ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Adult men (n = 582) and women (n = 765) from six nations (Spain, Peru, Venezuela, the United Kingdom, Australia, and the United States) completed two reliable and valid measures of chronic procrastination. Because both arousal and avoidant procrastination types were significantly related across the entire sample (r = .72, p < .001) and within each national sample, regression analyses calculated “pure” arousal and “pure” avoidant procrastinators, controlling for the scale scores of the other scale. Results indicated no significant sex or nationality differences within and between nations on self-reported arousal or avoidant procrastination. Overall, 13.5% and 14.6% of men and women self-identified as either arousal or avoidant procrastinators, respectively. These findings suggest that the tendency toward frequent delays in starting or completing tasks may be prevalent across diverse populations in spite of their distinct cultural values, norms, and practices.
Content may be subject to copyright.
http://jcc.sagepub.com
Psychology
Journal of Cross-Cultural
DOI: 10.1177/0022022107302314
2007; 38; 458 Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
Argumedo
Joseph R. Ferrari, Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales, Jean O'Callaghan, Karem Díaz and Doris
Chronic Procrastination
Frequent Behavioral Delay Tendencies By Adults: International Prevalence Rates of
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/38/4/458
The online version of this article can be found at:
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology
can be found at:Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology Additional services and information for
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/38/4/458#BIBL
SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms):
(this article cites 18 articles hosted on the Citations
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
458
FREQUENT BEHAVIORAL DELAY TENDENCIES BY ADULTS
International Prevalence Rates of Chronic Procrastination
JOSEPH R. FERRARI
DePaul University
JUAN FRANCISCO DÍAZ-MORALES
Complutense University
JEAN O’CALLAGHAN
Roehampton University
KAREM DÍAZ
DORIS ARGUMEDO
Pontifical Catholic University
Adult men (n=582) and women (n=765) from six nations (Spain, Peru, Venezuela, the United Kingdom,
Australia, and the United States) completed two reliable and valid measures of chronic procrastination.
Because both arousal and avoidant procrastination types were significantly related across the entire
sample (r=.72, p<.001) and within each national sample, regression analyses calculated “pure” arousal
and “pure” avoidant procrastinators, controlling for the scale scores of the other scale. Results indicated
no significant sex or nationality differences within and between nations on self-reported arousal or
avoidant procrastination. Overall, 13.5% and 14.6% of men and women self-identified as either arousal
or avoidant procrastinators, respectively. These findings suggest that the tendency toward frequent delays
in starting or completing tasks may be prevalent across diverse populations in spite of their distinct
cultural values, norms, and practices.
Keywords: chronic procrastination; prevalence rates; cultural nationalities
Levine and colleagues (Levine, 2005; Levine & Barlett, 1984; Levine & Norenzayan,
1999) examined the pace of life for adults living in large cities from numerous countries
and found that fast-paced lives were more dominant in Westernized, economically devel-
oped nations and may have serious negative health consequences around meeting time
frames. According to Brislin and Kim (2003), cultural differences in norms and values
exist in the perception of time that may affect a person’s ability to examine the impact of
long-term consequences, avoid risks, and live in the here and now, focusing on short-term
perspectives. Two components of their model pertinent to the present study are how punc-
tual a person may be and how efficiently that person accomplishes several tasks within a
limited time frame. The present study focuses on a behavioral index of these components
through a person’s tendency to procrastinate—a purposive delay in failing to start or meet
deadlines (Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995).
AUTHORS’NOTE: The authors express gratitude to Clarry Lay and Henri Schouwenburg for comments on early portions of this
article and to Anthony Owens for assistance in data collection. Direct correspondence to the first author at DePaul University,
Department of Psychology, 2219 North Kenmore Ave., Chicago, IL 60614; e-mail: jferrari@depaul.edu.
JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 38 No. 4, July 2007 458-464
DOI: 10.1177/0022022107302314
© 2007 Sage Publications
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Ferrari et al. / PREVALENCE OF PROCRASTINATION 459
At present, two reliable and valid measures of chronic procrastination have been iden-
tified with men and women in the United States (Harriott & Ferrari, 1996), with equal
prevalence rates of about 15%. One scale seems to be related to a tendency to delay tasks
as a thrill-seeking experience to ward off boredom and work “best under pressure.” Lay’s
(1986) 5-point, 20-item, unidimensional General Behavioral Procrastination (GP) Scale
(containing statements such as “I always seem to shop for birthday gifts at the last minute,
“I often find myself performing tasks that I intended to do days before,” and “I usually buy
even an essential item at the last minute”; see Ferrari et al., 1995) identified such delays
as being motivated by a need for arousal (Ferrari, 1992, 2000). Because scores on this scale
have been related to external attributes or excuses for delays (Ferrari, 1993) and poor per-
formance when environmental stressors existed that heightened arousal at task deadlines
(Ferrari, 2001), this self-report measure seems to be an appropriate measure to assess
arousal procrastination.
