Content uploaded by Andrea Rodenbeck
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Andrea Rodenbeck on Jan 31, 2014
Content may be subject to copyright.
Available via license: CC BY 3.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14, 2573-2589; doi:10.3390/ijms14022573
International Journal of
Molecular Sciences
ISSN 1422-0067
www.mdpi.com/journal/ijms
Article
Out of the Lab and into the Bathroom: Evening Short-Term
Exposure to Conventional Light Suppresses Melatonin and
Increases Alertness Perception
Amely Wahnschaffe 1,*, Sven Haedel 1, Andrea Rodenbeck 1, Claudia Stoll 1, Horst Rudolph 2,
Ruslan Kozakov 3, Heinz Schoepp 3 and Dieter Kunz 1,4
1 Institute of Physiology, Charité–Universitätsmedizin Berlin (CBF), 10115 Berlin, Germany;
E-Mails: sven@haedel.de (S.H.); andrea.rodenbeck@charite.de (A.R.);
claudia.stoll@charite.de (C.S.); dieter.kunz@charite.de (D.K.)
2 Trilux GmbH & Co.KG, 59759 Arnsberg, Germany; E-Mail: hrudolph@trilux.de
3 Leibniz Institute for Plasma Science and Technology (INP), 17489 Greifswald, Germany;
E-Mails: kozakov@inp-greifswald.de (R.K.); Schoepp@inp-greifswald.de (H.S.)
4 German Heart Institute, 13353 Berlin, Germany
* Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: amely.wahnschaffe@charite.de;
Tel.: +49-30-2311-2901; Fax: +49-30-2311-2903.
Received: 23 November 2012; in revised form: 23 December 2012 / Accepted: 16 January 2013 /
Published: 28 January 2013
Abstract: Life in 24-h society relies on the use of artificial light at night that might disrupt
synchronization of the endogenous circadian timing system to the solar day. This could
have a negative impact on sleep–wake patterns and psychiatric symptoms. The aim of the
study was to investigate the influence of evening light emitted by domestic and work place
lamps in a naturalistic setting on melatonin levels and alertness in humans. Healthy
subjects (6 male, 3 female, 22–33 years) were exposed to constant dim light (<10 lx) for
six evenings from 7:00 p.m. to midnight. On evenings 2 through 6, 1 h before habitual
bedtime, they were also exposed to light emitted by 5 different conventional lamps for
30 min. Exposure to yellow light did not alter the increase of melatonin in saliva compared
to dim light baseline during (38 ± 27 pg/mL vs. 39 ± 23 pg/mL) and after light exposure
(39 ± 22 pg/mL vs. 44 ± 26 pg/mL). In contrast, lighting conditions including blue
components reduced melatonin increase significantly both during (office daylight white:
25 ± 16 pg/mL, bathroom daylight white: 24 ± 10 pg/mL, Planon warm white:
26 ± 14 pg/mL, hall daylight white: 22 ± 14 pg/mL) and after light exposure (office
daylight white: 25 ± 15 pg/mL, bathroom daylight white: 23 ± 9 pg/mL, Planon warm
OPEN ACCESS
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2574
white: 24 ± 13 pg/mL, hall daylight white: 22 ± 26 pg/mL). Subjective alertness was
significantly increased after exposure to three of the lighting conditions which included
blue spectral components in their spectra. Evening exposure to conventional lamps in an
everyday setting influences melatonin excretion and alertness perception within 30 min.
Keywords: melatonin; circadian rhythm; light; sleep disturbances; alertness
1. Introduction
Both nonorganic disturbances of the sleep–wake schedule (F51.2) and nonorganic insomnia (F51.0)
are discrete psychiatric diagnoses according to ICD-10 chapter V, as well as common symptoms of
various psychiatric disorders. Whereas the high incidence of the key feature—nonrestorative sleep—is
a widely accepted fact [1], it was not until recently that the enormous socioeconomic costs related to
sleep disorders were reported [2]. Recently approved melatonergic agents have proven efficient in
patients suffering from major depression, as well as nonorganic insomnia, and thereby introduced the
potential role of melatonin for psychiatric disorders into scientific discourse [3]. The hormone
melatonin is central for the linkage between environmental and internal rhythms, because its secretion
pattern depends on environmental light conditions, and an increase of its serum levels signals
“night-mode” to numerous body functions. The question arises whether light exposure at the wrong
time—e.g., artificial evening light—via suppression of melatonin may contribute to sleep-related
psychiatric disturbances by suppressing melatonin secretion.
The earth’s rotation causes the most reliably recurrent event in nature: the daily light–dark cycle.
The evolutionary result is a network of internal clocks governed by a master clock located in the
hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), which drives the predictable part of daily physiological
variations in a precise manner [4]. This circadian timing system (CTS) has been shown to be involved
in the daily variation of almost every physiological and psychological system evaluated thus far
(e.g., hormones, neurotransmitters, receptor densities and affinities, gene expression, mood, motor
activity, pharmacokinetics, pharmacodynamics, responses to pharmacological treatment) [5–10].
Maintaining synchronized circadian rhythms is important to health and well-being. A growing body
of evidence suggests that desynchronization of circadian rhythms may play a role in various tumoral
diseases, diabetes, obesity, depression, and Alzheimer’s disease [11–14]. Shift workers, who serve as a
model for internal desynchronization, are subject to increased morbidity and mortality from a number
of diseases, including cardiovascular disorders and cancer [15–17].
