ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: A self-sustaining worldview comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated in endorsement. In Study 1 (n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another but by broader beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Uncorrected manuscript
Dead and Alive: Beliefs in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories
Michael J. Wood, Karen M. Douglas & Robbie M. Sutton,
University of Kent, Canterbury, United Kingdom
Keywords: conspiracy theories, conspiracism, contradiction, explanatory coherence
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR:
Michael J. Wood
University of Kent
School of Psychology – Keynes College, University of Kent
Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP, United Kingdom
mw337@kent.ac.uk
AUTHOR BIOS:
Michael J. Wood is a Ph.D. candidate at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. His research
focuses on the psychology of conspiracy theories.
Karen M. Douglas is a Reader in Psychology at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. She
has published widely on topics such as language and stereotyping, persuasion, the psychology of
the internet and the psychology of conspiracy theories.
Robbie M. Sutton is a Reader in Psychology at the University of Kent, United Kingdom. His
research interests include the psychology of justice, gender, and intergroup processes.
DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST:
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or
publication of this article.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE/FUNDING:
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research and/or
authorship of this article: School of Psychology, University of Kent.
Dead and Alive: Beliefs in Contradictory Conspiracy Theories
Abstract:
Conspiracy theories can form a monological belief system: a self-sustaining worldview
comprised of a network of mutually supportive beliefs. The present research shows that even
endorsement of mutually incompatible conspiracy theories are positively correlated. In Study 1
(n = 137), the more participants believed that Princess Diana faked her own death, the more they
believed that she was murdered. In Study 2 (n = 102), the more participants believed that Osama
Bin Laden was already dead when U.S. special forces raided his compound in Pakistan, the more
they believed he is still alive. Hierarchical regression models showed that mutually incompatible
conspiracy theories are positively associated because both are associated with the view that the
authorities are engaged in a cover-up (Study 2). The monological nature of conspiracy belief
appears to be driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another, but by broader
beliefs supporting conspiracy theories in general.
Article text:
A conspiracy theory is defined as a proposed plot by powerful people or organizations
working together in secret to accomplish some (usually sinister) goal (Coady, 2006; Douglas &
Sutton, 2008; Goertzel, 1994). Popular contemporary examples include the theory that the 9/11
attacks were planned and carried out by elements within the American government (Kay, 2011)
and the belief that evidence of a causal link between autism and childhood vaccination is being
suppressed by an unscrupulous medical industry (Goertzel, 2010). Conspiracy theories are not
by definition false; indeed, many real conspiracies have come to light over the years. Suspicions
of President Nixon’s involvement in a burglary at the headquarters of the Democratic National
Committee began as a seemingly outlandish conspiracy theory, but turned out to be true (Bale,
2007). However, conspiracy beliefs, even when wrong, are notoriously resistant to falsification,
and can take on the appearance of a “degenerating research program” (Clarke, 2002, p. 136),
with new layers of conspiracy being added to rationalize each new piece of disconfirming
evidence.
Spurred in part by the growth of new media, conspiracism has become a major subcultural
phenomenon. This shift has not gone unnoticed in academia. In recent decades there has been
an explosion of research into the psychology of belief in conspiracy theories. Much of this
research interest has focussed on the individual correlates of conspiracy belief, but perhaps the
most consistent finding in the work on the psychology of conspiracy theories is that belief in a
particular theory is strongly predicted by belief in others – even ostensibly unrelated ones
(Douglas & Sutton, 2008; Goertzel, 1994 ; Swami, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Furnham, 2010;
Swami et al., 2011). For instance, someone who believes that the American government was
behind the 9/11 attacks is very likely to also believe that Princess Diana was deliberately
assassinated. One proposed explanation for this connection is that beliefs in conspiracy theories
somehow support one another (Goertzel, 1994). Even though the perpetrators may be different
in each case, the fact that one massive, sinister conspiracy could be successfully executed in
near-perfect secrecy suggests that many such plots are possible. Over time, the view of the
world as a place ruled by conspiracies can lead to conspiracy becoming the default explanation
for any given event – a unitary, closed-off worldview in which beliefs come together in a
mutually supportive network known as a monological belief system (Goertzel, 1994; Clarke,
2002; Swami et al., 2010; Swami et al., 2011).
However, some conspiracy theories emphatically do not support one another; indeed, many
provide mutually contradictory explanations for the same event. These contradictions among
conspiracy theories are the focus of the present article. For instance, the theories surrounding the
death of Princess Diana vary widely; some claim that she was killed by MI6, others allege that
she was killed by Mohammed al-Fayed’s business enemies, still others that she faked her own
death. How does a conspiracy-believing observer reconcile the presence of these competing,
mutually contradictory accounts? If beliefs in conspiracy theories are correlated with one
another because the theories are in direct agreement, one would not expect reliable correlations
between beliefs in theories that are mutually exclusive.
In the present research, we seek to determine whether the coherence of the conspiracist
belief system is driven not by direct relationships among individual theories, but by agreement
between individual theories and higher-order beliefs about the world. For instance, the idea that
authorities are engaged in motivated deception of the public would be a cornerstone of
conspiracist thinking due to its centrality in conspiracy theories. Someone who believes in a
significant number of conspiracy theories would naturally begin to see authorities as
fundamentally deceptive, and new conspiracy theories would seem more plausible in light of that
belief (Read, Snow and Simon, 2003; Simon, Snow, & Read, 2004). Indeed, the two conspiracy
theories mentioned above – an autism/vaccine connection and 9/11 as an inside job – both
revolve around that central proposition. Likewise, whether one believes that Princess Diana was
killed by MI6 or Mohammed Al-Fayed’s business enemies, belief in a cover-up would support
(and be supported by) both theories. In spite of that, the two theories contradict each other.