The other scale of frequent procrastination among adults seems to be a reflection of low
self-esteem and self-confidence such that a person delays completing tasks that might reveal
potential poor abilities. McCown and Johnson’s (1989; items found in Ferrari et al., 1995)
Adult Inventory of Procrastination (AIP), a 5-point, 15-item, unidimensional scale (sample
items include “I am not very good at meeting deadlines,” “My friends and family think I
wait until the last minute,” and “Putting things off until the last minute has cost me money
in the past year”) is a global measure of frequent procrastination examining a variety of
tasks to deflect potential disclosure of perceived inabilities and incompetence (Ferrari,
1993) and self-relevant information about one’s skills and competence (Ferrari,1991). This
scale then may be considered a good self-report measure of avoidant procrastination.
To examine the discriminant validity of both procrastination measures within a cross-
cultural context, Ferrari and Díaz-Morales (in press) analyzed the characteristic profile of
arousal and avoidant motives of procrastination related to past, present, and future time
conceptions. Results provided evidence that despite a high relationship in measures, there
are distinct motives for chronic procrastination. Avoidant procrastination was associated
negatively with present–fatalistic time orientation, and arousal procrastination was associ-
ated positively with present–hedonistic and negatively with future time orientation. It
should be noted that both arousal and avoidant procrastinations have been significantly
related (rrange =.65 to .75) when measured in the same study with the same adult
samples. To ascertain prevalence rates of both procrastination types among international
samples of adult men and women, it is necessary to separate the shared covariance between
both types and examine “pure” procrastination types. Pure in the present context refers to
understanding the separate contributions of both procrastination typologies in contributing
to task delays among adults. In personality research, the use of residual scores after con-
ducting regression analyses to examine related but separate variables is an accepted pro-
cedure (see Lutwak, Ferrari, & Cheek, 1998, for an example).
In the present study, we focus on the replication of general psychological measures and
processes related to procrastination across languages and countries among men and women
from six nations. To establish the cross-cultural equivalence of a questionnaire, satisfactory
translation (verified by a back translation) of the questionnaire must be applied (Paunonen &
Ashton, 1998). Fortunately, Díaz-Morales, Díaz, Argumedo, and Ferrari (2006) established
the validity of both procrastination scales used in the present study with varied Spanish-
speaking adult populations. It was expected in the present study that the prevalence of arousal
and avoidant procrastination would be similar for men and women within each nation, con-
sistent with previous research (e.g., Ferrari, O’Callaghan, & Newbegin, 2005). No published
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
460 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY
study compares prevalence rates of arousal and avoidant procrastination (measured by the
GP and AIP measures, respectively) between men and women across nations with differ-
ent languages. The present study, therefore, extends the notions of punctuality and task
completion related to perceptions of time (Brislin & Kim, 2003) within six nations.
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
Participants were women (n=765) and men (n=582) living in metropolitan areas
within one of six countries (Australia, Peru, Spain, the United Kingdom, the United States,
and Venezuela). More specifically, from the United States, there were 207 adults (122
women, 85 men); Australia, 214 adults (124 women, 90 men); United Kingdom, 239
adults (143 women, 96 men); Venezuela, 123 adults (76 women, 47 men); Spain, 276
adults (141 women, 135 men); and Peru, 254 adults (132 women, 122 men). To ensure
more matched samples, we recruited middle-aged adults (Mage =40.7 years old, SD =
12.35, range =30 to 65 years old) within each country. Most participants reported they
were married (80%) with two children (Mnumber of children =1.88, SD =0.34).
Participants also reported they were employed at their present position, on average, 2 to 3
years (M=2.66 years, SD =0.88), and all participants would be labeled as middle-class
employed adults similar to U.S. standards.
PSYCHOMETRIC SCALES
All participants completed Lay’s (1986) 20-item, 5-point (1 =not true of me,5 =very
true of me) GP, which examines behavioral tendencies to delay the start or completion of
everyday tasks for thrill-seeking experiences. Participants also completed the 15-item,
5-point Likert-type scale (1 =strongly disagree,5 =strongly agree) self-report AIP devel-
oped by McCown and Johnson (1989; see Ferrari et al., 1995, for details) to examine
chronic task delays across a variety of situations motivated by avoidance of task unpleas-
antness and personal performance anxieties. Both scales were translated into Spanish by
the second and fourth authors and then back translated from Spanish to English by a bilin-
gual expert who was not a psychologist. An independent translator did the verification of
the translation, as suggested by Paunonen and Ashton (1998; see Ferrari et al., 1995, for
English and Díaz-Morales et al., 2006, for Spanish versions of these scales, including
their psychometric properties). For the present samples, the median coefficient alpha on
the GP scale was 0.83 and on the AIP measure was 0.85, suggesting good internal consis-
tency within each country. Table 1 presents the mean score and coefficient alpha for each
of the six countries.
PROCEDURES
Demographic data were collected by asking each participant to voluntarily complete a
survey of their age, sex, marital status, number of children, and length of time at their cur-
rent employed position as well as the GP and AIP scales in counterbalanced order.
Participants were recruited from middle-class adults living in and around major metropol-
itan areas within each country, with the restriction that gender and age be maintained for
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Ferrari et al. / PREVALENCE OF PROCRASTINATION 461
balanced samples. For U.S., Australian, and Venezuelan adults, the first author asked
groups of adults attending community-based public meetings in and around Chicago,
Melbourne, and Caracas, respectively, to participate. The second author used a snowball
method assisted by undergrads to solicit participation in and around Madrid, Spain. The
third author solicited random samples of U.K. adults living in and around London. The
fourth and fifth authors solicited random samples of adults who shared similar middle-
class demographic profiles living in Lima, Peru.
