ArticlePDF Available

Managing Obsessive Thoughts During Brief Exposure: An Experimental Study Comparing Mindfulness-Based Strategies and Distraction in Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In mindful meditation, negative thoughts such as obsessive thoughts are observed simply as mental events that come and go, rather than as accurate reflections of reality. This experimental study tested the efficacy of a mindfulness-based instruction compared to distraction during brief exposure to obsessive thoughts in obsessive–compulsive patients. Thirty patients diagnosed with obsessive–compulsive disorder were asked to listen to their own obsessive thoughts through headphones during three time phases: at baseline, during an experimental condition and during a return to baseline. During the experimental condition, they were instructed to deal with their obsessive thoughts using either a mindfulness-based strategy or a distraction strategy (random allocation). Results showed that a mindfulness-based strategy reduced anxiety and urge to neutralize from first to second baseline, whereas a distraction strategy did not. Data offer initial evidence that using mindfulness-based metaphors during brief exposure with obsessive thoughts may be a useful alternative to distraction.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1 23
Cognitive Therapy and Research
ISSN 0147-5916
Cogn Ther Res
DOI 10.1007/s10608-012-9503-2
Managing Obsessive Thoughts During
Brief Exposure: An Experimental Study
Comparing Mindfulness-Based Strategies
and Distraction in Obsessive–Compulsive
Disorder
Karina Wahl, Jan O.Huelle, Bartosz
Zurowski & Andreas Kordon
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and all
rights are held exclusively by Springer Science
+Business Media New York. This e-offprint is
for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you
wish to self-archive your work, please use the
accepted author’s version for posting to your
own website or your institution’s repository.
You may further deposit the accepted author’s
version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s
request, provided it is not made publicly
available until 12 months after publication.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Managing Obsessive Thoughts During Brief Exposure:
An Experimental Study Comparing Mindfulness-Based Strategies
and Distraction in Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder
Karina Wahl Jan O. Huelle Bartosz Zurowski
Andreas Kordon
Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012
Abstract In mindful meditation, negative thoughts such
as obsessive thoughts are observed simply as mental events
that come and go, rather than as accurate reflections of
reality. This experimental study tested the efficacy of a
mindfulness-based instruction compared to distraction
during brief exposure to obsessive thoughts in obsessive–
compulsive patients. Thirty patients diagnosed with
obsessive–compulsive disorder were asked to listen to their
own obsessive thoughts through headphones during three
time phases: at baseline, during an experimental condition
and during a return to baseline. During the experimental
condition, they were instructed to deal with their obsessive
thoughts using either a mindfulness-based strategy or a
distraction strategy (random allocation). Results showed
that a mindfulness-based strategy reduced anxiety and urge
to neutralize from first to second baseline, whereas a dis-
traction strategy did not. Data offer initial evidence that
using mindfulness-based metaphors during brief exposure
with obsessive thoughts may be a useful alternative to
distraction.
Keywords Obsessive–compulsive disorder Exposure
Mindfulness Distraction Experimental study
Introduction
Exposure with obsessive thoughts using a recorded version
of the thought (‘‘loop tape exposure’’, Salkovskis 1983;
Salkovskis and Westbrook 1989) is the recommended
treatment for patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder
(OCD; Freeston et al. 1997). It can be combined with
in vivo exposure to specific situations if necessary
(Ladouceur et al. 1993; Freeston et al. 1997). Typically, it
is carried out under certain conditions (e.g., with the
instruction not to neutralize, lasting until the anxiety
decreases noticeably) in order to maximize habituation and
incorporation of correcting information (Foa and Kozak
1986), which are supposed to underlie the reduction of
anxiety during exposure.
Data from case and uncontrolled studies (e.g., Abra-
mowitz 2002; Hoogduin et al. 1987) and from a controlled
trial (Freeston et al. 1997) show that particularly patients
without overt compulsions (‘pure obsessionals’) are likely
to benefit from a combined treatment of loop-tape exposure
and cognitive restructuring. However, drop-out rates were
19–22 % in the Hoogduin and Ladouceur studies and the
authors argued that these might be accounted for by the
demanding processes of (loop tape) exposure. This raises
the question whether the conditions of exposure can be
varied so that it becomes more tolerable for patients and
maximizes the chances of anxiety reduction at the same
time.
Variations of exposure and their effects have been
studied in patients diagnosed with overt compulsive rituals
such as compulsive washing or checking. For example, Foa
et al. (1984) found that exposure without deliberate
blocking of rituals reduces anxiety but not the frequency of
compulsive rituals. Grayson et al. (1982,1986) compared
distraction and focused attention during exposure and
K. Wahl (&)
Department of Psychology, University of Hamburg,
Von-Melle-Park 5, 20146 Hamburg, Germany
e-mail: karina.wahl@uni-hamburg.de
J. O. Huelle
Universitaetsklinikum Eppendorf, Martinistr. 52,
20246 Hamburg, Germany
B. Zurowski A. Kordon
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy,
Medical University of Luebeck, Ratzeburger Allee 160,
23538 Lu
¨beck, Germany
123
Cogn Ther Res
DOI 10.1007/s10608-012-9503-2
Author's personal copy
found that both conditions resulted in reduced subjective
and physiological anxiety during one exposure session.
However, habituation under distraction conditions did not
generalize to between session habituation. To our knowl-
edge, variations of loop tape exposure and their effects on
efficacy and tolerability have not been studied systemati-
cally yet.
Recently, it has been suggested that patients diagnosed
with OCD might benefit from mindful awareness during
exposure (Didonna 2009; Fairfax 2008; Hannan and Tolin
2005), thus making exposure less stressful and possibly
decreasing drop-out rates. Kabat-Zinn (1990) described
mindfulness as paying attention in a particular way: on
purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally. A
mindfulness mode of processing thus involves a nonjudg-
mental, accepting observation of an ongoing stream of
internal and external stimuli as they arise. Stimuli include
personal thoughts, also upsetting ones, which are perceived
simply as temporary mental events that come and go, rather
than as accurate reflections of reality. Empirical evidence
from case studies (Patel et al. 2007; Singh et al. 2004;
Wilkinson-Tough et al. 2010), from an analogue study
(Hanstede et al. 2008) and from a controlled trial (Twohig
et al. 2010) shows that mindfulness based interventions can
be effective in individuals bothered by intrusive thoughts
and patients diagnosed with OCD. These studies have
discussed and applied different aspects and interventions of
mindfulness during various stages of therapy.
In our view, a mindfulness-based strategy seems par-
ticularly suitable for the management of obsessive
thoughts during brief exposure. First, bringing deliberate
attention towards the obsessive thoughts might enhance
habituation processes, prolong their effects and generalize
to between session habituation (Grayson et al. 1982,
1986). Second, observing and describing thoughts in a
mindful way might stop individuals from giving in
automatically to thought suppression or neutralization,
thus fostering habituation processes further, as suggested
by Najmi et al. (2009). Third, metaphorical ‘letting go
experiences’ (Hanstede et al. 2008) such as imaging the
obsessive thought pass by like clouds in the sky might
change the short term meaning of the content of the
obsessive thoughts, as less threatening. This could initiate
an accepting attitude. Finally, in the long run, patients
diagnosed with OCD might develop a new relationship to
their obsessive thoughts, relating to them as transient
mental events and not as facts. This seems particularly
important in OCD, since cognitive theories of clinical
obsessions (e.g., Clark and Purdon 1993; Freeston and
Ladouceur 1997; Rachman 1997; Salkovskis 1985) posit
that the ways in which intrusive thoughts are interpreted
differentiates normal intrusions from obsessions. It seems
plausible to assume that the dysfunctional significance of
intrusions could be reduced by mindfulness through
continued practice (Baer 2003).
