ArticlePDF Available

The Relationship between Perfectionism and Rumination in Post Traumatic Stress Disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: To date no research has investigated the link between Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and perfectionism in a clinical sample. Aims: The aim of the current study was to examine whether there is a relationship between PTSD and perfectionism. This is important to address as many studies have demonstrated a link between other anxiety disorders, eating disorders, depression and perfectionism. The research also aimed to examine whether rumination was a mediator of the relationship between PTSD and perfectionism. Method: The sample consisted of 30 participants who were currently in treatment for PTSD. Results: The results suggest that perfectionism and PTSD symptoms were significantly correlated. In addition, rumination was a significant mediator of the relationship between Concern over Mistakes and PTSD. Conclusions: These findings help increase understanding about the relationships of perfectionism and rumination in PTSD and have implications for the treatment of PTSD.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 2014, 42, 211–223
First published online 15 February 2013 doi:10.1017/S1352465812001129
The Relationship between Perfectionism and Rumination
in Post Traumatic Stress Disorder
Sarah J. Egan, Mary Hattaway and Robert T. Kane
Curtin University, Perth, Australia
Background: To date no research has investigated the link between Post Traumatic Stress
Disorder (PTSD) and perfectionism in a clinical sample. Aims: The aim of the current
study was to examine whether there is a relationship between PTSD and perfectionism. This
is important to address as many studies have demonstrated a link between other anxiety
disorders, eating disorders, depression and perfectionism. The research also aimed to examine
whether rumination was a mediator of the relationship between PTSD and perfectionism.
Method: The sample consisted of 30 participants who were currently in treatment for PTSD.
Results: The results suggest that perfectionism and PTSD symptoms were significantly
correlated. In addition, rumination was a significant mediator of the relationship between
Concern over Mistakes and PTSD. Conclusions: These findings help increase understanding
about the relationships of perfectionism and rumination in PTSD and have implications for
the treatment of PTSD.
Keywords: Post traumatic stress disorder, perfectionism, rumination, transdiagnostic.
Introduction
Population estimates report as many as 65% of individuals have experienced a traumatic event,
with 5% estimated to have experienced Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) in their lives
(Australian Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health, 2007). Recognizing that most people
recover in the first 3 months following trauma poses the question as to why only some people
develop PTSD? Furthermore, which factors aid and which impede recovery (Cahill and Foa,
2007)? A potentially useful line of inquiry may be to examine cognitive factors that are known
to be risk and maintaining mechanisms in order to determine the influence of these on PTSD as
they may be particularly salient processes that have been overlooked in understanding PTSD.
Rumination and perfectionism
This study examines whether cognitive processes that have been found to be elevated
across numerous disorders, namely perfectionism and rumination, are associated with PTSD
symptoms. Perfectionism is elevated in eating disorders, depression, and the anxiety disorders
of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, Panic Disorder, and Social Phobia (Egan, Wade and
Reprint requests to Sarah Egan, School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Curtin University, GPO Box U1987,
Perth, WA 6845, Australia. E-mail: s.egan@curtin.edu.au
© British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2013
212 S. J. Egan et al.
Shafran, 2011); consequently it is plausible that perfectionism is also correlated with PTSD.
To the authors’ knowledge, no research to date has investigated whether there is a relationship
between perfectionism and PTSD in a clinical sample. Rumination has also been shown to be
elevated across numerous disorders (Ehring and Watkins, 2008) and is an important variable
to consider as it has been shown to be related to both PTSD and perfectionism (Michael,
Halligan, Clark and Ehlers, 2007; Harris, Pepper and Maack, 2008). Given rumination has
been found to be related to perfectionism and PTSD, if a relationship does exist between
perfectionism and PTSD it is possible that rumination may serve as a mediator.
Perfectionism
A review has identified perfectionism as a risk and maintaining factor across anxiety disorders,
eating disorders and depression (Egan et al., 2011). Of the anxiety disorders, PTSD is an
exception, with no studies examining perfectionism in clinical samples (Egan et al., 2011).
Kawamura, Hunt, Frost and DiBartolo (2001) did find a relationship between a PTSD factor
and perfectionism; however the methodology involved a college student population and factor
derived PTSD.
Rumination has been associated with perfectionism (Flett, Madorsky, Hewitt and Heisel,
2002) and maladaptive perfectionism has been shown to be correlated with rumination and
depressive symptoms (Frost, Marten, Lahart and Rosenblate, 1990). Harris et al. (2008)
found that rumination mediated the relationship between maladaptive perfectionism and
depressive symptoms. This finding is important as those individuals scoring higher on
perfectionism reported higher depressive symptoms through a mechanism of rumination and
this suggests that it would be useful to determine whether rumination is also an important
mediator across other disorders where it is seen as an important clinical feature, such as
PTSD. O’Connor, O’Connor and Marshall (2007) found that the ruminative response style
of brooding was an important mechanism that helped to explain the relationship between
perfectionism and psychological distress. In a longitudinal study Olson and Kwon (2008)
examined the prediction of depressive symptoms over a 4-week period and found that
individuals with high self-oriented and socially-prescribed perfectionism on the Hewitt and
Flett Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (HMPS; Hewitt and Flett, 1991) as well as
brooding rumination experienced the greatest increases in depressive symptoms over time.
It was concluded that the role of perfectionism as a diathesis for depression is dependent on
rumination and that rumination needs to be targeted in those with elevated perfectionism as
they are at risk compared to those low in rumination who did not show the same increases
in depression over time. It is thought therefore that rumination may explain how maladaptive
perfectionism contributes to future depressive symptoms. Hewitt and Flett (2002) explain that
perfectionists engage in rumination about failure. They state that individuals high on Concern
over Mistakes (CM) on the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al.,
1990) may be more likely to ruminate.