Would it be possible for their contradiction to be overruled by their coherence with a broader
conspiracist worldview, such that they display a positive correlation in endorsement?
Some literature on stereotyping suggests that coherence with strongly held worldviews
may well be sufficient to overwhelm contradictions between individual beliefs. Adorno (1954)
found strong positive correlations in endorsement between contradictory negative stereotypes of
Jews, such that highly prejudiced participants found them to be both too isolated from the rest of
society and too eager to participate in it. Adorno proposed that this paradoxical perception has
its roots in “a relatively blind hostility which is reflected in the stereotypy, self-contradiction, and
destructiveness” of anti-Jewish stereotyping (p. 76). In spite of their contradictory nature, both
stereotypes drew enough credibility from their one common element – a negative perception of
Jewish people – to end up with a strong positive association.
The same may well be true of
contradictory conspiracy theories; conspiracy advocates’ distrust of official narratives may be so
strong that many alternative theories are simultaneously endorsed in spite of any contractions
between them.
The phenomenon of global coherence overruling local contradictions is perhaps best
understood in the context of Thagard’s (1988) explanatory coherence model of social inference.
Explanatory coherence theory characterizes explanations and pieces of evidence about actors and
events as either coherent or incoherent with one another. These elements are represented by
nodes in a connectionist network. Activation flows from evidence nodes and higher-order
knowledge structures (Read, 1987) to the various explanations, which in turn excite or inhibit
one another depending on whether they are mutually coherent or contradictory. This process of
excitation and inhibition continues until the system reaches a stable equilibrium, at which point
the highly activated explanations are accepted and those with little activation are discarded.
Activation has been shown to flow the other way, as well: not only do evidence and higher-order
knowledge structures change one’s perception of explanations, emerging conclusions in the
network also change perceptions of evidence and alter broad worldviews (Read & Miller, 1993;
Read et al., 2003).
For instance, imagine that someone is heavily invested in conspiracism and strongly
believes in a wide variety of different conspiracy theories. A view of authority as fundamentally
deceptive is coherent with all of these theories, and as such draws activation from them until it
becomes a strongly held belief in itself. When a novel conspiracy theory is presented, it
immediately seems more credible because it agrees with this now strongly held view and
disagrees with the officially endorsed narrative. Such higher-order beliefs may be so strongly
held that any conspiracy theory that stands in opposition to the official narrative will gain some
degree of endorsement from someone who holds a conspiracist worldview, even if it directly
contradicts other conspiracy theories that they also find credible. In other words, a natural
consequence of the explanatory coherence approach to social explanation is an instantiation of
the principle “the enemy of my enemy is my friend.”
Indeed, this is a principle found explicitly in Heider's (1958) theory of psychological
balance, which shares a considerable common ground with explanatory coherence. In balance
theory, perceptions of an object or social actor are affected by its relationship with other actors
about which opinions already exist. For instance, people's evaluations of a novel product
endorsed by a known celebrity are more positive if they view the celebrity positively, or more
negative if their views of the celebrity are negative. In the case of conspiracy theories, we
propose that a similar mechanism is at work: officials are seen as deceptive, perhaps even
actively malevolent, so any explanation that they endorse is at a disadvantage, while alternative
explanations are more credible from the start. Explanatory coherence has been shown to
naturally instantiate many of the Gestalt principles on which balance theory is based (Read et al.,
2003), and others have noted the applicability of balance theory to the study of conspiracy belief,
such as Inglehart (1987)
.
Thus, we predict that for someone with a conspiracist worldview, nearly any theory that
assumes deception by officialdom in its explanation for a world event and stands in opposition to
the “mainstream” account will garner some agreement. This relationship may hold even to the
point that people who believe in a world governed by conspiracy are likely to endorse
contradictory conspiracy theories about the same topic. Just as Adorno (1954) found positive
correlations in endorsement of contradictory stereotypes, we expect to see positive relationships
between endorsement of contradictory conspiracy theories about the same event. For example,
the more that participants believe that a person at the centre of a death-related conspiracy theory,
such as Princess Diana or Osama Bin Laden, is still alive, the more they also tend to believe that
the same person was killed, so long as the alleged manner of death involves deception by
officialdom.
Study 1
We first elected to examine the relationship between contradictory conspiracy theories
regarding the same event by asking about several rival accounts of Princess Diana’s death.
Method
Participants
One hundred thirty seven undergraduate psychology students (83% female, mean age 20.4)
were recruited from a second-year research methods class at a British university. Participation
was voluntary and no compensation was given.
Materials and Procedure
For the purposes of the present study, we used the conspiracy theory belief scale used by
Douglas & Sutton (2011). The questionnaire was 17 items long and used a 7-point Likert scale
(1 = “strongly disagree,” 7 = “strongly agree,”), to ascertain participants’ agreement with a
variety of different conspiracy theories. These included 9/11 as an inside job, global warming as
a hoax by scientists and politicians, and the idea of a fake moon landing. Crucially, there were
five items regarding the death of Princess Diana (Douglas & Sutton, 2008; 2011; α = .83):
1. One or more rogue “cells” in the British secret service constructed and carried out a plot
to kill Diana
2. There was an official campaign by MI6 to assassinate Diana, sanctioned by elements of
the establishment
3. Diana faked her own death so that she and Dodi could retreat into isolation
4. Business enemies of Dodi and his father Mohammed Al-Fayed assassinated Dodi, with
the death of Diana a cover-up for the operation
5. Diana had to be killed because the British government could not accept that the mother of
the future king was involved with a Muslim Arab
Not all of these items are mutually contradictory. Diana might conceivably have learned of
a plot to kill her and faked her own death in response, so #3 and #2 do not necessarily contradict
one another. #1 and #2 differ in the degree to which the operation to kill Diana was officially
sanctioned, and not all participants would necessarily pick up on that difference. Likewise, #5
indicates the existence of a plot to kill Diana but does not specify whether it was successful, so it
does not explicitly contradict any of the other theories. However, there are some unambiguous
contradictions. #1, #3, and #4 all propose different accounts of Diana’s apparent death: either
she was killed by a rogue cell of the British secret service (#1) or by business rivals of the
Fayeds (#4), or she faked her own death (#3). These three theories are mutually incompatible,
and will be the focus of analysis in the present study.
