ArticlePDF Available

The Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on the Attention and Memory of Bystanders

PLOS
PLOS One
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The pervasive use of cell phones impacts many people-both cell phone users and bystanders exposed to conversations. This study examined the effects of overhearing a one-sided (cell phone) conversation versus a two-sided conversation on attention and memory. In our realistic design, participants were led to believe they were participating in a study examining the relationship between anagrams and reading comprehension. While the participant was completing an anagram task, the researcher left the room and participants overheard a scripted conversation, either two confederates talking with each other or one confederate talking on a cell phone. Upon the researcher's return, the participant took a recognition memory task with words from the conversation, and completed a questionnaire measuring the distracting nature of the conversation. Participants who overheard the one-sided conversation rated the conversation as significantly higher in distractibility than those who overheard the two-sided conversation. Also, participants in the one-sided condition scored higher on the recognition task. In particular they were more confident and accurate in their responses to words from the conversation than participants in the two-sided condition. However, participants' scores on the anagram task were not significantly different between conditions. As in real world situations, individual participants could pay varying amounts of attention to the conversation since they were not explicitly instructed to ignore it. Even though the conversation was irrelevant to the anagram task and contained less words and noise, one-sided conversations still impacted participants' self-reported distractibility and memory, thus showing people are more attentive to cell phone conversations than two-sided conversations. Cell phone conversations may be a common source of distraction causing negative consequences in workplace environments and other public places.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on the Attention
and Memory of Bystanders
Veronica V. Galva
´
n
1
*, Rosa S. Vessal
1
, Matthew T. Golley
1 Department of Psychological Sciences, College of Arts and Sciences, University of San Diego, San Diego, California, United States of America, 2 Department of Liberal
Arts, D’Youville College, Buffalo, New York, United States of America
Abstract
The pervasive use of cell phones impacts many people–both cell phone users and bystanders exposed to conversations.
This study examined the effects of overhearing a one-sided (cell phone) conversation versus a two-sided conversation on
attention and memory. In our realistic design, participants were led to believe they were participating in a study examining
the relationship between anagrams and reading comprehension. While the participant was completing an anagram task,
the researcher left the room and participants overheard a scripted conversation, either two confederates talking with each
other or one confederate talking on a cell phone. Upon the researcher’s return, the participant took a recognition memory
task with words from the conversation, and completed a questionnaire measuring the distracting nature of the
conversation. Participants who overheard the one-sided conversation rated the conversation as significantly higher in
distractibility than those who overheard the two-sided conversation. Also, participants in the one-sided condition scored
higher on the recognition task. In particular they were more confident and accurate in their responses to words from the
conversation than participants in the two-sided condition. However, participants’ scores on the anagram task were not
significantly different between conditions. As in real world situations, individual participants could pay varying amounts of
attention to the conversation since they were not explicitly instructed to ignore it. Even though the conversation was
irrelevant to the anagram task and contained less words and noise, one-sided conversations still impacted participants’ self-
reported distractibility and memory, thus showing people are more attentive to cell phone conversations than two-sided
conversations. Cell phone conversations may be a common source of distraction causing negative consequences in
workplace environments and other public places.
Citation: Galva
´
n VV, Vessal RS, Golley MT (2013) The Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on the Attention and Memory of Bystanders. PLoS ONE 8(3): e58579.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579
Editor: Jan de Fockert, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom
Received August 8, 2012; Accepted February 6, 2013; Published March 13, 2013
Copyright: ß 2013 Galva
´
n et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reprod uction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: V.V. Galva
´
n received a Faculty Research Grant from her institution, the University of San Diego. The funders had no role in study design, data collection
and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
* E-mail: vgalvan@sandiego.edu
¤ Current address: Clinical Psychology Program, Palo Alto University, Palo Alto, California, United States of America
Introduction
People spent an estimated 2.30 trillion minutes using their
wireless devices over the last year [1]. Cell phones, one subset of
these devices, are relied upon heavily for personal communication,
and are often used in public spaces [2]. Recent surveys indicate
that people are becoming more dependent on their cell phones.
Cell phones are increasingly replacing land lines and participants
described experiencing ‘‘a personal connection towards their cell
phone’’ [3]. People have reported feeling so emotionally attached
to their cell phones that they feel anxiety without their phones [4]
or feel they ‘‘can’t live without’’ them [5]. With more technological
capabilities, such as personal organizers and navigation devices,
people see the cell phone as an essential part of their everyday lives
and even feel that they have a ‘‘personal relationship’’ with their
phones [6]. Seventy-six percent of respondents also said their cell
phones were always on or on most of the time, and 24% felt they
had to answer their cell phones ‘‘even when it interrupts a meeting
or meal’’ [5]. With the increased presence and personal use of cell
phones, there is a greater likelihood that an individual will be
frequently impacted by cell phones because either they or someone
around them is using a cell phone.
Most cell phone research has focused on how using a cell phone
impacts the user. Much of the research has used a simulated
driving task to examine these effects and has found that drivers
using cell phones are slower to change speed or direction [7], and
are slower to brake and miss more red lights, regardless of whether
the device is held or ‘‘hands free’’ [8]. These cell phone effects are
strong enough that the chances of being in an accident are similar
to the risks of drunk driving [9,10] and the effects are not likely to
lessen with practice [11]. It is important to note that deficits
appear to be related to cognitive, and not motor, effects since
holding and dialing the phone were not significant factors [8].
Instead, it appears that impairment of visual attention leads to
cognitive impairments such that drivers ‘see’ less [12] and
remember fewer objects they directly looked at while driving [13].
While most research on cell phones has focused on drivers,
more recent research has shown pedestrians are also affected by
their cell phone usage. Pedestrians talking on cell phones have
impaired visual attention while crossing the street [14,15,16] to
such an extent that most pedestrians on cell phones did not see a
clown riding on a unicycle nearby [17].
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 1 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
Cell phone use can clearly have a negative impact on the user’s
cognition particularly when the user is driving or crossing streets
on foot. A question worth considering is whether there is any
impact on bystanders who overhear cell phone conversations,
since there are possibly millions of bystanders to the millions of cell
phone conversations that take place yearly. Surveys have shown
that 82% of respondents are annoyed by others’ cell phone use in
public [5] and that the level of annoyance depends on the public
location [18]. However, only a few studies have experimentally
addressed the effects of cell phone conversations that are
overheard by bystanders. Monk and colleagues questioned
bystanders who overheard either a one-sided or two-sided
conversation at a bus stop or on a train [19,20]. Participants
found one-sided conversations more noticeable and intrusive. This
difference was not due to the presence of a cell phone since both
versions of the one-sided conversation (a person using a cell phone
or a person who spoke in a normal tone with a partner who
whispered) were viewed unfavorably [20]. Unfortunately, Monk
and colleagues were limited by the transportation authorities to
only five questions, and thus were unable to examine other issues
about the different types of conversations.
The only study thus far to examine the cognitive effects of one-
sided conversations on bystanders is a study by Emberson,
Lupyan, Goldstein, and Spivey [21]. In this well-controlled study,
participants were asked to ignore sixty seconds of speech that
occurred while they completed verbal tasks and motor tasks.
Participants were told they would hear speech from the computer
speakers and were asked to focus their attention on the computer
tasks. In the within-subject design, participants overheard different
types of auditory distractions (one-sided, two-sided, monologue,
and silence) over thirty-two verbal and motor trials. When
compared to the silent baseline, participants were more distracted
by overhearing a one-sided conversation than hearing either a
monologue or two-sided conversations. In a follow-up study,
Emberson and colleagues filtered the conversations, making the
speech incomprehensible. Unlike their first study, they found no
significant differences when comparing the monologue, two-sided
and one-sided conversations to the silent condition. The research-
ers concluded cell phone conversations are more distracting than a
typical dialogue because the content of a cell phone conversation is
less predictable [21].
Our study tests the cognitive effects of one-sided conversation
and serves as a bridge between research done in a well-controlled
laboratory setting and a completely naturalistic setting. We were
able, with the help of confederates, to have a realistic situation in a
controlled environment. In contrast to Emberson et al. [21],
participants were exposed to the conversation in a more natural
context; i.e. they were not informed that a conversation would take
place; they only heard one conversation one time and, more
importantly, were not aware that the conversation was part of the
study. The current study’s realistic context allows it to be more
generalizable to real-world situations in which people overhear a
conversation. In addition, participants were asked to work on an
attention task which they were told would impact their participa-
tion in the study, encouraging the participants to perform well on
the task. By using a laboratory setting, we were able to control
where the participant and confederates sat, the number of people
in the room, and outside distractors. The laboratory setting also
allowed us to test cognitive effects of overhearing the cell phone
conversation.