The daily light–dark cycle synchronizes the CTS with the natural 24-h day [18]. In 2000,
melanopsin was identified in a subgroup of retinal ganglion cells, which were originally only known to
integrate the information from rods and cones and transform it into neuronal activation [19]. This
allowed for a non-image-forming system to be characterized in accretive detail: light and darkness are
predominantly perceived by specialized intrinsic photosensitive retinal ganglion cells expressing
melanopsin. On the one hand, they react directly to light input; on the other hand, they gather light
information from rods and cones. This integrated measurement provides the internal clock with
information on the time and length of day [20]. The blue portion of the visible spectrum appears to
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2575
play the dominant role in the intrinsic photosensitivity of these cells, as well as for the integrated
non-image-forming response to light stimuli [21]. Two groups independently reported an action
spectrum for light-induced melatonin suppression by monochromatic light at night [22,23].
Additionally, monochromatic blue light has been shown to induce pupillary constriction, increase heart
rate, influence thermoregulation, enhance alertness, change the frequency of the electroencephalogram
and influence sleep architecture [24–26]. In contrast to green light, blue light was even shown to
increase human PER2 gene expression after 2 h of evening light exposure [27]. Enezi et al. recently
suggested a “melanopic” spectral efficiency function as an alternative to lux and photon density for
describing the impact of polychromatic light of different spectral distributions on melanopsin
expression and the proximate pupillomotor and circadian responses [28]. This melanopic function was
successfully applied to rod- and coneless mice, but not yet to wild-type mice or even humans. Effects
of long-term polychromatic light on human melatonin levels have already been shown: a study using
artificial room light demonstrated that light exposure <200 lux for 8 h until bedtime delayed
the melatonin onset without shifting melatonin offset [29]. Shanti et al. [30] reported a
wavelength-dependent influence of 4 h of evening light exposure on melatonin suppression, subjective
sleepiness, and sleep onset latency. However, data on short-term evening polychromatic light exposure
are still missing.
Many practical clinical trials have shown the effects of light as a therapeutic agent on depression,
age-related sleep problems and agitation in dementia (see, for example, [31–34]). Thus, evening light
exposure was successfully applied to increase alertness in older adults with evening sleepiness [35].
Nevertheless, most of the studies in the context of light influences on circadian rhythms were
performed using long-term, high intensity bright white, polychromatic or blue light in artificial settings
in a way that seldom occurs even in our world of artificial lighting [22,23,36]. Even when the design
for these studies was modeled on average exposition under natural conditions for the periods they
examined [30] they did not consider the impact of very short periods of light exposure on circadian
parameters. Yet the evening setting can include relevant changes in lighting conditions such as a
transition from a brightly lit gym to a moderately lit home, or from a dimly-lit reading corner or
television light in the living room to the neon-lit bathroom, where you get ready for bed. Their findings
are therefore difficult to generalize to such conditions. Other studies addressed the therapeutic
potential of strengthening circadian rhythms with light. The question could be raised if simple behavior
recommendations regarding “light hygiene” could already work as a therapeutic agent on circadian
rhythm associated disturbances.
The hypotheses of the present study are that (1) light emitted by conventional home and work place
lamps in a naturalistic evening setting can suppress melatonin excretion in healthy human subjects
after very short periods of time; and (2) the strength of this influence is wavelength and
intensity-dependent and can be minimized by blue depletion and intensity reduction. In a second step,
we conduct an exploratory investigation of different lamps on subjective calmness, contentedness
and alertness.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2576
2. Results and Discussion
2.1. Raw Data and Descriptive Statistics
Graphical analysis of melatonin raw data in most of the nine single subjects and in group means
shows differences between baseline (dim light) and yellow bathroom lamp, on the one hand, and
between baseline and the four lamps with blue components of the visible spectrum, on the other hand
(e.g., Figure 1, subject 13).
Figure 1. Raw data of melatonin levels in saliva in subject number 13 (subjects in the
present study were numbered 7–15 following a pilot study with subjects 1–6). Lines
represent the different lighting conditions.
Mean group changes of melatonin levels relative to “lights on” are given in Figure 2. While
melatonin concentrations continuously rise during the time of light exposure under baseline and yellow
bathroom lamp conditions, the increase of melatonin concentrations stops or is even reversed during
exposure to any of the four lamps, including blue portions, starting already as early as 20 min after
“lights on”. After termination of light exposure, melatonin concentrations start rising again, but not to
the level reached within 30 min after baseline or yellow bathroom lamp concentrations, which are
within 30 min after lights exposure. Nearly the same changes between lighting regimes are reached by
using the mean between the melatonin concentrations 10 min before “lights on” and “lights on” as zero
point (see means and standard deviations in Table 1). This applies both to the analysis of all subjects
and to the analysis including the six subjects with complete data for all lighting conditions. The mean
difference of melatonin concentration between 10 min before “lights on” and “lights on” was
0.68 ± 0.47 pg/mL (range: −0.12 to 1.34 pg/mL per subject).
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2577
Table 1. Group means and standard deviations of saliva melatonin concentrations. Data are given relative to the zero point representing the
mean of melatonin concentrations in saliva 10 min before and at “lights on”. The first part of the table gives values for the entire sample, and
the second part only for the six subjects with complete data sets.