Mindfulness is a multidimensional construct and has
been integrated into Western clinical psychology with
varying operationalizations and degrees of complexity
(Brown et al. 2007). For this study the mindfulness
instruction was adapted from the ‘thoughts are not facts’
module of the mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for
depression (MBCT; Segal et al. 2002). Participants are
encouraged to view their thoughts as transient mental events
by bringing deliberate awareness towards them and
reminding themselves that thoughts are just thoughts and not
facts. Additionally, participants are instructed to use meta-
phors such as observing their thoughts like clouds moving
across the sky. The study is in line with other research
investigating the effects of an experimentally induced
mindful self-focus on emotional and cognitive processes
(e.g., Broderick 2005; Huffziger and Kuehner 2009; Marcks
and Woods 2005). In contrast to intensive mindfulness
intervention programs, the experimental manipulation of the
study presented here included a short induction period of
selected mindfulness elements only (e.g., an observing,
nonjudgmental state of mind). Importantly, this study does
not claim to capture the complex processes involved in
therapeutic mindfulness interventions.
Two recent studies have examined the effects of varying
strategies (thought suppression, acceptance, and focused
distraction) in the management of naturally occurring
obsessive thoughts. Najmi et al. (2009) investigated the
relative effectiveness of thought suppression, focused dis-
traction and acceptance on distress and frequency associ-
ated with obsessive thoughts in patients diagnosed with
OCD. While an acceptance based strategy reduced distress
significantly, thought suppression resulted in an increase in
distress. No significant changes were found for the focused
distraction strategy. Marcks and Woods (2005) demon-
strated that, in non-clinical individuals, an acceptance-
based strategy in response to personally relevant intrusive
thoughts resulted in a decrease in anxiety levels, whereas
thought suppression did not. Both studies monitored the
occurrence and distress of naturally occurring obsessive
thoughts. Our study extends these results by comparing a
mindfulness-based and a distraction strategy not in response
to naturally occurring obsessive thoughts, but during brief
exposure with obsessive thoughts via headphones. Our main
variables were anxiety and urge to neutralize.
The time of assessing the mindfulness effects is an
important issue. Since mindful awareness encourages
patients diagnosed with OCD to deliberately pay attention
to their obsessive thoughts and associated feelings, it seems
plausible that an immediate rise in anxiety occurs. How-
ever, the rise in anxiety should be followed by a decline at
a later time, when individuals let the thought pass by
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
without appraising it or reacting upon it. If mindfulness
works by enhancing habituation, then a reduction in anxi-
ety should not only be observed within an exposure session
but also during a further exposure session (Grayson et al.
1982,1986). Therefore we chose a delayed time period of
renewed exposure after a short break to assess the main
effects.
The aim of the present experimental study was to
investigate the efficacy of a mindfulness-based strategy
compared to distraction during brief exposure to obsessive
thoughts. It was hypothesized that patients diagnosed with
OCD who had previously engaged in a mindfulness-based
strategy while listening to their own obsessive thoughts
would be less anxious and report a lower urge to neutralize
than those who had previously engaged in distraction.
Secondly, it was expected that the immediate effect of the
mindfulness-based instruction would be a rise in anxiety, as
opposed to a reduction in anxiety in the distraction
condition.
Materials and Methods
Overview
Thirty patients diagnosed with OCD were asked to listen to
their own obsessive thoughts through headphones during
three phases: at baseline, during an experimental condition
and during a return to baseline. During the experimental
condition, they were instructed to deal with their obsessive
thoughts using either a mindfulness-based strategy or a
distraction strategy (random allocation). Dependent vari-
ables were anxiety and urge to neutralize, which were
assessed on visual analog scales at three times points
within the three phases.
Participants
Thirty-six patients diagnosed with OCD were recruited at
an inpatient clinic specialized in cognitive behavioral
treatment of OCD. Patients were assessed by experienced
clinicians using the structured clinical interview for DSM-
IV axis i disorders (SCID-I; First and Gibbon 2004) and
were included if they met the following criteria: (1) primary
diagnosis of OCD, defined by DSM-IV (American Psychi-
atric Association 1994), (2) aged 18–65, (3) could report an
identifiable obsessive thought, (4) confirmed that listening
to their own intrusive thoughts through headphones in the
laboratory would cause them discomfort, (5) were not
acutely suicidal, (6) had no history of substance dependency
or psychotic symptoms, (7) had not already started with
exposure and response prevention interventions and, (8) did
not use any form of counting as a neutralizing ritual. Four of
the recruited patients did not meet inclusion criteria and two
patients (one of each condition) did not complete the
experimental procedure satisfactorily. As became evident
during the course of the experiment, one patient was suf-
fering from a specific animal phobia, which was activated
during the animal movie between assessment phases. The
second participant did not give any reasons for dropping out
of the experiment. Participants were randomly allocated to
the experimental (mindfulness-based coping) or control
(distraction) condition using sampling without replacement.
Table 1presents demographic characteristics and psycho-
pathology measures of the participants who completed the
experiment successfully. There were no significant differ-
ences in any of the participants’ characteristics (all
ps[.05) between the experimental and control condition
group. Additionally, initial anxiety levels (STAI-State;
Spielberger et al. 1983) did not differ between groups. In
each group, seven participants (46.7 %) were acutely
diagnosed with comorbid psychiatric disorders (mindful-
ness condition: five [33.3 %] patients with major depression
and two [13.3 %] patients with anxiety disorders; distrac-
tion condition: six patients [40 %] with major depression,
one [6.7 %] of whom was also suffering from three
additional anxiety disorders, and one patient [6.7 %]
with tic-disorder). Thirteen patients (86.7 %) were taking
Table 1 Characteristics of participants in the two experimental
groups
Measure Mindfulness
(N=15)
Distraction
(N=15)
M(%) SD M (%) SD
Age 30.67 10.93 39.73 13.36
Female (%) 60 40
Y-BOCS total 22.93 4.76 24.33 4.27
Y-BOCS compulsions 10.67 2.50 11.73 1.94
Y-BOCS obsessions 12.27 2.71 12.60 2.87
STAI-S 40.40 5.70 39.73 6.15
STAI-T 49.80 5.59 50.26 6.89
BDI 18.78 12.52 20.60 11.90
BAI 27.96 19.34 31.92 16.29
OCI-R total 27.47 11.86 30.86 12.89
OCI washing 4.53 4.87 5.93 4.93
OCI checking 5.73 4.11 6.73 4.13
OCI ordering 3.40 2.77 4.13 3.68
OCI obsession 7.53 3.54 8.40 3.58
OCI neutralising 3.67 3.81 2.80 3.00
OCI hoarding 2.60 2.87 2.86 3.22
Y-BOCS Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale, STAI-Sstate-
trait anxiety inventory, state, STAI-Tstate-trait anxiety inventory,
trait, BDI Beck depression inventory, BAI Beck Anxiety inventory,
OCI-Robsessive–compulsive inventory, revised
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
anti-depressant medication in the mindfulness group and 14
(93.3 %) in the distraction group.