Rumination
Ehring and Watkins (2008) reviewed the literature and demonstrated that rumination is
implicated in 13 different disorders, including depression, PSTD, social phobia, obsessive-
compulsive disorder, insomnia, eating disorders, pain disorder, hypochondriasis, alcohol use
PTSD and perfectionism 213
disorder, bipolar disorder and psychosis. Ehring and Watkins (2008) argue that rumination
plays a significant role in the maintenance of psychological disorders. Michael et al. (2007)
explained that rumination can maintain PTSD due to focusing on “why” and “what if”
questions instead of the actual traumatic experience, having positive metacognitive beliefs
about rumination maintaining the PTSD symptoms and cognitive avoidance of emotional
issues. A clinical example of how rumination and perfectionism is relevant in PTSD can be
considered through the example of Kate, who was in treatment for trauma following a sexual
assault. Preceding the trauma Kate described herself as being a perfectionist and having very
high standards across numerous areas of her life (e.g. work, appearance, musical performance)
and she had always excelled in her studies and work. After the trauma she reported significant
rumination over the event, focusing on why it should have happened to her, what she should
have done differently for the event not to have occurred, and rumination over feeling like a
failure as she should have done something different so as to not be in the situation. Kate also
reported feeling that she had failed to meet her standards of being in control of her life and her
emotional states, stating that she viewed the PTSD symptoms she was experiencing as a sign
of her failure to be in control and not be a “strong” person and that she should cope better.
She stated that she was also feeling like a failure as she was not performing in her work to her
standards as she was spending a lot of time ruminating over the trauma and feeling anxious. In
this case, the two processes of rumination and perfectionism appear to be maintaining factors
of her symptoms, and also to interact with each other to keep the symptoms going in a vicious
cycle.
Avoidance is a component of the diagnosis of PTSD and is an important variable in
rumination and perfectionism. Avoidance is a factor that maintains rumination and may
include procrastination, suppression of feelings, and not taking risks (Moberly and Watkins,
2006). Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant (1998) examined thought suppression and rumination as
possible perpetuating factors for PTSD. They found increases in rumination about an event,
in conjunction with suppression of memories, were strongly associated with PTSD. Negative
interpretations of intrusions, persistent medical problems, and rumination at 3 months were
the most significant predictors of PTSD symptoms at one year. They suggested rumination
may impede changes in negative appraisals of trauma events, in addition to the nature of
the trauma memory, similar to the process of thought suppression increasing reliving of
symptoms. Several other studies have found rumination to be one of the strongest predictors
of persistent PTSD (Clohessy and Ehlers, 1999; Murray, Ehlers and Mayou, 2002: Mayou,
Ehlers and Bryant, 2002).
Self-regulation models indicate that negative affect is produced when an individual feels
they are not reaching a goal they are trying to achieve (Ehring and Watkins, 2008) and
Martin and Tesser (1996) purport that rumination occurs when a goal has not been attained.
Ehring and Watkins (2008) explain that cognitive models for rumination suggest people
are “driven” by unresolved goals, with repetitive negative thoughts reflecting unattained
goals, and individuals with psychological disorders who get stuck in rumination may have
more extreme, unattainable or perfectionist goals. This conceptualization is important in
highlighting that rumination may be more likely when an individual displays inflexible
perfectionist standards, which result in unattainable goals. Consequently, this literature links
perfectionism and rumination and suggests the importance of considering the combined roles
of these processes in a range of disorders, not just depression as in studies to date (e.g. Harris
et al., 2008; Olson and Kwon, 2008; O’Connor et al., 2007).
214 S. J. Egan et al.
Rationale
Rumination and perfectionism are two important processes demonstrated to be elevated
across a range of disorders (Ehring and Watkins, 2008; Egan et al., 2011) and given that
the relationship between perfectionism and PTSD in a clinical sample remains unexamined,
the aim of this study was to examine whether perfectionism and rumination account for
PTSD symptoms. Research has not considered that PTSD and perfectionism are both related
to rumination in the same sample nor considered the relative roles of these variables.
Consequently, the purpose of this study is to ascertain if there is an association between
PTSD and perfectionism and, if so, to examine the role of rumination in the process. It was
hypothesized that perfectionism and PTSD would be significantly correlated. Additionally,
it was hypothesized that rumination would act as a significant mediator of the relationship.
The overarching aim of this research was to determine if the processes of rumination and
perfectionism are important in the prediction of PTSD symptoms in a preliminary attempt to
define their role, with a view to exploring potential intervention for these processes should
they be found to be important in explaining PTSD. If rumination and perfectionism are found
to be salient processes in PTSD this may suggest that it would be useful for future research
to examine whether targeting these processes would confer benefit in removing maintaining
factors of PTSD and comorbid disorders.
Method
Participants
There were a total of 30 participants recruited from the Western Australian Department
of Health Sexual Assault Resource Centre (SARC) which is a government public sector
agency located in Perth, Australia that provides free psychological assessment and treatment
for sexual assault. Participants consisted of clients who self-referred to the SARC, were
18 years and older (range 18–81 years), and 75% of the sample were female. Participants
were approached on a voluntary basis, including new referrals and individuals currently in
treatment over a 3-month period. Inclusion criteria were experience of a sexual trauma and
current engagement in psychological counselling at the SARC. The exclusion criterion was
less than one month since experiencing the trauma, so that individuals with Acute Stress
Disorder (ASD) were excluded from the study. Participants were not reimbursed for the study.
Measures
Personal Standards and Concern over Mistakes subscales of the Frost Multidimensional
Perfectionism Scale (FMPS; Frost et al., 1990). The FMPS is a 35-item, self-report measure
of perfectionism consisting of five subscales: (1) Concern over Mistakes (CM); (2) Personal
Standards (PS); (3) Parental Expectations (PE); (4) Parental Criticism (PC); and (5) Doubts
about Actions (DA). The sixth subscale, Organization is not included in the total perfectionism
scoring. A 5-point Likert scale is used, ranging from 1 =Strongly disagree to 5 =Strongly
agree. Higher scores indicate higher levels of perfectionism. This study used the PS and CM
subscales to measure perfectionism as these are the two scales that are most closely linked
to the clinical definition of perfectionism (Shafran and Mansell, 2001). There is extensive
PTSD and perfectionism 215
evidence supporting the reliability and validity of the scale (Enns and Cox, 2002). The internal
consistency in this study was good for both PS; alpha =.84 and CM; alpha =.90.
Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire (CPQ; Fairburn, Cooper and Shafran, 2003) is a 12-
item self-report measure that assesses the degree to which individuals establish self-worth
through achievement of high standards, avoidance of performance related goals and feelings
of failure over the past month. A 4-point Likert scale is used, where 1 =Notatall,and4=
All the time. Higher scores indicate high clinical perfectionism. Steele, O’Shea, Murdock and
Wade (2011) investigated the CPQ in 39 women with eating disorders and found acceptable
reliability (alpha =.83), and convergent validity with the CPQ being significantly correlated
with PS (r=.73) and CM (r=.76) on the FMPS (Frost et al., 1990). The internal consistency
for the CPQ in this study was acceptable with a Cronbach’s alpha of .81.
Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) from the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ; Nolen-
Hoeksema, 1991). The RRS is a self-report measure of rumination. Respondents rate each
questionnaire item on a scale of ranging from 1 =Almost never to 4 =Almost always. The
RRS was criticised for some of the items being highly correlated with the Beck Depression
Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelsohn, Mock and Erbaugh, 1961). Treynor, Gonzalez and
Nolen-Hoeksema (2003) factor analysed the RRS, identifying the principal components of
depression and now these items are able to be removed (to make the 10 items). Thus for this
study the depression related items were removed, leaving 10 items. The 10 items selected from
the RSQ’s RRS self-report measure reflect two principle components of rumination, Reflective
Pondering and Brooding. Reflective Pondering is defined as engagement in problem-solving
to help alleviate negative affect and Brooding is considered to be thinking anxiously or
worrying, in a maladaptive fashion, over some unachieved standard (Harris et al., 2008).
Moberly and Watkins (2006) calculated the Cronbach’s alpha for the subscales of Brooding
and Reflective Pondering at .76 and .76 respectively. The internal consistency in this study
was alpha =.89 for Brooding and .67 for Reflective Pondering. The internal consistency for
the combined subscales of Reflective Pondering and Brooding of the RRS in this study was
good, with alpha =.87.
Post Traumatic Stress Checklist (Civilian) for DSM-IV (PCL-C; Weathers, Huska and
Keane, 1991) is a 17-item self-report measure of the DSM-IV symptoms for PSTD.
Respondents are asked to rate how much they were “bothered by that problem in the past
month”. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 =Not at all to 5 =Extremely;
higher scores indicate higher levels of PTSD. This study used the recommended cut-off of 50
to indicate a diagnosis of PTSD (Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska and Keane, 1993). The PCL
has been found to have good psychometric properties, with high internal consistency (alpha =
.96) (Keane, Brief, Pratt and Miller, 2007). Weathers et al. (1993) found test-retest reliabilities
were robust (.96). The internal consistency in this study was a Cronbach’s alpha of .89.
Procedure
Ethics approval for the study was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC)
at Curtin University and King Edward Memorial Hospital. Clients engaging in counselling
for PTSD at SARC were offered the opportunity to complete questionnaires anonymously by
the receptionist prior to entering their appointment for counselling. Counsellors mentioned
to their clients at the end of the counselling session that the research study was taking place.
SARC also services clients at various metropolitan offices around Perth. Counsellors at these
216 S. J. Egan et al.
Table 1. Mean (and Standard Deviation) of measures of PTSD,
perfectionism and rumination
Measure Mean SD Range
PCL-C 54.17 12.19 33–82
RRS 28.57 6.87 16–39
CM 29.73 7.89 9–45
PS 24.13 5.71 13–32
CPQ 28.83 6.61 18–42
Notes: PCL-C =Posttraumatic Stress Checklist Civilian Version
(Weathers, Huska and Keane, 1991); RRS =Ruminative Response
Scale (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991); CM =Concern over Mistakes
subscale of FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); PS =Personal Standards
subscale of FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); CPQ =Clinical Perfectionism
Questionnaire (Fairburn et al., 2003)
outreach offices offered clients the opportunity to participate in the study, with clients able
to mail back their responses in the reply paid envelope to Curtin University. The participant
information sheet was given to each participant; this outlined the aims and procedures of the
study. The second author worked at SARC as a counsellor but due to ethical issues did not
recruit her own clients to participate in the research.
Results
An adequate sample for testing the proposed model was calculated to be 30 participants. The
most complex regression model consisted of four predictors and GPower 3 suggested that 30
participants were required for an 80% chance of capturing a “large” relationship between the
Dependent Variable and each of the predictors (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and Lang, 2009). No
missing data were found and all correlation and regression assumptions were met.
Descriptive data
Means and standard deviations are displayed in Table 1. The mean that was found for PTSD
in this sample (M=54.17) slightly surpassed the Weathers et al. (1991) criteria of 50 which
indicates a diagnosis of PTSD. The sample range on the PTSD measure was from 33 to 82,
with 11 participants not reaching the criteria and 19 reaching the criteria, or 63% reaching
the cut-off for PTSD criteria. This emphasized that a majority of the population was indeed
a clinical PTSD population, and the mean score of the sample indicated that on average the
sample was in a clinical PTSD range.
Correlation analysis
A series of bivariate correlations were conducted, first in order to establish whether the
variables were significantly associated and the proposed mediation model could be tested. As
seen in Table 2, all variables were significantly associated, with the exception of PS and PTSD.
It is noteworthy that perfectionism had a strong correlation with PTSD on both measures
PTSD and perfectionism 217
Table 2. Summary of bivarate correlations for PTSD, rumination
and perfectionism measures
Measure 12 3 4 5
1. PCL-C .68∗∗ .57∗∗ .24 .69∗∗
2. RRS .69∗∗ .44.67∗∗
3. CM .45.70∗∗
4. PS .50∗∗
5. CPQ
Notes: PCL-C =Posttraumatic Stress Checklist Civilian Version
(Weathers et al., 1991); RRS =Ruminative Response Scale (Nolen-
Hoeksema, 1991); CM =Concern over Mistakes subscale of FMPS
(Frost et al., 1990); PS =Personal Standards subscale of FMPS
(Frost et al., 1990); CPQ =Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire
(Fairburn et al., 2003), p<.05,∗∗p<.01, two tailed.