Pre −60 Pre −30 Mean −10 to 0 min before lights on 10 20 30 Post +10 Post +30
Part 1
Baseline (n = 9) (−4.21) ± 1.89 (−2.26) ± 1.67 0 0.94 ± 2.47 1.08 ± 2.20 1.525 ± 1.69 3.48 ± 2.34 3.41 ± 1.78
bathroom yellow (n = 9) (−4.40) ± 2.68 (−2.37) ± 2.04 0 0.93 ± 1.56 1.95 ± 0.64 2.08 ± 1.15 2.075 ± 2.96 4.085 ± 4.26
office daylight white (n = 8) (−3.33) ± 2.23 (−1.29) ± 1.88 0 1.97 ± 2.90 0.56 ± 1.55 0.56 ± 2.48 0.94 ± 1.57 1.54 ± 2.23
bathroom daylight white (n = 8) (−4.05) ± 1.99 (−2.03) ± 1.44 0 0.97 ± 1.15 (−0.12) ± 1.00 (−0.13) ± 1.72 0.62 ± 2.96 2.03 ± 4.28
Planon warm white (n = 9) (−3.68) ± 2.94 (−1.77) ± 1.98 0 0.41 ± 2.12 (−0.43) ± 1.72 (−0.91) ± 1.64 (−0.83) ± 2.07 0.09 ± 2.83
hall daylight white (n = 8) (−2.66) ± 1.12 (−1.15) ± 0.76 0 (−0.30) ± 1.30 (−1.475) ± 1.42 (−2.50) ± 2.11 (−3.23) ± 4.675 (−0.98) ± 6.09
Part 2
n = 6
Baseline (−3.40) ± 1.54 (−1.88) ± 1.06 0 1.58 ± 1.87 1.42 ± 1.44 1.66 ± 1.92 3.84 ± 2.48 2.67 ± 1.58
bathroom yellow (−5.18) ± 2.91 (−2.76) ± 2.33 0 0.55 ± 1.80 2.02 ± 0.74 2.29 ± 1.39 1.18 ± 3.30 2.38 ± 3.72
office daylight white (−2.96) ± 2.39 (−0.97) ± 2.11 0 2.45 ± 3.24 0.37 ± 1.78 0.61 ± 2.94 0.885 ± 1.85 1.24 ± 2.14
bathroom daylight white (−4.295) ± 2.07 (−2.16) ± 1.65 0 0.58 ± 0.85 (−0.11) ± 1.01 (−0.505) ± 1.86 (−0.535) ± 1.79 0.22 ± 2.47
Planon warm white (−2.95) ± 1.43 (−0.97) ± 1.31 0 0.98 ± 0.93 0.02 ± 0.69 (−0.89) ± 1.45 (−0.68) ± 1.17 0.27 ± 1.39
hall daylight white (−2.31) ± 1.06 (−1.02) ± 0.83 0 (−0.26) ± 1.53 (−1.29) ± 1.58 (−2.09) ± 2.09 (−1.69) ± 2.05 1.04 ± 3.60
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2578
Figure 2. Group means of saliva melatonin concentrations; lines represent the different
lighting conditions. Data are given relative to the zero point that represents melatonin
concentrations at “lights on”. Values on X-axis represent mean differences of melatonin
concentrations to “lights on”.
2.2. Data Analysis
2.2.1. Melatonin Levels in Different Lighting Regimes
Friedman ANOVA was only applied to six of the nine subjects, because the melatonin in saliva data
sets of three subjects were incomplete due to an insufficient amount of saliva in the assays. In the six
subjects with complete melatonin data sets, Friedman ANOVA revealed significant group differences
in melatonin levels during (χ
2
= 15.5238, p < 0.009) and after (χ
2
= 18.09524, p < 0.003) light
exposure. In contrast, groups did not differ prior to light exposure with respect to the different lighting
regimes (χ
2
= 9.04719, p < 0.11), indicating comparable baselines each evening (see Figure 3). Thus,
an order effect induced by the experiment can be ruled out. Two out of three Friedman ANOVAs
showed significant differences between lighting conditions, thereby indicating a high overall
significance (p = 0.0072) of the comparisons.
Regarding the time during light exposure, post-hoc comparisons indicate significant decreases of
melatonin levels during lighting with three of the four lamps including blue components (daylight
white hall, daylight white bathroom, and warm white Planon) as compared to baseline (p < 0.03 each).
The decrease during exposure to daylight white office failed to reach significance (p = 0.07), while the
yellow bathroom showed no effect on melatonin levels. Post–hoc analysis of the time after “lights off”
showed significant (p < 0.03 each) lower melatonin levels for daylight white hall, daylight white
bathroom, and warm white Planon as compared to baseline. Furthermore, AUC of melatonin levels
was diminished following daylight white office (p < 0.05), while no changes following yellow
bathroom could be detected (Figure 3).
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2579
Figure 3. Melatonin levels before, during, and after light exposure. Areas under curve
(AUC) of melatonin saliva levels before (AUC pre, 30–0 min before lights on), during
(AUC light, 0–30 min after lights on), and after (AUC post, 0–30 min after lights off) the
light exposure. Data are expressed as means ± standard deviation; * p < 0.05 compared to
baseline, n = 6.
2.2.2. Subjective Alertness, Calmness and Contentedness in Different Lighting Regimes
The exploratory analysis of subjective calmness and contentedness did not yield any significant
results. The results regarding subjective alertness are given in Figure 4. There were no significant
differences between the baseline condition and the yellow bathroom condition, nor between baseline
and daylight white office. In contrast, subjective alertness after light exposure was significantly
increased compared to the baseline condition in three of the four lighting conditions that included a
blue portion of the visible spectrum.
Figure 4. Subjective alertness after light exposure. Subjective alertness was measured by
visual analog scales [37]. Data are expressed as means ± standard deviation; * p < 0.05
compared to baseline (Wilcoxon signed ranks-Test), n = 9.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2580
2.3. Discussion
The results of the present study show that short-term polychromatic light emitted by conventional
lamps in the evening in a naturalistic setting can suppress melatonin secretion and increase subjective
alertness. It is noteworthy, however, that subjects in our study were exposed to only 30 min of light in
addition to dim light from 7:00 p.m. until midnight. On a descriptive level, these differences begin to
appear after only 20 min of light exposure (see Figure 2).