Measures
Standardized Questionnaires
A German version of the YaleBrown Obsessive
Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS; Goodman et al. 1989; Hand
and Buettner-Westphal 1991) was used to assess OC
symptom severity. The Y-BOCS is a widely used semi-
structured clinician interview that includes a symptom
checklist and a 10-item symptom severity scale. After the
patient’s main obsessions and compulsions are identified
with the symptom checklist, the clinician rates the severity
of obsessions (items 1–5) and compulsions (items 6–10) on
a 5-point scale from 0 (no symptoms) to 4 (extreme) based
on the past week. The Y-BOCS has satisfactory psycho-
metric properties (Goodman et al. 1989; Storch et al. 2005)
and is considered the gold standard measure of obsessive–
compulsive symptoms.
The obsessivecompulsive inventory, revised (OCI-R;
Foa et al. 2002;Go
¨nner et al. 2008) is an 18-item self-
rating questionnaire of obsessive–compulsive symptom
severity in six domains: cleaning and washing, checking,
obsessions, neutralizing, hoarding and symmetry and order,
with good validity and reliability in the German version
(Go
¨nner et al. 2008).
The Beck depression inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al.
1996; Hautzinger et al. 1995) is a 21-item self-report
instrument intended to assess the severity of depressive
symptoms according to DSM-IV (APA 1994). For the
German version of the BDI-II (Hautzinger et al. 1995),
good internal consistency and test–retest reliability as well
as convergent and discriminant validity have been dem-
onstrated (Ku
¨hner et al. 2007).
The Beck anxiety inventory (BAI; Beck et al. 1988;
Margraf and Ehlers 2003) assesses somatic and cognitive
aspects of anxiety in a 21-item self-report questionnaire.
The German version showed good internal consistency and
temporal stability as well as convergent and discriminant
validity (Margraf and Ehlers 2003).
The state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI; Spielberger,
et al. 1983; Laux et al. 1981) is comprised of two scales,
with 20 items each, measuring trait anxiety and state
anxiety. The German versions of both scales demonstrated
high internal consistency, satisfactory test–retest reliability
and validity (Laux et al. 1981).
Visual Analog Scales (VAS)
Two 0–100 VAS were used to assess the dependent vari-
ables. Anxiety was rated on a 0–100 scale with 0 labeled
‘no anxiety at all’ and 100 labeled ‘the most anxious I have
ever felt’. Urge to neutralize was rated on a 0–100 scale
with 0 labeled ‘no urge to neutralize at all’ and 100 labeled
‘extreme urge to neutralize’.
Manipulation Check
Compliance with instructions and perceived helpfulness of
the coping strategies were assessed on 0–4 Likert scales. At
the end of the experiment, participants were asked to rate
(1) to what extent they managed to comply with the
instructions (0 =not at all and 4 =very much so) and
(2) how helpful they found the coping strategy in dealing
with the obsessive thoughts (0 =not at all and 4 =very
much so) during the baseline and separately for each of the
three instructions during the experimental condition, thus
resulting in four ratings per question. Percentage spent
neutralizing was assessed on a 0–100 scale separately for
each of the three phases (baseline, experimental condition
and return to baseline).
Procedure
Diagnostic evaluation (SCID-I and Y-BOCS) took place
within the first 2 weeks of the CBT inpatient treatment.
Participants were informed about the purpose of the study
(‘to investigate the effects of different coping strategies in
response to obsessive thoughts’) and written informed
consent was obtained from all participants prior to partic-
ipation. The study had been approved by the local ethics
committee of the University of Luebeck. Participants were
tested separately in a single experimental session in the
presence of the investigator. The experimental session
started with the identification of the most troublesome
obsessive thought. If necessary, the investigator and par-
ticipant elaborated on the obsessive thought so that it lasted
for about 10–15 s when spoken out loud. It was written
down on a form devised for this purpose. The average
duration of the intrusive thoughts did not differ between
experimental groups. Finally, participants read aloud the
written version of the intrusion verbatim onto an Apple
Macintosh Computer. Then they were asked to fill in the
STAI-State subscale. Before the experiment started, par-
ticipants were told that ratings of anxiety and urge to
neutralize would be taken at several points during the
experiment, and all participants were given practice in
rating the three verbal analog scales. Afterwards, partici-
pants were asked to put on the headphones and a power
point presentation was started. With the first slide, partic-
ipants were given the following instruction:
During the experiment, you will be listening to your
obsessive thoughts through the headphones. At the
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
same time, you will be given written instructions on
the screen on how to respond to these thoughts. Once
in a while, I will ask you to rate how anxious you are
and how much you want to neutralize at this partic-
ular time.
Once these instructions had been given, the investigator
ensured that the participant fully understood what was
required of him or her, and the experiment continued.
During the subsequent baseline (5 min), the obsessive
thought was presented through headphones every 30 s,
while the participants were given the following written
instructions on a slide:
During the next 5 min, you may think about anything
you like.
Ratings of anxiety and urge to neutralize were taken at
three time points after 45 s, 2.45 and 4.45 min. A 5-min
interval followed in which an animal movie was presented
in order to prevent further cognitive processing of the
obsessive intrusion or instruction. The animal movie was
followed by the experimental phase (6 min) in which
repeated audio-presentation of the obsessive intrusion was
accompanied by either a mindfulness-coping instruction or
distraction. Ratings of anxiety and urge to neutralize were
taken 15 s before the next slide was presented, i.e., at the
three time points after 45 s, 2.45 and 5.45 min. Then the
animal movie continued for another 5 min and was fol-
lowed by a return to baseline phase (5 min) that was
identical to the first baseline. Once the power point pre-
sentation was finished, participants removed the head-
phones and were asked to complete ratings of compliance
and perceived helpfulness. Finally, participants filled in the
set of standardized questionnaires. They were then
debriefed and the experiment ended.
Mindfulness-Based Coping
The mindfulness-based coping and the distraction instruc-
tion were presented on three power-point slides each, with
a presentation time of 1 min for the first slide, 2 min for the
second slide and 3 min for the third slide. The instructions
for the mindfulness-coping strategy were adapted from the
module ‘Thoughts are not facts’ used in MBCT (Segal
et al. 2002). The instruction focused on deliberate non-
judgmental awareness of thoughts and a metaphorical
‘letting go’ experience. The three statements were as
follows (1) ‘Thoughts are thoughts and not facts.’
(2) ‘Become aware of your thoughts at this moment’
(3) ‘Let your thoughts pass by like clouds in the sky.’
These three instructions had been rated most helpful in
pilot studies. Content validity of the mindfulness-based
coping strategy was determined by expert ratings.
Distraction
The three distraction instructions were as follows (1) ‘At
this moment your thoughts are causing you distress.’
(2) ‘Distract yourself from your thoughts.’ (3) Silently count
backwards in sevens from 700, i.e., 700, 693, 686 ’.