RESULTS
Table 1 presents the mean avoidant and arousal procrastination score for each of the six
international countries. A 2 (sex) ×6 (nation) MANOVA was performed on the GP and
AIP scores to assess whether there were significant differences in chronic procrastination
between men and women from each of the six countries. Consistent with other research
(see Ferrari et al., 1995; Ferrari & Pychyl, 2000), there were no significant sex main or
interaction effects. However, there was a significant main effect for nation, multivariate
F(10, 2646) =20.44, p<.001, Wilks’s Lambda =.862. Subsequent one-way ANOVA
between the six nations indicated a significant difference between nations on the GP scale,
F(5, 1430) =27.01, p<.001, and the AIP measure, F(5, 1438) =19.13, p<.001. Post
hoc analyses (Newman-Keuls, p<.05) indicated that adults living in the United Kingdom
reported significantly higher chronic arousal procrastination compared to adults from
Peru, the United States, and Spain, with adults from Venezuela and Australia claiming the
lowest prevalence. In addition, adults living in the United Kingdom reported significantly
higher chronic avoidant procrastination compared to adults from Peru, the United States,
and Australia, with the lowest reported tendencies among adults from Spain and Venezuela
(see Table 1).
We examined the magnitude of the differences between mean procrastination scores for
these six nations (Cohen, 1988). For arousal procrastination, the mean difference between
the United Kingdom versus Peru, Spain, plus the United States had a medium effect size,
TABLE 1
Mean Score, Coefficient Alpha, and Zero-Order Correlates for Arousal (Ars.)
and Avoidant (Avd.) Procrastination Measures for Each Nation
Within
Ars. Procrastination Avd. Procrastination Country
Ars.
×
Avd.
Country MSD
α
MSD
α
Correlate
Spain (141 women, 135 men) 46.51 10.16 .86 32.38 8.52 .82 .717**
Peru (132 women, 122 men) 50.01 10.02 .79 35.98 8.88 .79 .664**
Venezuela (76 women, 47 men) 44.33 10.25 .78 31.29 10.50 .83 .754**
Australia (124 women, 91 men) 44.72 11.65 .89 33.97 8.22 .86 .724**
United Kingdom (196 women, 52.46 12.65 .79 39.01 8.61 .86 .700**
115 men)
United States (103 women, 65 men) 49.22 11.30 .87 35.64 9.64 .85 .747**
**p<.001.
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
462 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY
d=.339; between Peru, Spain, plus the United States versus Venezuela plus Australia,
there was a medium effect size, d=.373; and between the United Kingdom versus
Venezuela plus Australia, there was a large effect size, d=.671. For avoidant procrastina-
tion, the mean difference between the United Kingdom versus Peru, the United States, plus
Australia had a medium effect size, d=.435; between Peru, the United States, plus
Australia versus Spain plus Venezuela, also a medium effect size, d=.364; and between
the United Kingdom versus Spain plus Venezuela, a large effect size, d=.811.
Nevertheless, these medium and large effect sizes still accounted for less than 50% of
the distribution of nonoverlapping scores on GP and AIP measures. Also, both sets of
chronic procrastination scores within each country were significantly interrelated. The
overall mean and median coefficient was 0.72 (p<.001) collapsing across all six nations.
This fact indicates that for the present adult samples, chronic procrastination behavior pat-
terns were not mutually exclusive. Consequently, to estimate the prevalence rates of pure
procrastination types among adults of each country (to compare independently GP and AIP
scores), we regressed GP scores on AIP scores, and then vice versa, to obtain standardized
zresidual scores for the sole variance of the specific procrastination types. We then calcu-
lated the percentage of people that obtained a Zresidual score >1.00 for both procrasti-
nation types. Setting our criteria to scores >1.00 permitted us to select what we labeled as
pure arousal and pure avoidant procrastinators independent of crossover effects between
typologies. We applied this method to compute residual scores separately for each country
using its own population, to correct for cultural sensitivity/interpretation of behavioral
delays. In other words, the use of within-culture zscores allowed us to identify pure
interindividual variances within cultures while correcting for between-culture variance.
The analysis of between-culture variance, although significant and large, was not the focus
of the present study. Instead, our aim was to explore cross-cultural replication of possible
common behavioral tendencies. The percentages of pure types of procrastination estimated
by this procedure are presented in Table 2.
A 2 (sex) ×6 (nation) MANOVA then was performed on the percentage of pure arousal
and avoidant procrastination types to assess whether there were significant differences in
procrastination among adults from each of the six countries. There was no significant sex
main or interaction effect. Using the residual scores that obtained orthogonal procrastination
types, there was no significant main or interaction effect by nation. As noted by Table 2, the
overall percentage of adults who reported behavioral procrastination tendencies associated
with an arousal motive was 13.5%, and with avoidant tendencies, 14.6%.
