ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Several research studies investigating the effectiveness of the different treatments have demonstrated that exposure-based therapies are more suitable and effective than others for the treatment of anxiety disorders. Traditionally, exposure may be achieved in two manners: in vivo, with direct contact to the stimulus, or by imagery, in the person’s imagination. However, despite its effectiveness, both types of exposure present some limitations that supported the use of Virtual Reality (VR). But is VR always an effective stressor? Are the technological breakdowns that may appear during such an experience a possible risk for its effectiveness? To answer these questions we compared changes following the exposure to an academic examination, one of the most universal examples of real-life stressors, in a sample of 39 undergraduate students. The same experience was offered using text (TX), audio (AU), video (VD), and VR. However, in the virtual environment we manipulated the experience introducing technological breakdowns. The Post Media Questionnaire (PMQ) and the Slater-Usoh-Steed Presence Questionnaire (SUS) were administered to each participant in order to evaluated self-report measures of anxiety and relaxation and the level of presence experience during media exposure. Electrocardiogram (ECG), Thoracic Respiration Signal (RSP) and Facial corrugator supercilii muscle Electromyography (EMG) were recorded in order to obtain objective measures of subjects’ emotional state. Analyses conducted on PMQ showed a significant increase in anxiety scores and a mirror decrease in relax scores after all our emotional procedures, showing that all the condition were effective in inducing a negative emotional response. Psychometric scores and psychophysiological indexes showed that VR was less effective than other procedures in eliciting stress responses. Moreover, we did not observe significative difference in SUS scores: VR induced a sense of presence similar to that experienced during the exposition to other media. Technological breakdowns significantly reduce the possibility of VR eliciting emotions related to complex real-life stressors. Without a high sense of presence, the significant advantages offered by VR disappear and its emotional induction abilities are even lower than the ones provided by much cheaper media. Trial registration Trial registration number: NCT01683617
Content may be subject to copyright.
R E S E A R C H A R T I C L E Open Access
Is virtual reality always an effective stressors for
exposure treatments? some insights from a
controlled trial
Federica Pallavicini
1*
, Pietro Cipresso
1
, Simona Raspelli
1
, Alessandra Grassi
2
, Silvia Serino
1
, Cinzia Vigna
1
,
Stefano Triberti
1
, Marco Villamira
3
, Andrea Gaggioli
1
and Giuseppe Riva
1,2
Abstract
Background: Several research studies investigating the effectiveness of the different treatments have demonstrated
that exposure-based therapies are more suitable and effective than others for the treatment of anxiety disorders.
Traditionally, exposure may be achieved in two manners: in vivo, with direct contact to the stimulus, or by imagery,
in the persons imagination. However, despite its effectiveness, both types of exposure present some limitations
that supported the use of Virtual Reality (VR). But is VR always an effective stressor? Are the technological
breakdowns that may appear during such an experience a possible risk for its effectiveness?
Methods: To answer these questions we compared changes following the exposure to an academic examination,
one of the most universal examples of real-life stressors, in a sample of 39 undergraduate students. The same
experience was offered using text (TX), audio (AU), video (VD), and VR. However, in the virtual environment we
manipulated the experience introducing technological breakdowns. The Post Media Questionnaire (PMQ) and the
Slater-Usoh-Steed Presence Questionnaire (SUS) were administered to each participant in order to evaluated self-
report measures of anxiety and relaxation and the level of presence experience during media exposure.
Electrocardiogram (ECG), Thoracic Respiration Signal (RSP) and Facial corrugator supercilii muscle Electromyography
(EMG) were recorded in order to obtain objective measures of subjectsemotional state.
Results: Analyses conducted on PMQ showed a significant increase in anxiety scores and a mirror decrease in relax
scores after all our emotional procedures, showing that all the condition were effective in inducing a negative
emotional response. Psychometric scores and psychophysiological indexes showed that VR was less effective than
other procedures in eliciting stress responses. Moreover, we did not observe significative difference in SUS scores:
VR induced a sense of presence similar to that experienced during the exposition to other media.
Conclusions: Technological breakdowns significantly reduce the possibility of VR eliciting emotions related to
complex real-life stressors. Without a high sense of presence, the significant advantages offered by VR disappear
and its emotional induction abilities are even lower than the ones provided by much cheaper media.
Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01683617
Keywords: Stress, Emotion elicitation, Virtual reality
* Correspondence: f.pallavicini@auxologico.it
1
Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Applied Technology for Neuro-
Psychology Laboratory, Milan, Italy
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
© 2013 Pallavicini et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
Background
Different research studies have shown that exposure-
based therapies are more suitable and effective than others
in the treatment of anxiety disorders [1-9].
Exposure is a process in which the patient is progres-
sively exposed to the feared stimulus or the situation that
provokes anxiety. Traditionally, exposure is experienced
in two ways: by imagery in the persons imagination, or
in vivo, with direct contact to the stimulus.
Nevertheless, all roses have thorns, and this is true for
both types of exposure, too. On one side, some patients re-
port difficulties when asked to imagine the feared situation;
furthermore, emotions have been shown to modulate
visual imagery and perception [10], impairing visualization
of detailed scenes. On the other side in vivo exposure
is not fully under the control of the therapist and
requires a high effort in terms of money and time
expenditure.
To overcome these issues an emerging approach is the
use of Virtual Reality Exposure Treatments (VRET) [11-15].
Compared to traditional methods, VR has the advantage
to elicit a sense of presence in users, defined as the feeling
of being in a world that exists outside of the self[16,17].
Another advantage of VR is that in such a setting, as in
the real world, the subject can manipulate and interact
with the environment differently from all the other techni-
ques in which users are passive and not active [13,18]. It is
also possible to grade the intensity of the stimulus follow-
ing the personal needs of each patient. In this way, the
patient feels less uncomfortable about the treatment and
his/her motivation increases [19]. Recently different quan-
titative meta-analyses, have been conducted on studies
reporting VRET treatments. Parson and Rizzo [20] ana-
lyzed data from 21 studies who have evaluated anxiety
and/or phobia before and after VRET. They conclude
that VRET has a statistically large effect on all affective
domains, and thus it is a relevant approach to reduce
anxiety-related symptoms. They conclude that VRET has a
statistically large effect on all affective domains, and thus it
is a relevant approach to reduce anxiety-related symptoms.
Similarly, Powers and Emmelkamp [21] provide effect size
estimates for virtual reality treatment in comparison to
in vivo exposure and other control conditions. They
found a predictable larger effect of VRET compared to
the control conditions; but more interestingly, VRET
outperformed in vivo exposure.
But is VR always an effective stressor? Are the techno-
logical breakdowns that may appear during such an ex-
perience a possible risk for its effectiveness?
Unfortunately only a few studies compared the efficacy
of VR in inducing an emotional response with other
media and/or with real-life reactions [22-24]. These
studies also have the limitation to investigate very
simple emotive reactions, not so complicated as might
be a response related to a real-life stressor. Recently,
Hartanto and colleagues investigated the effects of three
different interruption mechanisms on subjective experi-
ence during a VRET session for social phobia with
scripted avatar-patient dialogues [25]. Specifically, the
study examined three interruption mechanisms: (1) dia-
logue dependent, that occurs freely in the flow of the
dialogue; (2) speech dependent, that occurs during the
pauses in the dialogue; and (3) context independent,
that is randomly. Although, the dialogue dependent
interruption seems superiors on the perceived dialogue
flow, on the user preference and on the dialogue replies,
participants rated presence highest for the context inde-
pendent mechanism. According to Riva and Mantovani
[26] indeed, we are present in an environment - real
and/or virtual - when we are able, inside it, to intuitively
transform our intentions in actions.