The explanatory power of the presented data is limited by the small sample size, the non-systematic
selection of lamps, and the merely subjective measurement of behavioral variables. To draw
substantiated conclusions on adverse effects of light regarding health conditions, more lamps with a
systematic variation of light intensity and spectral distribution reflecting the whole range of naturalistic
evening light conditions should be tested on a bigger sample and with additional outcome variables
such as validated vigilance tasks, polysomnographic sleep parameters, or cortisol levels. In our present
sample, the influence of a lamp with no blue component and low light intensity (130 lx) on melatonin
concentrations and on subjective alertness was close to zero (see yellow bathroom lamp in Table 2 and
Figure 5), while melatonin secretion was suppressed by light with equal low intensity but a high blue
component (white bathroom, 130 lx, 6000 K). In addition, all lamps with high (500 lx) light intensities
had a detectable influence on melatonin concentrations after light exposure. Even the lamp with the
lowest blue proportion (warm white Planon, 2000 K) of the high-intensity (500 lx) lights had strong
effects on both melatonin secretion and subjective alertness. The rather low blue component of the
warm white Planon—at this intensity (see Table 3 and Figure 5) and the relatively close distance of
subjects’ eyes to the lamp (1 meter)—seems to be sufficient to cause this effect. The lamp with high
blue component and high intensity (hall daylight white) produces, as expected, even more pronounced
effects. In spite of the high light intensity and high blue component of the daylight white office lamp, it
only reduced melatonin concentrations significantly after, but not during light exposure, and the
increase of subjective alertness was not significant. The values of irradiance or melanopic lux (see
Table 3), a measure that integrates light intensity and spectral distribution in one unit on an empirical
basis [28], would imply a different ranking of the lamps than the amount of their effects in our sample.
Thus, the expected combined influence of light intensity and spectral distribution [37] is not reflected
in detail by our sample. This is probably due to the small sample size, combined with the pronounced
individual differences [30].
Still, in none of the conditions exposing subjects to light containing a high proportion of blue or
bright light was the rise in melatonin levels as large as they were in conditions exposing only to dim or
yellow light.
Nevertheless, subjects varied substantially in the magnitude of melatonin suppression induced by
any one lighting condition. Because all subjects were studied during the same week and exposed to
their assigned lighting condition each evening during the experimental period, light history would only
differ slightly between subjects according to their daytime activities [38]. Chemical agents or
age-related changes in the lens of the eye can be ruled out [39,40]. It might therefore be argued that the
observed differential responses to light have predominantly been due to individual variations in light
sensitivity [41,42]. Experimental data in extreme chronotypes have shown that circadian rhythms
differ in their response to chemical and environmental phase-shifting agents at the cellular level [43].
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2581
Though only suppression of melatonin as a circadian signal was investigated and not phase shifts of
melatonin, it would be interesting to validate the diagnostic properties of the setting and paradigm used
in the present study. This might help detect circadian rhythm sleep disorders by determining patients’
sensitivity to melatonin suppression by light. Regarding future research, it also seems worthwhile to
think about potential long-term consequences. Though still not generally accepted, several studies
suggest that melatonin in humans plays a similarly pivotal role to that already demonstrated in
animals [44,45]. Low melatonin is associated with numerous diseases [46], such as cancer [17],
and melatonin concentrations are reduced in the elderly [47], in patients with chronic primary
insomnia [48–50], and even more so in patients suffering from Alzheimer’s dementia [51,52]. Even the
neuroprotective properties of melatonin receive increasing evidence [53]. It is also well established
that the increase in melatonin levels that occurs in the evening facilitates sleep induction [54–56].
Thus, it is tempting to speculate that melatonin suppression by artificial light during the evening plays
a role in the sleep-related problems from which millions of individuals currently suffer. Nevertheless,
the general causal relations of light, melatonin-levels, and the health conditions mentioned above still
need to be determined.
The current study demonstrates that light affects not only melatonin levels, but also subjective
alertness. However, this is thought to rely on a different pathway than the retinal–pineal pathway. As
an fMRI study suggests, light directly activates cortical networks that underlie alertness [57]. This
relationship requires further investigation with objective measurements.
Diurnal species such as humans are obliged to use their eyes to find their way about the world.
Unlike nocturnal species like rats or bats, humans are at the mercy of their enemies when moving
about during the nighttime hours. It is most likely for this reason that the CTS promotes motor
inactivity and sleep during this period [4]. Although artificial light at night is clearly of great benefit to
society, its inappropriate timing might alter human physiology. In December 2007, the World Health
Organization added overnight shift work to its list of probable carcinogens [58]. One possible, but yet
to be proven, cause of cancer in overnight shift workers is the reduction of nighttime melatonin
resulting from exposure to artificial light. The results of the present study raise a question that should
be addressed in detail by future research: do we all suffer from these effects to some degree due to
nighttime exposure to blue and/or bright light?
3. Experimental Section
The study was performed in the sleep laboratory of the Institute of Physiology at the Charité in the
Sankt Hedwig Hospital, Berlin, in February of 2007. The protocol was approved by the local ethics
committee. Experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki,
and all participants gave their written, informed consent.
A total of 9 healthy subjects (3 women, 6 men, aged 22–33 years, mean = 26.3, SD = 4.2)
participated in the study. Subjects were recruited by word-of-mouth recommendation and received
pecuniary compensation. Medical, psychiatric, and sleep assessments were performed, including
evaluations of sleep quality (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, or PSQI [59]), of chronotype (Horne and
Östberg [60]), and of seasonality (Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire, or SPAQ [61]). The
inclusion criterion was a habitual bedtime between 10:00 p.m. and 1:00 a.m. (PSQI: Item 1—“When
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2582
did you habitually go to sleep during the last four weeks?”). Exclusion criteria were age <18 or >35;
current pregnancy; current or recent shift work (during the last year); poor sleep hygiene (sleep log or
actigraphic proof of a more than 1-h deviation from habitual bedtime during a 7-day entrainment
period); definite morning or evening types; seasonality score greater than 5; transmeridian travel
(during, or within 1 month of, the study); any psychiatric, medical, or sleep disorder, including drug or
alcohol abuse; and the intake of any medication, including over-the-counter medication, over the
past month.