Table 3. Unstandardized (B) and standardized (β) regression coefficients and
squared-semi partial correlations (sr2) for each predictor in the regression models
predicting rumination and PTSD for the CPQ (N=30)
Var i a b le B[95% CI] SE βsr2p
RRS (DV)
CPQ .70 [.397, .995] .15 .67 .45 .001∗∗
PTSD (DV)
RRS .70 [.074, 1.323] .30 .39 .29 .03
CPQ .79 [.138, 1.436] .32 .43 .32 .02
Notes: PCL-C =Posttraumatic Stress Checklist Civilian Version (Weathers et al., 1991);
RRS =Ruminative Response Scale (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991); CM =Concern over
Mistakes subscale of FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); PS =Personal Standards subscale of
FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); CPQ =Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire (Fairburn et al.,
2003), p<.05; ∗∗ p<.01; ∗∗∗ p<.001. SE =Standard Error, CI =Confidence Interval
of clinical/negative perfectionism, on CM (r=.57) and the CPQ (r=.69). It can also be
seen that the relationship between PTSD and rumination was strong (r=.68). Furthermore,
there was also a significant association between rumination and all perfectionism measures.
This indicates, as hypothesized, that significant relationships do exist between the variables
of PTSD, perfectionism and rumination.
Standard multiple regression analyses and mediation models
In order to determine the beta-weights of the proposed mediation model for the two
perfectionism measures, a series of two-step standard multiple regression analyses were
conducted, as seen in Tables 3 and 4. As PS was not correlated with PTSD it was not added
to the standard multiple regression analyses.
The following set of standard regression analyses used the CPQ as the perfectionism meas-
ure to determine the beta-weights to test the proposed rumination mediation model between
the perfectionism and PTSD relationship. Rumination was the dependent variable in the first
218 S. J. Egan et al.
.669
,
p
= .000 .393
,
p
= .030
PTSD
(PCL-C)
Rumination
(RRS)
Clinical
perfectionism
(CPQ)
.427, p = .019
Figure 1. Model for rumination as a mediator between clinical perfectionism and PTSD
regression analysis for the predictor CPQ. Clinical perfectionism accounted for a significant
45% of the variance in rumination, R2=.45, adjusted R2=.43, F(1, 28) =22.74, p=.0001.
The second regression analysis placed PTSD as the dependent variable and rumination and
CPQ as predictors. Rumination and the CPQ accounted for a significant 56% of the variance
in PTSD, R2=.56, adjusted R2=.53,F(2, 27) =17.32, p=.0001. Unstandardized (B) and
standardized (β) Regression Coefficients and Squared-Semi Partial (or “part”) correlations
(sr2) for each predictor in the regression models are reported in Table 3.
The next step was to apply the path coefficients generated by the aforementioned regression
analyses to the mediation model as can be seen in Figure 1. The beta weights (i.e. the
standardized regression weights) for the predictors provided the path coefficients. The
model displayed in Figure 1 revealed that significant pathways exist between the CPQ and
rumination, the CPQ and PTSD, and rumination and PTSD. Given that a significant direct
pathway existed between the CPQ and PTSD it can be concluded that rumination does not
fully mediate the relationship between the CPQ and PTSD. The next step was to determine
the impact of rumination on the CPQ and PTSD. In order to test the mediation model Sobel’s
test statistic (Sobel, 1982) was used. The test was significant, z=2.087, p=.036, indicating
a partial mediation (Baron and Kenny, 1986).
Rumination was the dependent variable in the first regression analysis when CM was the
predictor. CM accounted for a significant 47% of the variance in rumination, R2=.47,
adjusted R2=.45,F(1, 28) =25.03, p=.0001. The second regression analysis placed
PTSD as the dependent variable and rumination and CM as predictors. Rumination and CM
accounted for a significant 48% of the variance in PTSD, R2=.48, adjusted R2=.44, F (2,
27) =12.59, p=.0001. Unstandardized (B) and standardized (β) Regression Coefficients and
Squared-Semi Partial (or “part”) correlations (sr2) for each predictor in the regression models
are reported in Table 4.
Following these regression analyses the next step was to again apply the path coefficients
to the mediation model. The model displayed in Figure 2 revealed that significant pathways
exist between CM and rumination, rumination and PTSD, but not for CM and PTSD. Given
that a nonsignificant pathway existed between the CM and PTSD, it can be concluded that
rumination fully mediated the relationship between CM and PTSD.
Discussion
The hypothesis that there would be a significant relationship between PTSD and perfectionism
was supported. In addition, it was found that rumination was a mediator of the relationship
between PTSD and perfectionism when measured using the CM subscale of the FMPS
PTSD and perfectionism 219
Table 4. Unstandardized (B) and Standardized (β) regression coefficients and
squared-semi partial correlations (sr2) for each predictor in the regression models
predicting rumination and PTSD for the CPQ, (N=30)
Var i a b le B[95% CI] SE βsr2p
RRS (DV)
CM .60 [.353, .843] .12 .69 .47 .001∗∗
PTSD (DV)
RRS .96 [.267, 1.655] .34 .54 .16 .008
CM .31 [.294, .913] .29 .20 .02 .30
Notes: PCL-C =Posttraumatic Stress Checklist Civilian Version (Weathers et al., 1991);
RRS =Ruminative Response Scale (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991); CM =Concern over
Mistakes subscale of FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); PS =Personal Standards subscale of
FMPS (Frost et al., 1990); CPQ =Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire (Fairburn et al.,
2003), p<.05; ∗∗ p<.01; ∗∗∗ p<.001. SE =Standard Error, CI =Confidence Interval
.542, p= .008
PTSD
(PCL-C)
Rumination
(RRS)
CM
.687, p= .000
.200, p = .302
Figure 2. Model for rumination as a mediator of Concern over Mistakes and PTSD
(Frost et al., 1990). Consequently, results supported the hypothesis that perfectionism would
be a significant predictor of PTSD symptoms, but this relationship would be mediated by
rumination.