Results
Experimental Condition
In order to get more reliable measures of anxiety and urge
to neutralize, data were averaged for the three time points
during each phase. A 3 92 mixed-model ANOVA with
phase (baseline, experimental condition, return to baseline)
as the within-subject factor and experimental condition
(mindfulness vs. distraction) as the between subject factor
was conducted on anxiety and urge to neutralize. Alpha
was set at a=.05. To explore significant interactions,
results were followed up with ttests for dependent vari-
ables. Descriptive statistics of the original scores by phase,
time point and experimental condition are shown in
Tables 2and 3. For anxiety, there was a main effect of
phase, F(2, 56) =20.68, p\.001, modified by a signifi-
cant interaction of phase and experimental condition,
F(2, 56) =6.77, p\.01. For individuals in the mindful-
ness condition, anxiety declined significantly from baseline
(M=57.78, SD =16.41) to return to baseline (M=
28.67, SD =17.81), t(14) =7.25, p\.001, and from
experimental condition (M=47.78, SD =25.19) to return
to baseline, t(14) =6.67, p\.001. For individuals in the
distraction condition, reduction in anxiety between baseline
(M=50.23, SD =25.65) and return to baseline (M=
42.22, SD =27.45) and between experimental condition
Table 2 Means and standard deviations for anxiety (0–100) by
phase, time point and experimental condition
Descriptives/
time (min)
Mindfulness-
based
instruction
Distraction
MSDMSD
Baseline (5 min) 0.45 56.67 21.27 47.33 27.64
2.45 61.33 15.06 52.00 27.83
4.45 55.33 22.00 51.33 26.69
Experimental
condition
(6 min)
0.45 44.67 23.56 46.67 26.90
2.45 54.00 27.98 48.67 26.42
5.45 44.67 27.48 46.00 30.43
Return to
baseline
(5 min)
0.45 33.33 21.93 42.00 27.05
2.45 28.67 18.07 44.00 28.23
4.45 24.00 16.39 40.67 28.65
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
(M=47.11, SD =26.33) and return to baseline were
smaller and not significant, t(14) =0.93, and t(14) =
1.22, ns, respectively. For urge to neutralize, there was a
similar pattern of results: a significant effect of phase, F(2,
56) =10.22, p\.001, was modified by a significant
interaction of phase and experimental condition, F(2,
56) =3.24, p\.05. Specifically, individuals who used the
mindfulness strategy experienced a decrease in urge to
neutralize from baseline (M=47.11, SD =23.80) to
return to baseline (M=26.00, SD =17.92), t(14) =3.62,
p\.01, and from experimental condition (M=
43.78, SD =27.02) to return to baseline, t(14) =4.39,
p\.01. In contrast, individuals who used the distraction
strategy, did not experience a significant decline in urge to
neutralize, neither from baseline (M=46.00, SD =30.56)
to return to baseline (M=39.78, SD =31.33), t(14)
=.41, ns, nor from experimental condition (M=44.22,
SD =31.61) to return to baseline, t(14) =1.63, ns. Fig-
ures 1and 2shows the means and 95 % confidence inter-
vals for self-reported anxiety and urge to neutralize by
phase and experimental condition.
Manipulation Check
Ttests for independent samples were calculated to analyze
differences in compliance with instructions and perceived
helpfulness of the instructions. There were no significant
differences between the two experimental conditions in any
of the compliance variables (all ps[.05). Additionally,
instructions were seen as equally helpful in both experi-
mental conditions (ps[.05), except for the first slides of
the experimental phase (‘Thoughts are thoughts and not
facts.’ vs. ‘At this moment your thoughts are causing you
distress.’). Participants in the mindfulness-based instruc-
tion (M=2.87, SD =1.36) found the sentence more
helpful than those in the distraction condition (M=.93,
SD =.96), t(22) =-4.51, p\.001. Finally, percentage
of time spent neutralizing the obsessive thought during
each time phase did not demonstrate any significant dif-
ferences between experimental and control group
(ps[.05). The average time percentage spent neutralizing
was low in both groups (M=22.22, SD =11.97 for the
mindfulness condition, M=36.94, SD =11.9 for the
distraction condition).
Additional Analyses
To test whether there was an initial increase in anxiety
levels in the mindfulness condition as opposed to a
reduction in anxiety levels in the distraction condition, a
392 mixed-model ANOVA for the three time points
within the experimental condition was conducted on anx-
iety, with time points (0.45, 2.45, and 5.45) as the within-
subject factor and experimental condition (mindfulness vs.
Table 3 Means and standard deviations for urge to neutralize
(0–100) by phase, time point and experimental condition
Descriptives/
time (min)
Mindfulness-
based
instruction
Distraction
MSDMSD
Baseline (5 min) 0.45 45.33 26.96 46.00 31.58
2.45 48.67 25.03 46.67 32.22
4.45 47.33 26.58 45.33 32.92
Experimental
condition (6 min)
0.45 41.33 23.26 46.00 30.43
2.45 49.33 32.40 44.67 32.48
5.45 40.67 28.40 42.00 35.70
Return to baseline
(5 min)
0.45 30.00 21.04 40.00 33.17
2.45 25.33 18.07 40.67 31.50
4.45 22.67 17.51 38.67 30.91
Fig. 1 Mean anxiety and 95 % confidence intervals across phases by
experimental condition
Fig. 2 Mean urge to neutralize and 95 % confidence intervals across
phases by experimental condition
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
distraction) as the between subject factor. If predictions
were correct, anxiety should have increased during the
presentation of the second slide in the mindfulness condi-
tion (‘Become aware of your thoughts at this moment.’)
and declined during the presentation of the second slide in
the distraction condition (‘Distract yourself from your
thoughts.’). This would lead to a significant interaction of
time point and experimental condition. Table Two shows
the means and standard deviations per time point and
condition. Although the means show a marked rise in
anxiety at time two in the mindfulness condition relative to
the control group, no significant interactions or significant
main effects were evident (all ns. at a=.05). To assess the
possibility that we had inadequate power to detect a 3 92
interaction—given our sample size, a medium effect size,
and an a=.05—we found that power was .63. Thus, we
may have had insufficient power to detect a medium-sized
effect.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the efficacy of a
mindfulness-based strategy compared to distraction during
brief exposure to obsessive thoughts. As predicted, par-
ticipants experienced a greater decline in anxiety and urge
to neutralize after having engaged in a mindfulness-based
strategy than those who had engaged in distraction. Thus, it
appears that deliberately focusing on one’s obsessive
thoughts and then imagining these passing by like clouds in
the sky, is an effective way of dealing with the accompa-
nying anxiety and urge to neutralize when these thoughts
are presented a short time later without any explicit
instructions as to how to deal with them.
Results are unlikely to be accounted for by different
levels of compliance with the instructions between groups,
since there were no significant differences between groups
regarding compliance measures. Additionally, participants
rated the instructions as being equally helpful in both
groups, with the exception of one introductory instruction,
which was found to be more helpful in the mindfulness
group than in the distraction group. However, an additional
analysis demonstrated that there were no differences
between the groups in either anxiety or urge to neutralize
immediately after presenting this particular introductory
slide in the experimental condition [anxiety: t(28) =.21,
ns, urge to neutralize: t(28) =.47, ns]. Therefore, it
appears unlikely that differences in perceived helpfulness
of the introductory slide in the experimental period caused
the differences between groups at return to baseline
The mechanisms of change through which a mindful-
ness-based strategy reduces anxiety and urge to neutralize
during brief exposure can only be speculated upon. It is
possible that focused attention towards the obsessive
thoughts was the moderating factor and facilitated habit-
uation processes. We hypothesized that focused attention
towards the obsessive thoughts in the mindfulness group
would result in an initial rise in anxiety, followed by a
reduction. This prediction was not confirmed. However,
the study may have had insufficient power to detect
medium size effects and conclusions about the course of
anxiety would be premature. In the present study, group
differences in anxiety reduction are unlikely to be
accounted for by different levels of neutralizing, since
percentage time spent neutralizing during any phase of the
experiment did not differ significantly between groups.
One additional mechanism of change remains increased
‘letting go capacity’ in the mindfulness group through the
use of metaphors (Hanstede et al. 2008). ‘Letting go’
might have decreased the risk of attaching significance to
the thought, increasing its acceptance. It remains to be
seen in future studies if certain aspects of mindfulness-
based strategies (e.g., focused attention, accepting obser-
vation, particular metaphors) are associated with particular
mechanisms of change and which are most useful during
brief exposure.