During a 7-day entrainment period, subjects were asked to keep a regular sleep–wake schedule with
a tolerance range of ± 60 min of their self-estimated habitual bedtime. Compliance was monitored
using a sleep log and actimetry (Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology).
During the 6-day experimental phase, we tried to create conditions that were as natural as possible:
subjects followed their habitual daytime schedules, only attending the laboratory in the evening hours
from 7 p.m. until midnight (monitored by sleep log and actimetry). Subjects were allowed to eat until
7 p.m. and to drink caffeinated beverages until 3 p.m. Alcoholic beverages were forbidden during lab
days. Drinking water was accepted until 10 min prior to saliva collection. Subjects spent their time in
the laboratory pursuing self-chosen activities (e.g., playing games, talking, watching videos on a
dim-light screen) in a dimly lit room (<10 lx) by ordinary dimmable halogen lamps. Because circadian
phase is associated with habitual bedtime, light exposure was timed individually to occur 1 h before
habitual bedtime [62].
Table 2. Study design.
Group Day 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6
I Baseline: hall lighting office lighting bathroom bathroom Planon
II office lighting bathroom bathroom Planon hall lighting
II bathroom bathroom Planon hall lighting office lighting
daylight white warm white yellow
During evening 1, subjects were exposed only to dim light (baseline condition). During evenings 2
through 6, everyday lamps of different types (office, bathroom, industry), with two different intensities
(130 vs. 500 lx at the cornea) and spectral distributions (4 with various and 1 without blue portions)
were used to expose subjects, in randomly assigned groups of 3, to each lighting condition for a total
of 30 min (for study design, see Table 2; for a detailed description of the lamps, see Table 3 and for
their spectral distributions, see Figure 5). Three of the lamps were selected by their original application
as examples to reflect naturalistic lighting conditions as occurring in bathrooms (bathroom daylight
white), office work places (office daylight white) and industrial work places or gyms (hall daylight
white). Two of the lamps were custom-made to produce: (1) a zero blue component bathroom light
situation (bathroom yellow); and (2) a low blue component but high light intensity work place light
situation (Planon warm white).
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2583
Table 3. Lighting conditions.
No. lamp name
Lux
(lx)
Kelvin
(K)
Irra-
diance
(W/m
2)
Lux
m
(lx
m
)
1
Type Model
Blue
component
Position
Eye (cm)
distance
2
Company
0 baseline
(dim light) <10 normal
pointing to
ceiling 150 cm
1 bathroom
yellow 130 2000 0.319 118 fluorescent Custom-
made zero wall over
mirror 100 cm Narva
BEL
2
bathroom
daylight
white
130 6000 0.385 581 fluorescent 6641
L18/W high wall over
mirror 100 cm Narva
BEL
3
office
daylight
white
500 6000 1.437 2069 fluorescent
(3 pieces)
T5
54W/860 high ceiling 150 cm
Trilux
Narva
BEL
4 hall daylight
white 500 5000 1.391 1724 metal
halogenid NCT 70W high ceiling 150 cm
Trilux
Narva
GLE
5 Planon warm
white
3
500 2800 1.432 1826 dielectric
inhibited
custom
made low wall 100 cm Osram
1
The unit melanopic lux (lx
m
) was recently suggested by al Enezi et al. to predict the sensitivity of melanopsin
photoreceptors to polychromatic light [28]. It was derived from action spectra of monochromatic lights in several species;
2
Due to reflection, the whole visual field was exposed with a maximum in the center;
3
The Planon is an advanced
prototype dielectric inhibited light source. It is being developed by Osram, Munich. So far, more than 150 lamps have
been built. It is currently being used as a rugged light source in CNC Machines.
Figure 5. Spectral distributions. Numbers represent the lamps as follows: 1. bathroom
yellow; 2. bathroom daylight white; 3. office daylight white; 4. hall daylight white;
5. Planon warm white.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2584
During light exposure, subjects were sitting at an assigned spot and were instructed to keep their
gaze straight (at approximately 90°) over the 30-min light exposure. No other activity was permitted
except talking. Subjects were informed that bright light may suppress melatonin; no information was
given regarding the effects of light spectrum. Lighting conditions were characterized using a luxmeter
(LMT Berlin), a spectrometer (StellarNet EPP-2000), and a luminance camera (LMK Mobile
Advanced, Ilmenau). Light intensity and spectral distribution were determined for each lamp at the site
of the experiment from the position of subjects’ eyes, i.e., in the direction and angle of the gaze
(performed by INP, Greifswald, Germany).
Each evening during the study period, self-assessment of alertness, calmness and contentedness was
performed every 30 min using a paper and pencil version of visual analogue scales [63]. The VAS
assessment at the start of light exposure was brought forward by 5 min to avoid disturbing the
exposure procedure. In addition, saliva was collected with salivettes every 30 min during dim-light
exposure, as well as 10 min before, every 10 min during, and 10 min after light exposure. Saliva
collection never took more than one minute. Melatonin concentration in saliva was determined by a
commercial radioimmunoassay (Bühlmann Laboratories, Allschwill, Switzerland) with an analytical
least detectable dose of 0.15 pg/mL, intraassay precision: 6.7% (range: 4.9%–8.8%), interassay
precision: 10.4% (range: 8.5%–13.9%).
As a consequence of the design, subjects’ bedtimes were slightly delayed during the experiment, as
they still needed to get home after midnight. We tried to rule out possible order effects by using a
crossover design and by also controlling for differences between lighting conditions and light
exposition (see results section for details).