Implications
To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to date to show a relationship between
perfectionism and PTSD in a clinical sample, and this was using both the CM subscale of
the FMPS (Frost et al., 1990) and the CPQ (Fairburn et al., 2003). This finding is consistent
with Egan et al.’s (2011) review of perfectionism where CM is associated with symptoms
across many disorders, and gives further evidence to this aspect of perfectionism being a
transdiagnostic process. The finding that PS was not associated with PTSD symptoms is
also consistent with Egan et al.’s review that showed PS is generally not associated with
anxiety disorder symptoms. Moreover, the results lend support to the notion of PS being not
consistently associated with psychopathology, and the distinction between PS representing
“positive striving” and CM representing “maladaptive evaluative concerns” (see Stoeber and
Otto, 2006 for a review).
The general pattern of significant bivariate correlations existing across most measures
is noteworthy as it highlights the related and to some degree overlapping nature of the
variables. The processes of rumination and perfectionism are associated, and both of these
220 S. J. Egan et al.
processes are also associated with PTSD symptoms. The finding of a significant relationship
between rumination and PTSD is consistent with previous research (Michael et al., 2007). The
finding that rumination and perfectionism were significantly correlated also supports previous
research (Flett et al., 2002).
Rumination did fully mediate the relationship between perfectionism and PTSD, when the
CM subscale of the FMPS (Frost et al., 1990) was used, but not when the CPQ (Fairburn
et al.,2003) was utilised as the measure of perfectionism. The finding that rumination acted
fully as a mediator with CM but not the CPQ in PTSD requires examination. This finding may
indicate the CPQ warrants further work on validity as a measure of perfectionism. The FMPS
scales are a widely used and validated measure of perfectionism, while evidence supporting
the validity of the CPQ is scant. Hewitt and Flett (2002) have explained that perfectionists
engage in rumination about failure; hence perfectionists high on Concern over Mistakes are
likely to ruminate. The current result of rumination acting as a mediator between CM and
PTSD is therefore in keeping with this previous research. The results are also consistent with
rumination being found as a mediator between maladaptive perfectionism and depressive
symptoms (see Harris et al., 2008) and the brooding ruminative response style mediating
perfectionism and psychological distress (O’Connor et al., 2007). The findings overall lend
support to the rationale of both perfectionism and rumination acting as transdiagnostic
processes across different disorders (Egan et al., 2011; Ehring and Watkins, 2008).
Limitations of the study
The following limitations are acknowledged. First, the sample consisted of sexual violence
survivors who may compose a unique interpersonal trauma population compared to that of
other traumas. Second, the study was voluntary and it is possible that the sample selection
is biased; for example, individuals with higher degrees of perfectionism may have chosen to
participate in the study. Third, the internal consistency of the Reflective Pondering scale was
poor (Cronbach’s alpha of .67). Finally, as only 63% of the sample had a diagnosis of PTSD,
it may be useful in the future to restrict a further sample to only those who meet a diagnosis
of PTSD. Despite this, the mean score of the sample was in a clinical PTSD range, and as it
was a preliminary study, it was useful to include all cases including those that did not fully
reach diagnostic criteria.
A major limitation of the study was that a cross-sectional design was utilised and while the
results are useful in identifying correlations between variables, causal statements regarding
the relationship between perfectionism, rumination and PTSD cannot be made. This was a
preliminary attempt to establish whether relationships exist. However, in the context of the
data being cross-sectional, conclusions need to be interpreted with caution as the design limits
the interpretation of the mediation models, as they do not show temporal precedence. It is
unclear whether perfectionism predicts rumination or vice versa, and thus future longitudinal
research is required to determine the direction of these relationships.
Strengths of the study
The aim to investigate whether a relationship existed between perfectionism and PTSD in
a clinical sample addressed a significant gap in the literature. In addition, it was the first
study to date to investigate rumination as a mediator of the relationship. The present results
PTSD and perfectionism 221
may be useful in suggesting future research in to the role of perfectionism and rumination
in PTSD and if intervening with these processes may potentially be useful in treatment for
PTSD.
Future directions
In terms of understanding the relationship between rumination, perfectionism and PTSD,
future research could aim to investigate whether perfectionism has a unique contribution to the
relationship between perfectionism and PTSD. This could be achieved by using a measure of
PTSD related rumination as a dependent variable, and to include perfectionism measures, in
addition to other important variables including responsibility and self-esteem as independent
variables, with a larger sample. It would be useful also if possible to conduct longitudinal
research to determine if levels of perfectionism preceding a trauma can account for greater
likelihood of development of PTSD symptoms post trauma. Despite this being a difficult study
to conduct as it needs to follow people pre- and post-trauma, it may be possible to conduct
the research within groups where there are more frequent experiences of trauma (for example
emergency workers).
It would also be useful for future research to examine the content of rumination in PTSD to
determine if this is specific to PTSD rather than the typical rumination pattern in depression.
For example, it would be useful to determine if perfectionism is related to specific trauma-
related negative appraisals.
The clinical implications of this research indicate that a useful direction for future research
would be to investigate interventions for the relationship between perfectionism and PTSD,
and the additional impact of rumination that this study has established. This may be important
given that perfectionism has been shown to be a maintaining factor for other disorders, and
also impedes treatment (Egan et al., 2011). Furthermore, the preliminary findings from this
study suggest that it would be useful for future research to determine whether targeting
rumination and perfectionism in PTSD treatment is effective. Treatments exist for both of
these processes but as far as the authors are aware there have not been any studies for
treatment of perfectionism in PTSD. There has been some research examining the treatment
of rumination in PTSD; for example Wells (2009) described a treatment using metacognitive
therapy to address rumination in PTSD. This encompasses analysing the advantages and
disadvantages of rumination, with provision of training to acknowledge symptoms as they
are occurring, referred to as detached mindfulness and rumination/worry postponement.