Based on the current status of presence research, much
uncertainty remains about its usefulness for emotional
responses induction and VRET. Only weak evidence for a
relationship between presence and emotional responses
has been found [22,27], and no study has yet addressed
this relationship. In other words, it is still unclear whether
VR with low measured presence is characterized by a
reduced effectiveness in inducing negative emotional
responses compared to other traditional procedures.
According to these theoretical premises, we manipu-
lated the level of presence by creating a virtual environ-
ment with two technical breakdowns: the head tracking
was randomly reversed for 20 seconds during the experi-
ence and the avatarslips didn't move at the same time
as their voice. We argued that these two kinds of tech-
nical breakdowns reduce the capability of VR in support-
ing the intentions of the user and its capability to elicit
emotive reactions.
Hence, we compared changes following the exposure to
an academic examination, one of the most universal exam-
ples of real-life stressors, in a sample of 39 undergraduate
students. The same experience was offered using text,
audio, video, and impoverishedVR.
Methods
Aim of the study
The general goal of this study was to compare the effi-
cacy of different media in eliciting negative emotional
responses. In particular, we wanted to examine if techno-
logical breakdowns that appeared during VR could reduce
its effectiveness in comparison with other media (text,
audio, and video) in arousing emotions related to a real-
life stressor.
To compare the efficacy of these techniques, we
compared the relative effectiveness of different pres-
entation modalities of the same stressor (an oral aca-
demic examination).
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 2 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
In particular the following hypotheses were tested:
a. The four experimental situational emotional
induction procedures are effective in eliciting a
negative emotional response in undergraduate
students.
b. The four experimental situational emotional
induction procedures elicit different intensities of
emotional response.
c. The VR experience, when associated to technology
breakdowns, is less effective than other media in
eliciting the negative emotional response.
d. ImpoverishedVR induces a sense of presence
similar to those experienced during the exposure to
other media.
Participants
The experimental sample included 41 students. The mean
age of the sample was 21.15 ± 3.6, with a mean years of
school education of 13.59 ± .498.
Students were recruited from the University of IULM,
Italy. In order to be included in the study, students had
to meet the following criteria: (1) absence of heart, blood
pressure, or major medical diseases; (2) Age between 20
and 30 years; (3) Absence of pharmacotherapy or other
medications that might interfere with the measures being
assessed (e.g, psychoactive medications; antihypertensives);
(4) Absence of psychiatric disorders; (5) Absence of neuro-
logical diseases, mental retardation, psychosis, alcohol or
drug dependence; (6) No migraine, headache, or ves-
tibular abnormalities. Subject eligibility was determined
via subject-reported medical history.
Of these, two did not fulfill the inclusion criteria and
were excluded. Therefore, 39 students were included in
the statistical analysis of psychometric questionnaires,
while only 34 were included in the physiological one.
Before participating in the study, each participant was
provided with written information about the study and
was invited to give written consent for the inclusion.
Participants were also asked to not smoke or assume
caffeine during the day of the experiment. The study
received ethical approval by the Ethical Committee of
the Istituto Auxologico Italiano.
Negative emotions elicitation
According to an emotional narrative validated in a prelim-
inary study [28], we created an audio, a text, a video and a
virtual environment based on that script. The emotional
imagery narrative was based on methods developed by
Lang and his colleagues [29] and further adapted by Sinha
and colleagues [30,31]. In the emotional imagery script,
alluding to an oral academic examination, specific details of
the situation were elicited referring to specific stimulus and
response particulars, including physical and interpersonal
context details, verbal and cognitive attributions regarding
the people involved. A sample of the script used in this
study follows above:
Its your last exam before graduating. The next exam
session will be only the next year. You know that, if
you fail, you will have to wait some months to
graduate. You feel very nervous. In the exams room
you see four of your classmates. They are talking
about the examination and they seem to be very
worried and troubled. They tell you that all of them
failed the exam and that the professor is very strict.
Your anxiety increases. You feel your breath
becoming heavy. Your heart is faster and faster (...).
Laboratory assessment
Psychometric assessment
The following psychometric questionnaires were admi-
nistered to each participant at the start of the experi-
ment and after each experimental condition:
Post Media Questionnaire (PMQ, [32,33]).The Post
Media Questionnaire (PMQ) was used to assess the
emotions elicited by media exposure. In the
questionnaire participants rate the greatest emotions
experienced during the media experience (anxiety,
disgust, anger, fury, surprise, relax, happiness,
sadness) on a 7-point likert scale
Slater-Usoh-Steed Presence Questionnaire (SUS,
[34]): The Presence Questionnaire was used to
assess the level of presence experience during media
exposures. In the questionnaire participants rate the
characteristics of the media experience (feeling of
being there, realism, involvement) and on a 7-point
likert scale.
Psychophysiological assessment
At the beginning, during and at the end of the laboratory
session, electrocardiogram (ECG), Thoracic Respiration
Signal (RSP) and Facial corrugator supercilii muscle Elec-
tromyography (EMG) were recorded in order to obtain
objective measures of subjectsemotional state.
Collected data were analyzed using Matlab 7.0 (The
Mathworks, Natick, MA). Cardiovascular and respiratory
activities were monitored to evaluate both voluntary and
autonomic effect of respiration on heart rate, analyzing
both inter-beat (R-R) interval extracted from electrocar-
diogram and respiration (from a chest strip sensor). The
respiration signal was filtered to produce a smooth sinus-
oidal signal. The Respiration Rate (RSP_Rate) index repre-
sents the peak-to-peak time (max-to-max distance of
the sinusoid). Furthermore, following the guidelines of
Task force of the European Society of Cardiology and the
North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology,
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 3 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
a typical Heart Rate Variability (HRV) spectral method
indexeswereusedtoevaluatetheautonomicnervous
system response [35-40]. At this purpose spectral ana-
lysis was performed using Fourier spectral methods with
custom software. The rhythms have been classified as
very low frequency (VLF, <0.04 Hz), low frequency (LF,
from 0.04 to 0.15 Hz), and high frequency (HF, from
0.15 to 0.5 Hz) oscillations. This procedure enabled us
to calculate the HF index [39,40].
The raw electromyography is a collection of positive and
negative electrical signals; their frequency and amplitude
give us information on the contraction or rest state of
themuscle.AmplitudeismeasuredinμV(micro-Volts).
As the subject contracts the muscle, the number and
amplitude of the lines increases; as the muscle relaxes, it
decreases. We considered the EMG Root Mean Square
(EMG_RMS) for rectifying the raw signal and converting
it to an amplitude envelope. In this study, we were not
interested in frequency related to muscle fatigue.
To summarize psychophysiological measures used in
this study:
HF: High Frequency, 0.15 to 0.5 Hz oscillations in
R-R interval extracted from Electrocardiogram. In
stressful situations this index tends to decrease from
baseline.
EMG_RMS: Root Mean Square EMG from facial
corrugator supercilii muscle Electromyography. In
stressful situations this index tends to increase from
baseline
RSP_Rate: peak-to-peak time from Respiration
signal. In stressful situations this index tends to
increase from baseline
Psychophysiological data were obtained using the
NeXus-4 equipment, with the BioTrace+ software for
recording signals, developed by Mind Media. The module
gets analog data coming from different physiological sen-
sors and, after conditioning and digitalizing, sends them to
a host Personal Computer (PC) using a wireless connection.
Experimental protocol
A within-subjects design was used to compare self-reported
anxiety and relax measures from a pre-condition baseline
to each of the four experimental conditions. Participants
acted as their own controls.
