ArticlePDF Available

The myth of hidden ovulation: Shape and texture changes in the face during the menstrual cycle

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In recent years, evidence has been gathered indicating increased attractiveness of female faces at the point of ovulation. In this paper, we asked what changes in facial appearance occur during menstrual cycle that lead to this shift in attractiveness. We analysed facial photographs of 20 young women with a normal cycle. We found evidence for textural changes, as well as shape changes that might account for the ovulatory peak in attractiveness. Generally, facial shape at ovulation is perceived as more attractive — and ovulating women are perceived as more attractive the closer their face shape is to the “ovulation shape”. These findings support the hypothesis that attractive signals might be rooted in signals of fertility.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 2012, 163–175
DOI: 10.1556/JEP.10.2012.4.1
1789–2082 © 2012 Akadémiai Kiadó, Budapest
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION:
SHAPE AND TEXTURE CHANGES IN THE FACE
DURING THE MENSTRUAL CYCLE
DR. E. OBERZAUCHER
1
, DR. S. KATINA
2,3
, MAG. S. F. SCHMEHL
1
,
M
AG. I. J. HOLZLEITNER
1
, DR. K. GRAMMER
1,*
1
Department of Anthropology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
2
Department of Anthropology and Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Faculty of Science,
Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
3
Department of Applied Mathematics and Statistics, Faculty of Mathematics, Physics and
Informatics, Comenius University, Bratislava, Slovakia
Abstract. In recent years, evidence has been gathered indicating increased attractiveness of fe-
male faces at the point of ovulation. In this paper, we asked what changes in facial appearance oc-
cur during menstrual cycle that lead to this shift in attractiveness. We analysed facial photographs
of 20 young women with a normal cycle. We found evidence for textural changes, as well as
shape changes that might account for the ovulatory peak in attractiveness. Generally, facial shape
at ovulation is perceived as more attractive – and ovulating women are perceived as more attrac-
tive the closer their face shape is to the “ovulation shape”. These findings support the hypothesis
that attractive signals might be rooted in signals of fertility.
Keywords: ovulation, facial attractiveness, symmetry, geometric morphometric methodology
INTRODUCTION
The fact that human females do not appear to show any visible signs of ovulation –
unlike other primates – has led to the development of a rich theoretical body of
work attempting to explain this phenomenon in terms of evolutionary constraints.
Has hidden oestrus evolved to trick males into forming a pair bond (A
LEXAN-
DER
and NOONAN 1979)? Following this line of reasoning, males unaware of fe-
males’ fertility would remain bonded to ensure impregnation and paternity; a fe-
male providing clues to her ovulation might risk losing male investment (T
RIVERS
1972). Or, quite the opposite, has hidden oestrous arisen to increase paternal insecu-
rity to allow females to “escape the negative consequences of being pawns in mar-
riage games” (G
RAY and WOLFE 1983)? Once monogamy is established, an op-
tional female strategy would be to copulate outside their long-term relationships,
*
Address for correspondence:
1
Dr. E. OBERZAUCHER, University of Vienna Vienna, Austria,
e-mail: elisabeth.oberzaucher@univie.ac.at.
2
DR. S. KATINA, Department of Anthropology and
Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Faculty of Science, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech
Republic, e-mail: stanislav.katina@gmail.com
3
MAG. S. F. SCHMEHL, University of Vienna, De-
partment of Anthropology, e-mail: susanne.schmehl@univie.ac.at.
1
MAG. I. J. HOLZLEITNER, Uni-
versity of Vienna, Dept. of Anthropology, e-mail: iris.holzleitner@univie.ac.at.
1
DR. K. GRAM-
MER
, University of Vienna, Dept. of Anthropology, e-mail: grammer@univie.ac.at
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
164
thus obtaining superior genes for their offspring while ensuring paternal investment
by her long-term partner (B
ELLIS and BAKER 1991). Indeed, increased extra-pair
copulations coincide with an increase in females’ self-reported arousal to sexual
stimuli (L
USCHEN and PIERCE 1972), peaks in sexual receptivity (ADAMS, GOLD
and B
URT 1978), and an increased amount of attraction to, and fantasies about, men
who are not their primary partners. Yet another hypothesis speculates that signals of
oestrus might have been reduced to counter infanticide by increased paternal uncer-
tainty (H
RDY 1981). Unfortunately, these theories cannot be tested empirically.
In recent years, evidence has accumulated indicating that women’s mate pref-
erences change during ovulation (R
OBERTS et al. 2004). In this context, the term
hormone-mediated adaptive design has been introduced: In their fertile phase,
women prefer more masculine features than in their non-fertile phase (J
OHNSTON et
al. 2001). In addition, male body odour smells most intense and least unpleasant to
women at ovulation (D
OTY et al. 1975; SINGH and BRONSTAD 2001).
Female behaviour changes, too: Walking style becomes more feminine
(G
RAMMER et al. 2003), and women dress more ‘sexily’ during the fertile window
of their cycle (G
RAMMER, RENNINGER and FISCHER 2004). Not only do women be-
have differently, they are also perceived differently: Several recent studies show
that women are judged to be more attractive when in their fertile phase of the men-
strual cycle (R
OBERTS et al. 2004; LAW SMITH et al. 2006).
The fact that women lack ostentatious sexual swellings does not imply that
women do not signal their fertility status. Both the sexual swellings in primates, as
well as the shifts in human behaviour and facial appearance during ovulation, ap-
parently make females more attractive to males. Thus, we think the term ‘concealed
ovulation’ to be inappropriate and misleading.
The fact that women are most attractive in the fertile window of their cycle
(R
OBERTS et al. 2004; LAW SMITH et al. 2006) causes us to question whether we
have been approaching the whole topic from the wrong end: Traits associated with
attractiveness could well be perceived as attractive because they are signals of ovu-
lation. Instead of ovulation being hidden in humans, signals of ovulation might have
spread over the whole menstrual cycle. Thus, fertility signals may have become part
of overall attractiveness.