Results & Discussion
We first performed an exploratory principal components analysis to investigate the factor
structure of the scale. Based on a scree plot, we extracted two unrotated factors which together
accounted for 46.9% of scale variance. All items had loadings of at least .35 on the first factor in
the unrotated solution, suggesting that it represents generic conspiracy belief; the second factor
drew loadings only from the five items concerning climate change conspiracy theories, and thus
appears to be related to beliefs in these conspiracies in particular.
In line with this factor structure, and with previous findings of high correlations among
beliefs in different conspiracy theories, the scale showed reasonable reliability (α = .78). Most of
the questions were significantly correlated with one another despite covering different topics; for
instance, a belief that a rogue cell of MI6 was responsible for Diana’s death was correlated with
belief in theories that HIV was created in a laboratory (r = .39), that the moon landing was a
hoax (r = .34), and that governments are covering up the existence of aliens (r = .23) (all ps <
.01). In line with this general pattern, there was a network of significant positive relationships
among the majority of the Princess Diana conspiracy theories (see Table 1). People who
believed that Diana faked her own death were marginally more likely to also believe that she was
killed by a rogue cell of British Intelligence (r = .15, p = .075) and significantly more likely to
also believe that she was killed by business enemies of the Fayeds (r = .25, p = .003). Similarly,
participants who found it likely that the Fayeds’ business rivals were responsible for the death of
Diana were highly likely to also blame a rogue cell (r = .61, p < .001).
As can be seen in Table 1, the correlations in endorsement with the idea that Diana faked
her own death appear much lower than the rest, to the point that the only non-significant
correlation involves that theory. We believe this to be due to a floor effect rather than any sort of
response to contradiction; endorsement of the faked-own-death theory was extremely low in this
sample, with a mean of only 1.52 on a 7-point scale. This level of endorsement was significantly
lower than that of the other theories, for which agreement ranged from 2.51 (business rivals) to
2.98 (rogue cell) (all ps < .001). As an alternative approach to the relationship between the
faked-death theory and the rogue cell theory, we dichotomized responses to the faked-death item,
comparing those who gave the lowest possible response with those who responded more
positively. In accordance with the general pattern of results, participants who strongly disagreed
with the faked-death theory showed a lower level of agreement with the rogue cell theory (M =
2.75) than those who responded otherwise (M = 3.47; t(134) = -2.56, p = .01).
In line with our hypothesis, the results show mostly clear positive correlations in
endorsement of contradictory conspiracy theories. Intuitively, this does not make sense. One
would think that there ought to be a negative correlation between beliefs in contradictory
accounts of events – the more one believes in a particular theory, the less likely rival theories
will seem. One possible alternative explanation for these results is acquiescence bias:
participants may have simply replied in the same way to every question, resulting in positive
correlations across the scale regardless of the questions’ content. However, the scale included a
reverse-coded Diana conspiracy item which read, “The death of Princess Diana was an
accident.” Contrary to the acquiescence hypothesis, this item was consistently negatively
correlated with the rest of the scale, most notably r = -.75 with the rogue-cell item and r = -.65
with the MI6 item (both ps < .001).
These results suggest that those who distrust the official story of Diana’s death do not tend
to settle on a single conspiracist account as the only acceptable explanation; rather, they
simultaneously endorse several contradictory accounts. In Study 2, we set out to conceptually
replicate these findings in another setting, and also to ask why mutually contradictory conspiracy
theories are simultaneously endorsed.
Study 2
On May 2
nd
, 2011, it was reported in the news media that Osama bin Laden had been
killed in an American raid on a compound in Abbottabad, Pakistan. Conspiracy theories alleging
that bin Laden had not actually been killed in the raid immediately started to propagate
throughout the Internet and traditional media, mostly. Proponents claimed that their suspicions
were aroused by several actions of the Obama administration, including a refusal to release
pictures of bin Laden’s body and the decision to bury him at sea shortly after the raid.
The conspiracy theories surrounding the death of Osama bin Laden can be divided into two
major categories: those that propose he was already dead at the time of the raid, and those that
propose he is still alive (Kingsley & Jones, 2011). The former seems to have currency among
the 9/11 conspiracist Truth Movement; many “Truthers” allege that bin Laden died in 2000 or
even earlier, and his video appearances since then were in fact staged productions made with a
body double. The latter theory varies; some people believe that he is still at large, while others
think that he was captured alive and is being secretly held for interrogation by the CIA.
Naturally, these two theories are irreconcilable; bin Laden cannot be both alive and dead at the
same time. However, as in Study 1, we predicted that belief in the two conspiracy theories
would be positively correlated.
Further, in order to test the idea that perceived deception by authorities underlies the
positive correlation between contradictory conspiracy theories, we asked participants to what
degree they found the American government’s actions surrounding the raid to be suspicious and
indicative of a cover-up. This was intended to operationalize the central principle of
conspiracism outlined above: the idea that authorities are engaged in motivated deception. If
belief in a cover-up is indeed responsible for the positive association between contradictory
conspiracy theories, controlling for it should cause the correlation between the contradictory
theories to disappear.
Method
Participants
One hundred two undergraduate students (58% female, mean age 21) at a British university
were recruited to participate in the study between one and six weeks after the announcement of
bin Laden’s death. In exchange for their participation they received a randomized prize of either
a snack or a small monetary reward of GB£1.00 or 2.00 (~US$1.50 or 3.00).