Specifically, the study compared the following conditions:
Condition 1: Virtual Reality (VR). In the VR
condition participants were asked to wear a head-
mounted display (Vuzix VR Bundle with twin high-
Figure 1 Screenshot from VR condition. Figure illustrates virtual
environment representing an academic oral examination in
a classroom.
Figure 2 Time scheduled of the experiment.
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 4 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
resolution 640x480 LCD displays, 920,000 pixels,
iWear
W
3D compliant) in order to have a 3D view of
the virtual scene. The virtual environment was
rendered using a portable computer (ACER ASPIRE
with CPU Intel
W
Core
i5 and graphic processor
Nvidia GeForce GT 540M). Participants also had a
joystick (Xbox Controller), which allowed them to
explore and to interact with the environment. The
virtual scene represented an academic oral examination
in a classroom (the virtual environment is included
in NeuroVR 2.0 [23], a free open source software
that was previously used in different emotion inducing
experiments [22,24]. In all of them the scenes of the
environment were able to elicit a strong emotional
response and feeling of presence that was similar [22]
or even higher [24] than the real scene. Based on
these previous studies, we assumed that the virtual
scene without technical breakdowns would be very
effective in eliciting negative emotional response.
In the selected scene the individual was asked to
enter in the class and meet a group of students
who commented on the difficulty of the exam and
how the professor was very tough. Then, he/she
had to approach the professor who was just
rejecting a student. Finally his/her name was called
by the professor and asked to start the exam.
During the experience the participant also experienced
two technological breakdowns: the head tracking was
randomly reversed for 20 sec during the experience
and the studentslips didn't move at the same time
as their voice. The complete experience was about
3minuteslong.
Condition 2: Audio (AU). Subjects were asked to
imagine themselves as vivid as possible during an
academic oral examination, following an audio
narrative matching the VR experience. Script
development procedures was based on methods
developed by Lang and his colleagues [29], and
further adapted and validated in our previous study
[28]. During the imagery exposure subjects were
asked to close their eyes and to imagine the situation
described. The audio narrative was about 3 minutes
long and was played through the computer speakers.
(Compliant Standards High Definition Audio)
Condition 3: Texts (TX). Based on the script used in
the Imagery Exposure, a script in a written form was
developed. Participants were asked to read a text in
which an academic oral examination is described.
The script was defined with sufficient details to
induce subjects to enter in the story, as
hypothesized by Geerig [41]. The text was read in
about 3 minutes on the 17computer screen.
Condition 4: Video (VD). In the VD condition
participants were exposed to a video, based on the
Imagery Exposure script, and using the same actors
involved in the VR scene, showing the same situation
described before. The video was about 3 minutes long,
too. Participants watched the video on the 17"
(1920x1200) computer screen (Figure 1).
Participants were randomly assigned to the order in
which conditions were presented. The order of presenta-
tion of each experimental condition was counterbalanced
for each participant following an established randomization
schema obtained from http://www.randomizer.org/.
Before starting the experiment each participant was
provided with written information about the study and
was invited to give written consent for the inclusion. Then,
each participant was submitted to a subject-reported med-
ical history interview.
At the start of the experimental session, participants
were seated at a desk in front of a computer monitor. The
eye-to-display distance was about 0.75 meters. Participants
were connected with biosensors for recording their psy-
chophysiological parameters (heart rate, respiration, and
facial corrugator supercilii muscle activity). A baseline
measure of these signals was registered for 3 minutes in
rest condition, with eyes opened. Once the physiological
baseline was recorded, the experimental session started
and psychophysiological signals were recorded until the
end of all the tasks. Then, in order to measure the psycho-
logical variations occurring during the different exposure
conditions, subjects completed an adapted version of the
PMQ in order to assess their perceived level of anxiety
Table 2 Mean and Standard deviation of relax scores
assessed trough the PMQ Questionnaire before and after
all the four conditions
Time
Condition Pre Post
Virtual Reality 4.16 (1.14) 2.84 (1.6)
Video 4.14 (1.25) 3 (1.87)
Audio 4.24 (1.34) 2.43 (1.53)
Text 4.14 (1.22) 3.03 (1.6)
Table 1 Mean and Standard deviation of anxiety scores
assessed trough the PMQ Questionnaire before and after
all the four conditions
Time
Condition Pre Post
Virtual Reality 2.65 (1.16) 3.78 (1.88)
Video 2.41 (1.18) 4.27 (1.83)
Audio 2.51 (1.3) 4.76 (1.94)
Text 2.49 (1.23) 4.35 (1.94)
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 5 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
and relax immediately before and after each condition.
After each exposure condition they completed also the
SUS to assess presences level experienced. Before each
condition there was a 3 min baseline during which par-
ticipants were asked to stay completely relaxed with
open eyes, while their physiological parameters were
recorded (Figure 2).
Results
Data were entered into Microsoft Excel and analyzed using
SPSS and STATA. Change in psychometric and physio-
logical measures within subjects were calculated using
repeated measure ANOVAs.
Psychometric variables
To confirm that the stress induction was successful and
that participants were stressed and experienced a nega-
tive emotional state after the exposure to each condition
(VR, AU, VD, TX), we evaluated self-report measures of
anxiety and relaxation assessed through PMQ before and
after each condition. A 2 (time) × 4 (condition) repeated
measures ANOVAs on the mean scores were conducted
in order to compare the effect of time on anxiety and relax
scores depending on the specific condition.
Regarding anxiety, results showed a significant main ef-
fect of time, F (1,36) = 44.2, p < .001, ηp2 = .522. In particu-
lar, the mean score showed before (M = 2.51, SD = .168)
was lower than that assessed after (M = 4.29, SD = .275)
theexposuretoconditions.Themaineffectofcondition
was non-significant, F (3,108) = 2.56, p = ns, ηp2 = .066.
However, the interaction effect between time x condition
was significant, F (3,108) = 4.93, p < .05, ηp2 = .121, sug-
gesting that anxiety mean scores changed over time de-
pending on condition (Table 1).
A mirror result was obtained regarding relax level.
Results showed a significant main effect of time, F (1,36) =
41.1, p < .001, ηp2 = .533. In particular, the mean scores
showed before (M = 4.16, SD = .162) was higher than that
assessed after (M = 2.82, SD =. 233) the exposure to
conditions. The main effect of condition was non-
significant, F (3,108) = .887, p = ns, ηp2 = .024. However,
the interaction effect between time x condition was signifi-
cant, F (3,108) = 2.75, p < .01, ηp2 = .07, indicating that
relax mean scores changed over time depending on condi-
tion (Table 2).
To test for simple effects, we calculated the mean dif-
ference of self-report anxiety measures (score at the
baseline minus score after condition) for each condition.
A repeated measure ANOVA with condition as the
within factor revealed a significant main effect of condi-
tion in anxiety scores, F (3,114) = 4.59, p < .005, and in
relax scores, F (3,114) = 2.82, p < .05 (Table 3).
In particular, repeated-measures t-tests (using a Bonferroni
adjustment α= .05/3 = .017) showed that the mean differ-
ence in anxiety scores was significantly lower in the VR
condition (M = 1.02, SD = .325) than in the AU condition
(M = 2.28, SD = .322), t(36) = 1.256, p < .01, ES = .385;
Then, in order to evaluate self-report measures of pres-
ence, we analyzed the SUS total score (sum of item 1, 2
and 3) assessed after each condition. A one-way ANOVA
on the mean scores was conducted in order to test
whether SUS scores changed depending on the specific
condition.