In our experimental paradigm, participants were led to believe
they were participating in a study examining the relationship
between anagrams and reading comprehension. While the
participant was completing an anagram task, the researcher left
the room under the pretext of needing to make more copies. The
participant overheard confederate(s) engaged in either a one-sided
or two-sided conversation while the researcher was out of the
room. After the researcher returned, she explained that the
purpose of the study was to see how a conversation affected a
person. The participant took a recognition test, assessing their
memory for words from the conversation, and completed a
questionnaire regarding the distractibility of the conversation.
Based on abundant experimental and survey research showing the
distracting nature of cell phones, we predicted one-sided
conversations would be more distracting than two-sided conver-
sations because of the unpredictable nature of a one-sided
conversation. It was hypothesized that participants exposed to
the one-sided conversation would be more distracted by it and
would thus make more mistakes on the anagram task, but perform
better on the memory task.
Methods
Ethics Statement
This study and consent process received approval from the
University of San Diego’s Institutional Review Board. Participants
provided their written consent to participate in this study. All
participants were 18 years of age or older.
Participants
Participants were 164 undergraduate students taking an
Introduction to Psychology class at the University of San Diego.
Fifteen participants guessed the purpose of the study and their data
was not analyzed. Of the remaining 149 participants, 110 were
female and 39 were male, with a mean age of 18.48 years
(SD = .778, range = 18–21; one participant declined to list age).
The ethnicities of the 149 participants were: 117 Caucasian, 15
Asian-American, 14 Hispanic, and 2 African American, and 1
Pacific Islander. Participants were recruited from the Psychology
Department’s participant pool. Introductory Psychology students
were required to either earn five research participation credits or
perform alternative assignments. This study counted for two
credits.
Materials
Demographics. The demographics contained questions
about age, gender, ethnicity, and primary spoken language. It
also contained four questions about reading interests and word
puzzles to maintain the perception that the study was about the
relationship between anagram performance and reading compre-
hension.
Anagrams. Anagrams were used as an attention measure
since the participant attempted to complete them while the
conversation was taking place. Two types of scrambled words were
used: easy anagrams, where first and last letters were in the correct
position so that only middle letters are scrambled (e.g., hosue = -
house), and hard anagrams, where all of the letters were scrambled
(e.g. suohe = house). This procedure was based on the methodol-
ogy of Foley, Foley, Wilder, and Rusch [22]. The two levels of
anagram difficulty have resulted in measureable differences in
performance, such as in the time it takes to solve easy versus hard
anagrams [23]. There were 15 easy anagrams and 15 hard
anagrams. Words varied in length from 4–7 letters. The easy and
hard categories had equal numbers of 4–7 letter words. In addition
the average meaningfulness (easy = 6.66, hard = 6.61), imageability
(easy = 6.18, hard = 6.36) and concreteness (easy = 6.08,
hard = 6.46) were similar across the easy and hard categories
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 2 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
according to the word ratings from Paivio, Yuille, and Madigan
[24] and Fear [25].
Conversation. The conversation was a scripted conversation
that lasted approximately seven minutes and covered three topics:
a birthday party for dad, shopping for furniture, and meeting a
date at the shopping mall. A one-sided conversation was
conducted by a confederate talking on the cell phone; whereas
two-sided conversations were carried out by two confederates
speaking to each other. Confederates read the conversation off of a
computer screen during the study. Each speaker said 506 words.
Words that were tested on the recognition test only came from the
conversation uttered by the speaker seated next to the participant.
In both the one-sided and two-sided conversations, the confeder-
ate sitting next to the participant read from the same role in the
script. None of the words on the recognition tests were repeated by
the second speaker so that the participant only heard the
recognition words one time, even if he/she overheard a two-sided
conversation.
Manipulation Check. Participants answered the following
three questions to determine if they knew the purpose of the study:
‘‘What do you think the purpose of this study is?’’, ‘‘Do you have
any questions about this study?’’ and ‘‘Do you think there is any
other purpose to the study? If yes, please describe fully.’’
Recognition Test. A recognition test was used to assess
memory. This test took place approximately four minutes after the
conversation ended. It consisted of seventy words from five
categories. Participants were tested on their ability to discriminate
(1) actual words from the conversation, (2) related words (same
category as actual words), but not part of the conversation, and (3)
other categories with varying relatedness to the conversation topic.
Relatedness to the conversation topic was operationally defined as
the number of words from that category that appeared in the
conversation. For example, no words from the ‘beach’ category
occurred during the conversation. In contrast, 10 words, such as
‘‘potatoes,’’ that fit into the food category appeared in the
conversation. There were also six conversation words in the
‘school’ category and zero conversation words in the ‘household
items’ category. None of these conversation food words appeared
in the recognition test; for example ‘‘potatoes’’ did not appear in
the recognition test, but a new word ‘‘salad’’ did. Mistakes were
tracked to see if participants were more likely to make false alarms
for food words as opposed to beach words. This procedure was
meant to assess not only how many conversation words filter into
the participant’s memory, but also what type of content was
‘picked up’ by the participant. During the recognition test, one
word was shown on the computer screen at a time, and
participants were asked to determine whether each word was
part of the conversation they had just heard. To respond, the
participant was asked to choose their confidence level on a scale of
1–6, from ‘‘definitely not’’ to ‘‘definitely yes.’’ Responses included:
definitely not, probably not, maybe not, maybe yes, probably yes,
and definitely yes.
Distractibility Scale. A seven item self-report questionnaire
was administered on the distracting nature of the scripted
conversation. The questionnaire was adapted from Monk et al.
[19] and asked participants to rate how they felt while the
conversation was taking place. It included the following state-
ments: ‘‘I was surprised that the conversation was going on’’, ‘‘The
conversation was noticeable’’, ‘‘I found myself listening to the
conversation’’, ‘‘Did you believe that the conversation was real?’’,
‘‘The conversation was distracting’’, ‘‘I found the volume of the
conversation annoying’’, and ‘‘I found the content of the
conversation annoying.’’ The answers were on a seven point
Likert scale which ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree.
Post-Experiment Interview. At the end of the study,
participants were asked about their thoughts regarding the study
(‘‘what were you thinking/feeling/considering doing?’’), whether
they were able to look at all the anagrams, and whether they were
able to hear both sides of the one-sided conversation. They were
also asked whether they had heard talk on campus about this
particular study and were asked again about the purpose of the
study.
Procedure
Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three conditions:
(1) two-sided conversation participant heard both sides of a
conversation taking place between two confederates.
(2) one-sided/cell phone conversation participant heard one
side of a cell phone conversation taking place between a
confederate and the researcher who was not in the room.
(3) one-sided conversation with silent confederate participant
heard one side of a cell phone conversation, but there was also
a second, silent, confederate present. This condition was used
to determine if any differences between condition 1 and 2
were due to the different number of people in the room. It was
thought that if the silent confederate appeared to ignore the
conversation, the participant might also, making the results
more similar to a two-sided conversation.
Participants were led to believe they were participating in a
study examining the relationship between the ability to unscram-
ble anagrams and reading comprehension. Trained confederates
also appeared to be participating in the same study for research
credits. For the two-sided conversation (Condition 1) and the one-
sided conversation with silent confederate (Condition 3), two
confederates showed up for the study; while one confederate
showed up for the one-sided conversation (Condition 2). One
participant was tested at a time.
The researcher indicated where each participant should sit. The
room in which the experiment took place had three desks adjacent
to each other, each with its own desktop computer. The desks were
separated by a two-inch wide partial partition that extended
approximately three feet from the wall and was five and a half feet
high; the partition extended seven inches past the edge of the
desks. The participant sat closest to the door and the confederates
sat in the seats next to the participant, separated by the partition.
The participant and the confederates always sat in the same seats
so that the participant sat next to the confederate saying the half of
the conversation that all participants would hear. This seating
arrangement minimized differences between the volume of the
words overheard that would be later tested on in the recognition
task.
After seating the participant and confederate(s) and adminis-
tering the informed consent, the researcher described the study as
investigating the relationship between anagrams and reading
comprehension. The researcher explained that the results of the
anagram task would be used to determine which group each
participant would be placed in for the reading comprehension
portion of the study, thus imparting some consequences on the
participant’s anagram performance. The researcher gave the
participant a copy of the anagrams to unscramble. The researcher
then pretended to realize that the other anagram copies were bad
copies in which half the anagrams were not printed. The
researcher showed the participant and confederate(s) the bad
copies, apologized, and explained that he/she needed to leave the
room to make more copies of the anagrams for the other subject(s)
(the confederate(s)). The researcher instructed the participant to go
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 3 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
ahead and complete the anagrams since they were only part of the
‘pre-experiment’, and that the researcher would pick up the
participant’s anagram answer sheet upon the researcher’s return.