TABLE 2
Percentage of Adults With Pure Arousal or Avoidant Procrastination Tendencies
Procrastination Type
Country Arousal Avoidant
Spain 13.1 14.6
Peru 12.4 14.9
Venezuela 15.6 15.2
Australia 14.4 15.3
United Kingdom 10.9 13.8
United States 16.1 15.5
Total 13.5 14.6
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Ferrari et al. / PREVALENCE OF PROCRASTINATION 463
DISCUSSION
The tendency toward delaying the start or completion of tasks, known as procrastina-
tion, is common among the adults studied in six countries, consistent with previous litera-
ture reviews (van Eerde, 2003). Large and significant country effects emerged in the
present study when we examined raw procrastination scores. But the focus of our study
was within-group rather than between-group variance, and further analyses on prevalence
rates used standardized residuals. We used regression analyses to compute standardized
residual scores to calculate pure procrastination types to statistically control for the shared
variance found between procrastination types within and between each of the nations.
Results found men and women self-reported rates of 13.5% for arousal procrastination and
14.6% for avoidant procrastination, despite using different data collection methods and
different item translations. Our results found the rates of arousal and avoidant procrastina-
tion similar across nations, when controlled statistically, showing cross-cultural common-
ality rather than cross-cultural differences.
Although previous research indicated that both self-report scales are reliable and valid
measures (Ferrari & Pychyl, 2000), more research is needed that includes direct behavioral
indices with adults in different nations. The present study was the first systematic investi-
gation of chronic procrastination with adults from six different nations. Men and women
in varied nations are prone to a “mañana” lifestyle, but the causes and consequences within
each culture need further investigation to ascertain an indigenous psychology (Adair &
Diaz-Loving, 1999; Kim, Park, & Park, 2000). The present study provides an initial mea-
surement of how people from varied nations behave on the basis of their conception of
time lines and their relatively busy lifestyles (Brislin & Kim, 2003).
REFERENCES
Adair, J. G., & Diaz-Loving, R. (1999). Indigenous psychologies: The meaning of the concept and its assessment.
Applied Psychology,48, 397-402.
Brislin, R. W., & Kim, E. S. (2003). Cultural diversity in people’s understanding and uses of time. Applied
Psychology,52, 363-382.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Díaz-Morales, J. F., Díaz, K., Argumedo, D., & Ferrari, J. R. (2006). Factorial structure of three procrastination
scales with Spanish adult population. European Journal of Personality Assessment,22, 132-137.
Ferrari, J. R. (1991). A preference for a favorable public impression by procrastination: Selecting among cogni-
tive and social tasks. Personality and Individual Differences,12, 1233-1237.
Ferrari, J. R. (1992). Psychometric validation of two procrastination inventories for adults: Arousal and avoid-
ance measures. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,14, 97-110.
Ferrari, J. R. (1993). Christmas and procrastination: Explaining lack of diligence at a “real-world” task deadline.
Personality and Individual Differences,14, 25-33.
Ferrari, J. R. (2000). Procrastination and attention: Factor analysis of attention deficit, boredomness, intelligence,
self-esteem and task delay frequencies. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality,15, 185-196.
Ferrari, J. R. (2001). Procrastination as self-regulation failure of performance: Effects of cognitive load, self-
awareness, and time limits on “working best under pressure.European Journal of Personality,15, 391-406.
Ferrari, J. R. & Díaz-Morales J. F. (in press). Procrastination: Different time perspectives predict different
motives. Journal of Research in Personality.
Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J., & McCown, W. (1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and
treatment. New York: Plenum.
Ferrari, J. R., O’Callahan, J., & Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in the United States,
United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and avoidance delays among adults. North American Journal of
Psychology,7, 1-6.
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
464 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY
Ferrari, J. R., & Pychyl, T. A. (2000). Procrastination: Current issues and new directions. Corte Madre, CA:
Select.
Harriott, J., & Ferrari, J. R. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of adults. Psychological
Reports,78, 611-616.
Kim, U., Park, Y. S., & Park, D. (2000). The challenges of cross-cultural psychology: The role of the indigenous
psychologies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,31, 63-75.
Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research on procrastination. Journal of Research in Personality,20, 479-495.
Levine, R. (2005). A geography of busyness. Social Research,72, 355-370.
Levine, R., & Bartlett, K. (1984). Pace of life, punctuality, and coronary heart disease in six countries. Journal
of Cross-Cultural Psychology,15, 233-255.
Levine, R., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,30,
178-205.
Lutwak, N., Ferrari, J. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1998). Shame, guilt, and identity in men and women: The role of iden-
tity orientation and processing style in moral affects. Personality and Individual Differences,25, 1027-1036.
McCown, W., & Johnson, J. (1989). Validation of an adult inventory of procrastination. Unpublished scale. Available
in Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown (1995).
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (1998). The structured assessment of personality across cultures. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology,29, 150-170.
van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of procrastination. Personality and
Individual Differences,35, 1401-1418.