Analyses showed no significant main effect of condition,
F(3,111)=.084,p=ns(Table4).
Psychophysiological variables
A repeated measures analysis of variances was also con-
ducted in order to test whether psychophysiological indexes
changed depending on the specific experimental procedures.
Due to complexity of signal processing some subjects
turned in a problematic bio signal, composed by many
artifacts and/or missing values. Thus, some subjects have
been excluded from analysis, in particular we excluded
nine subjects form HF computation and eight subjects
from EMG_RMS and RSP_Rate computation. So, even-
tually, the psychophysiological statistical analyses have
been performed on 32 or 33 subjects, a number well-
Table 4 SUS total score after each condition
Condition Virtual Reality Audio Video Text Fp
Presence 12.1 (4.03) 12.4 (4.15) 12.3 (4.35) 12.1 (3.75) .084 .969
ANOVA did not reveal a significative main effect of condition.
Table 3 Mean and Standard Deviation of main difference in anxiety and relax scores (score at the baseline minus score
after condition)
Condition
Virtual Reality Audio Video Text Fη
p
2
p
Anxiety 1.02 (.325) 2.28 (.322) 1.64 (.37) 1.82 (.32) 4.59 .525 .000***
Relax 1.07 (2.03) 1.89 (1.41) 1.12 (1.52) 1.28 (1.94) 2.82 .069 .042*
***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 6 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
recognized to be of good level in these kinds of studies.
More, a statistical power analysis confirmed the fairness
of our sample size (Table 5).
Results showed a main effect of condition for HF,
F(96,3) = 3.16, p < .028, EMG_RMS, F(99,3) = 16.1,
p < .001, and RSP_Rate, F(99,3) = 17.1, p < .001. In
particular, adjustment for multiple comparisons, with
Bonferroni correction, showed the significant differences
reported in Table 4 (Figure 3).
Discussion
Virtual Reality Exposure Treatment (VRET) is an emer-
ging approach for the treatment of anxiety disorders that
offers different advantages over classical imaginative and
in vivo exposure. Recently, different quantitative meta-
analyses have been conducted on studies reporting VRET
treatments supporting their efficacy in reducing anxiety-
related symptoms [11,12].
But is VR always an effective stressor? Are the techno-
logical breakdowns that may appear during such an experi-
ence a possible risk for its effectiveness? Different studies
suggested a possible link between emotional response and
the level of presence experienced in a virtual environment.
For example, Michaud and colleagues [42] experimentally
manipulated presence in a sample of heights phobics who
had to take an elevator and perform tasks on a scaffold out-
side of a 15-story building. When the immersion in the vir-
tual environment was conducted in a high-presence setting,
the level of anxiety was significantly higher than when the
immersion was conducted in a low-presence setting.
Table 5 Differences in main effects for HF, EMG_RMS and RSP_Rate
Measure (I) Condition (J) Condition Mean difference (I-J) Std. error p
HF Virtual Reality Audio 5.554 1.916 .041*
Video 5.477 1.719 .020*
Text 3.745 2.338 .714
EMG_RMS Virtual Reality Audio 5.489 1.092 .001***
Video 3.61 1.133 .020*
Text 4.898 1.032 .001***
RSP_Rate Virtual Reality Audio 3.626 .604 .001***
Video 2.22 .460 .001***
Text 2.808 .585 .001***
This table reports the comparison between Virtual Reality and the other stimuli (Audio, Video, Text), showing statistical significant differences for all the three
measures. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.
Figure 3 A comparison within conditions with significance level showed accordingly. HF, EMG_RMS and RSP. Rate graphics per each
condition, showing deviation from baseline. The arrow (Stress zone) indicates the sense of the variation to indicate an increasing in stress in that
direction. It is clear from Figure 3 that Virtual Reality condition differs from other conditions in the direction quality, confirming that technological
breakdowns significantly reduce the possibility of Virtual Reality of eliciting emotions related to complex real-life stressors, besides the stressful
script used.
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 7 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
Riva and colleagues [43] explored, too, the relationship
between presence and emotions experienced in VR. Their
data showed a circular interaction between presence and
emotions: on one side, the feeling of presence was greater
in the "emotional" environments; on the other side, the
emotional state was influenced by the level of presence.
Taken together these results underline the existence of
a bi-directional relationship between presence and emo-
tions. However, no previous studies investigated if the
reduction of presence induced by technological break-
downs may be accompanied by a reduced results efficacy
of VR as a stressor for exposure treatments.
To answer this question we compared changes following
the exposure to an academic examination in a sample of
39undergraduatestudents.Thesameexperiencewas
offeredusingtext,audio,video,andVR.However,inthe
virtual environment we manipulated the experience intro-
ducing two technological breakdowns: the head tracking
was randomly reversed for 20 seconds during the experi-
ence and the avatarslips didn't move at the same time.
In the study we first investigated if our emotional induc-
tion procedures were really effective in eliciting an emo-
tional stress-related response. Results, in accordance with
our hypothesis, showed that conditions were effective in
inducing a stress-related response. We found a significant
increase in anxiety scores measured through PMQ and a
mirror decrease in relax scores after all our emotional
procedures.
In addition, we assumed that the four emotive induction
procedures elicits different intensity of responses and, in
particular, we hypothesized that VR, when experienced
with breakdowns, was less effective than traditional proce-
dures. As hypothesized, results showed significative differ-
ences in the psychometric scores and psychophysiological
correlates between the four experimental conditions. In
particular, results on anxiety scores assessed through the
PMQ showed that negative emotional response elicited
through VR was less effective than AU procedure. Psycho-
physiological indexes confirmed this interesting result and,
moreover, showed that VR was less effective in inducing a
stress-related response compared to the other conditions.
Results, in fact, showed an increased level from baseline in
EMG_RMS index, a well-known index of emotional
valence, in AU, VD and TX conditions, clearly indicate a
higher negative emotion for the participants. On the other
hand, VR condition showed no increased level from base-
line. Also RSP_Rate showed similar results, indicating
an accelerated respiration pattern in AU, VD and TX
condition and a decelerated one in the VR condition. This
confirms that Virtual Reality containing technological
breakdowns has not been able to induce stress, besides
the negative script. Finally, a further confirmation arises
from HF, an important index of Heart Rate Variability.
Decreases in HF is a well-recognized index of stress, more
it has been recognized as an index that works well with
different kind of stress, from a more cognitive working
load to a situational stress due to psychological individual
perception of environmental demands that exceed one's
own adaptive ability to meet them [44]. In our study,
lower level of HF for Audio, Video, and Text indicate a
stress measured by the means of this cardiovascular index.
Virtual Reality, on the opposite, showed an increased level.
Thus also for this index we can confirm that technological
breakdowns in Virtual Reality worked well.
Finally, results confirmed our fourth hypothesis. As we
expected, impoverishedVR induced a sense of presence
similar to that experienced during the exposition to other
media. This data confirms the hypothesis that techno-
logical breakdowns impacted the VR efficacy in inducing a
sense of presence in the user, creating break in presence
[26,44]. Moreover, this fact was associated in our study
with less effectiveness of VR in eliciting a negative emo-
tional response compared to other media.
Conclusions
In conclusion, even if using VR it is possible to experience
more presence than in real life [24,45], or to generate a
body transfer illusion [46], it is also possible to experience
less presence than reading a text or watching a video.