After the researcher left the room, the confederate either received
a phone call from the researcher or engaged in a conversation with
the other confederate. For one-sided conversations, the cell phone
was placed out of view on the desk and was answered after the first
vibration. During the one-sided conversations, the confederate sat
next to the participant and read from the script. For the two-sided
conversations, only the confederate sitting next to the participant
said the words that were later included in the recognition task.
Upon the researcher’s return, the participant was given the
manipulation check in which they were asked to write down what
they thought was the purpose of the study. The confederates were
then excused and the participant was given a surprise recognition
memory test on the computer using MediaLab (Empirisoft).
Finally, the participant was asked a series of questions regarding
the distracting nature of the conversation (whether it was loud,
annoying, believable, etc.) on a Likert scale of 1–7, and were
interviewed about their thoughts regarding the study. The
participant was then debriefed, asked to maintain confidentiality
regarding the study, and thanked for their time.
Results
Participants’ data were not analyzed if the participant guessed
the purpose of the study (n = 15). This left 60 participants who
overheard the two-sided conversation, 56 who overheard the one-
sided conversation, and 33 participants who overheard the one-
sided conversation with a silent confederate. A three-way analysis
of variance (ANOVA) showed that the one-sided and one-sided
with silent confederate conditions were not significantly different
from each other on either the attention or memory tests (see
Recognition test and d’) so these two conditions were combined,
leaving 60 participants who overheard the two-sided conversation,
and 89 who overheard the one-sided conversation. A Chi-square
test comparing participants in the three conditions revealed that
participants in the three conditions were not significantly different
in regards to gender, ethnicity, and primary spoken language
(Table 1) or age (Table 2). A second Chi-square test comparing
participants in the combined one-sided group versus the two-sided
group also showed that participants were not significantly different
in their demographic characteristics (Tables 3 and 4).
Distractibility Scale
A Cronbach’s alpha was used to determine whether the seven
statements of the distractibility scale were measuring the same
concept. The distractibility scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of.752.
The item measuring the believability of the conversation had a
corrected item-total correlation of.072. Since the believability was
not related to the distractibility of the conversation, it was dropped
from further distractibility analysis. Believability was significantly
different between groups (one-sided, M = 6.31, SD = 1.21; two-
sided, M = 5.71, SD = 1.93; F(1) = 5.102; p = .025. However,
further analyses revealed that believability was not related to
performance on either the anagram or recognition task, or to any
performance differences between the one-sided and two-sided
conditions. The Cronbach’s alpha for the remaining six questions
on the distractibility scale was.796.
An overall distractibility score was calculated for each partic-
ipant by summing each participant’s responses to the remaining
six items of the distractibility scale. An independent sample t-test
indicated participants exposed to the one-sided conversation
reported more distraction (M = 28.517, SD = 5.872) than those
exposed to the two-sided conversation (M = 22.700, SD = 7.797;
t(102.682) = 4.915, p ,.001). A comparison between conditions
indicated that the distractibility scale did not meet Levene’s test of
Table 1. Demographic information for participants in the
one-sided, two-sided, and one-sided with silent confederate
conditions.
Conversation Type
one-sided two-sided one-sided silent
N%N%N %
Gender Female 45 80.4 43 71.7 22 66.7
Male 11 19.6 17 28.3 11 33.3
Ethnicity White 46 82.1 47 78.3 24 72.7
Hispanic 4 7.1 6 10.0 4 12.1
Asian 5 8.9 6 10.0 4 12.1
Pacific
Islander
1 1.8 0 0.0 0 0.0
African
American
0 0.0 1 1.7 1 3.0
First
Language
English 52 92.9 54 9.0 29 87.9
Other 4 7.1 6 1.0 4 12.1
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.t001
Table 2. Average age for participants in the one-sided, two-
sided, and one-sided with silent confederate conditions.
Conversation Type
one-sided two-sided one-sided silent
m (SD) m (SD) m (SD)
Age 18.4 (0.71) 18.5 (0.79) 18.6 (0.87)
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.t002
Table 3. Demographic information for participants in the
one-sided and two-sided conditions.
Conversation Type
one-sided two-sided
N%N%
Gender Female 67 75.3 43 71.7
Male 22 24.7 17 28.3
Ethnicity White 70 78.7 47 78.3
Hispanic 8 9.0 6 10.0
Asian 9 12.5 6 10.0
Pacific Islander 1 1.4 0 0.0
African American 1 1.4 1 1.7
First Language English 81 91.0 54 9.0
Other 8 9.0 6 1.0
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.t003
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 4 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
equality of variances (F = 5.566 and p = .020) and therefore, we
adjusted the degrees of freedom from 147 to 102.682.
A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) compared the
responses between conditions to the individual questions on the
distractibility scale (Figure 1). Participants who overheard the one-
sided conversation were more surprised that the conversation took
place (p,.0001), and rated the conversation as more noticeable
(p,.0001), and distracting (p = .037) than those who overheard the
two-sided conversation. They were also more likely to rate the
content (p = .020) and volume (p = .005) of the conversation as
annoying.
Anagram Data
The number of anagrams that were solved correctly and
incorrectly was tallied. Incorrect anagrams included responses in
which participants crossed out or erased their answers as well as
wrong answers. Paired-samples t-test revealed that participants
correctly solved more easy anagrams (M = 12.557, SD = 2.613)
than hard anagrams (M = 5.826, SD = 3.106; t(148) = 26.150,
p,.001; Figure 2). There was a significant difference between the
total number of anagrams that females (M = 19.000, SD: 4.195)
and males (M = 16.641, SD: 5.932) correctly solved;
t(52.10) = 2 2.289, p = .026 (Figure 3). The number of anagrams
solved correctly did not meet Levene’s test of equality of variances
(F = 6.854 and p = .01) and therefore, we adjusted the degrees of
freedom from 147 to 52.10. However, there were no significant
differences between the two-sided and one-sided conversations in
regards to number of anagrams solved correctly or incorrectly. In
addition, the number of easy versus hard anagrams that were
solved correctly also did not vary by condition (one-sided easy/
hard anagrams: 12.43/6.11; two-sided easy/hard: 12.75/5.4) as
revealed by a 262 ANOVA (anagram type6conversation type).
Recognition Test and d’
For the recognition task, participants’ responses were measured
in terms of d’, a formula that takes into account each participant’s
ability to correctly discriminate stimuli amongst novel distractors
[26]. d’ takes into account ‘‘hits’’ (correct identifications of
repeated words), ‘‘misses’’ (failures to identify repeated words),
‘‘correct rejections’’ (correct identifications of novel words), and
‘‘false alarms’’ (mistakenly identifying a novel word as a repeated
Table 4. Average age for participants in the one-sided and
two-sided conditions.
Conversation Type
one-sided two-sided
m (SD) m (SD)
Age 18.5 (0.77) 18.5 (.79)
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.t004
Figure 1. Average responses to distractibility scale. Participants’ averaged responses to statements on the distractibility scale. For example,
responses to the statement ‘‘While the conversation was taking place, the conversation was noticeable’’ could range from 1, ‘‘not at all noticeable,’’ to
7, ‘‘very noticeable.’’ A response of 4 indicated ‘‘neither noticeable nor not noticeable.’’
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.g001
Figure 2. Average number of correct easy and hard anagrams.
The average number of easy and hard anagrams that each participant
completed correctly, regardless of which conversation they overheard.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.g002
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 5 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
word). The formula is d’ = Z(hit rate) 2 Z(false alarm rate). Hit
rate = hits/(hits+misses). False alarm rate = false alarms/(false
alarms+correct rejections). In cases where the response rate was
0, the response was converted to 1/(2N) where N = the number of
trials [27].
A three way ANOVA comparison of the d’ for the on-sided
condition versus the two-sided condition and one-sided condition
with silent confederate revealed a main effect for conversation
type. (one-sided conversation, M = .65, SD = .56; two-sided
conversation, M = .31 SD = .48; and one-sided with silent confed-
erate, M = .57, SD = .69; F(2) = 5.487, p = .005). Post hoc tests
revealed that one-sided condition vs. one-sided with silent
confederate condition were not significantly different from each
other, p = .521. However, the one-sided condition was significantly
different from the two-sided condition, p = .002, and the one-sided
condition with silent confederate was also significantly different
from the two sided condition, p = .038. Since the one-sided
condition and the one-sided condition with silent confederate were
not significantly different from each other but both of these groups
were significantly different from the two-sided group, we collapsed
the data for the one-sided group with the one-sided with silent
confederate group.
