistic need to be recognized as better than others underlies generic science denial.
Article
People's choices of food and drink, the attitudes they express, and the beliefs that they state are influenced by their political and other identities. At the same time, people's everyday choices depend on the context of available options in ways that are difficult to explain in terms of the choosers’ preferences and beliefs. Such phenomena provoke various questions. Do partisans or conspiracy theorists really believe what they are saying? Given the systematic inconsistency of their choices, in what sense do consumers prefer the items they purchase? More generally, how “flat” is the mind—do we come to decision‐making and choice with pre‐existing preferences, attitudes, and beliefs, or are our explanations for our behavior mere post‐hoc narratives? Here, we argue that several apparently disparate difficulties are rooted in a failure to separate psychologically different types of preferences, attitudes, and beliefs. We distinguish between underlying, inferred, and expressed preferences. These preferences may be expressed in different coordinate spaces and hence support different types of explanatory generalizations. Choices that appear inconsistent according to one type of preference can appear consistent according to another, and whether we can say that a person “really” prefers something depends on which type of preference we mean. We extend the tripartite classification to the case of attitudes and beliefs, and suggest that attributions of attitudes and beliefs may also be ambiguous. We conclude that not all of the mental states and representations that govern our behavior are context‐dependent and constructed, although many are.
Article
Full-text available
Even as the consensus over the reality and significance of anthropogenic climate change (ACC) becomes stronger within the scientific community, this global environmental problem is increasingly contested in the political arena and wider society. The spread of debate and contention over ACC from the scientific to socio-political realms has been detrimental to climate science. This article provides an overview of organized climate change denial. Focusing primarily on the US, where denial first took root and remains most active, this article begins by describing the growth of conservative-based opposition to environmentalism and environmental science in general. It then explains why climate change became the central focus of this opposition, which quickly evolved into a coordinated and well-funded machine or 'industry'. It also examines denialists' rationale for attacking the scientific underpinnings of climate change policy and the crucial strategy of 'manufacturing uncertainty' they employ.
Article
Full-text available
Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Although most experts agree that CO2 emissions are causing anthropogenic global warming (AGW), public concern has been declining. One reason for this decline is the `manufacture of doubt' by political and vested interests, which often challenge the existence of the scientific consensus. The role of perceived consensus in shaping public opinion is therefore of considerable interest: in particular, it is unknown whether consensus determines people's beliefs causally. It is also unclear whether perception of consensus can override people's `worldviews', which are known to foster rejection of AGW. Study 1 shows that acceptance of several scientific propositions--from HIV/AIDS to AGW--is captured by a common factor that is correlated with another factor that captures perceived scientific consensus. Study 2 reveals a causal role of perceived consensus by showing that acceptance of AGW increases when consensus is highlighted. Consensus information also neutralizes the effect of worldview.
Article
If you would be a real seeker after truth, it is necessary that at least once in your life you doubt, as far as possible, all things. (Descartes, Principles of Philosophy (1644)) “Simply stated, there is no doubt that Saddam Hussein now has weapons of mass destruction”; “we know where they are.” These statements, made in 2002 and 2003 by the US Vice President, Dick Cheney, and the US Secretary of Defense, Donald Rumsfeld, respectively, turned out to have no basis in fact when the post-invasion search for weapons of mass destruction (WMDs) in Iraq failed to turn up any tools of chemical or biological warfare, let alone the massive stockpiles that US officials insisted had been hidden by the Iraqi regime. Notwithstanding, polls conducted in the United States for up to a year after the invasion, by which time the absence of WMDs had become fully evident and made public, continued to reveal a persistent belief in their existence among 20% to 40% of respondents (Kull et al ., 2004; PIPA, 2004). Indeed, for several months after President Bush declared the war to have ended (May 1, 2003), some 20% of respondents additionally believed that Iraq had in fact used chemical or biological weapons on the battlefield during the immediately preceding conflict (Kull et al ., 2004.) Lest one think that these figures merely represent some inertia of opinion, with those erroneous beliefs inexorably fading over time, polls conducted in March, 2006, revealed that nearly a quarter of Americans continued to believe that Iraq possessed WMDs just before the invasion (PIPA, 2006).
Article
This article reports the development and validation of a scale to measure global life satisfaction, the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS). Among the various components of subjective well-being, the SWLS is narrowly focused to assess global life satisfaction and does not tap related constructs such as positive affect or loneliness. The SWLS is shown to have favorable psychometric properties, including high internal consistency and high temporal reliability. Scores on the SWLS correlate moderately to highly with other measures of subjective well-being, and correlate predictably with specific personality characteristics. It is noted that the SWLS is suited for use with different age groups, and other potential uses of the scale are discussed.
Article
HEATH recently completed a Ph.D. in environmental/social psychology at the University of Victoria. She is currently investigating human psychological factors to reduce wood-burning smoke. After the current study, she has been documenting sustainable business practices that bring about a win-win solution to sustainability and economic prosperity. She is an avid believer of spirituality and quantum theory. ROBERT GIFFORD is a professor of psychology at the University of Victoria, a Fellow of the American Psychological Association and the Canadian Psychological Association, author of three editions of Environmental Psychology: Principles and Practice, and editor of another journal in the field. His current research includes stud-ies of human encounters with large wild animals in the wilderness and humans on board the International Space Station. He thinks everyone should have a grocery store within a 10-minute walk. ABSTRACT: The effects of support for free-market ideology and environmental apa-thy were investigated to identify some bases for not believing in global climate change. A survey of community residents' (N = 185) beliefs about global climate change also assessed ecocentrism, anthropocentrism, perceived knowledge about cli-mate change, and self-efficacy. The beliefs that global climate change is not occur-ring, is mainly not human caused, will also have positive consequences and that weaker intentions to undertake ameliorative actions were significantly associated with greater support for free-market ideology, greater environmental apathy, less ecocentrism, and less self-efficacy. About 40% of the variance in each belief and 56% of the variance in the behavioral intention was explained by these factors. The results suggest that the relation between support for free-market ideology and the beliefs about global climate change is mediated by environmental apathy.
Article
The SWLS consists of 5-items that require a ratingon a 7-point Likert scale. Administration is rarely morethan a minute or 2 and can be completed by interview(including phone) or paper and pencil response. The in-strumentshouldnotbecompletedbyaproxyansweringfortheperson.Itemsofthe SWLSaresummedtocreatea total score that can range from 5 to 35.The SWLS is in the public domain. Permission isnot needed to use it. Further information regardingthe use and interpretation of the SWLS can be foundat the author’s Web site http://internal.psychology.illinois.edu/∼ediener/SWLS.html. The Web site alsoincludes links to translations of the scale into 27languages.