Data are consistent with results by Grayson et al. (1982,
1986), who showed that focused attention but not distrac-
tion during exposure generalized to between session
habituation. Data are also in line with previous research
showing that exposure alone (without explicitly blocking
compulsive rituals) leads to a reduction in anxiety (Foa
et al. 1984). Our data replicate and extend findings by
Marcks and Woods (2005) and Najmi et al. (2009), who
investigated the management of naturally occurring intru-
sive thoughts in the laboratory. In both studies, the use of a
mindfulness-based strategy had an immediate beneficial
effect (reduction in anxiety) in healthy individuals (Marcks
and Woods 2005) and also in patients diagnosed with OCD
(Najmi et al. 2009). Our results extend these findings by
showing that individuals also profit from a mindfulness-
based strategy when applied during brief exposure to
obsessive thoughts.
Clinical implications of these findings include consid-
erations of how mindfulness-based strategies can be best
integrated into empirically validated therapy for patients
diagnosed with OCD. The study suggests that conditions of
loop tape exposure can be varied in such a way that
mindfulness elements are added and the explicit instruction
to prevent neutralizing can be omitted. If our results were
replicated and found to have a lasting effect, they might be
incorporated into other exposure interventions, particularly
for patients without overt compulsions, in order to reduce
drop-out rates. Additionally, it can be discussed whether
patients might use mindfulness-based strategies in response
to their naturally occurring thoughts, or as a supplementary
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
strategy for relapse prevention when obsessive thoughts
re-occur.
Limitations of the current study should be taken into
consideration. Pilot work and previous studies (Grayson
et al. 1982,1986; Salkovskis et al. 2003) have demon-
strated that distraction during exposure is a powerful
mechanism that results in short term anxiety reduction. It
could be argued that applying metaphors such as clouds in
the sky simply act as distraction from the immediate dis-
tress of the thoughts. Therefore, we chose a counting
backward task as an appropriate control, since it is a cog-
nitively demanding task that distracts the person to roughly
the same degree as a mindfulness-based strategy would.
However, if the mindfulness instruction did not distract
participants, they performed more exposure to their
obsessional thoughts than participants in the distraction
condition. In this case the study was not designed to answer
questions about the efficacy of mindfulness. Any conclu-
sions about the efficacy of mindfulness could be explained
in terms of the effects of exposure. Whether a mindfulness-
based instruction is better than brief exposure alone can
only be answered in future studies including a third control
condition with exposure only. Second, only short term
effects of the mindfulness-based instruction were assessed,
whether these last cannot be concluded. Future studies with
three groups (mindfulness, distraction and listening to the
thought without any instructions) and a later assessment of
the effects (e.g., 1 day later) are needed in order answer
these questions. Further methodological weaknesses
include the exclusive reliance on subjective reports of
anxiety and urge to neutralize on VAS. Although the use of
such ratings is typical in experimental studies investigating
psychopathological processes, future research should
incorporate ratings that possess good psychometric prop-
erties. The sample size was small and might compromise
generalizability of results.
Finally, it should be emphasized again that implement-
ing brief aspects of mindfulness in experimental studies is
not representative of the complex multi-facetted construct
of mindfulness, which is typically integrated into an
intensive training, including mindful meditation practice
over several weeks. We think that—if at all—mindful
meditation is only an alternative to cognitive restructuring
for changing faulty appraisals of obsessive thoughts in case
it could be achieved through continued intensive practice.
Data offer initial evidence to support the idea that a
mindfulness based strategy during brief exposure might be
advantageous compared to a distraction strategy. Results
are in line with studies finding preliminary evidence of the
effectiveness of mindfulness and acceptance-based inter-
ventions in the reduction of OC symptoms (Hanstede et al.
2008; Patel et al. 2007; Singh et al. 2004; Twohig
et al. 2010; Wilkinson-Tough et al. 2010) and a recent
meta-analysis on the effects of mindfulness-based therapy
on anxiety and depression (Hofmann et al. 2010). Ulti-
mately, it is hoped that knowledge provided from this study
and future research may result in significant clinical
implications relevant to cognitive behavioral therapy.
Acknowledgments We are grateful to Johanna Schriefer and
Dr. Bernhard Osen for their help with data collection.
Conflict of interest None.
References
Abramowitz, J. S. (2002). Treatment of obsessive thoughts and
cognitive rituals using exposure and response prevention.
Clinical Case Studies, 1, 6–24. doi:10.1177/153465010200100
1002.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Amer-
ican Psychiatric Association.
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A
conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 10, 125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015.
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An
inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric proper-
ties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56,
893–897. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the
Beck depression inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psycho-
logical Corporation, Harcourt, Brace.
Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric
mood: Contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 29, 501–510. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-
3888-0.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness:
Theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects.
Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211–237. doi:10.1080/104784007
01598298.
Clark, D. A., & Purdon, C. (1993). New perspectives for a cognitive
theory of obsessions. Australian Psychologist, 28, 161–167. doi:
10.1080/00050069308258896.
Didonna, F. (2009). Mindfulness and obsessive–compulsive disorder:
Developing a way to trust and validate one’s internal experi-
ences. In F. Didonna (Ed.), Clinical handbook of mindfulness
(pp. 189–220). Berlin: Springer.
Fairfax, H. (2008). The use of mindfulness in obsessive–compulsive
disorder: Suggestions for its application and integration in
existing treatment. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 15,
53–59. doi:10.1002/cpp.557.
First, M. B., & Gibbon, M. (2004). The structured clinical interview
for DSM-IV axis I disorder (SCID-I) and the structured clinical
interview for DSM-IV axis II disorder (SCID-II). In M.
J. Hilsenroth & D. L. Segal (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook
of psychological assessment: Personality assessment (Vol. 2).
Hoboken: Wiley.
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R.,
Hajcak, G., et al. (2002). The obsessive–compulsive inventory:
Development and validation of a short version. Psychological
Assessment, 14, 485–496. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485.
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear:
Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99,
20–35. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20.
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G. S., Grayson, J. B., Turner, R. M., & Latimer,
P. R. (1984). Deliberate exposure and blocking of obsessive–
compulsive rituals: Immediate and long-term effects. Behavior
Therapy, 15, 450–472. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(84)80049-0.
Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1997). What do patients do with
their obsessive thoughts? Behaviour Therapy and Research, 35,
335–348. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00094-0.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., Thibodeau, N.,
Rhe
´aume, J., Letarte, H., et al. (1997). Cognitive-behavioral
treatment of obsessive-thoughts: A controlled study. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 405–413. doi:10.
1037/0022-006X.65.3.405.
Go
¨nner, S., Leonhart, R., & Ecker, W. (2008). The obsessive–
compulsive inventory-revised (OCI-R): Validation of the Ger-
man version in a sample of patients with OCD, anxiety disorders,
and depressive disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22,
734–749. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.07.007.
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C.,
Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). The Yale–Brown
Obsessive–Compulsive Scale: Development, use and reliability.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 1006–1011. doi:
10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007.
Grayson, J. B., Foa, E. B., & Steketee, G. S. (1982). Habituation
during exposure treatment: Distraction vs attention-focusing.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 20, 323–328. doi:10.1016/
0005-7967(82)90091-2.