Joseph R. Ferrari is a professor of psychology and Vincent de Paul Distinguished Professor at DePaul
University, Chicago. He is the editor of the Journal of Prevention and Intervention in the Community
(Haworth Press). His research interests include experimental social–personality psychology (procrastina-
tion, shame/guilt, impostor phenomena, attitude change and persuasion, attribution processes) and applied
social–community psychology (caregiver stress/satisfaction, community volunteerism, addiction recovery,
sense of community, and higher education mission statements). In procrastination research, he is author of
three scholarly volumes, more than 60 peer-reviewed publications, and more than 120 conference presenta-
tions. He also is an invited motivational speaker on procrastination and life change issues.
Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales is a professor of psychology at Complutense University of Madrid. His gen-
eral research interests concern individual differences, including personality styles, chronopsychology,
time psychology, and procrastination. He was a lecturer at the National Program for Researcher
Formation from 1998 to 2001. He is a coauthor of the Spanish adaptation of the Millon Index of
Personality Styles and has conducted research in France and Peru. He is a member of the official research
group Personality Styles, Gender, and Health at Complutense University of Madrid.
Jean O’Callaghan is a principal lecturer in psychology and counseling at Roehampton University,
London, and an integrative psychotherapist. She has research interests in self-regulation, procrastination,
and disability in higher education.
Karem Díaz is a professor of psychology at Pontifical Catholic University in Lima, Peru, and a rational
emotive behavioral therapist trained at the Albert Ellis Institute in New York. Her research interests lie in
the areas of cognitive and personality psychology, procrastination, and motivation.
Doris Argumedo is a professor in the Department of Psychology at the Pontifical Catholic University of
Peru in the areas of psychometric methodology and psychopathology. Her research interests concern psy-
chological measurement and cross-cultural testing and assessment.
© 2007 SAGE Publications. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.
at Universiteit van Tilburg on June 19, 2007 http://jcc.sagepub.comDownloaded from
... Academic procrastination is often associated with online learning due to the nature of the content delivery mode and the absence of in-person meeting times with multiple verbal and written reminders of assignment due dates. Procrastination has been studied extensively in the academic context (for a review, see Steel, 2007), and it is prevalent across numerous contexts, nationalities, and segments of the population, irrespective of cultural norms and values (Ferrari et al., 2007;Steel, 2007). Approximately 20% of all adults experience chronic forms of procrastination (Harriott & Ferrari, 1996), including professional workers (Hammer & Ferrari, 2002), but procrastination seems to be most prevalent in undergraduate university students. ...
Article
Full-text available
The rise of online and hybrid learning in universities, especially due to the pandemic, has not only revolutionized how students learn, but also blurred the boundaries between work, school, and leisure activities because of the pervasive use of technology surrounding students in their daily lives. The purpose of this study is to investigate, using coping theory, the relationships among academic self-efficacy, academic procrastination, and techno-overload, three constructs especially crucial in the current education climate. Using a sample of 179 undergraduate students, we found that techno-overload partially mediates the relationship between academic self-efficacy and academic procrastination. This finding demonstrates that techno-overload helps further explain how academic self-efficacy is negatively associated with procrastination. Specifically, academic self-efficacy is negatively related to techno-overload which is then positively linked to procrastination. Consistent with coping theory, our findings highlight the importance of mitigating technology-related stress in order to reduce academic procrastination. Implications and future research directions are discussed.
... Thus, it is assumed that procrastinators chronically delay tasks that they experience as unpleasant or difficult [9]. Approximately 20 to 30% of adults have been found to procrastinate chronically [10][11][12]. Prevalence estimates for students are similar [13]. It is believed that students do not procrastinate more often than other groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background It is generally assumed that procrastination leads to negative consequences. However, evidence for negative consequences of procrastination is still limited and it is also unclear by which mechanisms they are mediated. Therefore, the aim of our study was to examine the harmful consequences of procrastination on students’ stress and mental health. We selected the procrastination-health model as our theoretical foundation and tried to evaluate the model’s assumption that trait procrastination leads to (chronic) disease via (chronic) stress in a temporal perspective. We chose depression and anxiety symptoms as indicators for (chronic) disease and hypothesized that procrastination leads to perceived stress over time, that perceived stress leads to depression and anxiety symptoms over time, and that procrastination leads to depression and anxiety symptoms over time, mediated by perceived stress. Methods To examine these relationships properly, we collected longitudinal data from 392 university students at three occasions over a one-year period and analyzed the data using autoregressive time-lagged panel models. Results Procrastination did lead to depression and anxiety symptoms over time. However, perceived stress was not a mediator of this effect. Procrastination did not lead to perceived stress over time, nor did perceived stress lead to depression and anxiety symptoms over time. Conclusions We could not confirm that trait procrastination leads to (chronic) disease via (chronic) stress, as assumed in the procrastination-health model. Nonetheless, our study demonstrated that procrastination can have a detrimental effect on mental health. Further health outcomes and possible mediators should be explored in future studies.
... While procrastinating, the anticipated task is typically perceived as unpleasant or stressful and is deliberately postponed in order to engage in more pleasant activities, such as watching television , sleeping (Klingsieck, 2013) or other leisure activities (Burka & Yuen, 1983). Prevalence rates of procrastination behavior are as high as 20-25% in the general population (Ferrari et al., 2007) and are estimated to range from 70 to 95% in college students (Steel, 2007). Most studies have investigated procrastination in young adults in the academic context (e.g., putting off performance-related tasks such as studying for exams at university; Grunschel et al., 2018;Katz et al., 2014;Klassen et al., 2008;Watson, 2001). ...