Materials and Procedure
Participants were directed to read a brief summary of the official story of Osama bin
Laden’s death, including the details regarding the refusal to release pictorial evidence and the
burial at sea, followed by a short paragraph explaining that some people doubt the official story.
They were then asked about their opinion of the official story, followed by three conspiracy
items:
1. Osama bin Laden was killed in the American raid.
2. Osama bin Laden is still alive.
3. When the raid took place, Osama bin Laden was already dead.
4. The actions of the Obama administration indicate that they are hiding some important
or damaging piece of information about the raid.
Each of these statements was followed by a series of questions based on the composite
endorsement measure used by Douglas and Sutton (2011). This asked participants to rate their
agreement with each statement on a scale from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 6 (“strongly agree”), as
well as to what degree they found the statements plausible, convincing, worth considering, and
coherent, again on a scale from 1 (“not at all”) to 6 (“very much”). These ratings were then
averaged to obtain a composite measure of endorsement for each statement (α > .87 for each
statement). While the original measure used by Douglas and Sutton also asked participants to
judge the interestingness of each statement, there is no contradiction in finding two rival theories
equally interesting, so we excluded interestingness from the present study in order to avoid
artificially inflating the relevant correlations.
Results & Discussion
The idea that bin Laden was killed in the raid enjoyed a high level of endorsement (M =
5.00, SD = 1.19), indicating a fairly high level of trust in the official story, though participants on
average found the Obama administration’s actions to be suspicious (M = 4.74, SD = 1.41).
Participants seemed less likely to endorse the idea that bin Laden is still alive (M = 3.05, SD =
1.39) or was already dead (M = 3.19, SD = 1.39). In a replication of our Study 1 result, a
correlational analysis revealed a significant positive correlation between composite endorsement
ratings of the two contradictory conspiracy theories, r = .21, p = .04.
We next examined the contribution of belief in a cover-up to the positive relationship
between the two contradictory theories using a hierarchical multiple regression analysis.
Endorsement of the cover-up item significantly predicted endorsement of the “bin Laden is still
alive” theory, β = .373, t(97) = 4.04, p < .001 (the same was true of the already-dead theory, β =
.346, t(97) = 3.63, p < .001). Adding endorsement of the contradictory theory “bin Laden was
already dead” to the regression equation, however, explained no additional variance (R
2
=
.006), and this theory was not itself a significant predictor, β = .086, t(96)= 0.86, p = .40. This
indicates that the correlation in endorsement of the two contradictory theories is explainable
entirely by their connection with belief in a deceptive cover-up by authority (see Figure 1). The
degree to which someone believes in a cover-up helps to determine their endorsement of the
official story, and of both conspiracy theories as well. This result is in line with our predictions,
and supports the idea that conspiracy theories are defined not by adherence to a particular
alternative account, but by opposition to the official story and a belief that deception is taking
place.
General Discussion
While it has been known for some time that belief in one conspiracy theory appears to be
associated with belief in others, only now do we know that this can even apply to conspiracy
theories that are mutually contradictory. This finding supports our contention that the
monological nature of conspiracism (Goertzel, 1994; Swami et al., 2010; Swami et al., 2011) is
driven not by conspiracy theories directly supporting one another, but by the coherence of each
theory with higher-order beliefs that support the idea of conspiracy in general. As demonstrated
in Study 2, perceived deception by authority is one such belief, and it is likely that there are
many others as well. For those who hold such beliefs, the specifics of a conspiracy theory do not
matter as much as the fact that it is a conspiracy theory at all.
There are strong parallels between this conception of a monological belief system and
Adorno’s (1954) work on prejudice and authoritarianism. In an attempt to explain the strong
positive correlations between contradictory anti-Semitic beliefs, Adorno suggested that
incompatibilities between beliefs at a local level are dwarfed by coherence with broader beliefs
about the world - “nuclear ideas” which “tend to ‘pull in’ numerous other opinions and attitudes
and thus to form a broad ideological system.” (p. 92). Such a system “provides a rationale for
any specific idea within it and a basis for meeting and assimilating new social conditions” (p.
93). Our findings support an equivalent explanation for beliefs in contradictory conspiracy
theories, with a belief in deceptive officialdom as the nuclear idea in question.
If Adorno’s explanation for contradictory anti-Semitic beliefs can indeed be applied to
conspiracy theories, conspiracist beliefs might be most accurately viewed as not only
monological, but also ideological in nature. Just as an orthodox Marxist might interpret major
world events as arising inevitably from the forces of history, a conspiracist would see the same
events as carefully orchestrated steps in a plot for global domination. Conceptualizing
conspiracism as a coherent ideology, rather than as a cluster of beliefs in individual theories, may
be a fruitful approach in the future when examining its connection to ideologically relevant
variables such as social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism.
Although we have demonstrated the importance of a belief in deception by authority as an
important antecedent of conspiracy belief and a partial explanation for correlations between
contradictory theories, there are certainly other broad beliefs which could make a similar
contribution. For instance, conspiracy theories would seem much more plausible to those with a
belief in the effectiveness of intimidation and bribery. In a more abstract sense, a belief in the
essential malevolence of officialdom - or in the specific malevolence of a certain powerful entity
- would make many conspiracies seem more likely. The social element must not be neglected
either; many conspiracy theories are associated with specific groups or even organized
movements, such as the 9/11 Truth Movement. Clarke (2007) found a trend of increasing
vagueness in these modern conspiracist communities, which he characterised as a reaction to the
antagonistic atmosphere of Internet discourse. Our results suggest an alternative possibility: a
genuine uncertainty within individuals regarding the true nature of the conspiracy behind a
particular event (beyond the fact that there was one), and a willingness to consider and even
endorse mutually contradictory accounts as long as they stand in opposition to the officially
sanctioned narrative. There may also be an element of self-presentation and conflict avoidance
in the vagueness observed by Clarke: if multiple contradictory theories are simultaneously
believed by many in a conspiracist community, endorsing one in particular is tantamount to
denying the others, and may provoke a backlash. In any event, the development of conspiracy
theories almost certainly owes a great deal to social engagement and discussion of alternative
narratives, and the dynamics of conspiracist communities may be a fruitful avenue for future
investigation with reference to previous work on opinion-based groups (e.g. Musgrove &
McGarty, 2008).