Grayson, J. B., Foa, E. B., & Steketee, G. S. (1986). Exposure in vivo
of obsessive–compulsives under distraction and attention-focus-
ing conditions: Replication and extension. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 24, 475–479. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(86)90013-6.
Hand, I., & Buettner-Westphal, H. (1991). Die Yale–Brown Obses-
sive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS): Ein halbstrukturiertes Inter-
view zur Beurteilung des Schweregrades von Denk- und
Handlungszwa
¨ngen [The Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive
Scale (Y-BOCS): A semistructured interview for assessing
severity of compulsive cognitions and behavior]. Verhaltens-
therapie, 1, 226–233. doi:10.1159/000108846.
Hannan, S. E., & Tolin, D. F. (2005). Mindfulness- and acceptance-
based behaviour therapy for obsessive–compulsive disorder. In
S. M. Orsillo & L. Roemer (Eds.), Acceptance and mindfulness-
based approaches to anxiety. Conceptualization and treatment.
Series in anxiety and related disorders (pp. 271–299). doi:
10.1007/0-387-25989-9_11.
Hanstede, M., Gidron, Y., & Nyklicek, I. (2008). The effects of a
mindfulness intervention on obsessive–compulsive symptoms in
a non-clinical student population. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 196, 776–779. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181
8786b8.
Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H., & Keller, F. (1995). Beck-
depressions-inventar (BDI) [Beck depression inventory (BDI)]
(2nd ed.). Bern: Huber.
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010). The
effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression:
A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 78, 169–183. doi:10.1037/a0018555.
Hoogduin, K., de Haan, E., Schaap, C., & Arts, W. (1987). Exposure
and response prevention in patients with obsessions. Acta
Psychiatrica Belgica, 87(6), 640–653.
Huffziger, S., & Kuehner, C. (2009). Rumination, distraction, and
mindful self-focus in depressed patients. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 47, 224–230. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.005.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of
your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York:
Delta.
Ku
¨hner, C., Bu
¨rger, C., Keller, F., & Hautzinger, M. (2007). Reliabil-
itaet und Validitaet des revidierten Beck-Depressionsinventars
(BDI-II) [Reliability and validity of the revised beck depression
inventory (BDI-II)]. Nervenarzt, 78, 651–656. doi:10.1007/
s00115-006-2098-7.
Ladouceur, R., Freeston, M., Gagnon, F., Thibodeau, N., & Dumont,
J. (1993). Idiographic considerations in the behavioral treatment
of obsessional thoughts. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 24, 301–310. doi:10.1016/0005-7916
(93)90054-Z.
Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C. D. (1981).
Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). Theoretische Grundlagen
und Handanweisung [The state-trait-anxiety-inventory (STAI).
Theoretical background and instructions]. Weinheim: Beltz.
Marcks, B. A., & Woods, D. W. (2005). A comparison of thought
suppression to an acceptance-based technique in the manage-
ment of personal intrusive thoughts: A controlled evaluation.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 433–445. doi:10.1016/j.
brat.2004.03.005.
Margraf, J., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Beck-Angst-Inventar. Deutschsp-
rachige Adaptation des Beck Anxiety Inventory von A.T.Beck
und R.A. Stern [Beck-anxiety-inventory. German adaptation of
the Beck anxiety inventory from A. T. Beck and R. A. Stern].
Bern: Huber.
Najmi, S., Riemann, B. C., & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Managing
unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive–compulsive disorder:
Relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and
acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 494–503. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.015.
Patel, S. R., Carmody, J., & Simpson, H. B. (2007). Adapting
mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of obses-
sive–compulsive disorder: A case report. Cognitive and Behav-
ioral Practice, 14, 375–380. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2006.08.006.
Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 35, 793–802. doi:10.1016/S0005-79
67(97)00040-5.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1983). Treatment of an obsessional patient using
habituation to audiotaped ruminations. British Journal of
Clinical Psychology, 22, 311–313. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.
1983.tb00618.x.
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional–compulsive problems: A
cognitive–behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Ther-
apy, 23, 571–583. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6.
Salkovskis, P. M., Thorpe, S. J., Wahl, K., Wroe, A. L., & Forrester,
E. (2003). Neutralizing increases discomfort associated with
obsessional thoughts: An experimental study with obsessional
patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 709–715. doi:
10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.709.
Salkovskis, P. M., & Westbrook, D. (1989). Behaviour therapy and
obsessional rumination: Can failure be turned into success?
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 149–160. doi:10.1016/
0005-7967(89)90073-9.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002).
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new
approach to preventing relapse. New York: The Guilford Press.
Singh, N. N., Wahler, R. G., Winton, A. S., Adkins, W., & The
Mindfulness Research Group. (2004). A mindfulness based
treatment of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Clinical Case
Studies, 3, 275–287. doi:10.1177/1534650103259646.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. K., &
Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory
for adults. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Storch, E. A., Shapira, N. A., Dimoulas, E., Geffken, G. R., Murphy,
T. K., & Goodman, W. K. (2005). Yale–Brown Obsessive–
Compulsive Scale: The dimensional structure revisited. Depres-
sion and Anxiety, 22, 28–35. doi:10.1002/da.20088.
Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., Plumb, J. C., Pruitt, L. D., Collins, A. B.,
Hazlett-Stevens, H., et al. (2010). A randomized clinical trial of
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
acceptance and commitment therapy vs. progressive relaxation
training for obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 78, 705–716. doi:10.1037/a002
0508.
Wilkinson-Tough, M., Bocci, L., Thorne, K., & Herlihy, J. (2010). Is
mindfulness-based therapy an effective intervention for obses-
sive intrusive thoughts? Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy,
17, 250–268. doi:10.1002/cpp.665.
Cogn Ther Res
123
Author's personal copy
... Additionally, the studies that were included met the aims and objectives of the current review. of aims, types of intervention components and how interventions for OCD were adapted/implemented to address STBs-related outcomes Studies varied in their target sample, type of intervention, period and indicators to improve treatment outcomes. For instance, one study conducted in Germany(Wahl et al., 2013) on 30 in-patients with OCD used mindfulness-based strategies compared distraction strategies during brief exposures to obsessive thoughts. Patients were asked to listen to their obsessive thoughts using headphones throughout three time periods: at baseline, during the intervention, and post-intervention. ...
... Therefore, to achieve that, the role of mental health professionals needs to be improved through comprehensive training. Thus, psychotherapy treatments for OCD are recommended to enhance treatment outcomes, minimize STBs, and raise the quality of life for individuals with OCD.Our finding is supported by the study ofWahl et al. (2013), which reported that mindfulness-based strategies and distraction strategies were most effective for alleviating the symptoms of OCD and minimizing the STBs.Şafak et al. (2014) used CBGT to minimize clinical aspects of OCD such as STB. Another study found that ...
Article
Background: Suicidal thoughts and behaviours (STBs) are significant public health challenges that affect a variety of individuals and communities. Despite numerous efforts to discover and refine psychotherapy treatments to minimize STBs, the efficacy of STB treatments remains unclear. Objective: Conduct a scoping review to assess the efficacy of using psychotherapy treatments to minimize STBs among individuals with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Method: A scoping review was conducted using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Extension for Scoping Reviews guidelines (PRISMA-ScR) to screen 163 studies published between 2010 and 2021. Results: A total of seven articles that fulfil the eligibility criteria reported that psychotherapy treatments for obsessive–compulsive disorder were found to be effective and applicable approaches to minimize the severity of the OCD symptoms and STBs, despite variance in studies' target samples, types of interventions, periods and indicators. Conclusion: The current review has provided evidence showing the significant effects of psychotherapy treatments on various health-related aspects of life for individuals diagnosed with obsessive–compulsive disorder, and it is recommended to use them for enhancing treatment outcomes and minimizing STBs. Implication for Practice: This scoping review verifies the formalization and incorporation of psychotherapy treatments for OCD to minimize STBs into standard practice and highlights the importance of mental health professionals being part of the implementation of these treatments.