Article
Full-text available
Procrastination as an intentional delay of personal tasks in spite of anticipating negative consequences as a result is a widespread behavior, particularly among young adults. Previous research points to a reduction of procrastinatory tendencies across the adult lifespan; it is unclear to date which mechanisms underlie this decrease of procrastination behavior. Given evidence of fear of failure strongly influencing younger adults to postpone the start or delay of intended actions, as well as a possible decrease of fear of failure with increasing age, this study set out to explore a potential mediating effect of fear of failure on procrastination. A total of 197 participants, aged 18 to 90 years, took part in this study via online questionnaires. Statistical analyses showed that procrastination behavior decreased across the adult lifespan. Higher levels of fear of failure were linked to more procrastination behavior. A mediation analysis indicated that the relationship between age and procrastination was mediated by fear of failure. Results are discussed in terms of theoretical and practical implications.
Article
Although some people procrastinate more than others, most of us occasionally postpone tasks until tomorrow that we could or should have done today. The present study proposes a volitional-task attractiveness framework including both personality and task-related predictors of actual procrastinatory behavior. Data were collected in a three-wave design around the Christmas break. Using a representative sample of the Dutch population, qualitative data were collected on tasks/situations that induce procrastination ( N = 1835) and the validity of the volitional-task attractiveness framework was explored regarding procrastination on a Christmas break task ( n = 712). Most respondents (95.6%) reported to at least occasionally procrastinate (e.g., on housekeeping, social, and administrative tasks; when busy/stressed, tasks conflict, and being tired). While personality (i.e., trait procrastination) positively predicted procrastinatory behavior, task-related factors (i.e., task importance and task motivation) explained 12% unique additional variance. Whereas intrinsic task motivation negatively and amotivation positively predicted procrastinatory behavior, extrinsic task motivation showed a more complex pattern. Lastly, low intrinsic motivation was less harmful for those higher on trait self-control. The findings align with the proposed volitional-task attractiveness framework, suggesting that both personality and task-related factors as well as their interaction explain actual procrastinatory behavior.
Article
Full-text available
The recent affect-regulation (AR) model emphasizes the relevance of context and the diversity of AR strategies, which deepens our understanding of emotional processes. This study, conducted with 228 Chinese undergraduates, aims to support the AR model by investigating the connection between cognitive emotion regulation (CER) and procrastination, as suggested by the temporal mood regulation (TMR) model. The findings discovered a positive correlation between past negative (PN) time perspective and procrastination. Acceptance showed no significant correlation with procrastination, while refocus on planning exhibited a weak negative correlation. Surprisingly, PN acted as a suppressor in the relationship between CER strategies and procrastination. This study highlights the diverse impact of CER strategies on procrastination, providing support for both the AR and TMR models.
Article
Full-text available
Chronic procrastination, which is increasingly prevalent in adult routines, may have implications for mental and physical health. Family dynamics significantly influence procrastination tendencies. This study investigates the influence of family functioning on chronic procrastination, focusing on the mediating role of self-efficacy. We examined the proposed relations using Partial Least Square Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) with Smart PLS on a sample of 596 emerging adults. The results confirm the goodness of fit for the theoretical model. Notably, a positive and significant association exists between family functioning and self-efficacy. The study’s results further strengthen the idea that both self-efficacy and family functioning significantly influence chronic procrastination. Moreover, self-efficacy acts as a mediator in the relations between chronic procrastination and family functioning. Overall, the study concluded that self-efficacy could effectively mediate the association between family functioning and chronic procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
Procrastination is the deliberate, unjustified postponing of an intended course of action despite its costs or unfavorable effects. The present study used a self-report online survey and collected data from a large convenience sample of the general adult population (N = 2,076; females = 55.73%; Mage = 35.1 years [SD ± 12.7]) with diverse demographics. Following the ring-curve distribution, the results indicated a 15.4% prevalence rate of procrastination among the Iranian community, which was significantly higher among women and divorced individuals and lower among nomadic individuals and those with higher academic degrees. A latent profile analysis demonstrated two distinct profiles, one for procrastinators (high scores on chronic procrastination, psychological distress, neuroticism, and extraversion; and low scores on general self-efficacy, self-esteem, satisfaction with life, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) and one for non-procrastinators (demonstrating a reverse pattern compared to procrastinators). Moreover, additional network analysis suggested that the examined networks were invariant across procrastination status and gender. The results indicate that procrastination differs by demographic characteristics and is associated with a unique psychological profile. However, none of the aforementioned key study variables were considered a potential vulnerability for procrastinators due to the finding that all variables were peripheral and none were central in the examined networks. Therefore, relying on the differences in mean scores on psychometric scales does not appear to be an optimal way of determining the most important variables in a therapeutic context when treating procrastination.