Conspiracist belief systems may also be well-captured by connectionist models of social
inference such as Thagard’s (1989) explanatory coherence model (ECHO). ECHO has been
shown to accurately predict the degree to which higher-order beliefs about social actors affect
judgements of their actions as sinister or innocent, honest or deceptive (Read & Miller, 1993).
However, there has been little or no investigation into the ability of ECHO to model the
influence of broad worldviews. Based on the present research, one would expect that when
broad beliefs are relevant to the interpretation of a particular situation, they serve as a constraint
on the conclusions that are likely to be drawn from it in the same way as specific beliefs about
the actors and situations involved. A conspiracist belief system consisting of many such beliefs
would inhibit the acceptance of official narratives, but may not discriminate among several
different conspiracy theories. Some might be discarded, but even contradictory theories might
be simultaneously accepted. Almost any account of events which accords with the broader
beliefs in question is likely to garner some endorsement by adherents of a conspiracist
worldview. Modelling such a network might provide an instructive insight into the processes
underlying the development of conspiracist beliefs, and of other beliefs influenced by
superordinate ideological considerations.
It must be noted that not all conspiracy theories fall under the “deceptive officialdom”
umbrella. Anti-Semitic conspiracy theories are a notable and historically important exception;
instead of alleging abuse of power by elites, historical theories of Jewish conspiracy usually
detailed supposed attempts by a minority to seize power for themselves (Graumann, 1987). It
would be instructive to examine whether beliefs in such conspiracies are correlated with belief in
those that fit more closely into the “deceptive officialdom” template, and if such relationships are
mediated to the same degree by endorsement of that central belief.
In any case, the evidence we have gathered in the present study supports the idea that
conspiracism constitutes a monological belief system, drawing its coherence from central beliefs
such as the conviction that authorities and officials engage in massive deception of the public to
achieve their malevolent goals. Connectivity with this central idea lends support to any
individual conspiracy theory, even to the point that mutually contradictory theories fail to show a
negative correlation in belief. Believing that Osama bin Laden is still alive is apparently no
obstacle to believing that he has been dead for years.
References:
Bale, J.M. (2007). Political paranoia v. political realism: On distinguishing between bogus
conspiracy theories and genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns of Prejudice, 41, 45-60.
Clarke, S. (2002). Conspiracy theories and conspiracy theorizing. Philosophy of the Social
Sciences, 32, 131-150.
Clarke, S. (2007). Conspiracy theories and the Internet: Controlled demolition and arrested
development. Episteme: A Journal of Social Epistemology, 4, 167-180
Coady, D. (2006). Conspiracy theories: The philosophical debate. Aldershot: Ashgate
Publishing.
Douglas, K.M., & Sutton, R.M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and
actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. Journal of Social
Psychology, 148, 210-222.
Douglas & Sutton (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is
influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50,
542-552.
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742.
Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO Reports, 11, 493-499.
Graumann, C.F. (1987). Conspiracy: history and social psychology - a synopsis. In C.F.
Graumann and S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 245-251).
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Grzesiak-Feldman, M., & Suszek, H. (2008). Conspiracy stereotyping and perceptions of group
entitativity of Jews, Germans, Arabs and homosexuals by Polish students. Psychological
Reports, 102, 755-758.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Kay, J. (2011). Among the truthers: A journey through America’s conspiracist underground.
New York: HarperCollins.
Inglehart, R. (1987). Extremist political positions and perceptions of conspiracy. In C.F.
Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.) Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 231–244). New
York: Springer-Verlag.
Kingsley, P., & Jones, S. (2011, May 5). Osama bin Laden death: The conspiracy theories. The
Guardian. Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/world/2011/may/05/osama-bin-
laden-conspiracy-theories
Musgrove, L., & McGarty, C. (2008) Opinion-based group membership as a predictor of
collective emotional responses and support for pro- and anti-war action. Social Psychology,
39, 37-47.
Read, S. J. (1987). Constructing causal scenarios: A knowledge structure approach to causal
reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 288-302.
Read, S. J., & Miller, L.C. (1993). Rapist or "regular guy": Explanatory coherence in the
construction of mental models of others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
526-540.
Read, S. J., Snow, C. J., & Simon, D. (2003). Constraint satisfaction processes in social
reasoning. In The Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Cognitive Science Society
Conference.
Rowe, K. (Producer), Bermas, J. (Producer), Brown, M. (Producer), & Avery, D. (Director)
(2005). Loose Change [Motion Picture]. United States: Microcinema International.
Simon, D., Snow, C.J., & Read, S. J. (2004). The redux of cognitive consistency theories:
Evidence judgments by constraint satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 86, 814-837.
Smith, T.C., & Novella, S.P. (2007). HIV denial in the Internet era. PLoS Medicine, 4, 1312-
1316.
Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A
preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11
conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749-761.
Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011).
Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and
associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious
conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology.
Table 1. Correlations between endorsement of Princess Diana conspiracy theories in Study 1.