... Prior research on pain suggest that distraction as a general coping strategy can be adaptive or maladaptive-while distracting reduces experimentally evoked pain (such as cold pressor or thermal stimulation; McCaul and Malott 1984;Verhoeven et al. 2010), distracting from severe/chronic pain or catastrophizing about pain is maladaptive (Rosenstiel and Keefe 1983;Keefe et al. 1990;Johnson 2005). Greater distracting from aversive body sensations was associated with worse psychiatric outcomes (Brown et al. 2017;Rogers et al. 2018), and this was consistent with cognitive behavioral therapy studies revealing that distracting from uncomfortable events impedes the reduction of distress over time (Wahl et al. 2013) and is detrimental to the therapeutic process (Gillihan et al. 2012;McKay et al. 2015;Blakey and Abramowitz 2018). ...
... Our findings have implications for the treatment of OCD and future work. First, the finding that patients reporting more "adaptive" IS tended to have milder OCD symptoms is consistent with emerging evidence that mindfulnessbased interventions can be efficacious (Wahl et al. 2013;Selchen et al. 2018). Mindfulness training is often bodyfocused (Lee 2009;Kerr et al. 2013;Gibson 2019) and aims to teach practitioners to observe distressing events or sensation without acting on or feeling controlled by them (Teasdale et al. 2002;Crowe and McKay 2016;Crane 2017), thereby modulating attentional control and emotional regulation (Coffey et al. 2010;Marchand 2013;Guendelman et al. 2017;Gibson 2019). ...
Article
Patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) exhibit abnormality in their subjective perception of internal sensation, a process known as interoceptive sensibility (IS), as well as altered functioning of the insula, a key neural structure for interoception. We investigated the multivariate structure of IS in 77 OCD patients and 53 controls and examined associations of IS with resting-state functional connectivity (FC) of the insula within the OCD group. For each group, principal component analysis was performed on 8 subscales of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness assessing putatively "adaptive" and "maladaptive" aspects of IS. Associations between IS components and insula FC in the OCD group were evaluated using seed regions placed in each of 3 subdivisions of the insula (posterior, anterior dorsal, and anterior ventral). Behaviorally, controls showed a 2-component solution broadly categorized into "adaptive" and "maladaptive" IS, while OCD patients exhibited a 3-component solution. The general tendency to notice or be aware of sensation loaded onto an "adaptive" IS component in controls but loaded onto both "adaptive" and "maladaptive" IS components in OCD. Within OCD, insula FC was differentially associated with distinct aspects of IS, identifying network connections that could serve as future targets for the modulation of IS in OCD.
... Research in non-clinical participants (Marcks & Woods, 2005) and individuals with OCD (Najmi et al., 2009) has demonstrated that participants who were given an acceptance-based instruction experienced less distress in response to intrusive thoughts than those who were instructed to suppress them. Similarly, Wahl et al. (2012) found that individuals with OCD who were instructed to mindfully attend to their thoughts experienced greater declines in anxiety and urges to neutralize during a loop-tape exposure than those who were instructed to distract themselves during the exposure. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Despite growing research on mindfulness-based interventions for obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), it remains unknown which aspects of mindfulness are most beneficial and whether the effects vary for different OCD symptom domains. Methods To clarify these relationships, we examined the links between dispositional mindfulness, experimentally induced mindfulness, and obsessive compulsive (OC) symptoms in a sample of young adults selected for elevated OC symptoms (N = 97). First, we investigated the association between dispositional mindfulness on the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) and clinical interview-assessed OC symptoms, as well as anxiety and urge to ritualize in response to two OC symptom provocation tasks. Second, we examined the effects of a brief, computerized Mindful Attention (MA) training relative to a Control training on responses to two different OC symptom provocation tasks (harm- or contamination-related). Results FFMQ-nonjudgment negatively predicted obsession and compulsion severity, as well as post-task urge intensity. None of the other FFMQ indices was predictive of any OC symptom measures. The effect of MA training, relative to Control training, was moderated by the type of OC symptom (harm-related vs. contamination-related concerns) such that it appeared to have a beneficial effect for the former, but not the latter. Conclusions Our findings indicate that the nonjudgment facet of mindfulness may be especially important for individuals with OCD, and that MA training may be more helpful for harm-related unacceptable thoughts than for contamination concerns. Future research and clinical interventions would benefit from further examination of the relationship between specific facets of mindfulness and OC symptoms.
... 19 A study examining the effectiveness of mindfulness and meditation compared to the use of distraction in patients with OCD showed greater decrease and urge to perform compulsions using mindfulness-based strategy in comparison to those who had engaged in distraction. 20,21 The current study thus attempted to assess effectiveness of ACT in patients with OCD. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background:Obsessive-compulsive disorder is chronic and severity of its symptoms often make it a source of distress, and dysfunction and, disability; and has high comorbidity with mood as well as anxiety disorders. Exposure response prevention (ERP) is most researched and effective behavioral techniques despite this, 25% patients refuse to complete ERP due to distress associated with exposure. Objective: A study examining the effectiveness of mindfulness and meditation compared to the use of distraction in patients with OCD showed significant decrease and urge to perform compulsions. The current study thus attempted to assess effectiveness of acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) in patients with OCD. Method: It was a single group pre-post intervention study design using purposive sampling method. Fifteen participants with diagnosis of OCD were recruited. The pre-post assessment measures used were; Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS)Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF)Clinical Global Impression-Severity Scale (CGI-S). Results: The paired t-test analysis revealed significant change in pre-post test scores on symptomatology (YBOCS), functioning (GAF) and clinical symptoms (CGI) with p vales significant at .001 level. However, on computing the effect size it was found that the change in pre-post test scores of obsession and compulsion ranged from small to medium effect size while the change in scores of functioning was large (d=.83). Conclusion: ACT unlike behavioral and other cognitive therapies emphasize on the patient's functioning than the symptom alleviation. This shift is approach is believed to reduce the chances of relapse and enhance better generalization of skills in day-today life.
Article
Full-text available
Obsessive Compulsive Disorder is one of the most disabling mental disorders in the world and there are different interventions in the treatment strategy for obsessive compulsive disorder. A recent technique that has been gaining ground within positive psychology is Mindfulness, this technique has shown promising results and is being used as a new resource for adherence to the treatment of OCD symptoms. The aim of this study was to perform a systematic review of the literature to verify the therapeutic efficacy of mindfulness-based interventions for the treatment of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. The study was carried out through a systematic review of the literature in the following databases: Pubmed, Psycinfo, BVS and Web of Science. The keywords used were searched in the Descriptors in Health Sciences (DecS) terminology banks. These were complemented by keywords found in reference articles on the subject. The following string was used: “mindfulness” AND “obsessive compulsive disorder” OR “OCD”. Initially, a total of 320 articles were found, after applying the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 19 articles entered this review. Mindfulness-based interventions have emerged as an important therapeutic tool. Research on the subject points to its role in reducing the symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder and increasing quality of life. The results of different interventions point to the potential of mindfulness-based programs and the benefits of their application in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Article
Many studies tend to explore a link between obsessive-compulsive symptoms, stressful life events and the social support received when facing them and other developmental challenges. While the compulsions are the most obvious aspect of this disorder, stereotypical behavior is encountered as a symptom in several other disorders and the underlying obsessions are less obvious, In the present study, we investigated the associations between obsessing, different types of compulsions and the pathological aspects of narcissism with some aspects positively associated with weel-being, like feeling of belonging and mindfulness, with its two dimensions, the here-and-now awareness and the acceptance of this experience. The data showed that on obsessing had a significant effect exclusion/rejection, awareness and the acceptance of here-and-now-experience; washing was best predicted by the feeling of being accepted/included (negative correlation); neutralizing by awareness, acceptance of here-and-now-experience (negative correlations), and entitlement (positive correlation); checking by acceptance of here-and-now-experience (negative correlation) and entitlement (positive correlation). Also, there were significant negative correlations between hoarding and awareness, but also between ordering and acceptance of here-and-now experience. The findings suggest different psychological explanations for the different types of compulsive behaviors, but also ways of diminishing the obsessing process and compulsions by developing the awareness capacity.