Female attractiveness is characterised by a number of aspects, such as reduced
fluctuating asymmetry, markers of hormone levels, skin coloration, and neoteny
markers. According to the parasite theory of sexual selection (H
AMILTON and ZUK
1982), the absence of anomalies in ontogeny shows ‘developmental stability’: The
inability to cope with environmental and genetic perturbations is for example re-
flected in elevated levels of fluctuating asymmetry (G
ANGESTAD, THORNHILL and
Y
EO 1994; GRAMMER and THORNHILL 1994; THORNHILL and MOLLER 1998), a
symmetric face presumably indicates high immune competence (T
HORNHILL and
G
ANGESTAD 1993). Preferences for averageness have been linked to heterozygos-
ity, which represents a larger buffer against pathogens.
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
165
In addition, immune competence is highly relevant because the steroid repro-
ductive hormones responsible for the development of attractive, secondary sexual
traits, may negatively affect immunological functions (F
OLSTAD and KARTER
1992). Thus, markers developed with the help of high sex hormone levels may sig-
nal the ability of the immune system to deal with the deteriorating effect of oestro-
gen (S
ERVICE 1998). The display of handicaps such as hormone markers can signal
the ability to meet the costs imposed by this handicap. As another example, Z
AHAVI
and Z
AHAVI (1997) discuss red cheeks and lips. The red colouring is a result of pe-
ripheral blood circulation below the skin, which cannot be kept up in a cold envi-
ronment or during times of illness.
Skin texture is also known to affect the attractiveness of faces, and it too seems
to signal fitness. R
OBERTS et al. (2005) found skin homogeneity and attractiveness
to be positively associated with heterozygosity in MHC-genes. M
ATTS et al. (2007)
found that a different skin can make the same face look 20 years older. Skin dark-
ens in pregnancy, under the influence of hormonal contraception, which has been
taken to assume that paler skin – found to be attractive in previous studies – might
be an indicator of neoteny (F
ROST 1988).
J
ONES (1996) showed that female faces exhibiting certain – neotenous – facial
proportions – small lower face, lower jaw and nose, and large lips – are perceived
as more attractive. The strong association between youth and fecundity in adult fe-
males supposedly led to the male preference for facial markers of high, age-related
fecundity. Women’s faces are attractive when they possess small lower facial fea-
tures, especially a gracile jaw, and large lips, which manifest under the influence of
oestrogen. These features may also be signals of nulliparous status (S
YMONS 1995).
In the present study we address the question why ovulating women are per-
ceived as more attractive. Can increased attractiveness at ovulation be linked to
measurable parameters? And, are those parameters similar to those already known
to be positively linked to female attractiveness?
METHODS
20 females aged 19 to 31 (23.35 ± 3.15) were photographed every day at the same
time throughout an entire cycle after giving informed consent. Ovulation was de-
tected using ovulation kits. For this investigation we used the photo from the first
day of ovulation and a luteal picture from 14 days after ovulation.
In a two-alternative forced choice task, 15 men (aged 24.20 ± 4.57) and 10
women (aged 26.80 ± 4.49) were asked to identify from each ovulatory/luteal pair
the image which was rated more highly on a number of features (attractive, healthy,
sexy, sociable, trustworthy, young, and likeable). Ovulatory and luteal images were
presented simultaneously on a computer screen in random order, with no indication
of the image types.
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
166
The statistical analyses were performed in R software (R DEVELOPMENT,
C
ORE TEAM 2010). The results of hypotheses testing were considered as statisti-
cally significant at p-value<0.05. All p-values were calculated by a permutation
method (number of permutations = 5000).
The odds that the face in ovulatory state was rated more positively for a par-
ticular item was tested by a permutation Chi-square test. The null hypothesis is as
follows: odds are less than or equal to 1, where 1 means that ovulatory and luteal
state were chosen with the same probability.
We analysed the morphological changes using geometric morphometrics. On
the facial photographs, we measured Cartesian coordinates of 46 anthropological
landmarks and 26 semilandmarks (6 on the upper and lower outline of each eye-
brow, 7 on each half of the lower face, from the ear lobe to the turning point of left
and right chin, Table 1, Fig. 1).