Official MI6
campaign to kill
Diana
Dodi and Diana
killed by Al-
Fayeds’ business
enemies
Diana had to die
prevent her from
marrying an Arab
Diana faked her
own death
Diana killed by
rogue cell of
British
Intelligence
.749 *** .614 *** .670 *** 0.15
Official MI6
campaign to kill
Diana
1 .660 *** .622 *** .206 *
Dodi and Diana
killed by Al-
Fayeds’ business
enemies
1 .607 *** .253 ***
Diana had to die
to prevent her
from marrying an
Arab
1 .242 **
Note: *** p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05,
p < .10. Correlations between mutually
contradictory items are bolded. All correlation coefficients are Pearson r.
Figure 1. Illustration of the observed correlations in endorsement of Study 2 items. The two
conspiracy theories display a significant zero-order correlation (above), but have no significant
direct relationship when belief in a cover-up is taken into account (below).
... In an analogy to the Ultimatum Game, conspiracy theorists are rejecting a disadvantageous distribution of understanding in favor of a situation where anyone or anything could be equally correct (see Wood et al. 2012). ...
... Although conspiracy beliefs often encounter out-groups due to their counter-mainstream nature, an antagonistic out-group is not essential for initiating these beliefs. That manipulating these factors independently influences beliefs is consistent with this, as is the fact that a facet of conspiracy ideation is the rejection of the "mainstream opinion" regardless of the subject matter (Wood et al. 2012). Thus, we suggest that the relationship between the established predictors of belief might be more parsimoniously explained as a manifestation of an evolved general (spiteful) facultative response to many domains of (potential) individual competitive disadvantage, rather than as the causal outcome of sensitivity to specific cues of (potential) existentially threatening rival groups. ...
... Endnotes 1 This definition combines elements from Goertzel (1994), Wood et al (2012), and Brotherton and French (2014) to differentiate "conspiracy theory" beliefs from the arguably rational mistrust of the powerful (see Coady 2019). It highlights that many, if not all, beliefs identified as "conspiracy theories" are not simply illogical or unlikely suspicions of malfeasance but are accepted by adherents despite their grandiose nature and better evidenced or parsimonious explanations being available. ...
Article
Full-text available
Science denialism is at the heart of many conspiracy theory beliefs. We propose that such beliefs are manifestations of a distal social process: spite. In three pre‐registered studies, we test the hypothesis that established predictors of these beliefs (epistemic, existential, and social motives) are specific cues of competitive disadvantage that provoke a common facultative “spiteful” psychological response, making a person more open to believing in conspiracy theories. Study 1 ( N = 301; UK representative Prolific sample), found that spite mediated the relationship between realistic threat and in‐group narcissism (social motives), political powerlessness (existential motive), and intolerance for uncertainty (epistemic motive), and conspiracy theory belief and COVID‐19 conspiracies. This pattern was replicated in Study 2 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample). In Study 3 ( N = 405; UK representative Prolific sample), we found that those who engaged in a spite‐inducing task reported higher levels of spite which indirectly resulted in stronger beliefs in conspiracy theories. The overall pattern of results provides initial evidence that spite may play a role in why people engage with false information. Research and policy implications of these findings are discussed.
... Considering this hypothesis, theoretical perspectives emphasizing overarching cognitive patterns have been advanced to elucidate the nature of ESB adherence. Goertzel (1994) introduced the notion of a "monological belief system" and argued that endorsing a particular conspiracy belief invariably bolsters the propensity to believe in others, even when these beliefs lack logical coherence (Wood et al., 2012). An alternative perspective suggests that certain individuals possess a predisposition to interpret events as clandestine schemes masterminded by influential and malevolent entities, a construct referred to as "conspiracy mentality" (Bruder et al., 2013). ...
... In the current study, we investigated the relationship between fictitious and familiar beliefs with the objection of revealing that general receptivity operates independently of the familiarity. Previous studies also indicated that individuals may simultaneously endorse contradictory conspiracy beliefs (Swami et al., 2011;Wood et al., 2012; but also see van Prooijen et al., 2023) which suggest overall receptivity to ESBs operates independently of the contents of the findings. Thus, representative samples should be used in the future research to enhance the applicability of findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Belief in various types of Epistemically Suspect Beliefs (ESBs), such as conspiracy theories, paranormal phenomena, and pseudoscientific claims, tends to strongly correlate. However, the use of ESB scales in the literature, which often include phenomena frequently encountered in daily life with familiar content, challenges the clarity of inferences about this relationship. To address this issue, we developed a scale for Fictitious Epistemically Suspect Beliefs (FESBs), composed entirely of novel and fabricated statements related to conspiracy, paranormal activity, and pseudoscience. In Study 1, with a Turkish sample of 448 participants, we found that FESBs positively correlated with ESBs, despite consisting of less familiar claims. Moreover, both FESBs and ESBs showed similar associations with individual differences in worldview and cognition. These findings were replicated in a larger Turkish sample (N = 786) in Study 2, and a UK sample (N = 746) in Study 3. The results indicate that individuals with higher ESBs are more likely to endorse FESBs, despite having never encountered these claims before.
... Conspiracy beliefs refer to the belief that power elites have "coordinated in secret to achieve an outcome of their own interest" (Douglas and Sutton 2023: 282). Even though most of conspiracy beliefs are unfounded or debunked, they are notoriously resistant to correction (Wood et al. 2012). We argue that these beliefs can foster negative attitudes toward news authentication, hindering individuals from engaging in this behavior. ...
... Second, conspiracy believers likely devalue news authentication itself. Conspiracy beliefs stem from a monological belief system that guides conspiratorial thinking, where conspiracy is the default explanation for any event (Miller 2020;Tam and Lee 2024;Wood et al. 2012). Individuals with such beliefs routinely engage in motivated reasoning, selectively gathering and interpreting information to maintain or bolster their existing views (Miller et al. 2016). ...