Further research is required on the effectiveness of this approach, and it would be useful
to investigate if also targeting perfectionism may lead to further amelioration of PTSD
symptoms.
Conclusions
If future studies, using prospective designs, confirm the relationship between perfectionism
and PTSD and if treatments that target perfectionism and rumination are found to lessen the
impact of PTSD, then this research may potentially help to increase treatment efficacy in
PTSD.
222 S. J. Egan et al.
References
Australian Centre for Posttraumatic Mental Health (2007). The Australian Guidelines for the
Treatment of Adults with Acute Stress Disorder and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder [Brochure].
Melbourne, Australia: Author.
Baron, R. M. and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelsohn, M., Mock, J. and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory or
measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Cahill, S. P. and Foa, E. B. (2007). Psychological theories of PTSD. In M. J. Friedman, T. M. Keane
and P. A. Resick (Eds.), Handbook of PTSD: science and practice (pp. 55–77). New York: Guilford
Press.
Clohessy, S. and Ehlers, A. (1999). PTSD symptoms, response to intrusive memories and coping in
ambulance service workers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 251–265.
Egan,S.J.,Wade,T.D.andShafran,R.(2011). Perfectionism as a transdiagnostic process: a clinical
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 203–212.
Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A. and Bryant, B. (1998). Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic
stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508–519.
Ehring, T. and Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process.
International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1, 192–205.
Enns, M. W. and Cox, B. J. (2002). Nature and assessment of perfectionism. In G. L. Flett and P. L.
Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: theory, research and treatment (pp. 33–62). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Fairburn, C., Cooper, Z. and Shafran, R. (2003). The Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire. Oxford,
UK: Unpublished manuscript.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. and Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using
GPower 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–
1160.
Flett, G. L., Madorsky, D., Hewitt, P. L. and Heisel, M. J. (2002). Perfectionism cognitions,
rumination and psychological distress. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy,
20, 33–47.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P. A., Lahart, C. and Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468.
Harris, P. W., Pepper, C. M. and Maack, D. J. (2008). The relationship between maladaptive
perfectionism and depressive symptoms: the mediating role of rumination. Personality and Individual
differences, 44, 150–160.
Hewitt, P. L. and Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: conceptualization,
assessment and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
456–470.
Hewitt, P. L. and Flett, G. L. (2002). Perfectionism and stress processes. In G. L. Flett and Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: theory, research and treatment (pp. 255–284). Washington, DC. American
Psychological Association.
Kawamura, K., Hunt, S., Frost, R. and DiBartolo, P. (2001). Perfectionism, anxiety, and depression:
are the relationships independent? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 291–301.
Keane, T. M., Brief, D. J., Pratt, E. M. and Miller, M. W. (2007). Assessment of PTSD and its
comorbidities in adults. In M. J. Friedman., T. M. Keane and P. A. Resick (Eds.), Handbook of PTSD:
science and practice (pp. 279–305). New York: Guilford Press.
Martin, L. L. and Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Ruminative
Thought (pp. 306–326). New York: Guilford.
PTSD and perfectionism 223
Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A. and Bryant, B. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle
accidents: 3-year follow-up of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
40, 665–675.
Michael, T., Halligan, S. L., Clark, D. M. and Ehlers, A. (2007). Rumination in Posttraumatic Stress
Disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 24, 307–317.
Moberly, N. J. and Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing mode influences the relationship between trait
rumination and emotional vulnerability. Behavior Therapy, 37, 281–291.
Murray, J., Ehlers, A. and Mayou, R. A. (2002). Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder: two
prospective studies of motor vehicle accident survivors. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 363–368.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on their on the duration of
depressive episode. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.
O’Connor, D., O’Connor, R. and Marshall, R. (2007). Perfectionism and psychological distress:
evidence of the mediating effects of rumination, European Journal of Personality, 21, 429–452.
Olson, M. L. and Kwon, P. (2008). Brooding perfectionism: refining the roles of rumination and
perfectionism in the etiology of depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 788–802.
Shafran, R. and Mansell, W. (2001). Perfectionism and psychopathology: a review of research and
treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 879–906.
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models.
In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1982 (pp. 290–312). Washington DC: American
Sociological Association.
Steele, A. L., O’Shea, A., Murdock, A. and Wade, T. D. (2011). Perfectionism and its relation to
overevaluation of weight and shape and depression in an eating disorder sample. International Journal
of Eating Disorders, 44, 459–464.
Stoeber, J. and Otto, K. (2006). Conceptions of perfectionism: approaches, evidence, challenges.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 295–319.
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R. and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric
analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.
Weathers, F. W., Huska, J. A. and Keane, T. M. (1991). PCL-C for DSM-IV. Boston: National Center
for PTSD – Behavioral Science Division.
Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J. and Keane, T. (1993). The PTSD Checklist
(PCL): reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility. Paper presented at the Annual Convention of the
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, San Antonio, TX, October.
Wells, A. (2009). Metacognitive Therapy for Anxiety and Depression. New York: Guilford Press.
... In contrast, individuals with high separation anxiety are more worried about losing contact and connection with their significant others (Diamond & Keefe, 2024) following major disasters, tend to develop self-doubt and relationship alienation, and are overly sensitive and vigilant in social interactions (Holmbeck & Leake, 1999), which may trigger rumination (Rodríguez-Menchón et al., 2021). This may directly increase feelings of tension, dysphoria, or hopelessness (Ehlers & Clark, 2000;Watkins & Roberts, 2020) because it provides internal retrieval cues that may trigger memories of traumatic events, increase negative evaluations of events, and ultimately lead to PTSD (Egan et al., 2014;Moulds et al., 2020;Zhou et al., 2021;Zhou & Wu, 2016). Therefore, it was suggested that separation anxiety may predict PTSD via rumination. ...