There was a significant effect of conversation type on d’
(Figure 4). An independent samples t-test revealed that participants
exposed to a one-sided conversation had a larger d’ score on the
recognition test (M = .616, SD = .608) than those who overheard a
two-sided conversation (M = .311, SD = .481); t(147) = 3.256,
p = .001. We initially restricted data analysis to only those
participants who rated the conversation as moderately or highly
believable (believability . = 4, then believability . = 5, etc), but
the significance values on the recognition test did not change to
any notable degree. A univariate ANOVA revealed no interaction
between gender and conversation type. We analyzed the
individual components that are factored into d’ and found no
significant differences between conversation types on false alarms
made to the five word categories on the recognition task. However,
participants who overheard the one-sided conversation correctly
identified more words from the conversation, i.e. made more
‘‘hits’’, (M = 4.47, SD = 2.216) than those who overheard the two-
sided conversation (M = 3.75, SD = 1.819); t(147) = 2.092,
p = .038.
Recognition Test and Confidence Ratings
The confidence levels for different types of responses were
compared. Participants were more confident in their correct
rejections (M = 2.058, SD = .428) than their misses (M = 1.960,
SD = .545; paired samples t-test; t(148) = 2.993; p = .003;
Figure 5A), but there were no significant differences between
one-sided and two-sided conditions in regards to correct rejections
or misses. In contrast, participants who overheard the one-sided
conversation were more significantly more confidant in their ‘hits’
(M = 1.877, SD = .545) than participants who overheard the two-
sided conversation (M = 1.687, SD = .495; t(142) = 2.13, p = .035;
Figure 5B).
The accuracy for each type of confidence rating was also
calculated: 1) definitely (definitely yes and definitely not), 2)
probably (probably yes and probably not), and 3) maybe (maybe
yes and maybe not)(Figure 5C1 and 5C2). A multivariate anova
(MANOVA) revealed a significant difference for accuracy of
‘‘probably’’ ratings of words from the conversation (one-sided
conversation: M = 46.18, SD = 33.94; two-sided conversation:
M = 31.96, SD = 32.14; F(1) = 4.649, p = .033).
Discussion
We hypothesized that the self-report questionnaire, anagram
task, and recognition task would reveal that participants who
overheard the one-sided conversation would be more distracted
than participants who overheard the two-sided conversation. As
predicted, participants exposed to the one-sided conversation did
report being more distracted by the conversation than those who
overheard the two-sided conversation. In addition, those who
overheard the one-sided conversation had a more accurate
performance on the recognition task, and were more confident
and accurate in their responses to words from the conversation.
Although participants completed more easy than hard anagrams,
and females performed better on the anagram task than males,
there were no significant differences between the different
conversation conditions on the anagram task.
Figure 3. Average number of correct and incorrect anagram
responses by gender. The average number of easy and hard
anagrams that males and females answered correctly, regardless of
which conversation they overheard.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.g003
Figure 4. Average d’ for each conversation type. The average d’
score for participants who overheard a one-sided versus two-sided
conversation.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.g004
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 6 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
Several components of the distractibility questionnaire were
significantly different between the two conditions. Participants
who overheard the one-sided conversation rated the conversation
as more noticeable, and distracting. They were also more surprised
that the conversation took place than participants in the two-sided
condition. Participants who overheard the one-sided conversation
were also more likely to rate the content and volume of the
conversation as annoying than those who overheard the two-sided
conversation. These results are in agreement with those of Monk
et al. [19,20]. Their participants rated the one-sided conversation
as more noticeable and intrusive, and were more likely to find
themselves listening to it. One important note is that Monk’s
participants found the volume more annoying even though the
researchers controlled for volume level of the conversation [19].
The annoyance that participants who overheard the one-sided
conversation felt is consistent with surveys that have shown people
are annoyed by other’s cell phone use in public [5,18]. This
annoyance may be caused by the ‘‘blurring of the distinction
between the public and the private sphere’’ [28]. For example,
people typically have personal, not business, conversations while
they use cell phones in public [2]. Bystanders who are exposed to
these personal conversations may not have much control over the
situation, thereby increasing their levels of annoyance and
frustration. Research has shown that bystanders in situations
where they are not free to leave (for example, waiting for or using
public transportation) often find cell phone conversations annoy-
ing [18,19]. Other research investigating the effects of lack of
control have shown that lack of perceived control can, in turn, lead
to an increase in stress responses [29,30].
In regards to results of the anagram task, females have been
shown to perform better than males on anagrams tasks [31], and
it’s not surprising that participants would complete more easy than
hard anagrams. However, we did expect to see a difference in
anagram performance between conditions and did not. Emberson
et al. [21] observed significant differences between the silent
condition and one-sided conversation, but not between the two-
sided conversation and silence, on their choice reaction and motor
tracking tasks. It is interesting to note that Emberson et al. did not
report any differences across attentional measures (verbal or
motor) between their one-sided and two-sided groups. Since
previous research did not reveal differences between conversation
types on their primary attention tasks, we concluded that perhaps
a different task that allows more attentional resources to be paid to
distractors might reveal attentional differences. Lavie [32] has
suggested, with support from many studies [33], that distractors
have a larger impact on an easy task versus a harder task. The
reason is that less attentional resources are used in the easy task,
leaving participants free to be distracted by irrelevant stimuli. Not
only does task load affect performance on the primary task, but it
also affects memory for the irrelevant distractors [34]. Memory for
unattended distractors is greater on tasks with low loads. We had
equal number of easy and hard anagrams. In the easy anagrams,
the first and last letters were in the correct position so that the
middle letters were scrambled; in the hard anagrams, all of the
letters were scrambled. Perhaps a future study using a greater
number of easy anagrams would reveal differences between
conditions on our paradigm.
In this study, participants who overheard the one-sided
conversation performed better on the recognition memory task
than those who overheard the two-sided conversation, indicating
that they remembered more words that were said in the
conversation. The one-sided condition’s better memory for the
conversation occurred despite an experimental design in which
participants were not asked to focus on the stimuli of interest. In
memory studies, the researcher typically knows that the partici-
pants attended to the stimuli; for example, the participant is asked
to study the stimuli or there is only one set of stimuli (e.g., [35]).
Even in covert memory studies, participants are often asked to rate
the stimulus on a particular dimension (e.g., [36]). However, in this
study, whether or not participants paid attention to the stimulus
was a decision that was left to each participant. Many participants
said that they tried to ignore the stimulus. This type of design likely
minimized differences between the two conversation conditions.
Thus, our paradigm may have attenuated overall memory
Figure 5. Re cogn itio n Test a nd Confidenc e Ratings. 5A.
Confidence levels for correct rejections and misses, regardless of which
conversation participants overheard. 5B. Confidence levels for hits for
participants who overheard a one-sided versus two-sided conversation.
5C. The accuracy of different types of confidence ratings (definitely,
probably, maybe) on the recognition test. Responses are shown for
participants who overheard one-sided versus two-sided conversations.
5C1. Accuracy of responses to repeated words. 5C2. Accuracy of
responses to novel words.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058579.g005
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 7 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
performance, yet exposed unique, intrusive, "attention grabbing"
aspects of a one-sided cell phone conversation.
Many studies have examined performance regarding unattend-
ed stimuli (e.g., [37,38,39]). The responses to the unattended
conversation in this study were stronger than those in some studies
[34,39,40]. However, it is difficult to compare our results with
these studies since their stimuli were very brief, 170 to 500 ms, and
participants were instructed to attend to the target stimuli and
ignore distractors. Another factor that may have improved
performance in our study is that the stimuli were in two different
modalities, visually-presented anagrams and auditory distractors.
According to multiple resources theory [41], performance on tasks
with simultaneously-presented stimuli from multiple modalities
shows less decrement than competing stimuli from the same
modality. This occurs because the multimodal stimuli are using
different attentional resources, and thus are not competing for the
same resources. It could be that the visually-presented anagrams
did not use the same attentional resources as the conversation and
thus made it easier for participants to process the conversation.
Perhaps the primary reason we found differences in d’ perfor-
mance is that participants chose to attend to the conversation
despite the researcher telling them that their performance on the
anagram task would impact their participation in the rest of the
study and that their answer sheet would be picked up upon the
researcher’s return. Lavie notes that this may also be a factor in
studies which do explicitly ask their participants to ignore
irrelevant stimuli: ‘‘According to the load model, the allocation
of attention is an automatic process in the sense that it cannot be
simply withheld at will because of the instruction to ignore task-
irrelevant objects’’ [34]. In our study, every participant attempted
the anagrams, and many participants appeared to pay some
attention to the conversation during the anagram task based on
their performance on the memory task. This study adds to the
body of research by suggesting that overhearing a cell phone
conversation competes for attentional resources that may have
otherwise been devoted to other tasks.
There were also significant differences between conditions in the
accuracy of high confidence responses on the recognition test.
Participants in the one-sided condition were more likely to be
correct when they said that words were ‘‘probably’’ part of (or not
part of) the conversation. When participants were unsure about a
word’s presence in the conversation and chose ‘‘maybe’’, there
were no significant differences between groups. Hoffman et al.