Article
In an era of pervasive misinformation, equipping citizens to counter its spread is increasingly critical. This study examines news authentication—individuals’ proactive verification of news—as a key indicator of resilience to misinformation. Guided by the theory of planned behavior and the resilience model, we examine how individual characteristics and structural contexts interact to influence news authentication. To do so, we adopt a multilevel comparative approach, analyzing news authentication in three distinct societies: Hong Kong, the Netherlands, and the United States. Drawing on a preregistered, population-based survey conducted in 2022 (N = 6,082), we apply multigroup structural equation modeling to identify the influential factors. Our findings show that, at the societal level, news authentication is more prevalent in the United States and Hong Kong, where severe polarization and fragmented, low-trust media environments amplify misinformation risks. Conversely, the Netherlands exhibits lower levels of news authentication, potentially due to its relatively cohesive media environment and moderate polarization. At the individual level, political efficacy and institutional trust are consistent predictors across societies, underscoring the importance of political empowerment and trust in fostering resilience. Education significantly predicts news authentication only in the United States, where the complex information landscape necessitates higher cognitive engagement. Notably, conspiracy beliefs positively associate with news authentication in the Netherlands and the United States, reflecting a potential “dark side” of this behavior in contexts marked by growing anti-establishment sentiments. These findings highlight the interplay between individual capacities, political beliefs, and broader media and political environments in shaping resilience to misinformation.
... For instance, conspiracy theories tend to take counter-epistemic positions and consider organized science corrupt [18][19][20]. In addition, people with a conspiracist worldview believe that large networks of people with malevolent agendas try to secretly execute plots and mass hoaxes [16,18,21,22]. Furthermore, climate change is perceived as a hoax perpetrated by scientists to seek funding or to act out on a secret agenda by the believers of conspiracy theories [18]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Foreign pseudohistory, as one format of conspiracy theories, is an unverified discourse explicitly stating that a culture, civilization, or achievement outside one’s homeland country did not exist, does not have historical continuity, or was plagiarized from this homeland country. The current study analyzed 302 videos about foreign pseudohistory from a popular Chinese video-sharing website (Bilibili) and found 213 videos supporting foreign pseudohistory and 89 videos opposing foreign pseudohistory. Videos opposing foreign pseudohistory attract more viewers and comments than videos supporting foreign pseudohistory, but the latter videos are posted by a smaller group of core creators and also attract considerable numbers of views. The inductive thematic analysis identified three major themes from these videos, including: 1) how foreign civilizations and history were based on fabrication and plagiarism; 2) promoting foreign pseudohistory as a way to fight against Western-centrism; and 3) refuting and mocking foreign pseudohistory and enlightening the public about the real history. The implications of this study were discussed
Article
Objectives Despite the fast growth of the social scientific literature on conspiracy theories, fragmentation rather than dialogue is the norm across disciplines. One such division is between the individual/cognitive versus sociopolitical dimensions of conspiracy beliefs, which are often studied in isolation. This article aims to contribute to bridging the gap. Methods We carry out a selective review of the post‐2010 literature that approaches conspiracy theories from (social) psychological and political sociological perspectives to highlight and compare their main inquiries and findings. Results The examination finds that the psychological scholarship, which deals with individual and group‐based variables, is more attuned to studying the public “demand” for conspiracy theories. By contrast, research on conspiracy theories in collective phenomena such as populism and social movements is more inclined to elucidate the “supply” side of the equation. Conclusions In addition to the quantitative‐qualitative rift already identified in the literature, conspiracy theory scholarship is also shaped by the divides that pertain to the level of analysis and the supply and demand sides of the conspiracy “market” dynamics. The article argues for a closer dialogue between micro (individual), meso (interpersonal), and macro (national/global) levels of analysis to integrate the demand and supply factors nourishing conspiracy narratives.
Article
While the psychological dispositions that underlie conspiracy thinking are well researched, there has been remarkably little research on the political preferences of conspiracy believers that go beyond self‐reported ideology or single political issue dimensions. Using data from the European Voter Election Study (EVES), the relationship between conspiracy thinking and attitudes on three deeper‐lying and salient political dimensions (redistribution, authoritarianism, migration) is examined. The results show a clear picture: Individuals with economically left‐wing and culturally conservative attitudes tend to score highest on conspiracy thinking. People at this ideological location seem to long for both economic and cultural protection and bemoan a “lost paradise” where equalities had not yet been destroyed by “perfidious” processes of cultural modernization and economic neoliberalism. This pattern is found across all countries and holds regardless of socioeconomic characteristics such as education and income. While previous research has found that belief in conspiracies tends to cluster at the extremes of the political spectrum, our analysis opens up a more complex picture, showing that conspiracy thinking is not merely related to extremist orientations, but to specific combinations of political attitudes.
Book
This Element leverages a comparative approach to understand how conspiracy theories and their believers differ within and across countries. Using original survey data from eight varied cases (Australia, Brazil, Canada, Germany, Lebanon, Morocco, South Africa, and the United States) the authors present specific contemporary conspiracy theories, illustrate how these theories appeal in their national context, and determine whether the characteristics of the typical conspiracy theory believer vary across setting. They first demonstrate that there is a wide range of conspiracy theories, some of which have worldwide reach, whereas others are more context specific. Then, they show that the determinants of individual conspiracism are very similar in the Western world and Brazil, but do not necessarily travel to Lebanon, Morocco and South Africa. Lastly, they summarize the main conclusions of this Element and discuss the need for greater comparative research on conspiracy theories and propose clear areas for future research.
Article
Feeling positively connected to other people is a basic human need. If this need is threatened by feeling lonely, people might become more susceptible to conspiracy theories to help make sense of their surroundings. Simultaneously, conspiracy beliefs could lead to loneliness because they can strain existing relationships. Using two pre‐registered longitudinal studies, we investigated the reciprocal relationship between loneliness and conspiracy mentality (Study 1, N = 1604) and the more malleable specific conspiracy beliefs (Study 2, N = 1502) during the COVID‐19 pandemic. Random‐intercept cross‐lagged panel models showed that people who are, on average, lonelier are also more likely to believe in conspiracy theories. However, the data provided no support for the notion that conspiracy beliefs and loneliness predicted each other over time. The research helps to understand the thus far mixed evidence on loneliness and conspiracy beliefs and adds important insights to the literature on conspiracy beliefs and need deprivation.