Table 1. Definition of (semi)landmarks; left/right orientation – with respect to the observer
*
excluded from the analysis
1
*
Forehead right The right corner of the forehead, located in the hairline
2
*
Trichion The mid point of the hairline
3
*
Forehead left The left corner of the forehead, located in the hairline
4 Superciliare laterale right The most lateral point of the right eyebrow
5 Upper eyebrow right
6 Upper eyebrow right
7 Upper eyebrow right
Semilandmarks
8 Superciliare mediale right The most medial point of the right eyebrow
9 Lower eyebrow right
10 Lower eyebrow right
11 Lower eyebrow right
Semilandmarks
12 Superciliare mediale left The most medial point of the left eyebrow
13 Upper eyebrow left
14 Upper eyebrow left
15 Upper eyebrow left
Semilandmarks
16 Superciliare laterale left The most lateral point of the left eyebrow
17 Lower eyebrow left
18 Lower eyebrow left
19 Lower eyebrow left
Semilandmarks
20 Exocanthion right The outer corner of the right eye fissure where the eyelids meet
21 Iris top right
The highest point of the right iris; if not visible, point is located
on the approximated elongation of the circumference of the pupil
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
167
Table 1. (Continue)
22 Endocanthion right
The right inner corner of the right eye fissure where the eyelids
meet
23 Iris bottom right
The lowest point of the right iris; if not visible, point is located
on the approximated elongation of the circumference of the pupil
24 Iris laterale right The most lateral point of the right iris
25 Pupil right The mid point of the right pupil
26 Iris mediale right The most medial point of the right iris
27 Endocanthion left The inner corner of the left eye fissure where the eyelids meet
28 Iris top left
The highest point of the left iris; if not visible, point is located on
the approximated elongation of the circumference of the pupil
29 Exocanthion left The outer corner of the left eye fissure where the eyelids meet
30 Iris bottom left
The lowest point of the left iris; if not visible, point is located on
the approximated elongation of the circumference of the pupil
31 Iris mediale left The most medial point of the left iris
32 Pupil left The mid point of the left pupil
33 Iris laterale left The most lateral point of the left iris
34 Alare origin right
The most inner point of the right ala of the nose; or elongation of
the nasolabial folds on right ala of the nose
35 Alare right The most lateral point on the right nasal ala
36
Columella apex
(columella breakpoint,
nostril top point) right
(The most anterior or) the highest point on the columella crest at
the apex of the right nostril
37 Subnasale
On the local midline of the junction formed by the lower border
of the nasal septum (the partition that divides the nostrils) and the
cutaneous portion of the upper lip
38
Columella apex
(columella breakpoint,
nostril top point) left
(The most anterior or) the highest point on the columella crest at
the apex of the left nostril
39 Alare left The most lateral point on the left nasal ala
40 Alare origin left
Most inner point of the left ala of the nose; or elongation of the
nasolabial folds on left ala of the nose
41 Cheilion right
The right corner of the mouth where the outer edges of the up
p
er
and lower vermilions meet
42 Upper lip right
The point in the middle of Labiale superius (44) and right Cheil-
ion (41)
43 Crista philtri right
The point on the right crest of the philtrum, i.e. the vertical
groove in the median proportion of the upper lip, located on the
vermilion border
44 Labiale superius
The philtrum mid point, located on the vermilion border of the
upper lip
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
168
Table 1. (Continue)
45 Crista philtri left
The point on the crest of the left philtrum, i.e. the vertical groove
in the median proportion of the upper lip, located on the vermil-
ion border
46 Upper lip left
The point in the middle between Labiale superius (44) and left
Cheilion (47)
47 Cheilion left
The left corner of the mouth where the outer edges of the upper
and lower vermilions meet
48 Lower lip left
The point in the middle between Labiale inferius (49) and left
Cheilion (47)
49 Labiale inferius The mid point of the vermilion border of the lower lip
50 Lower lip right
The point in the middle between Labiale inferius (49) and right
Cheilion (41)
51 Cleft right
The point between Stomion (52) and right Cheilion (41), exactly
on the right cleft between upper and lower lip
52 Stomion The mid point of the labial fissure
53 Cleft left
The point between Stomion (52) and left Cheilion (47), on the
left cleft between upper and lower lip
54 Zygion right The most lateral point on the right zygomatic arch
55 Otobasion inferius right
The lowest point of attachment of the right ear lobe to the cheek,
which determines the lower border of right ear insertion
56 Lower face right
57 Lower face right
58 Lower face right
59 Lower face right
60 Lower face right
61 Lower face right
62 Lower face right
Semilandmarks
63 Gnathion The turning point of the right and left side of the chin
64 Lower face left
65 Lower face left
66 Lower face left
67 Lower face left
68 Lower face left
69 Lower face left
70 Lower face left
Semilandmarks
71 Otobasion inferius left
The lowest point of attachment of the left ear lobe to the cheek,
which determines the lower border of left ear insertion
72 Zygion left The most lateral point on the left zygomatic arch
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
169
Fig. 1. Design of the (semi)landmarks (left and middle; numbers correspond to Table 1) and
statistically significant (semi)landmarks (•: p-values < 0.05, not adjusted by any multiple post-hoc
correction, (semi)landmarkwise matched-pair Goodall F-test)
We calculated the Procrustes shape coordinates using a generalized Procrustes
analysis (B
OOKSTEIN 1991). Firstly, the centroid of each (semi)landmark configura-
tion was found, and its root mean square distance to the (semi)landmarks computed;
this is called centroid size (CS). Secondly, the forms were re-scaled to CS = 1, the
centroids superimposed, and the forms rotated around the common centroid, until
the sum of squared distances between corresponding (semi)landmarks was a mini-
mum over all such rotations. This resulted in Procrustes shape coordinates. Equidis-
tantly marked semilandmarks were slid with respect to reference curves using bend-
ing energy. Sliding was performed iteratively on tangents, in a locally linear way, to
get geometrically homologous semilandmarks on the curves.
Hypotheses about mean shape differences between ovulatory and luteal states
were tested by permutation matched-pair Goodall F-test and, additionally, for visu-
alisation purposes, by (semi)landmarkwise matched-pair Goodall F-test, where
p-values were not adjusted by any multiple post-hoc correction. The aim of point-
wise statistical inference was only to visualize the statistical and biological signal
present in the data. Additionally we investigated fluctuating asymmetry in both
states (luteal and ovulatory) separately by permutation one-sample Mardia–
Bookstein–Moreton test (M
ARDIA, BOOKSTEIN and MORETON 2000). The level of
asymmetry was determined as the sum of squares of differences between original
and reflected forms.
A shape space matched-pair principal components analysis (PCA, also called
relative warp analysis, RWA) of the covariance matrix of approximate tangent Pro-
crustes shape coordinates (centered Procrustes shape coordinates, Procrustes fit co-
ordinates) was conducted in several forms: 1) PCA in full shape space, 2) PCA in
subspace of global bending patterns, and 3) PCA in subspace of local bending pat-
terns. Variability was decomposed into orthogonal components and, subsequently,
these components of shape variation were examined (B
OOKSTEIN 1991) and the
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
170
main direction and size of the shape changes between luteal and ovulatory state
were identified. In the subspace of the first two PCs, a permutation one-sample test
was used to assess whether PC scores were significantly different from zero (in
which case the null hypothesis that ovulatory and luteal faces do not differ in the
particular PC subspace was rejected).