Article
Bu çalışmanın amacı; iş özellikleri ve örgüt kültürünün iş erteleme üzerindeki etkisini incelemek, varsa bu etkide dış kontrol odaklılığın düzenleyicilik rolünü belirlemektir. Bir görevin tamamlanmasının pişmanlık, baskı veya stres duyguları hissedinceye kadar geciktirilmesi olarak tanımlanabilen erteleme, günümüzde yaygın bir davranış haline gelmiştir. Örgütsel üretkenlik ve etkinlik üzerindeki potansiyel olumsuz etkilerine rağmen ertelemenin, büyük ölçüde kişisel faktörler açısından incelendiği buna karşın durumsal veya görevle ilgili faktörlerin etkileşimi olarak ortaya çıkma olasılığının dikkate alınmadığı görülmektedir. Bu noktadan hareketle çalışmanın modeli, erteleme davranışını öngörmede durumsal ve kişisel faktörlerin birlikte değerlendirmenin daha açıklayıcı olacağı görüşüyle oluşturulmuştur. Araştırmanın evrenini; Strateji ve Bütçe Başkanlığı kaynaklarına göre Türkiye genelinde 2022 yılı itibariyle görev yapan 5.052.409 adet kamu kurumu çalışanları oluşturmaktadır. Anket uygulaması sonucunda 714 katılımcıdan elde edilen veriler istatistiki olarak incelemeye alınmıştır. Çalışmanın temel hipotezlerini test etmek için kurulan yapısal eşitlik modelleri ise LISREL programı aracılığıyla analiz edilmiştir. Analizler sonucunda katılımcıların iş özellikleri, klan kültürü ve adhokrasi kültürü algılarının iş erteleme davranışı üzerinde negatif ve anlamlı bir etkisi olduğu tespit edilirken, hiyerarşi ve piyasa kültürü algılarının ise iş erteleme davranışı üzerinde pozitif ve anlamlı etkisi olduğu tespit edilmiştir. Ayrıca dış kontrol odaklılığın bu etkilerin tümünde düzenleyicilik rolü bulunduğu tespit edilmiştir.
Article
Günlük hayat içerisinde çoğu kişinin yaşamlarının her döneminde yapmaları gereken işlerini ve sorumluluklarını erteledikleri gözlemlenmektedir. Bireylerin erteleme davranışı göstermelerinin pek çok nedeni bulunmakla birlikte erteleme davranışındaki en temel nedenin, bireyin yeteri kadar zamanını iyi kullanabilme yeteneğine sahip olmadığı görülmektedir. Kişilerin erteleme davranışı arttıkça akademik başarıları düşmekte ve kaygı düzeyleri artmaktadır. Bu çalışmanın amacı, hazırlık sınıfı dâhil üniversite öğrencileri ile lisansüstü öğrenim gören bireylerin karar vermeyi erteleme (KVE) davranışlarının akademik başarı ve sürekli kaygı açısından incelenmesidir. Araştırma sürecinde çalışma grubu; İstanbul, Ankara ve Mersinde bulunan vakıf ve devlet üniversitelerinde öğrenim gören yaşları 18 ile 48 arasında olan 171’i kadın 29’u erkek toplam 200 üniversite öğrencisinden oluşturmaktadır. Araştırma verileri; Mann (1982) önderliğinde şekillendirilen ve Balkıs tarafından Türkçeye uyarlanan “Karar Vermeyi Erteleme Eğilimi Ölçeği”, Öner ile Le Compte tarafından Türkçeye uyarlanan “Sürekli Kaygı Ölçeği” ve demografik bilgi formu aracılığıyla toplanmıştır. Yapılan çalışmanın sonucunda karar vermeyi erteleme davranışı ile sürekli kaygı arasında orta seviyede pozitif, anlamlı bir ilişki bulunmuş olup, akademik başarıları ile anlamlı bir ilişki bulunmadığı görülmüştür. Cinsiyetler arasında ise karar vermeyi erteleme davranışlarının hem akademik olarak sergiledikleri başarı hem de sürekli kaygıları bakımından anlamlı fark olmadığı görülmüştür. Bununla birlikte yaş, sınıf düzeyi ve sosyoekonomik düzey değişkenleri ile KVE davranışları arasında da anlamlı bir ilişkinin varlığı bulunmamaktadır. Genel not ortalaması ve karar vermeyi erteleme davranışı arasında anlamlı korelasyon bulunmuştur. Tukey analiz sonuçlarına göre ise akademik ortalaması 2.01-3.00 ve 3.01-4.00 arasındaki üniversite öğrencilerinin karar vermeyi erteleme davranışı arasında anlamlı korelasyon bulunmuştur.