[38] have suggested that memory studies may have missed the
retention of unattended stimuli. They argue that the retention of
unattended stimuli is detectable if the confidence ratings for ‘‘no’’
responses (i.e., the participant says the word is not a repeated
word) are studied. They found that ‘‘no’’ responses to unattended
stimuli (misses) were less confident compared to ‘‘no’’ responses to
novel stimuli (correct rejections). Our comparison of confidence
levels for misses and correct rejections (mistakes versus correct
responses) revealed results similar to Hoffman et al.; participants
were more confident in their correct rejections than their misses.
In addition, participants who overheard the one-sided conversa-
tion were more confident in their ‘hits’ and their moderately high
(‘probably’) confidence responses were more accurate than those
in the two-sided condition. The stronger confidence ratings and
accuracy of the one-sided group could be due to some of the
factors that may have improved d’ performance in our study:
longer stimuli, different stimuli modalities, and participants
choosing to pay attention to the conversation.
The main strength of the current study is its realistic design.
Participants were led to believe that the conversation was not part
of the study. Similar to cell phone conversations in natural settings,
the conversations in our study had an element of surprise and
bystanders to the conversation decided themselves whether or not
to attend to the conversation. When people are distracted by a
conversation while working on a task, they are not usually warned
a conversation will take place. Moreover, if participants had been
asked to ignore the conversation, it might lessen the generaliz-
ability of the study. The current study is also unique in several
other regards. First, the participants heard the conversation only
one time as people would in naturalistic settings. Second,
participants were tested on two cognitive abilities, attention to
the anagram task during the conversation and memory for the
conversation after it ended.
A possible limitation of this study may be the difference in
number of words overheard in the two types of conversations.
Compared to the two-sided conversation, those who overheard the
one-sided conversation heard only half of the words. It’s possible
that participants who overheard the one-sided conversation
performed better on the recognition task because of experimental
confounds. Participants in the one-sided condition heard less noise
and had less information to remember, and possibly the
confederates spoke louder during the one-sided conversation than
the two-sided conversation. We did not control for the difference
in sound and word count which may have contributed to the
differences in distraction. Nevertheless, in Emberson et al. [21],
the participants in the one-sided conversation performed worse
than those in the two-sided conversation when both groups were
compared to the silent condition. The poorer performance of their
one-sided group occurred even though this group was exposed to
42% less words and noise. They attributed the poor performance
of the one-sided group to the unpredictability of the one-sided
conversation’s content, and not the difference in amount of speech
overheard. Emberson et al.’s [21] hypothesis was supported by
results in a follow-up study in which performance did not differ
between groups when conversation content was filtered out, even
though the two-sided group was exposed to almost twice as much
noise. Compared to predictable noises, unpredictable noises
appear to be better distractors because they caused slower reaction
times. However, when participants were given instructions to pay
attention to the sounds, all participants had slower reaction times.
Participants were able to tune out the predictable better than they
could random sounds [42]. In our study, the one-sided cell phone
conversation, with the missing content, could be described as
unpredictable. This supports previous research findings that one-
sided conversations are less predictable because they are missing
content [21].
Although the volume for each conversation type was not
recorded, attempts were made to ensure that each participant was
tested under similar conditions. For example, in order to reduce
between-subject volume differences, only the confederate sitting
next to the participant said the words that were tested on the
recognition task. While participants in the one-sided conversation
were more likely to rate the volume as annoying, the average
rating for the one-sided conversation was 4.35 on a scale of 1 to 7,
midway between a score of 4, neither distracting nor not
distracting and 5, slightly distracting. Thus it seems that the
volume of the one-sided conversation was only mildly annoying.
It’s possible that participants rated the volume of the one-sided
conversation as more annoying even though there were no volume
differences between the conversations. Prior research has shown
that participants can rate the volume of one-sided conversations as
more annoying even when volume is controlled [19].
Although, we did not record volume levels for the conversations
described in this study, we have completed a follow-up study in
which volume was recorded for 54 participants (n = 27 one-sided;
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 8 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
27 two-sided). The conversation in the follow-up study was not
identical to the one described in this paper, but was centered on
the same list of words and was thus tested with the same
recognition test. Although, there were no significant differences in
volume between the two groups (one-sided volume = 55.09dB;
two-sided volume = 54.38dB), the d’ for the 1 one-sided conver-
sation was significantly greater than the d’ for the two sided
conversation (p = .028). Since both studies were conducted using
the same procedure in the same room, and d’ was significantly
different despite equal volume levels in the follow-up study, we
have reason to think volume probably played a negligible role in
this study.
It is likely that cell phone conversations would impact a
bystander’s cognition. Monk and colleagues [20] theorize that
people have a tendency to want to complete information that is
missing in order to make it understandable. Similarly, Hughes,
Vachon, and Jones [43] believe that people attempt to process and
integrate peripheral information (even if it’s to be ignored), and
when it is not possible to process and integrate, attention is
captured. Thus, it is expected that a one-sided cellphone
conversation would negatively impact a bystander’s cognitive
processing. Emberson and colleagues [21] demonstrated that
unpredictable content, but not unpredictable noise, negatively
affect performance. They also explored whether errors were due to
participants’ attempts to fill in the missing conversation during the
silent periods of the one-sided conversation. Participants made
more errors when the one-sided speaker began speaking after a
period of silence, in comparison with periods of silence and two-
sided conversations. Emberson et al [21] reason that bystanders to
cell phone conversation, unlike those exposed to dialogues, do not
know what turn the conversation will take when the cellphone user
speaks again after a silence. Attempting to process the conversa-
tion without the context provided by the prior speaker increases
the cognitive processing that a bystander must use.
This study is the first to have observed cognitive effects of cell
phone conversations on bystanders in a realistic context.
Participants rated the cell phone conversation as more distracting
and were more likely to remember content from the conversation,
even though they were working on another task and were unaware
that the conversation was part of the study. These results have
implications for workplace environments, transportation hubs and
other public areas. Future studies should explore how attention
and cognitive effects of cell phone use vary as a function of
conversation volume and content. Additionally, it will be
important to determine what types of tasks are subject to
performance impairments by overheard cell phone conversations.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank A. Wray and A. Koenig for feedback on
experimental design, C. N. Smith for advice on d’, N. Goldschmeid for
statistical advice, and P. W. Vanderklish for insightful feedback on the
manuscript. The authors also wish to than k a large group of
undergraduates for help in designing and administering the study, and
also analyzing data.
Author Contributions
Edited manuscript: MTG. Conceived and designed the experiments:
VVG. Performed the experiments: VVG MTG RSV. Analyzed the data:
RSV MTG VVG. Wrote the paper: VVG RSV.
References
1. Association CTW (2012) CTIA-The Wireless Associa tionH Semi-Annual Survey
Shows Significant Demand by Americans for Wireless Broadband. http://www.
ctia.org/media/press/body.cfm/prid/2171.
2. Lenhart A (2010) Cell phones and American adults. Pew Research Center.
Washington, DC. http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2010/Cell-Phones-
and-American-Adults/Overview.aspx.
3. Ingenio & Harris Interactive (2007) That’s amore: US callers demonstrate new
attitudes towards cell usage.
4. Vincent J (2006) Emotional attachment and mobile phones. Philos Technol 19:
39–44.
5. Rainie L, Keeter S (2006) How Americans use their cell phones. Pew Internet
Project Data Memo. http://www.pewinternet.org/Reports/2006/Americans-
and-their-cell-phones/1-Data-Memo-Findings.aspx.
6. Kolsaker A, Drakatos N (2009) Mobile advertising: The influence of emotional
attachment to mobile devices on consumer receptiveness. J of Marketing
Communications 15: 267–280.
7. McKnight AJ, McKnight AS (1993) The effect of cellular phone use upon driver
attention. Accid Anal Prev 25: 259–265.
8. Strayer DL, Johnston WA (2001) Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of
simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychol Sci 12: 462–
466.
9. Redelmeier DA, Tibshirani RJ (1997) Association between cellular-telephone
calls and motor vehicle collisions. N Engl J Med 336: 453–458.
10. Strayer DL, Drews FA, Crouch DJ (2006) A comparison of the cell phone driver
and the drunk driver. Hum Fac: The journal of the huma n factors and
ergonomics society 48: 381–391.
11. Cooper JM, Strayer DL (2008) Effects of Simulator Practice and Real-World
Experience on Cell-Phone–Related Driver Distraction. Hum Factors Ergon
Manuf 50: 893–902.
12. Strayer DL, Drews FA, Johnston WA (2003) Cell phone-induced failures of
visual attention during simulated driving. J Exp Psychol Appl 9: 23.
13. Strayer DL, Drews FA (2007) Cell-phone–induced driver distraction. Curr Dir
Psychol Sci 16: 128–131.
14. Hatfield J, Murphy S (2007) The effects of mobile phone use on pedestrian
crossing behaviour at signalised and unsignalised intersections. Accid Anal Prev
39: 197–205.