Article
Written by internationally recognized experts, this comprehensive CBT clinician's manual provides disorder-specific chapters and accessible pedagogical features. The cutting-edge research, advanced theory, and attention to special adaptations make this an appropriate reference text for qualified CBT practitioners, students in post-graduate CBT courses, and clinical psychology doctorate students. The case examples demonstrate clinical applications of specific interventions and explain how to adapt CBT protocols for a range of diverse populations. It strikes a balance between core, theoretical principles and protocol-based interventions, simulating the experience of private supervision from a top expert in the field.
Chapter
Written by internationally recognized experts, this comprehensive CBT clinician's manual provides disorder-specific chapters and accessible pedagogical features. The cutting-edge research, advanced theory, and attention to special adaptations make this an appropriate reference text for qualified CBT practitioners, students in post-graduate CBT courses, and clinical psychology doctorate students. The case examples demonstrate clinical applications of specific interventions and explain how to adapt CBT protocols for a range of diverse populations. It strikes a balance between core, theoretical principles and protocol-based interventions, simulating the experience of private supervision from a top expert in the field.
Article
Full-text available
Mindfulness is non-judgmental awareness that arises as a result of conscious focus on the current moment. It may be seen as one of the useful therapeutic tools that helps to alleviate symptoms of OCD. In recent decades, there has been a growing interest in studying the concept of mindfulness as a psychological construct and as a form of psychotherapeutic intervention for the prevention and treatment of mental disorders. The purpose of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) like cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is to assist patients in developing awareness of their thoughts and reactions. MBCT teaches that the best way to spot these triggers and overcome stress and anxiety is to be aware of and accept the current moment. Instead of trying too hard to realize negative thoughts, fears, and anxieties, MBCT teaches to accept any thought in a non-judgmental way and allow it to disappear as easily as to appear. Mindfulness is a proven skill of awareness and a way of responding in a non-judgmental manner to unwanted thoughts, feelings, and urges. Clearing the mind, mindfulness helps to kill off habitual connections between neurons and develop new ones, to reduce the level of obsessive thoughts and stereotypes, to overcome automatic thinking and eradicate existing patterns of behavior that ultimately leads to a qualitatively new level of living and development of human abilities and talents. Complementing cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), the gold standard for the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) enhances and improves the therapeutic effect and opens new horizons for further research. The paper aims to analyze the place and role of mindfulness in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorders.
Article
Full-text available
In the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder, the effectiveness of two main treatment approaches, pharmacotherapy and cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), has been proven. However, the high rates of drop out from treatment and the presence of residual symptoms after CBT direct researchers to look for different treatment approaches. Therefore, emerging developments show that Third Wave Therapies are promising. In this review; it is aimed to summarize the theoretical background of Third Wave Therapies used in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder and to review the effectiveness of these approaches and examine these studies in terms of methodological perspective and therapy content. In accordance with this purpose, studies which examine the effectiveness of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Mindfullness-Based Therapy, Metacognitive Therapy, Schema Therapy and Dialectical Behavior Therapy which are published in English/Turkish between 2004-2020 years and accessed from 5 databases: Ebscohost, Ulakbim, Google Scholar, Science Direct and Web of Science are included. As a result of this review, it has been determined that Third Wave Therapies generally reduce obsessive-compulsive symptoms and result in both reductions on depression, anxiety and stress symptoms and improvements on skills such as self-compassion and mindfulness. Based on these results, it was concluded that the first evidences about the effectiveness of Third Wave Therapies in the adult population in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder begin to accumulate, but there is a need for randomize clinical controlled trials which is performed with larger samples and compared with cognitive behavioral therapy, exposure and response prevention or pharmacotherapy.
Article
Full-text available
This case study describes the application of cognitive-behavioral therapy by exposure and response prevention (EX/RP) to a young man with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) involving primarily obsessional thoughts and mental rituals. Although patients with primarily cognitive OCD symptoms have been previously considered treatment resistant, novel approaches to EX/RP have been developed and validated. Successful treatment of such symptoms requires a thorough and informed assessment. The theoretical and empirical basis for these procedures is described, along with a cognitive-behavioral analysis of the problem. The course of treatment, use of assessment data, and recommendations to clinicians are also discussed.
Article
• The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale was designed to remedy the problems of existing rating scales by providing a specific measure of the severity of symptoms of obsessivecompulsive disorder that is not influenced by the type of obsessions or compulsions present. The scale is a clinician-rated, 10-item scale, each item rated from 0 (no symptoms) to 4 (extreme symptoms) (total range, 0 to 40), with separate subtotals for severity of obsessions and compulsions. In a study involving four raters and 40 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder at various stages of treatment, interrater reliability for the total Yale-Brown Scale score and each of the 10 individual items was excellent, with a high degree of internal consistency among all item scores demonstrated with Cronbach's α coefficient. Based on pretreatment assessment of 42 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder, each item was frequently endorsed and measured across a range of severity. These findings suggest that the Yale-Brown Scale is a reliable instrument for measuring the severity of illness in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder with a range of severity and types of obsessive-compulsive symptoms.
Article
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is an illness characterized by intrusive and distressing thoughts, images, or impulses (i.e., obsessions) and by repetitive mental or behavioral acts (i.e., compulsions) performed to prevent or reduce distress. Efficacious treatments for OCD include psychotropic medications and exposure and response prevention (EX/RP). The following case report presents an individual diagnosed with OCD who refused treatment with medication or EX/RP and was treated using an adapted Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program. After an 8-week adapted MBSR program, the endpoint evaluation revealed clinically significant reductions in symptoms of OCD as well as an increased capacity to evoke a state of mindfulness. Discussion includes generalizability of these findings, potential mechanisms of action, and the role of an adapted MBSR in the treatment of OCD.
Article
Of those who suffer from obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), a substantial number do not respond well to the standard treatment of two trials of serotonin reuptake inhibitors and cognitive-behavioral therapy. In addition to being refractory to current treatments, these individuals often have comorbid disorders that contribute to a compromised quality of life. The authors present the case of such an individual who was assisted to improve her quality of life by accepting her OCD as a strength and enhancing her mindfulness so that she was able to incorporate her OCD in her daily life. Results showed that she successfully overcame her debilitating OCD and was taken off all medication within 6 months of intervention. Three years of postintervention follow-up showed that she was well adjusted, had a full and healthy lifestyle and that although some obsessive thoughts remained, they did not control her behavior.