Article
Full-text available
No systematic study has examined the global prevalence of chronic procrastination-the purposive delay in starting or completing tasks. In the present study, adult samples from the United States (122 women, 85 men), United Kingdom (143 women, 96 men), and Australia (124 women, 90 men) completed reliable and valid self-report measures of arousal procrastination (delays motivated by a "last-minute" thrill experience) and avoidant procrastination (delays related to fears of failure or success). Both men and women from the United Kingdom reported higher rates of arousal and avoidance procrastination compared to adults from the United States and Australia. However, when both procrastination types were separated statistically into "pure types" there were no significant differences across countries: 11.5% of adults self-identified as arousal procrastinators, and 9.9% of adults as avoidant procrastinators. Results indicated that chronic procrastination prevalence is common among westernized, individualistic, English-speaking countries; further epidemiological cross-cultural studies are needed. It has been estimated that procrastination (i.e., frequent delays in starting and/or completing tasks to deadline: Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995) is common by around 70% of college students for academic-specific tasks (Ellis & Knaus, 1977), yet as high as 20% among normal adult men and women for everyday, daily life events such as paying bills and planning for personal health issues (Harriott & Ferrari, 1996). While it seems that procrastination rates decrease with age, Ferrari et al. (1995) proposed that these rates reflect different forms of procrastination, with the former an exanqjle of situational-specific task delays and the latter indicative of chronic, dispositional delay behavior patterns. That is, college students may engage in delay of studying but
Article
Full-text available
Article
Full-text available
This article outlines the indigenous psychologies approach within the context of three research traditions in cross-cultural psychology: the universalist, contextualist, and integrationist approaches. The first section outlines the limitations of the universalist and contextualist approaches. The second section overviews the two integrationist approaches: the derived etics approach and the indigenous psychologies approach. In comparison to the former approach, the indigenous psychologies approach advocates a bottom-up, model-building paradigm that examines generative capabilities of human beings. It investigates human actions that occur in meaningful context. In this transactional approach, human consciousness, intentions, and goals are central aspects of the research design.
Article
Full-text available
The present study explored the factorial structure of three well-known procrastination measures: the General Procrastination Scale (GP; Lay, 1986), the Adult Inventory of Procrastination (AIP; McCown & Johnson, 1989), and the Decisional Procrastination Scale (DP; Mann, 1982) with 502 Spanish adults between 25 and 67 years old. The principal component analysis indicated a one-factor structure for GP and DP, and two factor components for AIP. The internal consistency of the three scales was satisfactory. Results suggested that the Spanish versions of the three procrastination scales were effective and reliable. Finally, a principal component analysis with the three scales indicated a four-factor model of procrastination, namely: dilatory behaviors, indecision, lack of punctuality, and lack of planning.
Article
The purpose of this article is to review several popular, structured, personality questionnaires in terms of their applicability in cross-cultural assessment contexts. Prior to our review, we describe the types of psychometric data that can be used to support claims of a measure's cross-cultural applicability. More important, we list several factors, not all of which have to do with the measure itself, that can undermine such cross-cultural evidence. We then review relevant cross-cultural data on the California Psychological Inventory, the Comrey Personality Scales, the 16 Personality Factors Questionnaire, the Pavlovian Temperament Survey, the Personality Research Form, and the Nonverbal Personality Questionnaire. We show that those inventories have each demonstrated mostly replicable factor structures across cultures. In contrast, relatively little data are available regarding the cross-cultural generality of their criterion validities.
Article
Cross-cultural differences in attitudes and behaviors concerning time, punctuality, and pace of life, and the relationship of these measures to coronary heart disease, were investigated in a series of studies in Japan, Taiwan, Indonesia, Italy, England, and the United States. Study 1 found that public clocks were most accurate in the most economically developed countries (Japan and the United States) and least accurate in the least developed country (Indonesia). Study 2 measured average walking speed during main business hours. Walking speed was fastest in Japan and slowest in Indonesia. Also, subjects in smaller cities tended to walk slower than those in larger cities. Study 3 measured average time taken to complete a standardized postal request. Work speed was again fastest in Japan. Italy was slowest. There were high intercorrelations within cities for the above three measures. Next, the findings from the first three studies were related to incidence of coronary heart disease. Despite the lack of available mortality statistics for Taiwan and Indonesia, some interesting trends questioning the cross-cultural generality of the relationship between Type A behaviors and coronary heart disease emerged. Finally, in a questionnaire study conducted mostly in the United States, the relationship between scores on a measure of Type A behavior characteristics and subjects' reported frequency of lateness, feelings after being late, perceptions of what constitutes late and early, attributions for lateness, and ratings of the importance of punctuality in a business person and in a friend were investigated.
Article
The personality disorder of dispositional procrastination was significantly related to a concern for appropriate self-presentations. Procrastinators (n = 39) and nonprocrastinators (n = 36) rated a social ability task more attractive, personally relevant and likely to be performed well than a cognitive ability task. Procrastinators and nonprocrastinators stated they would prefer to complete the social task, yet in a public performance feedback condition procrastinators only chose to complete the cognitive task. Results were interpreted in terms of self-presentation strategies by dispositional procrastinators, and draws attention to a pathological personality that has largely been ignored by research psychologists.
Article
200 women and 106 men (mean age 19.6 years) completed measures of shame, guilt, identity-orientation, and identity-processing styles. Women reported greater shame and guilt than men. Zero-order and partial correlates indicated that for both women and men shame was related positively to a social identity (one's public image as presented through roles and relationships) and a diffuse processing style (both self-relevant information and self-exploration about one's identity is avoided), while guilt was related to personal identity (conceptualizing oneself as unique) and an information-oriented style (self-exploration of personal issues occurs). Integration of identity orientation and cognitive processing styles in relation to shame and guilt was discussed. (For updates about the Aspects of Identity Questionnaire see: http://academics.wellesley.edu/Psychology/Cheek/identity.html ).