With a symmetrical two-block partial least square (PLS) analysis (B
OOKSTEIN
1994; S
AMPSON, STREISSGUTH, BARR and BOOKSTEIN 1989) we investigated the
correspondence between the set of centered Procrustes shape coordinates (block 1),
and attraction variables and degree of fluctuating asymmetry (block 2). The multi-
variate association between both blocks can be expressed by means of Pearson
product-moment correlation coefficients of singular warp (SW) scores.
Both PCA and PLS find low-dimensional linear combinations of high-
dimensional measurements by adapting one singular value decomposition of com-
mon and cross-block covariance matrices, respectively. The biologically meaning-
ful signal was investigated in the first two principal components (PCs) and the first
SW.
The visualisation of shape change from source to target was performed by thin
plate spline (TPS) deformation grids (B
OOKSTEIN 1991). The Procrustes mean
shape was chosen as a reference (source) form (Fig. 1). To quantify mean shape dif-
ferences, luteal and ovulatory mean shapes were chosen as targets. In both, PCA
and PLS, the matrix of scaled eigenvectors (PC loadings and singular vectors, resp.)
was used in the same way. If necessary, TPS deformation grids were extrapolated
(magnified) in a particular direction to ease the visualisation.
The analysis of skin texture homogeneity was carried out with co-occurrence
matrices from a 50×50 pixels sized patch from the left cheek (H
ARALICK, SHAN-
MUGAM
and DINSTEIN 1973). The co-occurrence matrix allows measuring the spa-
tial interrelationships of grey tones in a textural pattern to be measured and this
provides objective measurements for skin texture. The colour characteristics in skin
textures where calculated in a HSV-colour space as an average over the whole
patch. These procedures have been employed successfully in previous studies
(F
INK, GRAMMER and MATTS 2006; GRAMMER and THORNHILL 1994; MATTS,
F
INK, GRAMMER and BURQUEST 2007).
Hypotheses about mean differences between ovulatory and luteal state in par-
ticular texture variables were tested by permutation matched-pair student’s t-test.
The null hypothesis was one-sided with the mean difference being less than or equal
to zero.
Reliability of landmarks was calculated for both x- and y-coordinates simulta-
neously as total variance (trace of covariance matrix of particular landmark). It re-
flects intra- and inter-observer error scaled by sample total variance (n = 20). Me-
dian intra-observer error was 3.60%, median inter-observer error 6.61%. The less
reliable measurements were, not surprisingly, Type II and III landmarks (B
OOK-
STEIN
, 1991), namely forehead
*
, trichion
*
, superciliare, alare, alare origin, colu-
mella apex, iris mediale and laterale, and gnathion
*
(excluded from the analyses).
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
171
RESULTS
Ovulatory faces were chosen significantly more often as being more attractive,
healthy, sexy, sociable, trustworthy, young, and likeable than luteal faces (Table 2).
Table 2. Frequencies of ovulatory face picture being chosen in the forced choice task
item
absolute
frequency
relative frequency in % ±
sd
Chi-square
stat
p-value
attractive 288 57.60 ± 2.21 11.25 < 0.001
healthy 306 61.20 ± 2.18 24.64 < 0.001
sexy 301 60.20 ± 2.19 20.40 < 0.001
sociable 273 54.60 ± 2.23 4.05 0.022
trustworthy 275 55.00 ± 2.22 4.80 0.014
young 293 58.60 ± 2.20 14.45 < 0.001
likeable 282 56.40 ± 2.22 7.94 0.002
Ovulatory and luteal faces were significantly different in PC2 of full shape
space (p = 0.002), and in PC1 and PC2 of local bending patterns (p = 0.001 and p =
0.032, resp.). In all PCs (see Fig. 2), the lower face is more robust in the luteal
phase, the nose is broader, and the eyebrows are more pronounced; in the ovulatory
phase, the lips are fuller and the whole face is more gracile (Fig. 2).
Fig. 2. TPS deformation grids; PCA in full shape space (first column; PC2, explained
variance 12.17%); PCA for global bending patterns (second column; PC1, 47.16%), and PCA for
local bending patterns (last two columns; PC1 and PC2, 14.46% and 12.24%, resp.)
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
172
Shape corresponds to attractiveness ratings in more or less the same direction:
Among ovulatory faces, those which have the least deviation from the average ovu-
latory face are perceived as most attractive, whereas those whose shape is similar to
the average luteal face are perceived as least attractive (Fig. 3).
Fig. 3. TPS deformation grids of PLS in SW1 (ovulatory faces only, explained variance
54.84%, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient of attractiveness ratings and shape
coordinates = 0.622, calculated based on SW1 scores)
Ovulatory faces were not significantly asymmetric (p = 0.4), while luteal faces
were strongly asymmetric (p < 0.0001).
Ovulatory skin had significantly lower hue (it is redder), lower contrast, and
increased homogeneity compared to luteal skin (Table 3).
Table 3. Results of the texture analysis
item mean ovulatory ± sd mean luteal ± sd t-stat p-value
homogeneity 0.257 ± 0.028 0.243 ± 0.025 2.936 0.008
correlation 7.020×10
11
± 2.520×10
11
8.13×10
11
± 2.210×10
11
–2.097 0.050
hue 0.026 ± 0.059 0.029 ± 0.050 –2.875 0.010
DISCUSSION
Our findings confirm earlier studies insofar as facial pictures of ovulatory women
were chosen significantly more often as being more attractive, healthy, sexy, socia-
ble, trustworthy, young, and likeable than luteal faces. We could identify the shape
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
173
changes that occur between the luteal and ovulatory state. The lower face is more
robust in the luteal phase, the nose is broader, and the eyebrows are more pro-
nounced. This corresponds to what have been described as masculine features in the
literature (R
OBERTS et al. 2004). In the ovulatory phase the lips are fuller and the
whole face is less robust. Fuller lips and a fragile lower face have been previously
associated with youthfulness and high levels of oestrogen (G
RAMMER et al. 2003;
S
YMONS 1995). The increased redness of the face is probably due to higher periph-
eral blood circulation.