Statistical Analysis
In order to calculate descriptive group differences, the reference point for differential melatonin
levels was set to zero for each individual: (1) at time of “lights on” (see Figure 2); and (2) at the mean
of 10 min before “lights on” and “lights on” (see Table 1). For statistical analysis, melatonin
concentrations were calculated as areas under the curve (AUC) before, during, and after light exposure.
AUC before light exposure (AUC pre) included 30 to 0 min before “lights on,” based on
20- and 10-min intervals. AUC during lighting (AUC light) included 10 to 30 min after “lights on,”
based on 10-min intervals. AUC after light exposure (AUC post) included 0 to 30 min after “lights
off,” based on 10- and 20-min intervals. Friedman ANOVAs were calculated for each AUC (pre, light,
and post), followed by post–hoc Wilcoxon-tests. Friedman ANOVAs were alpha-adjusted using the
method of Cross and Chaffin [64]. Three subjects had to be excluded from statistical analysis of
melatonin levels due to single missing data caused by incorrect saliva collection.
For explorative purposes, subjective alertness was compared between baseline and the various
lighting conditions using Wilcoxon signed-rank test.
4. Conclusions
Low-intensity light emitted by everyday lamps in a naturalistic setting can influence melatonin
levels and alertness perception in healthy human subjects after only 30 min. The suppressive effect of
this light exposure on melatonin can be minimized by using a yellow lamp without blue component.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2585
The order of the other lamps in the degree of influence on alertness and melatonin concentrations in
our sample is not completely as expected, which could be due to the small sample size. It should be
replicated with bigger sample sizes, also including further outcome variables such as sleep parameters
and objective measures of behavioral variables. This could highlight the impact of evening light on
adverse health outcomes in more depth. Still, even with this small sample size, the melatonin
suppressing and alertness enhancing power of short exposition to ordinary lamps in contrast to dim
light or yellow light without blue component could be shown in this study. We suggest that potentially
adverse biological effects of untimely light exposure could be reduced or avoided by appropriate
tuning of the spectral content and intensity of lighting devices, similar to the way lamps with specially
designed spectral distributions can be used therapeutically in order to relieve various conditions. To
put it—albeit tentatively—in a nutshell: filter out blue or dim the light when you go brush your
teeth at night!
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by BMBF (German Ministry for Education and Research) FKZ: 13N8973.
We thank the BMBF for their support.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Ohayon, M.M.; Reynolds, C.F., III. Epidemiological and clinical relevance of insomnia diagnosis
algorithms according to the DSM-IV and the International Classification of Sleep Disorders
(ICSD). Sleep Med. 2009, 10, 952–960.
2. Olesen, J.; Gustavsson, A.; Svensson, M.; Wittchen, H.U.; Jonsson, B. The economic cost of brain
disorders in Europe. Eur. J. Neurol. 2012, 19, 155–162.
3. Hickie, I.B.; Rogers, N.L. Novel melatonin-based therapies: Potential advances in the treatment of
major depression. Lancet 2011, 378, 621–631.
4. Pittendrigh, C.S. Temporal organization: Reflections of a Darwinian clock-watcher. Annu. Rev.
Physiol. 1993, 55, 16–54.
5. Boivin, D.B.; Czeisler, C.A.; Dijk, D.J.; Duffy, J.F.; Folkard, S.; Minors, D.S.; Totterdell, P.;
Waterhouse, J.M. Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates
mood in healthy subjects. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1997, 54, 145–152.
6. Bruguerole, B. Chronopharmacology. In Biologic Rhythms in Clinical and Laboratory Medicine;
Touitou, Y., Haus, E., Eds.; Springer Verlag: Paris, France, 1992; pp. 114–137.
7. Shearman, L.P.; Sriram, S.; Weaver, D.R.; Maywood, E.S.; Chaves, I.; Zheng, B.; Kume, K.;
Lee, C.C.; van der Horst, G.T.; Hastings, M.H.; et al. Interacting molecular loops in the
mammalian circadian clock. Science 2000, 288, 1013–1019.
8. Van Cauter, E.; Plat, L.; Leproult, R.; Copinschi, G. Alterations of circadian rhythmicity and
sleep in aging: Endocrine consequences. Horm. Res. 1998, 49, 147–152.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2586
9. Wirz-Justice, A. Circadian rhythms in mammalian neurotransmitter receptors. Prog. Neurobiol.
1987, 29, 219–259.
10. Wyatt, J.K.; Ritz-De Cecco, A.; Czeisler, C.A.; Dijk, D.J. Circadian temperature and melatonin
rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. Am. J. Physiol.
1999, 277, R1152–R1163.
11. Davidson, A.J.; Sellix, M.T.; Daniel, J.; Yamazaki, S.; Menaker, M.; Block, G.D. Chronic jet-lag
increases mortality in aged mice. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, R914–R916.
12. Spiegel, K.; Leproult, R.; van Cauter, E. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine
function. Lancet 1999, 354, 1435–1439.
13. Stevens, R.G.; Blask, D.E.; Brainard, G.C.; Hansen, J.; Lockley, S.W.; Provencio, I.; Rea, M.S.;
Reinlib, L. Meeting report: The role of environmental lighting and circadian disruption in cancer
and other diseases. Environ. Health Perspect. 2007, 115, 1357–1362.
14. Hu, K.; Van Someren, E.J.; Shea, S.A.; Scheer, F.A. Reduction of scale invariance of activity
fluctuations with aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Involvement of the circadian pacemaker.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 2490–2494.
15. Hansen, J. Risk of breast cancer after night- and shift work: Current evidence and ongoing studies
in Denmark. Cancer Causes Control 2006, 17, 531–537.