Article
The monological belief system model suggests that—for at least a subset of people—developing a belief in one conspiracy theory will cause them to be more likely to believe in others. This model has been influential in the literature, but its core causal hypothesis has never been credibly tested. We therefore tested it in two longitudinal studies. Study 1 used a sample from New Zealand and Australia ( N = 498), with 7 monthly waves. Study 2 (preregistered) used a sample from New Zealand, Australia and the United Kingdom ( N = 978), with 13 monthly waves. We applied random intercept cross‐lagged panel models, permitting a credible causal identification strategy, albeit we cannot rule out time‐varying confounds. We find that increased belief in a conspiracy theory at one wave did (on average) predict increased belief in other conspiracies at the next wave, although the estimated coefficients were small.
Article
Full-text available
A model of causal reasoning based on Schank and Abelson's (1977) analysis of knowledge structures is presented. The first part of this article outlines the necessary characteristics of such a model. It is argued that a central attributional problem is to explain extended sequences of behavior. To do this people must relate actions in a sequence to one another and construct a coherent scenario from them. Because the relation among actions is not given, people must use detailed social and physical knowledge to make connecting inferences. The resulting scenario typically includes information about the plans and goals of the actor. The second part of this article analyzes how the knowledge structures outlined by Schank and Abelson (1977) —scripts, plans, goals, and themes—can be used to construct such causal scenarios, and it presents a process model for the construction of such scenarios. The last part of this article examines the implications of this model and its relations to other attribution models by Kelley (1967, 1971a, 1971b) and Jones (Jones & Davis, 1965; Jones & McGillis, 1976).
Article
Full-text available
Surveyed 348 residents of southwestern New Jersey and found that most believed that several of a list of 10 conspiracy theories were at least probably true. Ss who believed in 1 conspiracy were more likely also to believe in others. Belief in conspiracies was correlated with anomia, lack of interpersonal trust, and insecurity about employment. Blacks and Hispanics were more likely to believe in conspiracy theories than were Whites. Younger Ss were slightly more likely to believe in conspiracy theories, but there were few significant correlations with gender, educational level, or occupational category. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Following Clarke (2002), a Lakatosian approach is used to account for the epistemic development of conspiracy theories. It is then argued that the hypercritical atmosphere of the internet has slowed down the development of conspiracy theories, discouraging conspiracy theorists from articulating explicit versions of their favoured theories, which could form the hard core of Lakatosian research programmes. The argument is illustrated with a study of the “controlled demolition” theory of the collapse of three towers at the World Trade Center on September 11, 2001.
Chapter
Social psychologists, when first introduced into the field (or rather, underground world) of conspiracy and conspiratorial conceptions, experience two kinds of surprise. First, they will be struck and then puzzled by the obvious fact that there is a large and complex field of potential social-psychological research of which they have been unaware. Neither in the broad context of the general problems of their field nor in the special domains of the attribution of guilt and blame have they come across conspiracy Not for the first time they will have to concede that there is a topic of intrinsic psychological interest that has been left to history and to other social sciences, as has happened with most social issues that could not easily be tailored into experimental size.
Chapter
Conspiracy implies secret communication. It occurs when a group is plotting to attain some goal, and keeping their actions secret from those who would otherwise oppose them.
Article
The dismissive attitude of intellectuals toward conspiracy theorists is considered and given some justification. It is argued that intellectuals are entitled to an attitude of prima facie skepticism toward the theories propounded by conspiracy theorists, because conspiracy theorists have an irrational tendency to continue to believe in conspiracy theories, even when these take on the appearance of forming the core of degenerating research program. It is further argued that the pervasive effect of the “fundamental attribution error” can explain the behavior of such conspiracy theorists. A rival approach due to Brian Keeley, which involves the criticism of a subclass of conspiracy theories on epistemic grounds, is considered and found to be inadequate.
Article
Scholars and intellectuals often fail to pay sufficient attention to the historical and political importance of conspiratorial politics, that is, real-world covert and clandestine activities. This is primarily because they rarely make an effort to distinguish conceptually between such activities, which are a regular if not omnipresent feature of national and international politics, and bogus 'conspiracy theories', elaborate fantasies that purport to show that various sinister, powerful groups with evil intentions, operating behind the scenes, are secretly controlling the course of world events. Bale's purpose is to provide a clear analytical distinction between actual conspiratorial politics and 'conspiracy theories' in the pejorative sense of that term, and to suggest that research methods appropriate to investigating and analysing the former have long been available. In a world full of secret services, surreptitious pressure groups, criminal cartels and terrorist organizations, academics can no longer afford to ignore bona fide conspiratorial activities of various types, which have often had considerable historical significance in the past and are likely to continue to exert an impact on events in the future.
Article
A model is presented of how people construct coherent representations of others. It integrates work on knowledge representations with Kintsch's construction-integration model of discourse comprehension and Thagard's model of explanatory coherence. A major claim is that parallel constraint satisfaction processes, fundamental to connectionist modeling, play a major role in the development of coherent representations. Several topics are examined: (a) the role of making goal inferences in trait inferences, (b) how people combine apparently inconsistent traits to arrive at a coherent impression, and (c) how this parallel process model can account for findings that have been given a serial interpretation in Trope's two-stage model of dispositional inference and Gilbert's work on cognitive busyness. It is argued that this model provides a more parsimonious but broader explanation for attributions than alternatives.