... This leads to the tendency to assume a lack of support from others (Flannery, 1990;Manicavasagar & Silove, 2020), which triggers separation anxiety in adolescents. In addition, rumination directly increases tension, dysphoria, or hopelessness (Ehlers & Clark, 2000;Watkins & Roberts, 2020) in adolescents and also triggers intrusive memories of the earthquake, thereby enhancing later PTSD symptoms (Egan et al., 2014;Moulds et al., 2020;Zhou & Wu, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and separation anxiety often show high comorbidity after trauma, which complicates the course of disease and treatment response. However, it is unclear how PTSD and separation anxiety comorbidity occur. According to the cognitive model of PTSD, rumination may be a key cognitive process linking the two constructs. Thus, this study aimed to examine the relation between PTSD and separation anxiety and to assess the mediating role of rumination in this relation among adolescents after trauma. Method: A follow-up survey was conducted among 204 adolescent survivors of the Ya’an earthquake, at 6 months (T1), 12 months (T2), and 18 months (T3). A random-intercept cross-lagged panel model was used for data analysis to disentangle within-person dynamics from stable between-person differences. Results: The results showed that at the within-person level, PTSD increased separation anxiety via rumination, and early PTSD further exacerbated later PTSD through increased rumination and separation anxiety. Conclusions: These findings highlight the mediating role of rumination, elucidate the relation between PTSD and separation anxiety, and provide theoretical support for the development of transdiagnostic clinical interventions and treatments.
... When considering the outcomes of traumatic events, two essential factors that can foster potential growth are the extent to which the event poses a significant challenge and the degree to which this challenge activates cognitive processes that focus on the life crisis event and its implications (Cann et al., 2009). The cognition at the onset of trauma is spontaneous, and it may focus primarily on the negative aspects of the traumatic event (Lindstrom et al., 2013), leading individuals to experience negative emotional reactions which may induce posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Egan et al., 2014). With increasing posttraumatic time, people's cognition may shift from the negative aspects of the traumatic event to engagement in active cognitive processing of trauma-related cues (Janoff-Bulman, 2010), which can help individuals reconstruct their understanding of the events. ...
... Children who are exposed to physical abuse and psychological maltreatment may react to adversity by becoming perfectionistic to reduce humiliation or to gain control in an unpredictable environment (Flett, Hewitt, Oliver & Macdonald, 2002). Rumination, which seems to be the maintaining factor of traumatic experience is also a feature in perfectionism and therefore could be the link (Egan, Hattaway & Kane, 2013). Recent research suggests that living in a dysfunctional family and childhood abuse can predict socially prescribed perfectionism (Chen, Hewitt & Flett, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Due to its impact on mental health and well‐being, fear of happiness is beginning to receive more attention in research. This study, conducted in Hungary, explored the relationship between fear of happiness, perfectionism, loneliness, hopelessness, and academic burnout. Participants aged between 18 and 35 years ( N = 1,148, M = 22 years, SD = 4.5) completed an online questionnaire that included self‐report measures of these constructs. In the present study, males showed higher levels of fear of happiness and perfectionism than did females. Analyses also revealed that both adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism, academic burnout, loneliness, and hopelessness were positively related to fear of happiness. In multiple regression analysis, gender had a positive and age had a negative role in the levels of fear of happiness. Maladaptive perfectionism, academic burnout, loneliness, and hopelessness were positive predictors, while adaptive perfectionism almost reached statistical significance. These findings highlight the predictive role of previously unexplored variables in fear of happiness.
... Moreover, correlation analyses suggested that higher endorsement of repetitive negative thinking (i.e., worry, rumination and anger rumination) was associated with higher levels of personal standards perfectionism and concern over mistakes perfectionism. These findings are congruent with those of previous studies, which were run with general population samples (Kawamura et al., 2001;Santanello & Gardner, 2007;Stöber & Joormann, 2001), undergraduate students (Besharat & Shahidi, 2010;Flett et al., 2002;Harris et al., 2008) and in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (Egan et al., 2014). To our knowledge, our study is the first to report on correlation between repetitive negative thinking and perfectionism in patients with EDs. ...
Article
Introduction: Using the Self‐Regulatory Executive Function model as a basis, this study explored whether, in patients with eating disorders (EDs), metacognitions and repetitive negative thinking are associated with higher levels of perfectionisms. Methods: One hundred twenty‐three outpatients with EDs were recruited. Perfectionism, metacognitive beliefs, worry, rumination, anger rumination, affective and eating symptoms were assessed. Correlation and hierarchal regression analyses were run. Results: Higher endorsement of positive beliefs about worry was associated with higher levels of ‘personal standards perfectionism’. Higher endorsement of positive beliefs about worry, need to control thoughts, worry and rumination was associated with higher levels of ‘concern over mistakes perfectionism’. Conclusions: Among patients with EDs, perfectionism appears to be associated with the endorsement of dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs, worry and rumination. Dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs and repetitive negative thinking could be suitable therapeutic targets to reduce the levels of perfectionism among patients with EDs.