While we took all measures to ensure that the faces were photographed in ex-
actly frontal position, any two-dimensional approach is vulnerable to tilting effects.
Part of the shape changes we find might be due to changes in head pose, i.e. the
luteal faces might be tilted back more. In order to rule out this possibility, we will
collect 3D data as a next step, which is not affected by head pose.
The findings of our study support the idea that some characteristics of what we
perceive as an attractive female face are actually synonymous to signals of ovula-
tion. Thus, the notion of hidden ovulation has to be challenged: First, as there are
detectable changes in the appearance of the female face at the point of ovulation, it
cannot be considered hidden. Second, the signal value of those characteristics has to
be reinvestigated: What we perceive as being attractive might just signal ovulation,
conveying no information beyond the hormonal state.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Statistical analyses were supported by VEGA grant Nr. 2/0038/12 to SK.
REFERENCES
A
DAMS, D.B., GOLD, A.R. and BURT, A.D. (1978): Rise in female initiaed sexual activity at
ovulation and its suppression by oral contraceptives. New England Journal of Medicine,
299, 1145–1150.
A
LEXANDER, R. and NOONAN, K. (1979): Concealment of ovulation, parental care, and human
social evolution. In: Chagnon, N.A., Irons, W.G. (eds): Evolutionary Biology and Human
Social Behavior: an anthropological perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press,
pp. 436–453.
B
ELLIS, M. and BAKER, R. (1991): Do females promote sperm competition? Data for humans.
Animimal Behavior, 40(5), 997–999.
BOOKSTEIN, F.L. (1991): Morphometric Tools for Landmark Data: Geometry and Biology.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
BOOKSTEIN, F.L. (1994): Partial least squares: a dose-response model for measurement in the
behavioral and brain sciences. Psychology 5, 1–10.
D
OTY, R.L., FORD, M., PRETI, G. and HUGGINS, G.R. (1975): Changes in intensity and
pleasantness of human vaginal odors during the menstrual cycle. Science, 190(4221), 1316–
1318.
E. OBERZAUCHER et al.
JEP 10(2012)4
174
F
INK, B., GRAMMER, K., and MATTS, P.J. (2006): Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the
perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27(6),
433–442.
F
OLSTAD, I. and KARTER, A.J. (1992): Parasites, bright males, and the immunocompetence
handicap. Am. Nat., 139(3), 603–622.
FROST, P. (1988): Human skin color: A possible relationship between its sexual dimorphism and
its social perception. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 32(1), 38–58.
GANGESTAD, S.W., THORNHILL, R. and YEO, R.A. (1994): Facial attractiveness, developmental
stability, and fluctuating asymmetry. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(2), 73–85.
GRAMMER, K. and THORNHILL, R. (1994): Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual
selection – The role of symmetry and averageness. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
108(3), 233–242.
G
RAMMER, K., KEKI, V., STRIEBEL, B., ATZMÜLLER, M. and FINK, B. (2003): Bodies in motion: a
window to the soul. In: Voland, E., Grammer, K. (eds) Evolutionary Aesthetics. Heidelberg,
Berlin, New York: Springer, pp 295–324.
G
RAMMER, K., RENNINGER, L. and FISCHER, B. (2004): Disco clothing, female sexual motivation,
and relationship status: Is she dressed to impress? Journal of Sex Research, 41(1), 66–74.
GRAY, J.P. and WOLFE, L.D. (1983): Human female sexual cycles and the concealment of
ovulation problem. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 6, 345–352.
HAMILTON, W.D. and ZUK, M. (1982): Heritable true fitness and bright birds – A role for
parasites. Science, 218(4570), 384–387.
HARALICK, R.M., SHANMUGAM, K., and DINSTEIN, I. (1973): Textural features for image
classification. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. SMC 3(6), 610–621.
H
RDY, S.B. (1981): The Woman that Never Evolved. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
J
OHNSTON, V.S., HAGEL, R., FRANKLIN, M., FINK, B. and GRAMMER, K. (2001): Male facial
attractiveness: evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 22(4), 251–267.
J
ONES, D. (1996): An evolutionary perspective on physical attractiveness. Evolutionary
Anthropology, 5(3), 97–109.
LAW SMITH, M.J., PERRETT, D.I., JONES, B.C., CORNWELL, R.E., MOORE, F.R., FEINBERG, D.R.,
BOOTHROYD, L.G., DURRANI, S.J., STIRRAT, M.R., WHITEN, S., PITMAN, R.M. and HILLIER,
S.G. (2006): Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proceedings of the
Royal Society B, 273(1583), 135–140.
L
USCHEN, M.E. and PIERCE, D.M. (1972): Effect of menstrual cycle on mood and sexual
arousability. Journal of Sex Research, 8(1), 41–47.
M
ARDIA, K.V., BOOKSTEIN, F.L., and MORETON, I.J. (2000): Statistical assessment of bilateral
symmetry of shapes. Biometrika 87(2), 285–300.
M
ATTS, P.J., FINK, B., GRAMMER, K. and BURQUEST, M. (2007): Color homogeneity and visual
perception of age, health, and attractiveness of female facial skin. Journal of the American
Academy of Dermatoogy., 57 (6), 977–984.
R
DEVELOPMENT CORE TEAM (2010): R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing
(R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
ROBERTS, S.C., HAVLICEK, J., FLEGR, J., HRUSKOVA, M., LITTLE, A.C., JONES, B.C., PERRETT, D.I.
and PETRIE, M. (2004): Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the
menstrual cycle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 270–272.
ROBERTS, S.C., LITTLE, A.C., MORRIS GOSLING, L., PERRETT, D.I., JONES, B.C., PENTON-VOAK, I.
and PETRIE, M. (2005): MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 26(3), 213–226.