Therefore, if the focus is on designing applications capable
of eliciting emotions with the goal of reducing or modify-
ing them (as in psychological therapy), the environments
must be able to induce a high feeling of presence. For this
reason an important goal of VR developer is to guarantee
that the virtual experience offered to their user is smooth
and transparent enough to provide a high level of presence
through a full support to the intentions of the user. With-
out it, the significant advantages offered by VR disappear
and its emotional induction abilities are even lower than
the ones provided by much cheaper media.
Abbreviations
AU: Audio; ECG: Electrocardiogram; EMG: Electromyography;
EMG_RMS: Electromyography Root Mean Square; HF: High Frequency;
HRV: Heart Rate Variability; LF: Low Frequency; μV: Micro-Volts; PC: Personal
Computer; PMQ: Post Media Questionnaire; R-R: Inter-beat; RSP: Thoracic
Respiration Signal; RSP_Rate: Respiration Rate; SUS: Slater-Usoh-Steed
Presence Questionnaire; TX: Texts; VD: Video; VLF: Very Low Frequency;
VR: Virtual Reality; VRET: Virtual Reality Exposure Treatments.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authorscontributions
FP prepared the first draft of the manuscript. SR, AG and FP supervised the
study in its clinical aspect. FP, GR, SS and ST collected literature material and
supervised the background of the study. PC supervised the
psychophysiological aspect of the study. CV created the study technological
materials. GR, PC and SS developed the first draft manuscript into the final
version suitable for publication. MV, AG and GR conceived the idea of the
study and supervised its scientific design. All authors read and approved the
final manuscript.
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 8 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
Acknowledgements
The present work was supported by the European funded project
"Interstress”–Interreality in the management and treatment of stress-related
disorders (FP7- 247685).
Author details
1
Istituto Auxologico Italiano IRCCS, Applied Technology for Neuro-
Psychology Laboratory, Milan, Italy.
2
Department of Psychology, Catholic
University of Milan, Milan, Italy.
3
Iulm University, Milan, Italy.
Received: 18 July 2012 Accepted: 7 February 2013
Published: 11 February 2013
References
1. Deacon BJ, Abramowitz JS: Cognitive and behavioral treatments for
anxiety disorders: a review of meta-analytic findings. J Clin Psychol 2004,
60(4):429441.
2. Emmelkamp PM: Behavior therapy in adults.InHandbook of psychotherapy
and behavior change. 5th edition. Edited by Lambert M. New York: Wiley;
2003:393446.
3. Rothbaum BO, Schwartz AC: Exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress
disorder. Am J Psychother 2002, 56(1):5975.
4. Asukai N, Saito A, Tsuruta N, Kishimoto J, Nishikawa T: Efficacy of exposure
therapy for Japanese patients with posttraumatic stress disorder due to
mixed traumatic events: A randomized controlled study. J Trauma Stress
2010, 23(6):744750.
5. Barlow JH, Ellard DR, Hainsworth JM, Jones FR, Fisher A: A review of self-
management interventions for panic disorders, phobias and obsessive-
compulsive disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005, 111(4):272285.
6. Craske M, Barlow D: Panic disorder and agoraphobia.InClinical handbook
of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual. 4th edition.
Edited by Barlow DH. New York: Guilford; 2007:164.
7. Landon TM, Barlow DH: Cognitive-behavioral treatment for panic
disorder: current status. J Psychiatr Pract 2004, 10(4):211226.
8. Olatunji BO, Cisler JM, Deacon BJ: Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy
for anxiety disorders: a review of meta-analytic findings. Psychiatr Clin
North Am 2010, 33(3):557577.
9. Franklin M, Foa E: Obsessive compulsive disorder.InClinical handbook of
psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual. 4th edition. Edited
by Barlow DH. New York: Guilford; 2007:164215.
10. Borst G, Kosslyn SM: Fear selectively modulates visual mental imagery
and visual perception. Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester) 2010, 63(5):833839.
11. Krijn M, Emmelkamp PM, Olafsson RP, Biemond R: Virtual reality exposure
therapy of anxiety disorders: a review. Clin Psychol Rev 2004, 24(3):259281.
12. Krijn M, Emmelkamp PMG, Olafsson RP, Biemond R: Virtual reality exposure
therapy of anxiety disorders: A review. Clin Psychology Rev 2004,
24(3):259281.
13. Opris D, Pintea S, Garcia-Palacios A, Botella C, Szamoskozi S, David D: Virtual
reality exposure therapy in anxiety disorders: a quantitative meta-
analysis. Depress Anxiety 2012, 29(2):8593.
14. Riva G: Virtual reality in psychotherapy: review. Cyberpsychol Behav 2005,
8(3):220230. discussion 231240.
15. Repetto C, Riva G: From virtual reality to interreality in the treatment of
anxiety disorders. Neuropsychiatry 2011, 1(1):3143.
16. Riva G, Waterworth JA, Waterworth EL, Mantovani F: From intention to
action: The role of presence. New Ideas in Psychology 2011, 29(1):2437.
17. Waterworth JA, Waterworth EL, Mantovani F, Riva G: On Feeling (the)
Present: An evolutionary account of the sense of presence in physical
and electronically-mediated environments. J Conscious Stud 2010,
17(12):167178.
18. Villani D, Riva G: Does interactive media enhance the management of
stress? Suggestions from a controlled study. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw
2012, 15(1):2430.
19. Botella C, GarcÌa-Palacios A, Villa H, Baños RM, Quero M, Alcañiz M, Riva G:
Virtual reality exposure in the treatment of panic disorder and
agoraphobia: A controlled study. Clin Psychol Psychother 2007, 14(3):164175.
20. Parsons TD, Rizzo AA: Affective outcomes of virtual reality exposure
therapy for anxiety and specific phobias: a meta-analysis. J Behav Ther
Exp Psychiatry 2008, 39(3):250261.
21. Powers MB, Emmelkamp PMG: Virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety
disorders: A meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord 2008, 22(3):561569.
22. Gorini A, Griez E, Petrova A, Riva G: Assessment of the emotional
responses produced by exposure to real food, virtual food and
photographs of food in patients affected by eating disorders. Ann Gen
Psychiatry 2010, 9:30. Online: http://www.annals-general-psychiatry.com/
content/39/31/30.
23. Riva G, Gaggioli A, Grassi A, Raspelli S, Cipresso P, Pallavicini F, Vigna C,
Gagliati A, Gasco S, Donvito G: NeuroVR 2a free virtual reality for the
assessment and treatment in behavioral health care. Stud Health Technol
Inform 2011, 163:493495.
24. Villani D, Repetto C, Cipresso P, Riva G: May I experience more presence in
doing the same thing in virtual reality than in reality? An answer from a
simulated job interview. Interacting with Computer 2012, doi:10.1016/j.
intcom.2012.1004.1008.
25. Hartanto D, Kang N, Brinkman WP, Kampmann IL, Morina N,
Emmelkamp PMG, Neerincx MA: Automatic Mechanisms for Measuring
Subjective Unit of Discomfort. Annu Rev Cybertherapy Telemed 2012,
181:192196.
26. Riva G, Mantovani F: From the body to the tools and back: a general
framework for presence in mediated interactions. Interacting with
Computers 2012, 24(4):203210.
27. Gorini A, Capideville CS, De Leo G, Mantovani F, Riva G: The role of
immersion and narrative in mediated presence: the virtual hospital
experience. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2011, 14(3):99105.
28. Raspelli S, Pallavicini F, Grassi A, Cipresso P, Balgera A, Meazzi D, Gaggioli A,
Villamira M, Riva G: Validation of a narrative as an emotional-induction
technique through different non-invasive psychophysiological
monitoring devices: preliminary results. J Cybertherapy Rehabil 2011,
4(2):261.