15. Nasar J, Hecht P, Wener R (2008) Mobile telephones, distracted attention, and
pedestrian safety. Accid Anal Prev 40: 69–75.
16. Stavrinos D, Byington KW, Schwebel DC (2011) Distracted walking: Cell
phones increase injury risk for college pedestrians. J Safety Res.
17. Hyman Jr IE, Boss SM, Wise BM, McKenzie KE, Caggiano JM (2010) Did you
see the unicycling clown? Inattentional blindness while walking and talking on a
cell phone. Appl Cogn Psychol 24: 597–607.
18. Wei R, Leung L (1999) Blurring public and private behaviors in public space:
policy challenges in the use and improper use of the cell phone. Tele Info 16:
11–26.
19. Monk A, Carroll J, Parker S, Blythe M (2004) Why are mobile phones annoying?
Behav Inf Technol 23: 33–41.
20. Monk A, Fellas E, Ley E (2004) Hearing only one side of n ormal and mobile
phone conversations. Behav Inf Technol 23: 301–305.
21. Emberson LL, Lupyan G, Goldstein MH, Spivey MJ (2010) Overheard Cell-
Phone Conversations. Psychol Sci 21: 1383–1388.
22. Foley MA, Foley HJ, Wilder A, Rusch L (1989) Anagram solving: Does effort
have an effect? Mem Cognit 17: 755–758.
23. Mayzner M, Tresselt M (1962) Anagram solution times: A function of word
transition probabilities. J Exp Psychol 63: 510.
24. Paivio A, Yuille JC, Madigan SA (1968) Concreteness, imagery, and
meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. J Exp Psychol 76: 1.
25. Fear WJ (1997) Ratings for Welsh words and their English equivalents. Behav
Res Methods 29: 425–445.
26. Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New
York: Wiley.
27. Macmillan NA, Creelman CD (1991) Detection theory: a user’s guide
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
28. Decuypere M, Masschelein J, Simons M (2011) " Where are you?" Cell phones
and environmental self-understanding amongst students (In Press). Int J Qual
Stud Educ.
29. Hay EL, Diehl M (2010) Reactivity to daily stressors in adulthood: The
importance of stressor type in characterizing risk factors. Psychol Aging 25: 118.
30. Peters ML, Godaert GLR, Ballieux RE, van Vliet M, Willemsen JJ, et al. (1998)
Cardiovascular and endocrine responses to experimental stress: effects of mental
effort and controllability. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23: 1–17.
31. Hyde JS, Linn MC (1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis.
Psychol Bull 104: 53.
32. Lavie N (1995) Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21: 451.
33. Green CS, Bavelier D (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective
attention. Nature 423: 534–537.
34. Lavie N, Lin Z, Zokaei N, Thoma V (2009) The role of perceptual load in object
recognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35: 1346.
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 9 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
35. Smith CN, Wixted JT, Squire LR (2011) The hippocampus supports both
recollection and familiarity when memories are strong. J Neurosci 31: 15693–
15702.
36. Craik FIM, Tulving E (1975) Depth of processing and the retention of words in
episodic memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 104: 268.
37. Forster S, Lavie N (2011) Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and
captivate attention. Psychon Bull Rev18: 1064–1070.
38. Hoffman Y, Bein O, Maril A (2011) Explicit Memory for Unattended Words.
Psychol Sci 22: 1490–1493.
39. Ruz M, Wolmetz ME, Tudela P, McCandliss BD (2005) Two brain pathways
for attended and ignored words. Neuroimage 27: 852–861.
40. Rees G, Russell C, Frith CD, Driver J (1999) Inattentional blindness versus
inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. Science 286: 2504–2507.
41. Wickens CD (1984) Processing resources in attention; Parasuraman R, & Davies,
R, editor. New York: Academic Press. 63–101 p.
42. Matthews KA, Scheier MF, Brunson BI, Carducci B (1980) Attention,
unpredictability, and reports of physical symptoms: Eliminating the benefits of
predictability. J Pers Soc Psychol 38: 525.
43. Hughes RW, Vachon F, Jones DM (2007) Disruption of short-term memory by
changing and deviant sounds: Support for a duplex-mechanism account of
auditory distraction. J Exp Psychol: Learn, Mem, Cog 33: 1050.
Cell Phone vs. Normal Conversation Distraction
PLOS ONE | www.plosone.org 10 March 2013 | Volume 8 | Issue 3 | e58579
... As communication technology progresses, smartphones have become essential for our social life, entertainment, and education (Galván et al., 2013) and have become an indispensable component of people's life (Krasovsky et al., 2017). People used mobile phones for around 2.3 trillion minutes in 2013, with most of the usage scenarios taking place in public places (Galván et al., 2013). ...
... As communication technology progresses, smartphones have become essential for our social life, entertainment, and education (Galván et al., 2013) and have become an indispensable component of people's life (Krasovsky et al., 2017). People used mobile phones for around 2.3 trillion minutes in 2013, with most of the usage scenarios taking place in public places (Galván et al., 2013). In 2018, nine countries worldwide had a mobile phone usage coverage of more than 70% (Kim et al., 2020), with more than 25 million Americans using smartphones (Kim et al., 2020). ...
... On the one hand, the cognitive load in different mobile phone tasks differs (Krasovsky et al., 2017). Compared to dialing, talking, and calling tasks, texting and reading tasks require more coordinated cooperation of sensory organs to complete action tasks efficiently (Neider et al., 2010;Galván et al., 2013). Their occupancy of cognitive resources will be significantly higher than dialing, talking, and calling tasks (Tan et al., 2022), which may be the main reason for the differences in step speed, step length, and step time between different mobile phone use tasks. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Mobile phone usage while performing postural-locomotor tasks is everyday activity across persons of all ages in various environmental contexts and health conditions. However, it is also an important factor contributing to accidents. To lower the risk of pedestrian accidents, this meta-analysis aimed to examine how mobile phones affect pedestrian gait and identify how mobile phone tasks and participant age affect gait differently. Methods: Electronic database searches were performed in The Cochrane Library, PubMed, and Medline. Two examiners evaluated the eligibility and quality of included studies using the Downs and Black checklist. The mean differences (MD) or standardized mean differences (SMD) were calculated for each outcome. Subgroup analyses were used to compare the differential effects of mobile phone task and participant age on gait. Results: Among 22 eligible studies, 592 participants in 10 countries were analyzed in this meta-analysis. The overall meta-analysis showed that using a mobile phone significantly decreased pedestrian gait velocity (SMD = −1.45; 95% CI: −1.66 to −1.24; p < 0.00001; I² = 66%), step length (SMD = −1.01; 95% CI: −1.43 to −0.59; p < 0.00001; I² = 82%), and stride length (SMD = −0.9; 95% CI: −1.19 to −0.60; p < 0.00001; I² = 79%), significantly increased pedestrian step time (SMD = 0.77; 95% CI: 0.45 to 1.08; p < 0.00001; I² = 78%), stride time (SMD = 0.87; 95% CI: 0.69 to 1.06; p < 0.00001; I² = 24%), step width (SMD = 0.79; 95% CI: 0.34 to 1.24; p = 0.0006. I² = 75%), double support time (SMD = 1.09; 95% CI: 0.86 to 1.31; p < 0.00001; I² = 42%), and double support (%gait cycle, %GC) (MD = 2.32; 95% CI: 1.75 to 2.88; p < 0.00001; I² = 26%). Conclusion: In summary, the effects of mobile phone tasks and participant age on gait were inconsistent. Our study found that resource-intensive tasks (texting and reading) significantly reduced gait velocity, and step time; however, small resource-intensive tasks (calling, talking, and dialing) did not affect these outcomes. In contrast to young adults, step length and step time were not affected by mobile phone use in older adults. Tips: Pedestrians should consider using a mobile phone in their daily lives according to the application scenarios (walking environment, the complexity of mobile phone tasks, pedestrians’ task processing abilities, etc.) as appropriate to avoid dangerous accidents. Systematic Review Registration: identifier CRD42022358963.
... Herein, the goal was to present non-repeating speech with rich content and where each talker represents one side of a telephone conversation (i.e., a halfalogue) at a workstation, which is a common scenario in OPOs [21,56]. Studies have shown that such halfalogues are not only perceived as more annoying and distracting compared to hearing both sides of a dialogue [62,63], but also impede performance in cognitive tasks involving semantic processing [56,64,65]. For the speech recordings, four native German talkers (1Female (F), 3 Male (M)) and two native Hindi talkers (1 M, 1F) volunteered. ...