THE MYTH OF HIDDEN OVULATION
JEP 10(2012)4
175
S
AMPSON, P.D., STREISSGUTH, A.P., BARR, H.M., and BOOKSTEIN, F.L. (1989): Neurobehavioral
effects of prenatal alcohol: Part II. Partial least squares analysis. Neurotoxicol. Teratol.
11(5), 477–491.
S
ERVICE, R.F. (1998): New role for estrogen in cancer? Science, 279(5357), 1631–1633.
S
INGH, D. and BRONSTAD, P.M. (2001): Female body odour is a potential cue to ovulation.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 268 (1469), 797–801.
S
YMONS, D. (1995): Neoteny reconsidered (commentary on “Sexual selection, physical
attractiveness, and facial neoteny” by D. Jones). Current Anthropology, 36, 741–743.
T
HORNHILL, R. and GANGESTAD, S.W. (1993): Human facial beauty – Averageness, symmetry,
and parasite resistance. Human Nature, 4(3), 237–269.
THORNHILL, R. and MOLLER, A.P. (1998): The relative importance of size and asymmetry in
sexual selection. Behavioral Ecology, 9(6), 546–551.
TRIVERS, R.L. (1972): Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell, B. (ed.): Sexual
Selection and the Descent of Men, 1871–1971. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, pp.
136–179.
Z
AHAVI, A. and ZAHAVI, A. (1997): The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwin’s
Puzzle. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
... Articles in which visual recognition behavior was measured with regard to facial attractiveness of facial stimuli and emotional expressions were found. 15,17,26,27,30,32,35,36,[38][39][40]47 These studies used a variety of stimuli: for example, prototypes of male and female facial stimuli of different ethnicities at different stages of the menstrual cycle. 39 In addition to these models, neurocognitive studies were found that evaluated visuospatial memory and visual working memory. ...
... According to the authors of that study, the preference for facial attractiveness in the ovulatory phase correlated with the hormonal fertility state. 40 Stimuli of male and female faces were used in a study aimed at measuring the facial attractiveness of women in the follicular and luteal phases. It was found that there was a higher attractiveness rate in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle than in the menstrual and luteal phases. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: This article systematically updates the literature on changes in visual functions during the phases of the normal menstrual cycle in women. Objectives: To update Guttridge's 1994 review of visual structures and functions associated with the menstrual cycle and broaden the search through psychophysical, neuroimaging and neurobehavioral measurements covering 1994-2020. Design and setting: Narrative review conducted in a neurosciences and behavior laboratory in Brazil. Methods: The PubMed, Cochrane Clinical Answers and Google Scholar databases were searched. After screening and applying the eligibility criteria, 32 articles were examined. Through this analysis, the following information was extracted: (1) geographical distribution of the study; (2) sample size (according to age and phase of the menstrual cycle); (3) type of measurements according to psychophysical, neuroimaging and neurobehavioral instruments; (4) vision testing model; (5) visual subcategory evaluated; (6) categories of processed visual stimuli; and (7) main findings. Results: The menstrual phases give rise to significant changes in visual functions, including in relation to orientation and spatial attention, visual campimetry and visual sensitivity. These relate specifically to the follicular and luteal phases. Conclusions: These findings theoretically expand the effects of menstrual cycles on visual functions found by Guttridge (1994). Despite some inconsistencies in the studies analyzed, it was found that visual processing during the follicular and luteal phases of the normal menstrual cycle of healthy women can explain physiological, cognitive, behavioral and social modulations.
... One potential explanation for the link between ovulation and attractiveness is the effect of hormonal changes on facial tissues and dimensions. Specifically, the hormone estrogen (as captured by estradiol, the main form of estrogen during reproductive years) increases during ovulation, leading to changes in face shape (Oberzaucher et al., 2012, but see Marchinkowska & Holzleitner, 2020), and alters skin color (although these changes may not be detectable; Burriss et al., 2015). ...
... This corresponds to reports [12,13] that suggest females are more attractive in the fertile phase of their sexual cycle. The enhanced beauty could be as result of changes in facial texture and shape, since study showed that there are changes in facial texture and shape during the various phases of menstrual cycle which could result to shift in the facial beauty to ovulation [17,18]. Hormonal system may have also played a role as reports showed increase in melanin secretion which is a hormone of skin coloration which can in turn induce beautification on the skin [19][20][21]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Facial photogrammetry during various phases of female sexual cycle was used to estimate the facial beauty/attractiveness of females at menstruation and ovulation phase of their monthly cycle; in order to form basis of using facial attractiveness as a biomarker of ovulation. Methods: Thirty-seven (37) volunteered female scholars in an institution in Southern Nigeria were purposively selected to include menstruating females of 17-28 years. Photographs of the selected subjects were taken during menstruation and two weeks after menstruation (ovulation period). Afterwards, photogrammetric analysis was performed; 350 males were asked to pick preferred facialpicture of each of the subjects. Results: There was significant differences between the facial proportions measured during the sexual cycle and ideal facial proportion of the subjects p<0.05. The estimation of attractiveness was higher in ovulation phase as there was positive significant correlation of facial proportions of the subjects during the various phases of their sexual cycle. Conclusion: The findings suggests that facial beauty/attractiveness may be used to estimate the sexual cycle of females as enhanced facial beauty/attractiveness could be a biomarker of ovulation.
... Work also suggests that effects of the color red may be connected to perceptions of a woman's underlying reproductive value [23]. The attractiveness-enhancing effect of the color red may reflect its link to perceived health, vitality, and sexual receptivity such as when women's sexual arousal is accompanied by facial blushing [24][25][26], and when women's facial skin becomes slightly redder during the fertile phase of their menstrual cycle [27,28]. This body of research is consistent with the possibility that the link between red and attraction is rooted in human evolutionary biology. ...