16. Knutsson, A.; Akerstedt, T.; Jonsson, B.G.; Orth-Gomer, K. Increased risk of ischaemic heart
disease in shift workers. Lancet 1986, 2, 89–92.
17. Schernhammer, E.S.; Schulmeister, K. Melatonin and cancer risk: Does light at night compromise
physiologic cancer protection by lowering serum melatonin levels? Br. J. Cancer 2004, 90,
941–943.
18. Czeisler, C.A.; Shanahan, T.L.; Klerman, E.B.; Martens, H.; Brotman, D.J.; Emens, J.S.;
Klein, T.; Rizzo, J.F., III. Suppression of melatonin secretion in some blind patients by exposure
to bright light. N. Engl. J. Med. 1995, 332, 6–11.
19. Provencio, I.; Rodriguez, I.R.; Jiang, G.; Hayes, W.P.; Moreira, E.F.; Rollag, M.D. A novel
human opsin in the inner retina. J. Neurosci. 2000, 20, 600–605.
20. Foster, R.G. Neurobiology: Bright blue times. Nature 2005, 433, 698–699.
21. Hanifin, J.P.; Brainard, G.C. Photoreception for circadian, neuroendocrine, and neurobehavioral
regulation. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007, 26, 87–94.
22. Brainard, G.C.; Hanifin, J.P.; Greeson, J.M.; Byrne, B.; Glickman, G.; Gerner, E.; Rollag, M.D.
Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: Evidence for a novel circadian
photoreceptor. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 6405–6412.
23. Thapan, K.; Arendt, J.; Skene, D.J. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: Evidence for a
novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J. Physiol. 2001, 535, 261–267.
24. Cajochen, C.; Munch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Krauchi, K.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Orgul, S.;
Wirz-Justice, A. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate
to short wavelength light. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2005, 90, 1311–1316.
25. Lockley, S.W.; Evans, E.E.; Scheer, F.A.; Brainard, G.C.; Czeisler, C.A.; Aeschbach, D.
Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness, vigilance, and the waking
electroencephalogram in humans. Sleep 2006, 29, 161–168.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2587
26. Munch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Steiner, R.; Oelhafen, P.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Cajochen, C.
Wavelength-dependent effects of evening light exposure on sleep architecture and sleep EEG
power density in men. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2006, 290, R1421–R1428.
27. Cajochen, C.; Jud, C.; Munch, M.; Kobialka, S.; Wirz-Justice, A.; Albrecht, U. Evening exposure
to blue light stimulates the expression of the clock gene PER2 in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006,
23, 1082–1086.
28. Enezi, J.; Revell, V.; Brown, T.; Wynne, J.; Schlangen, L.; Lucas, R. A “melanopic” spectral
efficiency function predicts the sensitivity of melanopsin photoreceptors to polychromatic lights.
J. Biol. Rhythms 2011, 26, 314–323.
29. Gooley, J.J.; Chamberlain, K.; Smith, K.A.; Khalsa, S.B.; Rajaratnam, S.M.; van Reen, E.;
Zeitzer, J.M.; Czeisler, C.A.; Lockley, S.W. Exposure to room light before bedtime suppresses
melatonin onset and shortens melatonin duration in humans. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2011, 96,
E463–E472.
30. Santhi, N.; Thorne, H.C.; van der Veen, D.R.; Johnsen, S.; Mills, S.L.; Hommes, V.; Schlangen, L.J.;
Archer, S.N.; Dijk, D.J. The spectral composition of evening light and individual differences in
the suppression of melatonin and delay of sleep in humans. J. Pineal Res. 2012, 53, 47–59.
31. Campbell, S.S.; Dawson, D.; Anderson, M.W. Alleviation of sleep maintenance insomnia with
timed exposure to bright light. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1993, 41, 829–836.
32. Riemersma-van der Lek, R.F.; Swaab, D.F.; Twisk, J.; Hol, E.M.; Hoogendijk, W.J.;
van Someren, E.J. Effect of bright light and melatonin on cognitive and noncognitive function in
elderly residents of group care facilities: A randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2008,
299, 2642–2655.
33. Van Someren, E.J.; Kessler, A.; Mirmiran, M.; Swaab, D.F. Indirect bright light improves
circadian rest-activity rhythm disturbances in demented patients. Biol. Psychiatry 1997, 41,
955–963.
34. Lieverse, R.; van Someren, E.J.; Nielen, M.M.; Uitdehaag, B.M.; Smit, J.H.; Hoogendijk, W.J.
Bright light treatment in elderly patients with nonseasonal major depressive disorder: A
randomized placebo-controlled trial. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2011, 68, 61–70.
35. Munch, M.; Scheuermaier, K.D.; Zhang, R.; Dunne, S.P.; Guzik, A.M.; Silva, E.J.; Ronda, J.M.;
Duffy, J.F. Effects on subjective and objective alertness and sleep in response to evening light
exposure in older subjects. Behav. Brain Res. 2011, 224, 272–278.
36. Lewy, A.J.; Wehr, T.A.; Goodwin, F.K.; Newsome, D.A.; Markey, S.P. Light suppresses
melatonin secretion in humans. Science 1980, 210, 1267–1269.
37. Figueiro, M.G.; Rea, M.S.; Bullough, J.D. Circadian effectiveness of two polychromatic lights in
suppressing human nocturnal melatonin. Neurosci. Lett. 2006, 406, 293–297.
38. Hebert, M.; Martin, S.K.; Lee, C.; Eastman, C.I. The effects of prior light history on the
suppression of melatonin by light in humans. J. Pineal Res. 2002, 33, 198–203.