... This study demonstrated that intrusive rumination not only exerts a significant positive predictive effect on academic burnout but also indirectly exacerbates academic burnout through PTSS. This aligns with prior research indicating a positive correlation between intrusive rumination and PTSS [60,61], whereby individuals experiencing trauma tend to engage in intrusive rumination and focus on negative aspects, leading to the development and persistence of post-traumatic stress symptoms. In addition, the study results are consistent with previous empirical research on ethnic minority adolescents in China and adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic [39,62], demonstrating a positive predictive relationship between PTSS and academic burnout. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The COVID-19 pandemic has had a significant negative impact on public health, prompting scholarly research in related fields. In this context, the present study reveals the psychological characteristics of adolescents in ethnic minority areas of China approximately five months after the 2020 outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic, explores the relationship between intrusive rumination and academic burnout, and examines the role of post-traumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) and cognitive reappraisal in the relationship to provide an empirical foundation for developing effective psychological interventions for adolescents in the wake of the pandemic. Methods Based on cluster sampling, 941 middle school students (65.36% female, 74.71% senior high, Mage=15.95) in ethnic minority areas of China were surveyed using the Event Related Rumination Scale, Adolescent Academic Burnout Scale, Post-traumatic Stress Checklist Scale, Emotion Regulation Strategy Scale, and a self-designed demographic questionnaire. Results During the COVID-19 pandemic, 7.44% of Chinese ethnic minority adolescents in our study sample were classified as PTSD positive, and 10.95% exhibited partial PTSD. Intrusive rumination significantly predicted academic burnout, and PTSS played a key mediating role between the two, accounting for 58.51% of the total effect. After controlling for PTSS, cognitive reappraisal moderated the effects of intrusive rumination on academic burnout. Specifically, the effect of intrusive rumination on academic burnout decreased with improvement in cognitive reappraisal. Conclusions Intrusive rumination indirectly affected academic burnout in adolescents through PTSS as a crucial mediator, and the remnant direct effect was alleviated by cognitive reappraisal. This finding emphasises the importance of adopting a comprehensive approach that encompasses cognitive, emotional, and physiological symptoms to understand and address academic burnout among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic.
... There is considerable literature on the role of rumination as a mediator of the perfectionism-psychological distress link. However, we could identify only two published studies that specifically examine this indirect pathway in a clinical sample (Smith et al., 2020;Egan et al., 2014), highlighting the need to explore these relationships in the same. Currently, there are no published studies from India on the mediating role of rumination in clinical samples. ...
Article
Full-text available
Rumination has been studied extensively as a transdiagnostic variable. The present study explored the relationship between rumination, perfectionism, self-compassion, depression, and anxiety severity in forty-nine adults, with a primary diagnosis of an anxiety disorder. They were assessed on the Ruminative Response Scale of the Response Style Questionnaire, Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale-Short form, Self-compassion scale, the Overall Anxiety Severity Scale, and the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Rumination was positively associated with all three dimensions of perfectionism, depression, and anxiety severity, and was negatively associated with self-compassion. Both socially prescribed perfectionism and self-compassion predicted rumination, and rumination was a significant predictor of depression and anxiety severity. Mediation analysis indicated that rumination mediated the relationship between socially prescribed perfectionism and depression severity. These findings reiterate our understanding of the role of transdiagnostic factors, such as, rumination, and perfectionism in anxiety disorders and the significance of self-compassion-based interventions in the alleviation of anxiety and depression.
... Especially, mistrust and punitiveness are schemas that may develop as a reaction to past traumatic experiences (i.e. mistrusting others or blaming oneself for harm being done) but also act as risk factors, while neglecting oneself (self-sacrifice) as well as setting high standards for yourself (unrelenting standards) could put one at risk for developing post-traumatic stress disorder after experiencing trauma (Egan et al., 2014;Reis et al., 2016). ...
Article
Purpose: Although schema therapy has been predominantly applied to treat personality disorders, interest into its application in other clinical disorders is growing. Central to schema therapy are Early Maladaptive Schemas (EMS) and Schema Modes. Since existing EMS and Schema Modes were primarily developed in the context of personality disorders, their relevance for clinical disorders is unclear. Methods: We conducted a systematic review of the presence of EMS and Schema Modes in clinical disorders according to DSM criteria. Per disorder, we evaluated which EMS and Schema Modes were more pronounced in comparison with clinical as well as non-clinical control groups and which EMS and Schema Modes were most highly endorsed within the disorder. Results: Although evidence concerning EMS was scarce for several disorders, and only few studies on Schema Modes survived inclusion criteria, we identified meaningful relationships and patterns for EMS and Schema Modes in various clinical disorders. Conclusions: The present review highlights the relevance of EMS and Schema Modes for clinical disorders beyond personality disorders. Depending on the theme of the representation, EMS act as vulnerabilities both across diagnoses and for specific disorders. Thus, EMS and resulting Schema Modes are potential, valuable targets for the prevention and treatment of clinical disorders.
Article
Objectives Perfectionism is a common transdiagnostic problem that may lead to substantial distress and functional impairments. Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) is an effective treatment for perfectionism. However, the existing significant barriers to access and utilization of mental health services, including among college students, demand the development of low‐intensity accessible interventions. The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of a low‐intensity CBT‐based self‐help gamified app developed specifically for perfectionism in a sample of college students. Methods Participants completed assessments of perfectionism, related symptoms, emotional burden and functional impairments at pretreatment, posttreatment and at one‐month follow‐up. Results Compared with the waitlist condition ( n = 35), the app condition ( n = 35) demonstrated a significant and greater reduction in perfectionism, obsessive‐compulsive symptoms, functional impairments and subjective ratings of emotional burden. Conclusions Results suggest that a brief, daily app‐based game‐like intervention targeting maladaptive perfectionistic beliefs may be a viable, low‐cost alternative to traditional CBT treatments for vulnerable populations on college campuses.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Dissociative symptoms during trauma predict post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but they are often transient. It is controversial whether they predict chronic PTSD over and above what can be predicted from other post-trauma symptoms. To investigate prospectively the relationship between dissociative symptoms before, during and after a trauma and other psychological predictors, and chronic PTSD. Two samples of 27 and 176 road traffic accident survivors were recruited. Patients were assessed shortly after the accident and followed at intervals over the next 6 months. Assessments included measures of dissociation, memory fragmentation, data-driven processing, rumination and PTSD symptoms. All measures of dissociation, particularly persistent dissociation 4 weeks after the accident, predicted chronic PTSD severity at 6 months. Dissociative symptoms predicted subsequent PTSD over and above the other PTSD symptom clusters. Memory fragmentation and data-driven processing also predicted PTSD. Rumination about the accident was among the strongest predictors of subsequent PTSD symptoms. Persistent dissociation and rumination 4 weeks after trauma are more useful in identifying those patients who are likely to develop chronic PTSD than initial reactions.