Article
The irrelevant sound effect (ISE) characterizes short-term memory performance impairment during irrelevant sounds relative to quiet. Irrelevant sound presentation in most laboratory-based ISE studies has been rather limited to represent complex scenarios including open-plan offices (OPOs) and not many studies have considered serial recall of heard information. This paper investigates ISE using an auditory-verbal serial recall task, wherein performance was evaluated for relevant factors in simulating OPO acoustics: the irrelevant sounds including the semanticity of speech, reproduction methods over headphones, and room acoustics. Results (Experiments 1 and 2) show that ISE was exhibited in most conditions with anechoic (irrelevant) nonspeech sounds with/without speech, but the effect was substantially higher with meaningful speech compared to foreign speech, suggesting a semantic effect. Performance differences in conditions with diotic and binaural reproductions were not statistically robust, suggesting limited role of spatial separation of sources. In Experiment 3, statistically robust ISE were exhibited for binaural room acoustic conditions with mid-frequency reverberation times, T 30 (s) = 0.4, 0.8, 1.1, suggesting cognitive impairment regardless of sound absorption representative of OPOs. Performance differences in T 30 = 0.4 s relative to T 30 = 0.8 and 1.1 s conditions were statistically robust. This emphasizes the benefits for cognitive performance with increased sound absorption, reinforcing extant room acoustic design recommendations. Performance differences in T 30 = 0.8 s vs. 1.1 s were not statistically robust. Collectively, these results suggest that certain findings from ISE studies with idiosyncratic acoustics may not translate well to complex OPO acoustic environments.
... Studies have shown that such halfalogues, when compared to both sides of a dialogue, are not only perceived as being more annoying and distracting [62,63], but also impede performance in cognitive tasks involving semantic processing [56,64,65]. lips, similar to previous research [12]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The irrelevant sound effect (ISE) characterizes short-term memory performance impairment during irrelevant sounds relative to quiet. Irrelevant sound presentation in most ISE studies has been rather limited to represent complex scenarios including open-plan offices (OPOs) and not many studies have considered serial recall of heard information. This paper investigates ISE using an auditory-verbal serial recall task, wherein performance was evaluated for relevant factors for simulating OPO acoustics: the irrelevant sounds including speech semanticity, reproduction methods over headphones, and room acoustics. Results (Experiments 1 and 2) show that ISE was exhibited in most conditions with anechoic (irrelevant) nonspeech sounds with/without speech, but the effect was substantially higher with meaningful speech compared to foreign speech, suggesting a semantic effect. Performance differences in conditions with diotic and binaural reproductions were not statistically robust, suggesting limited role of spatial separation of sources. In Experiment 3, statistically robust ISE were exhibited for binaural room acoustic conditions with mid-frequency reverberation times, T30 (s) = 0.4, 0.8, 1.1, suggesting cognitive impairment regardless of sound absorption representative of OPOs. Performance differences in T30 = 0.4 s relative to T30 = 0.8 and 1.1 s conditions were statistically robust, but not between the latter two conditions. These results suggest that certain findings from ISE studies with idiosyncratic acoustics may not translate well to complex OPO acoustic environments.
... Research on neural processing of incomplete and partial information made headlines recently in relation to the widely-noted phenomenon of irritation provoked by listening to one-sided cellphone conversations-a significantly more engaging (i.e. annoying) experience than listening to an entire conversation (Galvàn et al. 2013). Our human minds respond to puzzles, for reasons perhaps linked to evolution-an unknown sound could threaten danger. ...
Chapter
In an isolated region of the Canadian Maritimes, Jacques Lecoq’s program for actor training has borne fruit. Les Araignées du boui-boui, a university-community-based theatre troupe, has flourished for thirty-five years under the artistic direction of Lecoq-trained Normand Godin. Following the Lecoq insistence on direct observation of life, Les Araignées create theatre thatresponds creatively to the minority Acadian linguistic and cultural environment. Among Lecoq’s most critical teachings: actors must use their native tongues to elicit the intonations and gestures of their distinctive cultures. So, Les Araignées work in the beautiful but largely discredited minority French dialect of the region. This apparent limitation communicates in the most powerful way possible with the Acadian community, and has not stopped the troupe from successfully touring and winning international awards. For limitation, as Lecoq understood, is an artistic necessity: limitation of movements, objects, sets, paraphenalia, words. A few strokes of the pen, the space between: the spectator's mind “perks” and fills the void. When the play does not command meaning and detail, the spectator becomes a partner to the creation. Today, cognitive neuroscience supports this central artistic insight. Lecoq confirmed that actors mature with patience as they are invited into new territories by someone who has veered off the beaten path; they work together for what is elusive, truth, beauty, perfection. They quest for that equation between the complexity of life and the simplicity of its depiction. This is the story of Les Araignées du boui-boui, who, through Normand Godin, are following the path laid out by Jacques Lecoq. ! —Susan Knutson and Normand Godin, Université Sainte-Anne
... Relevant in this perspective is that in the present study, compared to a relatively controlled experimental environment, many uncontrollable factors may have influenced performance efficiency due to the dynamic nature of the actual office environment [40,41]. On the one hand, factors such as interruptions related to the presence of others [42] and uncontrollable requests for actions from electronic devices (e.g., online activity, telephone calls), might increase task demands [43], which could in turn increase the effects of mental fatigue on performance efficiency. However, on the other hand, work motivation [1] and enhanced autonomy with regard to setting one's own schedule and planning work-breaks if needed [37], might reduce experienced task demands and related levels of mental fatigue during regular office work compared to the lab setting. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental fatigue has repeatedly been associated with decline in task performance in controlled situations, such as the lab, and in less controlled settings, such as the working environment. Given that a large number of factors can influence the course of mental fatigue, it is challenging to objectively and unobtrusively monitor mental fatigue on the work floor. We aimed to provide a proof of principle of a method to monitor mental fatigue in an uncontrolled office environment, and to study how typewriting dynamics change over different time-scales (i.e., time-on-task, time-of-day, day-of-week). To investigate this, typewriting performance of university employees was recorded for 6 consecutive weeks, allowing not only to examine performance speed, but also providing a natural setting to study error correction. We show that markers derived from typewriting are susceptible to changes in behavior related to mental fatigue. In the morning, workers first maintain typing speed during prolonged task performance, which resulted in an increased number of typing errors they had to correct. During the day, they seemed to readjust this strategy, reflected in a decline in both typing speed and accuracy. Additionally, we found that on Mondays and Fridays, workers adopted a strategy that favored typing speed, while on the other days of the week typing accuracy was higher. Although workers are allowed to take breaks, mental fatigue builds up during the day. Day-to-day patterns show no increase in mental fatigue over days, indicating that office workers are able to recover from work-related demands after a working day.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Despite efforts to improve health equity, there is still limited knowledge about the number and characteristics of people with disabilities, particularly the Deaf community. Our aim is to use linguistically and culturally adapted research instruments to measure key health indicators and priorities within the Deaf community from a low- and middle-income country—Colombia, contributing to a better understanding of health inequities. Methods We used data from the Health Survey for Deaf (HSD) and National Quality of Life Survey (NQLS) from Colombia. We included various communication and technology-related indicators—usage of smartphone, modes of interacting with healthcare personnel, along with health indicators—self-perception of health, healthcare quality, hospitalisation and functional difficulties in various domains. ORs were computed to depict the differences in two groups, adjusted for both age and gender, using logistic regressions. Results We included 204 and 877 Deaf participants from HSD and NQLS, respectively. Owning a phone was significantly associated with a better self-perception of health (ORs=2.27, 95% CI 1.63 to 3.17 for NQLS-Deafs; 1.49, 1.43 to 1.54 for NQLS-general population) but also with more functional difficulties corresponding to most domains (all ORs >1). However, for HSD datasets, we found that phone ownership was associated with having significantly less functional difficulties in moving hands (0.34, 0.14 to 0.81) and cognition (0.36, 0.15 to 0.89). Access to professional interpreting services was correlated with increased communication-related functional difficulties (2.02, 1.00 to 4.08), for HSD participants. Better self-perception of health was linked to fewer functional difficulties (all ORs <1), while recent hospitalisation was associated with more functional difficulties (all ORs >1). Conclusions We found that Deaf individuals generally experience poorer health outcomes compared with hearing individuals. To address these disparities, we recommend (1) improving data quality that could lead to targeted responses and monitoring of it and (2) implementing personalised health surveys that account for the Deaf population’s fluency in Spanish and Colombian Sign Language and their specific understanding of health issues.
Article
Cognitive effects of smartphones have become a relevant research area. In two experiments, we investigated the effect of other‐owned phone vibrations on long‐term memory in undergraduate Colombian students. Participants watched an educational video while a nearby experimenter‐owned smartphone emitted vibrations at specific moments. After a few minutes, their memory for video content with and without vibrations was assessed. In Experiment 1, memory showed no significant effect of vibrations, but a non‐significant directional trend suggested potential disruption in memory regarding vibrating notifications. In Experiment 2, we adjusted some parameters and added a metamemory measure. We found a significantly worse memory performance and lower confidence on questions with vibrations. Experiment 2 also included a media‐multitasking measure that showed no relation to memory or metamemory results. Our findings provide new data on the effects of silent notifications on memory and metamemory, highlighting the potential disruption of nearby phones.