Article
Full-text available
The color red has been implicated in a variety of social processes, including those involving mating. While previous research suggests that women sometimes wear red strategically to increase their attractiveness, the replicability of this literature has been questioned. The current research is a reasonably powered conceptual replication designed to strengthen this literature by testing whether women are more inclined to display the color red 1) during fertile (as compared with less fertile) days of the menstrual cycle, and 2) when expecting to interact with an attractive man (as compared with a less attractive man and with a control condition). Analyses controlled for a number of theoretically relevant covariates (relationship status, age, the current weather). Only the latter hypothesis received mixed support (mainly among women on hormonal birth control), whereas results concerning the former hypothesis did not reach significance. Women (N = 281) displayed more red when expecting to interact with an attractive man; findings did not support the prediction that women would increase their display of red on fertile days of the cycle. Findings thus suggested only mixed replicability for the link between the color red and psychological processes involving romantic attraction. They also illustrate the importance of further investigating the boundary conditions of color effects on everyday social processes.
... Previously assumed fluctuating facial cues of fertility were facial shape and skin color (Bobst & Lobmaier, 2012;Burriss et al., 2015;Oberzaucher et al., 2012). However, changes in facial shape do not seem to replicate (Marcinkowska & Holzleitner, 2020) and changes in facial color are apparently too subtle to be detectable by the human visual system (Burriss et al., 2015). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The Oxford Handbook of Human Mating covers the contributions and up-to-date theories and empirical evidence from scientists regarding human mating strategies. The scientific studies of human mating have only recently risen, revealing fresh discoveries about mate attraction, mate choice, marital satisfaction, and other topics. Darwin’s sexual selection theory primarily guides most of the research in the scientific study of mating strategies. Indeed, research on the complexities of human mate competition and mate choice has centred around Darwin’s classic book. This book discusses theories of human mating; mate selection and mate attraction; mate competition; sexual conflict in mating; human pair bonding; the endocrinology of mating; and mating in the modern world.
Article
Research on attractiveness has identified eight pillars of beauty: symmetry, youthfulness, texture, proportions, hormone markers, averageness, movement and body odor. These properties are construction features associated with several characteristics of biological relevance. Attractive faces are connected to developmental stability, sexual hormones and nutritional factors. Recent research has shown that these properties are also linked to fertility and reproductive success. Thus, beauty only partially lies in the eyes of the beholder and also carries biologically relevant information.
Chapter
Relative to other species, human females invest considerable effort in attracting and retaining mates. Stroll the aisles of any bookstore and you may come across titles such as “Get the guy: Learn secrets of the male mind to find the man you want and the love you deserve” (Hussey, 2014), and “Texts so good he can't ignore: Sassy texting secrets for attracting high-quality men” (Bryans, 2018). A desire to attract and retain mates underlies diverse facets of women’s psychology and behavior, including displaying or enhancing aspects of one’s personality and physical appearance. Not surprisingly, these efforts correspond with men’s mate preferences. Human males are unique in their relative choosiness surrounding their mates, especially within the context of long-term pair-bonding. Look further in that bookstore aisle and you might come across a title such as “The man's handbook for choosing the right woman” (Daniels, 2009). In this chapter, we examine the theoretical rationale underlying female intersexual selection. We begin with a discussion of the theory underlying human mate choice, highlighting why men’s choosiness has been selected for, and why this compels women to exert effort toward attracting men. We then discuss specific characteristics of men’s short-term and long-term mate choice, and the multitude of tactics women utilize to better embody those traits. We describe preliminary evidence surrounding how intersexual selection may have shaped some phenotypic traits in women as costly signals of underlying fertility or immunocompetence. Finally, we discuss both individual and contextual differences among women in their mating effort and provide suggestions for future research directions aimed at further understanding how intersexual selection has shaped women’s mating psychology.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding how human mating psychology is affected by changes in female cyclic fertility is informative for comprehending the evolution of human reproductive behavior. Based on differential selection pressures between the sexes, men are assumed to have evolved adaptations to notice women's within-cycle cues to fertility and show corresponding mate retention tactics to secure access to their female partners when fertile. However, previous studies suffered from methodological shortcomings and yielded inconsistent results. In a large, preregistered online dyadic diary study (384 heterosexual couples), we found no compelling evidence that men notice women's fertility status (as potentially reflected in women's attractiveness, sexual desire, or wish for contact with others) or display mid-cycle increases in mate retention tactics (jealousy, attention, wish for contact or sexual desire towards female partners). These results extend our current understanding of the evolution of women's concealed ovulation and oestrus, and suggest that both might have evolved independently.
Article
Full-text available
Facial cognition serves an important role in human daily interactions. It has been suggested that facial shape can serve as a signal for underlining biological condition, and that it is correlated with, among others, health, fertility, and attractiveness. In this study, 14 women were photographed during three consecutive trimesters of pregnancy, and the levels of their facial sexual dimorphism, asymmetry, and averageness were computed. Facial sexual dimorphism in first trimester was higher than in the second trimester (F(2, 22) = 5.77; p = 0.01; ηp² = 0.34, post-hoc Tukey HSD test p = 0.007). Similar pattern was visible for asymmetry (F(2, 22) = 3.67; p = 0.04; ηp² = 0.25, post-hoc Tukey HSD test p = 0.05). No statistically significant changes in measurement of averageness were observed. Results from Bayesian complementary analyses confirmed the observed effects for sexual dimorphism. The evidence for trimester differences in asymmetry and averageness was inconsequential. Based on the preliminary results of this exploratory study, we suggest that previously found decrease in observed facial attractiveness during pregnancy can be related to the decrease in computed facial femininity (possibly mediated by the changes in facial adiposity).