29. Lang PJ, Davis M, Öhman A: Fear and anxiety: animal models and human
cognitive psychophysiology. J Affect Disord 2000, 61(3):137159.
30. Sinha R: Imaging stress-and cue-induced drug and alcohol craving:
association with relapse and clinical implications. Drug Alcohol Rev 2007,
26(1):2531.
31. Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ: Stress-induced cocaine craving and
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine
relapse outcomes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004, 1032:254257.
32. Gross JJ, Levenson RW: Emotion elicitation using films. Cognit Emot 1995,
9(1):87108.
33. Rottenberg J, Ray RD, Gross JJ: Emotion elicitation using films.InThe
Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment. 9th edition. Edited by Coan
JA, Allen JJB. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007:928.
34. Slater M, Usoh M, Steed A: Depth of presence in virtual environments.
Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 1994, 1(3):130144.
35. Ahuja ND, Agarwal AK, Mahajan NM, Mehta NH, Kapadia HN: GSR and HRV:
its application in clinical diagnosis. In Computer-Based Medical Systems
16th IEEE Symposium: 2003.; 2003:279283.
36. Barbieri R, Triedman JK, Saul JP: Heart rate control and mechanical
cardiopulmonary coupling to assess central volume: a systems analysi.
Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2002, 283:12101220.
37. Mauri M, Magagnin V, Cipresso P, Mainardi L, Brown EN, et al:
Psychophysiological signals associated with affective states. In Annual
International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology
Society 2010. 2010:35633566.
38. Lewicki RJ, Tomlinson EC, Gillespie N: Models of interpersonal trust
development: Theoretical approaches, empirical evidence, and future
directions. J Manag 2006, 32:9911022.
39. Malik M, Bigger JT, Camm AJ, Kleiger R, Malliani A, et al:Task Force of the
European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of
Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: standards of
measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation
1996, 93:10431065.
40. Magagnin V, Mauri M, Cipresso P, Mainardi L, Brown EN: Heart Rate
Variability and Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Assessment of Affective
States by Bivariate Autoregressive Spectral analysis. Comput Cardiol 2010,
37:145148.
41. Gerrig RJ: Experiencing narrative worlds: on the psychological activity of
reading. New Haven and London: Yale University Press; 1993.
42. Michaud M, Bouchard S, Dumoulin S, Zhong XW, Renaud P: Manipulating
presence and its impact on anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav 2004, 7(3):297298.
43. Riva G, Mantovani F, Capideville CS, Preziosa A, Morganti F, Villani D,
Gaggioli A, Botella C, Alcaniz M: Affective interactions using virtual reality:
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 9 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
the link between presence and emotions. Cyberpsychol Behav 2007,
10(1):4556.
44. Serino S, Cipresso P, Tartarisco G, Baldus G, Corda D, Pioggia G, Gaggioli A,
Riva G: An Event-Driven Psychophysiological Assessment for Health Care.
In 2nd International Workshop on Computing Paradigms for Mental Health:
669 2002, SciTe Press; 2012:2534.
45. Gutierrez-Maldonado J, Ferrer-Garcia M, Caqueo-Urizar A, Moreno E: The
validity of virtual environments for eliciting emotional responses in
patients with eating disorders and in controls. Behaviour Modification
2009, 33:830854.
46. Slater M, Spanlang B, Sanchez-Vives MV, Blanke O: First person experience
of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS One 2010, 5(5):e10564.
doi:10.1186/1471-244X-13-52
Cite this article as: Pallavicini et al.:Is virtual reality always an effective
stressors for exposure treatments? some insights from a controlled trial.
BMC Psychiatry 2013 13:52.
Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central
and take full advantage of:
Convenient online submission
Thorough peer review
No space constraints or color figure charges
Immediate publication on acceptance
Inclusion in PubMed, CAS, Scopus and Google Scholar
Research which is freely available for redistribution
Submit your manuscript at
www.biomedcentral.com/submit
Pallavicini et al. BMC Psychiatry 2013, 13:52 Page 10 of 10
http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-244X/13/52
... With the advent of emergent technologies, such as virtual reality, new techniques to use horror for entertainment have emerged. Due to its immersive capabilities, virtual reality is able to induce strong emotional responses in players that are comparable to those that occur in real scenarios [5,6,7]. Such a characteristic is useful for the treatment of phobias and anxiety disorders in serious applications [8,9], but can also be used to maximize the sense of fear in horror games [10]. ...
... After playing the game, participants filled a post-test questionnaire, in which they were asked to rate how much fear they have experienced with each horror elements that they faced during the game (darkness, apparitions, unknown voices, and unknown sounds). Each horror element was rated in a five-point Likert scale ranging from "not scared at all" (1) through "moderately scared" (3) to "extremely scared" (5). ...
... Percentage of time that the player is standing still (in relation to T) F 2 Percentage of time that the player is walking (in relation to T) F 3 Percentage of time that the player is colliding (in relation to T) F 4 Average walking speed F 5 Standard deviation of the walking speed F 6 Average rotation speed F 7 Standard deviation of the rotation speed F 8 Average position of the right-hand controller on the x-axis F 9 Average position of the right-hand controller on the y-axis F 10 Average position of the right-hand controller on the z-axis F 11 Standard deviation of the position of right-hand controller on the x-axis F 12 Standard deviation of the position of right-hand controller on the y-axis F 13 Standard deviation of the position of right-hand controller on the z-axis F 14 Average position of the left-hand controller on the x-axis F 15 Average position of the left-hand controller on the y-axis F 16 Average position of the left-hand controller on the z-axis F 17 Standard deviation of the position of left-hand controller on the x-axis F 18 Standard deviation of the position of left-hand controller on the y-axis F 19 Standard deviation of the position of left-hand controller on the z-axis F 20 Average rotation of the player's head on the x-axis F 21 Average rotation of the player's head on the y-axis F 22 Average rotation of the player's head on the z-axis F 23 Standard deviation of the rotation of player's head on the x-axis F 24 Standard deviation of the rotation of player's head on the y-axis F 25 Standard deviation of the rotation of player's head on the z-axis Voices whose source is unknown or originates from specific places, such as walls, rooms, or inanimate objects. Unknown sounds ...
Article
Full-text available
Fear is a basic human emotion that can be triggered by different situations, which vary from person to person. However, game developers usually design horror games based on a general knowledge about what most players fear, which does not guarantee a satisfying horror experience for everyone. When a horror game aims at intensifying the fear evoked in individual players, having useful information about the fears of the current player is vital to promote more frightening experiences. This work presents a new method to create adaptive virtual reality horror games, which combines player modeling techniques and an adaptive agent-based system that can identify what individual players fear and adapt the content of the game to intensify the fear evoked in players. The main contributions of this work are: (1) a new method to identify individual player’s fears using only gameplay data and machine learning techniques; and (2) a new agent-based adaptive game system that can track the horror intensity experienced by players and moderate the use of the horror elements feared by individual players in the game. The results show that the proposed method is capable of correctly identifying players’ fears (average accuracy of 79.4% for new players). In addition, results of a user study and statistical significance tests (ANOVA and post-hoc analyses) suggest that our method can intensify the fear evoked in players and positively improve immersion and flow.
... This has led to the development of rating scales for cybersickness, including the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ), Visual Analogue Scale, the Fast Motion Sickness Scale, and Virtual Reality Sickness Questionnaire [24][25][26]. It has also led to the repurposing of previously reported scales such as Subjective Units of Distress Scale [27] and, more recently, the development of the Virtual Reality Neuroscience Questionnaire [28], which additionally measures user experience with the software, allowing direct comparisons between different interventions. ...