Article
Full-text available
“I deemed this novel definitively unfinished on 27 January, 2011”: thus begins the postface of Catalan novelist Jaume Cabré’s Confessions (Jo confesso, 2011). Beyond the totality of the published work, the phrase evokes the infinity of possibility, and hints at the æsthetic and ethical matter of the novel. Despite the narrator Adrià’s painstaking quest for exhaustivity, completion and the last word in the long letter he writes to his beloved, the fictional universe denies him all of the above: in the end which Cabré deals him (not death, but Alzheimer’s disease), in the form of the epistolary monologue whose fate necessarily leaves his hands, and in the uncertainty which surrounds the interpretation of the novel’s end. This unfinished quality is revealed as valuable. It highlights life’s real continuity amid the artificiality of literary endings; it is dialogic, as highlighted by the letter-form, revealing the novel to be open to constant reinvention; finally, it is ethical, standing in opposition to the final solutions whose drive for ends and completions are the heart of darkness and the portrait of evil in the novel.
Article
Background Human walking is a highly automated motor task, however if the individual’s attention is divided, gait can be negatively affected. Although the effect of divided attention has been usually tested with standardised cognitive tasks, the common task of walking while talking on the phone may represent an ecological dual task scenario. Research question What is the effect of divided attention on locomotion when using a mobile phone? Methods Thirty-seven healthy participants were asked to walk while performing different cognitive tasks: counting and spelling backwards, talking on the phone (handset by the ear and hands-free), and texting. As a control, extra postural conditions were tested: holding the phone by the ear (without talking) and carrying the phone as in the texting task. These tasks were compared with normal walking (no other cognitive or postural task). Twenty participants also performed the same tasks with the addition of an obstacle halfway through the walkway. Gait performance was measured using non-invasive inertial sensors. Step time and mediolateral acceleration range were calculated. Results Step time increased when counting (mean ± standard error 0.63 ± 0.02 s, p < 0.001), spelling backwards (0.67 ± 0.03 s, p < 0.001) and texting (0.61 ± 0.02 s, p = 0.005) compared to normal walking (0.56 ± 0.02 s). Compared to normal walking (8.03 ± 0.58 m/s²), mediolateral acceleration decreased when counting (6.43 ± 0.39 m/s², p < 0.001), spelling backwards (6.67 ± 0.44 m/s², p < 0.001), when talking on the phone while holding the phone (7.28 ± 0.48 m/s², p = 0.003), or hands-free (7.28 ± 0.40 m/s², p = 0.004), or texting (6.71 ± 0.50 m/s², p < 0.001). Introducing an obstacle confirmed these results. Significance This study shows that even in young and healthy individuals, gait is affected by divided attention. Furthermore, the results show that common and ecological cognitive tasks, such as phone use, could induce measurable worsening of gait performance. Individuals should be careful when walking and performing other tasks that could distract them, by dividing their attention.
Article
Full-text available
Bystander effect is the phenomenon that describes how, when more people are around, each individual is less likely to intervene. While the bystander effect is an integral part of studying social behaviors and group thinking, the many caveats it presents itself with must be considered. Every situation differs based on location, people, and circumstance, so the idea of the bystander effect is not valid in every scenario, as evidenced by the various counter-examples and contradictory findings researchers have discovered. However, the bystander effect is still very important to study because understanding what encourages/prevents people from helping is critical to decrease the effect of the bystander effect to promote helping behavior. In this paper, we discuss the various factors that affect the prevalence of the bystander effect: perceived physical and social harm to the helper, responsibility diffusion, and perceived helpfulness.
Book
Full-text available
Detection Theory: A User’s Guide is an introduction to one of the most important tools for the analysis of data where choices must be made and performance is not perfect. In these cases, detection theory can transform judgments about subjective experiences, such as perceptions and memories, into quantitative data ready for analysis and modeling. For beginners, the first three chapters introduce measuring detection and discrimination, evaluating decision criteria, and the utility of receiver operating characteristics. Later chapters cover more advanced research paradigms, including: complete tools for application, including flowcharts, tables, and software; student-friendly language; complete coverage of content area, including both one-dimensional and multidimensional models; integrated treatment of threshold and nonparametric approaches; an organized, tutorial-level introduction to multidimensional detection theory; and popular discrimination paradigms presented as applications of multidimensional detection theory. This modern summary of signal detection theory is both a self-contained reference work for users and a readable text for graduate students and researchers learning the material either in courses or on their own.
Article
Full-text available
Nowadays, the cell phone is omnipresent in our society, certainly amongst youngsters. Its presence, and the possibility to be in constant touch, have some profound consequences on our experience (of our selves, of others and the world) and self-understanding. It is important for educators and scholars in the field of education to understand such consequences and develop an awareness of how students relate to the world they inhabit. Starting from the observation that people often want to know the position of the party they call, this explorative study reports on an analysis of text messages of 10 participants and tries to couple this and related questions heuristically to an environmental self-understanding, wherein a particular environmental kind of positioning becomes a major need and/or obsession. Results point to a particular potential dealing with the present and the future. See here: https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/262334
Article
Full-text available
Located 165 studies that reported data on gender differences in verbal ability. The weighted mean effect size was +0.11, indicating a slight female superiority in performance. The difference is so small that we argue that gender differences in verbal ability no longer exist. Analysis of tests requiring different cognitive processes involved in verbal ability yielded no evidence of substantial gender differences in any aspect of processing. Similarly, an analysis of age indicated no striking changes in the magnitude of gender differences at different ages, countering Maccoby and Jacklin's (1974) conclusion that gender differences in verbal ability emerge around age 11 yrs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Conducted 10 experiments to evaluate the notion of "depth of processing" in human memory. Undergraduate Ss were asked questions concerning the physical, phonemic, or semantic characteristics of a long series of words; this initial question phase was followed by an unexpected retention test for the words. It was hypothesized that "deeper" (semantic) questions would take longer to answer and be associated with higher retention of the target words. These ideas were confirmed by the 1st 4 experiments. Exps V-X showed (a) it is the qualitative nature of a word's encoding which determines retention, not processing time as such; and (b) retention of words given positive and negative decisions was equalized when the encoding questions were equally salient or congruous for both types of decision. While "depth" (the qualitative nature of the encoding) serves a useful descriptive purpose, results are better described in terms of the degree of elaboration of the encoded trace. Finally, results have implications for an analysis of learning in terms of its constituent encoding operations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
When are involved in a crash with a motor vehicle, their injuries are typically high if the vehicle is travelling at more than 40 km/h ( , 1981
Article
Our research examined the effects of hands-free cell-phone conversations on simulated driving. We found that even when participants looked directly at objects in the driving environment, they were less likely to create a durable memory of those objects if they were conversing on a cell phone. This pattern was obtained for objects of both high and low relevance, suggesting that very little semantic analysis of the objects occurs outside the restricted focus of attention. Moreover, in-vehicle conversations do not interfere with driving as much as cell-phone conversations do, because drivers are better able to synchronize the processing demands of driving with in-vehicle conversations than with cell-phone conversations. Together, these data support an inattention-blindness interpretation wherein the disruptive effects of cell-phone conversations on driving are due in large part to the diversion of attention from driving to the phone conversation.
Article
We investigated the effects of divided attention during walking. Individuals were classified based on whether they were walking while talking on a cell phone, listening to an MP3 player, walking without any electronics, or walking in a pair. In the first study, we found that cell phone users walked more slowly, changed directions more frequently, and were less likely to acknowledge other people than individuals in the other conditions. In the second study, we found that cell phone users were less likely to notice an unusual activity along their walking route (a unicycling clown). Cell phone usage may cause inattentional blindness even during a simple activity that should require few cognitive resources.
Article
Mobile advertising has grown rapidly over recent years, yet volume is not necessarily a good measure of consumer acceptance. Earlier studies have explored acceptance in terms of content; here we focus on the communication medium, the mobile device. Building upon Vincent's (2006) premise that emotional attachment to mobile phones is a likely key influence on future adoption of new services, we explore whether this holds true for mobile users' receptiveness to mobile advertising. Our research reveals that users are emotionally attached to their mobile devices and that attachment is to a large degree attributable to a sense that the device is an essential part of life with a value that goes beyond simple communication. Those with a strong sense of attachment are more receptive to mobile adverts, however overall users generally find them irritating. It appears that advertisers have hitherto been insufficiently user-centric; responses are as important as the message yet there is a tendency to concentrate on what is being sent rather than how it is received. We conclude that consumer receptiveness might improve by targeting users who are strongly attached their mobile devices.