Article
Full-text available
It is hypothesized that human faces judged to be attractive by people possess two features-averageness and symmetry-that promoted adaptive mate selection in human evolutionary history by way of production of offspring with parasite resistance. Facial composites made by combining individual faces are judged to be attractive, and more attractive than the majority of individual faces. The composites possess both symmetry and averageness of features. Facial averageness may reflect high individual protein heterozygosity and thus an array of proteins to which parasites must adapt. Heterozygosity may be an important defense of long-lived hosts against parasites when it occurs in portions of the genome that do not code for the essential features of complex adaptations. In this case heterozygosity can create a hostile microenvironment for parasites without disrupting adaptation. Facial bilateral symmetry is hypothesized to affect positive beauty judgments because symmetry is a certification of overall phenotypic quality and developmental health, which may be importantly influenced by parasites. Certain secondary sexual traits are influenced by testosterone, a hormone that reduces immunocompetence. Symmetry and size of the secondary sexual traits of the face (e.g., cheek bones) are expected to correlate positively and advertise immunocompetence honestly and therefore to affect positive beauty judgments. Facial attractiveness is predicted to correlate with attractive, nonfacial secondary sexual traits; other predictions from the view that parasite-driven selection led to the evolution of psychological adaptations of human beauty perception are discussed. The view that human physical attractiveness and judgments about human physical attractiveness evolved in the context of parasite-driven selection leads to the hypothesis that both adults and children have a species-typical adaptation to the problem of identifying and favoring healthy individuals and avoiding parasite-susceptible individuals. It is proposed that this adaptation guides human decisions about nepotism and reciprocity in relation to physical attractiveness.
Article
Full-text available
Examined the frequency of extra-pair copulations (EPCs) by 2,708 female humans in relation to the probability of conception on days 6–20 of a 28-day menstrual cycle. Data are interpreted as supporting a sperm competition theory of double-mating behavior. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Everyday experience suggests that physical attractiveness is important in personal—and especially sexual—relationships. This impression is confirmed by a large body of social psychological research.1,2 Cross-cultural surveys and ethnographic accounts show that concern with the attractiveness of potential mates is also common in non-Western societies and in tribal and peasant cultures.3 However, social psychologists and anthropologists have often had a hard time explaining why attractiveness should count for so much, or why some features rather than others should seem particularly attractive. The theoretical difficulties in accounting for physical attraction are brought out in a Brazilian saying, “Beleza nâo pôe na mesa” (“Good looks don't put anything on the table”), which points to the absence of any evident practical advantage to choosing an attractive mate. Faced with these difficulties, a growing number of researchers in biology, psychology, and anthropology have turned to the modern theory of sexual selection, which has been highly successful in explaining nonhuman animals attractions to traits of no direct ecological utility. In this article, I survey recent efforts to apply the theory of sexual selection to human physical attraction.
Book
The handicap principle, first proposed by Zahavi about 17 years ago, has in the past several years become widely accepted as a central unifying theory explaining many previously baffling aspects of animal signalling and communication. It is arguably the most important theoretical advance in animal behaviour in recent years. Basically, the theory states that to be effective, signals must be reliable, and to be reliable, they must be costly to the signaller. This fundamental insight is then developed to explain and illuminate much of animal and human behaviour - why the peacock’s tail is so ornate, and why antelope will spend energy stetting or leaping into the air, when they see a predator, instead of running away, but also how humans test each others’ commitment by imposing burdens during courtship. Signals are paid attention to only if the signal itself imposes a handicap on the signaller that would make cheating impossible or unprofitable, This book explores the very wide-ranging implications of the handicap principle, for predator-prey relations, sexual selection, parent-offspring relations, coalitions and alliances, and the persistence of altruism, in animals and also in human societies and intercellular signalling within multicellular organisms.
Article
The menstrual cycle is presumed to exert a powerful influence on the behavior of women. Dalton (1964) studied the effects of the menstrual cycle on female behavior and reported that 49% of acute medical and surgical hospital admissions occurred, the highest incidence of psychiatric admissions occurred, and that more suicide attempts are made on the four premenstrual and four menstrual days than during comparable periods of time in other parts of the month. He also found that 45% of the female industrial employees who report sick do so on the four premenstrual and four menstrual days. Sixty-two per cent of the crimes of violence committed by female prison inmates were committed in the premenstrual week according to Parker (1960). In a study of psychiatric admission rates, Janowsky (1969) reported findings indicating an aggravation of manic, catatonic, and schizophrenic symptoms during the premenstrual-menstrual phases of mental patients. Many women present variations in mood over the menstrual cycle with the most noticeable effects occurring during the premenstrual period (Parker, 1960). Many women become depressed or irritable. Accompanying physical symptoms are headache, backache, nausea, and edema. In some women these mood and physical changes are so extreme that gynecologists refer to the condition as premenstrual tension or the premenstrual tension syndrome (Parker, 1960). Apparently it is the psychological more often than the physical symptom that is the presenting complaint (Perr, 1958). Most women menstruate (Mazer and Israel, 1951). In view of the prevalence of the condition and the extreme physical and psychological effects reported, it is surprising that relatively few investigations of the phenomenon have been reported. * This report is based on an Honors Thesis submitted by the senior author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Arts degree with Honors at Cornell College. The research was conducted with the supervision of the junior author.
Article
Evolutionary psychologists have widely accepted that there are biological reasons for body shape and size preferences in potential sexual partners. Female physical attractiveness is considered to be largely a reflection of her potential reproductive success. Recent research suggests that male physical attractiveness is also based upon the same criterion. Reproductive success is defined as the optimum (for a given environment) number of children surviving to reach sexual maturity and to become parents themselves. Buss (1989) suggests that there are cultural universals in desired body size and shape for intersexual attraction, and that these derive from the division of labor between males and females during the course of evolution, where males specialized in hunting activities and females in food gathering and child rearing. Natural and sexual selection are believed to have operated in a way that men and women whose bodies were best suited for these tasks were most attractive to potential mates. We find that females consider their physical appearance as “efficient” when they attract mates, males consider their body as efficient when it promises success in male-male competition (Erikson 1968; Lerner et al. 1976).