... There are also tools specifically scoring the degree of "presence" such as the Igroup Presence Questionnaire [22]. This is important because the intervention's ability to influence the emotional state, fundamental to therapeutic effects, is related to the sense of presence and can be disrupted by adverse effects interrupting the simulation [25]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Virtual reality (VR) and augmented reality (AR) are emerging treatment modalities in psychiatry, which are capable of producing clinical outcomes broadly comparable to those achieved with standard psychotherapies. Objective: Because the side effect profile associated with the clinical use of VR and AR remains largely unknown, we systematically reviewed available evidence of their adverse effects. Methods: A systematic review was conducted in accordance with the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) framework across 3 mental health databases (PubMed, PsycINFO, and Embase) to identify VR and AR interventions targeting mental health diagnoses. Results: Of 73 studies meeting the inclusion criteria, 7 reported worsening clinical symptoms or an increased fall risk. Another 21 studies reported "no adverse effects" but failed to identify obvious adverse effects, mainly cybersickness, documented in their results. More concerningly, 45 of the 73 studies made no mention of adverse effects whatsoever. Conclusions: An appropriate screening tool would help ensure that VR adverse effects are correctly identified and reported.
... The extended SOR model asserts that in website settings, if perceived risk is greater, then patients will have more negative emotions toward digital health services. With respect to the virtual groups, patient's cognitive maps of the serviceable usefulness and system quality results in the positive attitudes toward these groups (Pallavicini et al. (2013), which means that patient's cognitive response is correlated to their affective response. Thus, based on the above evidence, the study in hand while employing the extended SOR theory added patient's attachment to VR-health in addition to the visit intention, especially the value of health services on the group-buying websites, for example, patients' cognitive response expressively and positively affects the patient' attachment to healthcare websites (Jerdan et al., 2018). ...
... According to the extended SOR model, patients' affective responses also significantly affect their behavioral intention. In VR-health, emotional engagement and positive emotions influence behavioral intentions positively (Pallavicini et al., 2013). Based on evidence from the existing studies, this study states that patients' affective response to health-related VR experience has a significant association with their engagement to VR-health experience inter alia to their visit intention with the health facility as shown in the VR-health experience. ...
Article
Full-text available
Virtual reality (VR) is a recently emerged tool in healthcare. However, there is dearth of research since literature lacks studies on factors that enhance patients' visits to VR-health facilities. The current study has proposed a theoretical framework to engulf this gap, employing the stimulus, organism, and response (SOR) framework to investigate patients' attachment, patient's cognitive and affective response, authentic experience, and intention to visit VR-health facilities. The study has found significant influences of the patients' authentic experience on their cognitive and affective responses. It shows that patients' authentic experience plays a significant role in VR health. The results further verified that patients' cognitive and affective responses have significant mediating effects to predict the attachment and intentions to VR visit. The patients' intention to visit health facilities reveal that attachment to the VR predicts VR-health. Likewise, cognitive response shows a powerful impact compared to the affective response on the intention to visit health facilities over virtual reality. This research brings to fore why prospective patients visit healthcare facilities using virtual reality.
... These results appear important since usability was considered one of the key qualities of the VR psychoeducational experience during its conception and development. Indeed, breakdowns or other interaction issues can reduce VR's ability to induce both emotions [111][112][113] and a satisfying sense of presence [112,114], with potential detrimental effect on learning outcomes [49,115]. ...
... These results appear important since usability was considered one of the key qualities of the VR psychoeducational experience during its conception and development. Indeed, breakdowns or other interaction issues can reduce VR's ability to induce both emotions [111][112][113] and a satisfying sense of presence [112,114], with potential detrimental effect on learning outcomes [49,115]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Virtual reality can help individuals to manage stress and anxiety. In addition to its efficacy in reducing stress and anxiety through relaxation, virtual reality can be helpful for psychoeducational. However, to date, few studies have explored this topic. Therefore, this mixed-methods sequential explanatory study aimed to investigate the usability, sense of presence, emotional response, and effect on learning of MIND-VR, a psychoeducational virtual experience created to offer information on stress and anxiety. Twenty healthcare workers (60% female; mean age 43 10) tried MIND-VR. Psychometric outcomes served as quantitative variables, while participant interviews provided qualitative data. Results showed that the virtual reality psychoeducational experience: (a) was highly usable and satisfying; (b) increased positive emotions (i.e., happiness and surprise) and decreased negative emotions (i.e., fear, sadness) and state anxiety; (c) elicited a strong sense of presence, especially spatial presence; and (d) was practical, simple, motivating, and engaging for learning information about stress and anxiety. These findings offer promising insights into that virtual reality may be an effective tool to provide psychoeducation.
... ave compared the effectiveness of VR in producing an emotional response to that of other media and/or real-life reactions, finding that virtual stimuli generate emotional and behavioral responses that are close to those seen in the real world. In reaction to complicated real-life pressures, virtual reality has lately been proven to elicit emotions (Pallavicini, et. al, 2013). When these systems are combined, they can provide aim and quantitative assessment of symptoms using biosensors and behavioral analysis, monitoring of patient behavior and general and psychological status, early detection of symptoms of critical evolutions, and timely activation of feedback in a closed loop approach, and Decision Suppor ...
Article
Construction operates under tight time constraints and deadlines and this worsens the stress situation in the construction industry. Stress is a condition or feeling experienced when an individual perceives demands exceed personal and social resources the individual can mobilize. According to studies, a large percentage of workers are experiencing unacceptably high levels of stress at work, and the impact on productivity levels cannot be overstated. Construction projects are notorious for being complex and involving tangled work connections, making this a difficult sector for all involved. Quantity surveying is one of the professional practices subject to the stressing situations in the construction industry. Therefore, with the ultimate goal of improving the performance of quantity surveyors in the industry, this study focuses on the impact of technologies on addressing stress-related problems among practicing quantity surveyors and the goal is to proffer solutions for reducing/eliminating stress/stressors in the profession. Primary data were collected through the administration of questionnaires to quantity surveyors (QSs). The total pollution was one thousand one hundred and thirty-five (1135). After using AusVet Animal Health Service formula a total of one hundred and nine (109) respondents were gotten and questionnaires were administered using the convenience sampling technique; seventy-eight (78) of them were returned and found suitable for analysis, representing a response rate of approximately 71%. The data collected were analyzed using Percentiles, Mean Item Score, and Kruskall-Wallis. The result shows that Mobile phones, Building Information Modeling (BIM), Smartwatches, Strap/band/patch, Wearable Sensors, and Virtual Reality (VR) are the most available technology for reducing stress levels among quantity surveyors. Kruskall-Wallis test was to identify the significant difference and it revealed that six (6) variables are significantly different while others are not. The study concludes that the availability and use of these technologies can help to reduce and identify the stress level imposed on quantity surveyors. Based on the findings, the study recommends that construction organizations or companies should ensure some of these technological tools are made available to quantity surveyors to ensure the stress level is well managed or prevented. Quantity surveyors should also try as much as possible to get (affordable ones) any of the available technologies to help themselves achieve better results. The study was limited to quantity surveyors in Lagos State; further research can also appraise the stress among other construction professionals and identify the available technologies for stress management.