39. Herljevic, M.; Middleton, B.; Thapan, K.; Skene, D.J. Light-induced melatonin suppression:
Age-related reduction in response to short wavelength light. Exp. Gerontol. 2005, 40, 237–242.
40. Wirz-Justice, A.; Reme, C.; Prunte, A.; Heinen, U.; Graw, P.; Urner, U. Lithium decreases retinal
sensitivity, but this is not cumulative with years of treatment. Biol. Psychiatry 1997, 41, 743–746.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2588
41. Lewy, A.J.; Lefler, B.J.; Emens, J.S.; Bauer, V.K. The circadian basis of winter depression.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 7414–7419.
42. Terman, J.S.; Terman, M. Photopic and scotopic light detection in patients with seasonal affective
disorder and control subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 1999, 46, 1642–1648.
43. Brown, S.A.; Kunz, D.; Dumas, A.; Westermark, P.O.; Vanselow, K.; Tilmann-Wahnschaffe, A.;
Herzel, H.; Kramer, A. Molecular insights into human daily behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
2008, 105, 1602–1607.
44. Arendt, J. Melatonin and the Mammalian Pineal Gland; University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995;
pp. 201–285.
45. Kunz, D.; Mahlberg, R. Melatonin: A Chronobiotic that not only Shifts Rhythms. In Melatonin:
Biological Basis of its Function in Health and Dieases; Pandi-Perumal, S.R., Cardinali, D.P.,
Eds.; Landes Biosciences: Austin, TX, USA, 2004; pp. 1–11.
46. Hardeland, R. Melatonin in aging and disease—Multiple consequences of reduced secretion,
options and limits of treatment. Aging Dis. 2012, 3, 194–225.
47. Mahlberg, R.; Tilmann, A.; Salewski, L.; Kunz, D. Normative data on the daily profile of
urinary 6-sulfatoxymelatonin in healthy subjects between the ages of 20 and 84.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006, 31, 634–641.
48. Riemann, D.; Klein, T.; Rodenbeck, A.; Feige, B.; Horny, A.; Hummel, R.; Weske, G.;
Al-Shajlawi, A.; Voderholzer, U. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary insomnia.
Psychiatry Res. 2002, 113, 17–27.
49. Rodenbeck, A.; Huether, G.; Ruther, E.; Hajak, G. Nocturnal melatonin secretion and its
modification by treatment in patients with sleep disorders. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1999, 467, 89–93.
50. Hajak, G.; Rodenbeck, A.; Staedt, J.; Bandelow, B.; Huether, G.; Ruther, E. Nocturnal plasma
melatonin levels in patients suffering from chronic primary insomnia. J. Pineal Res. 1995, 19,
116–122.
51. Mahlberg, R.; Walther, S.; Kalus, P.; Bohner, G.; Haedel, S.; Reischies, F.M.; Kuhl, K.P.;
Hellweg, R.; Kunz, D. Pineal calcification in Alzheimer’s disease: An in vivo study using
computed tomography. Neurobiol. Aging 2008, 29, 203–209.
52. Skene, D.J.; Swaab, D.F. Melatonin rhythmicity: Effect of age and Alzheimer’s disease.
Exp. Gerontol. 2003, 38, 199–206.
53. Pandi-Perumal, S.R.; Bahammam, A.S.; Brown, G.M.; Spence, D.W.; Bharti, V.K.; Kaur, C.;
Hardeland, R.; Cardinali, D.P. Melatonin Antioxidative Defense: Therapeutical Implications for
Aging and Neurodegenerative Processes. Neurotox. Res. 2012, doi:10.1007/s12640-012-9337-4.
54. Kunz, D. Chronobiotic protocol and circadian sleep propensity index: New tools for clinical
routine and research on melatonin and sleep. Pharmacopsychiatry 2004, 37, 139–146.
55. Tzischinsky, O.; Shlitner, A.; Lavie, P. The association between the nocturnal sleep gate and
nocturnal onset of urinary 6-sulfatoxymelatonin. J. Biol. Rhythms 1993, 8, 199–209.
56. Wirz-Justice, A.; Armstrong, S.M. Melatonin: Nature’s soporific? J. Sleep Res. 1996, 5, 137–141.
57. Vandewalle, G.; Balteau, E.; Phillips, C.; Degueldre, C.; Moreau, V.; Sterpenich, V.; Albouy, G.;
Darsaud, A.; Desseilles, M.; Dang-Vu, T.T.; et al. Daytime light exposure dynamically enhances
brain responses. Curr. Biol. 2006, 16, 1616–1621.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2013, 14 2589
58. International Agency for Research on Cancer, World Health Organization. Graveyard Shift Work
Linked to Cancer; The Asoociated Press: London, UK, 2007.
59. Buysse, D.J.; Reynolds, C.F., III; Monk, T.H.; Berman, S.R.; Kupfer, D.J. The Pittsburgh Sleep
Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989, 28,
193–213.
60. Horne, J.A.; Ostberg, O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness
in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. 1976, 4, 97–110.
61. Williams, J.B.W.; Link, M.J.; Rosenthal, N.E.; Amira, L.; Terman, M. Structured Interview Guide
for the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale- Seasonal Affective Disorder. Version (Sigh-SAD);
New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York, NY, USA, 1992.
62. Mongrain, V.; Lavoie, S.; Selmaoui, B.; Paquet, J.; Dumont, M. Phase relationships between
sleep-wake cycle and underlying circadian rhythms in Morningness-Eveningness. J. Biol. Rhythms
2004, 19, 248–257.
63. Bond, A.; Lader, M. The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. Br. J. Med. Psychol.
1974, 47, 211–218.
64. Cross, E.M.; Chaffin, W.W. Use of binomial theorem in interpreting results of multiple tests of
significance. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1982, 42, 25–34.
© 2013 by the authors; licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).