... In this regard, Allison Anderson et al. concluded that VR therapy improves the level of relaxation both objectively and subjectively [29]. Other studies identify the potential of immersive VR as a novel strategy in psychological interventions [30][31][32]. The trend in recent years is to study the applications of this resource in cognitive disorders such as dementia [33] or Alzheimer's disease [16]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Immersive therapy through virtual reality represents a novel strategy used in psychological interventions, but there is still a need to strengthen the evidence on its effects on health professionals’ mental health. Objective: To analyze the results of immersive therapy through virtual reality on the levels of anxiety and well-being of the health professionals working in a regional hospital in Olot (Spain). Methods: Pilot quasi-experimental study including a group of 35 female (mean age = 45.7, SD = 8.43) health professionals who undertook immersive therapy for 8 weeks. The intervention was implemented through virtual reality, and its effect on anxiety levels and well-being was evaluated through the Hamilton and Eudemon scales, respectively. Data on age, gender, active pharmacological or psychological treatment, mental health disorders and number of sessions were also collected. Results: A statistically significant (p < 0.001) improvement in anxiety and well-being was found, with large and moderate effect sizes (0.90 and 0.63 respectively). In addition, these changes were clinically significant. No significant associations were found between the improvements and the different variables, but a greater trend was identified among the group of professionals with untreated or unidentified levels of anxiety. Conclusion: This group of health professionals showed a statistically and clinically significant improvement in anxiety and well-being after the application of immersive therapy using virtual reality. Further studies with a control group are necessary to further analyze this novel intervention.
... Second, when the graphic quality of the virtual experience was perceived as too low, users had difficulties comprehending a target scenario [73,87]. To prevent graphical or technical problems (e.g., breakdown or problems with interaction) [140,141], with potential detrimental effects on treatment and learning outcomes [138,142], principles and practices of User-Centered Design (UCD) are recommended [143,144]. Many VR-based treatment and medical education and training programs are currently taking a technology rather than a human-centered approach, which can lead to limited impact of VR in these fields. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Virtual reality can play an important role during the COVID-19 pandemic in the health care sector. This technology has the potential to supplement the traditional in-hospital medical training and treatment, and may increase access to training and therapies in various health care settings. Objective: This systematic review aimed to describe the literature on health care–targeted virtual reality applications during the COVID-19 crisis. Methods: We conducted a systematic search of the literature on the PsycINFO, Web of Science, and MEDLINE databases, according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis guidelines. The search string was as follows: “[(virtual reality)] AND [(COVID-19) OR (coronavirus) OR (SARS-CoV-2) OR (healthcare)].” Papers published in English after December 2019 in peer-reviewed journals were selected and subjected to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. We used the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool to assess the quality of studies and the risk of bias. Results: Thirty-nine studies met the inclusion criteria. Seventeen studies showed the usefulness of virtual reality during the COVID-19 crisis for reducing stress, anxiety, depression, and pain, and promoting physical activity. Twenty-two studies revealed that virtual reality was a helpful learning and training tool during the COVID-19 crisis in several areas, including emergency medicine, nursing, and pediatrics. This technology was also used as an educational tool for increasing public understanding of the COVID-19 pandemic. Different levels of immersion (ie, immersive and desktop virtual reality), types of head-mounted displays (ie, PC-based, mobile, and standalone), and content (ie, 360° videos and photos, virtual environments, virtual reality video games, and embodied virtual agents) have been successfully used. Virtual reality was helpful in both face-to-face and remote trials. Conclusions: Virtual reality has been applied frequently in medicine during the COVID-19 pandemic, with positive effects for treating several health conditions and for medical education and training. Some barriers need to be overcome for the broader adoption of virtual reality in the health care panorama. Trial Registration: International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols (INPLASY) INPLASY202190108; https://inplasy.com/inplasy-2021-9-0108/
... In this regard, Allison Anderson et al. concluded that VR therapy improves the level of relaxation both objectively and subjectively [32]. Other studies identify the potential of immersive VR as a novel strategy in psychological interventions [33][34][35]. The trend in recent years is to study the applications of this resource in cognitive disorders such as dementia [36] or Alzheimer's disease [37]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Immersive therapy through virtual reality represents a novel strategy used in psychological interventions, but there is still a need to strengthen the evidence on its effects on health professionals' mental health. Objective: To analyze the results of immersive therapy through virtual reality in the levels of anxiety and secondly, well-being of the health professionals working in a regional hospital in Olot (Spain). Methods: Pilot quasi-experimental study including a group of 35 women (mean age=45.7, SD=8.43) health professionals who undertook immersive therapy for 8 weeks. The intervention was implemented through virtual reality, and its effect on anxiety levels and well-being was evaluated through the Hamilton and Eudemon scales, respectively. Data on age, gender, active pharmacological or psychological treatment, mental health disorders and number of sessions were also collected. Results: Statistically significant (p<0.001) improvement in anxiety and well-being was found, with large and moderate effect sizes (0.90 and 0.63 respectively). In addition, these changes were clinically significant. No significant associations were found between the improvements and the different variables, but a greater trend was identified among the group of professionals with untreated or unidentified levels of anxiety. Conclusion: This group of health professionals showed statistically and clinically significant improvement in anxiety and well-being after the application of immersive therapy using virtual reality. Further studies with a control group are necessary to further analyze this novel intervention.
... Moreover, a trial [24] studies experiential virtual scenarios with real-time monitoring for the management of psychological stress. Also, the trial [25] studies whether virtual reality is effective for exposure treatments. Using a virtual reality environment able to induce a high feeling of presence, cardiovascular and respiratory activities are monitored to evaluate both voluntary and autonomic effects of respiration on heart rate. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The application of Context Aware Computing (CAC) can be an effective, useful, feasible, and acceptable way to advance medical research and provide health services. Methods: This review was conducted in accordance with the principles of the development of a mixed methods review and existing knowledge in the field via the Synthesis Framework for the Assessment of Health Information Technology to evaluate CAC implemented by Evidence-Based Health Informatics (EBHI). A systematic search of the literature was performed during 18 November 2021-22 January 2022 in Cochrane Library, IEEE Xplore, PUBMED, Scopus and in the clinical registry platform Clinicaltrials.gov. The author included the articles in the review if they were implemented by EBHI and concerned with CAC technologies. Results: 29 articles met the inclusion criteria and refer to 26 trials published between 2011 and 2022. The author noticed improvements in healthcare provision using EBHI in the findings of CAC application. She also confirmed that CAC systems are a valuable and reliable method in health care provision. Conclusions: The use of CAC systems in healthcare is a promising new area of research and development. The author presented that the evaluation of CAC systems in EBHI presents positive effects on the state of health and the management of long-term diseases. These implications are presented in this article in a detailed, clear, and reliable manner.
Chapter
According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), the essential features of obsessive-compulsive disorder are recurrent obsessions or compulsions of significant severity. Obsessions are “persistent ideas, thoughts, impulses, or images that are experienced as intrusive and inappropriate and that cause marked anxiety or distress” (APA, 1994, p. 418). Compulsions are “repetitive behaviors... or mental acts... the goal of which is to prevent or reduce anxiety or distress” (p. 418). This DSM-IV conceptualization of OCD offers no fundamental reformation of the previous construct, but there have been some noteworthy shifts of emphasis that reflect recent thinking about the disorder.
Chapter
Research on emotion has undergone explosive growth during the past few decades, marked by new theories (e.g., evolutionary analyses; Tooby & Cosmides, 1990), methods (e.g., anatomically-based systems for coding facial expressive behavior, see Cohn this volume), and findings (see Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999). Some of the research in this area has been correlational, focusing on factors that naturally co-vary with emotional processes, such as chronological age, physical health, or social status. However, experimental research also has flourished, focusing on emotional processes in the context of relatively well-controlled laboratory environments. Our chapter on the use of emotion-eliciting films, like many of the contributions to the Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, lies squarely within this second, experimental tradition.