BookPDF Available

The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature

Authors:

Abstract

The mating mind' revives and extends Darwin's suggestion that sexual selection through mate choice was important in human mental evolution - especially the more 'self-expressive' aspects of human behavior, such as art, morality, language, and creativity. Their 'survival value' has proven elusive, but their adaptive design features suggest they evolved through mutual mate choice, in both sexes, to advertise intelligence, creativity, moral character, and heritable fitness. The supporting evidence includes human mate preferences, courtship behavior, behavior genetics, psychometrics, and life history patterns. The theory makes many testable predictions, and sheds new light on human cognition, motivation, communication, sexuality, and culture.
THE MATING MIND: HOW SEXUAL CHOICE SHAPED THE EVOLUTION OF
HUMAN NATURE
[Doubleday/Heinemann, 2000, 503 pp. ISBN: 0-434-00741-2]
Precis of Miller on Mating-Mind
Geoffrey F. Miller
Department of Psychology, Logan Hall
University of New Mexico
Albuquerque, NM 87131-1161
USA
gfmiller@unm.edu
[Note: this précis of my book “The mating mind” was published in the electronic journal
Psycoloquy in August 2001, at:
http://www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/psyc-bin/newpsy?12.008
Abstract
'The mating mind' revives and extends Darwin's suggestion that sexual selection
through mate choice was important in human mental evolution - especially the
more 'self-expressive' aspects of human behavior, such as art, morality,
language, and creativity. Their 'survival value' has proven elusive, but their
adaptive design features suggest they evolved through mutual mate choice, in
both sexes, to advertise intelligence, creativity, moral character, and heritable
fitness. The supporting evidence includes human mate preferences, courtship
behavior, behavior genetics, psychometrics, and life history patterns. The theory
makes many testable predictions, and sheds new light on human cognition,
motivation, communication, sexuality, and culture.
Keywords: sexual selection, human evolution, art, language, morality, creativity.
I. CHAPTER 1: CENTRAL PARK (US edition)/EXHIBITION ROAD (UK edition).
1. The human mind evolved somehow. Yet it remains unclear what selection pressures
favored our large brains, our creative intelligence, and our unique capacities for
language, art, music, humor, romantic love, and moral commitment. Following Herbert
Spencer's misleading phrase 'the survival of the fittest', most theorists throughout the
last 140 years have tried and failed to identify the 'survival value' of these capacities.
2. 'The mating mind' suggests that if one meta-theory doesn't work, we should try
another one. Darwin (1871) argued that sexual selection (for reproduction) is distinct
from natural selection (for survival), and that traits inexplicable in terms of 'survival value'
can often be explained as ornaments for attracting sexual partners. The revival of sexual
selection theory since about 1980 has confirmed the utility of Darwin's distinction
1
between natural and sexual selection. Sexual selection often creates an evolutionary
positive-feedback loop that is highly sensitive to initial conditions. It therefore tends to
produce extravagant traits that have high costs and complexity, yet these traits are often
unique to one species, and absent in closely-related taxa. By contrast, natural selection
for ecological utility tends to produce convergent evolution, where many lineages
independently evolve the same, efficient, low-cost solutions to the same environmental
problems.
3. Viewed from a macro-evolutionary perspective, the human brain fits this profile of
sexually-selected ornaments: it is unique among living primates, has high metabolic
costs and enormous complexity, and its capacities are conspicuously displayed during
courtship (especially verbal courtship). The brain's low level of sexual dimorphism is
congruent with evidence that human mate choice is mutual, with males and females
almost equally choosy about the mental traits of long- term sexual partners. Sexual
dimorphism is a distinctive outcome of sexual selection, but sexual selection does not
always produce dimorphism.
4. Mate choice suffices to explain many distinctive aspects of the human mind,
especially those that yield no apparent survival benefits. Darwin (1871) made the same
argument in suggesting that human language and music evolved, like bird song, for
courtship. This view of the mind as a set of courtship adaptations can now be extended
and refined in the light of modern sexual selection theory, evolutionary psychology, and
evolutionary anthropology. Although evolutionary psychology recognizes the powerful
role of sexual selection in shaping sex differences in motivation, emotion, and cognition,
it has neglected the possibility that sexual selection could produce psychological
adaptations (such as language and creativity) equally in both sexes.
5. Many researchers have suggested that human mental evolution was so fast and
unusual that it must have been driven by some sort of positive-feedback process. Other
candidates for the positive-feedback process have included gene-culture co-evolution,
inter-tribal warfare, and social selection for Machiavellian intelligence. Sexual selection
has been strangely neglected, though it is the best-established, most powerful form of
positive-feedback evolution known to biologists.
6. Evolutionary psychology has tended to overlook sexually-selected mental traits
because its criteria for recognizing psychological adaptations (e.g. efficiency, modularity,
low heritability, universality across individuals) have been too restrictive (Miller, 2000).
Sexual ornaments violate the efficiency criterion because they have high growth,
maintenance, and display costs precisely so they can function as reliable fitness
indicators according to Zahavi's (1975) handicap principle. They are only somewhat
modular at the genetic, developmental, and metabolic levels, because total modularity
would render them totally useless as indicators of general health and fitness. Ornament
quality tends to have moderate to high heritability, because ornaments often evolve to
advertise heritable genetic quality. Likewise, the whole point of sexual ornaments is to
amplify apparent individual differences, so they show extreme variability rather than
uniformity across individuals. Unfortunately, some evolutionary psychologists such as
Pinker (1997) have dismissed music, art, humor, and general intelligence as non-
adaptations because they did not fit the typical profile of survival-selected adaptations --
hardly surprising, if they were sexually selected.
2
7. In other respects, 'The mating mind' is standard biological adaptationism, focusing
much more on 'what' and 'why' than on 'how', 'when', or 'where'. It makes very few claims
about the geographical location, antiquity, or genetic changes associated with the
evolution of our sexually-selected mental traits. The emphasis, as in most animal
behavior research, is on characterizing various adaptations and seeing whether their
features are consistent with particular selection pressures hypothesized to have
produced them. The book's theory of mental evolution is more testable than most,
because the heritable sexual preferences of our ancestors are probably still manifest in
modern mate choice, so they can be assessed as selection pressures independently of
the courtship adaptations they are posited to have favored.
II. CHAPTER 2: DARWIN'S PRODIGY.
8. This chapter reviews the peculiar history of Darwin's theory of sexual selection
through mate choice, tracing its rejection by Victorian biologists, its neglect for over a
century, and its dramatic revival in the last couple of decades (also see Cronin, 1991).
9. Charles Darwin, like his grandfather Erasmus, recognized that sexual reproduction
was central to evolution. His theory of sexual selection was developed not so much to
explain sex differences, but to account for complex ornaments that seem useless for
survival, and therefore inexplicable through natural selection. He suggested that if
animals of a species came to prefer a particular trait when choosing sexual partners, that
trait would tend to grow in size, complexity, and quality over evolutionary time, even if
the trait had high costs in every other domain of evolutionary competition.
10. Darwin (1871) had a sophisticated view of the psychology of mate choice. He
emphasized that even relatively simple nervous systems (e.g. insects, fish, frogs) suffice
for mate choice -- but that the more complex an animal's brain, the more intelligent its
mate choice could be. As mental complexity increased, the discriminatory power of mate
choice would increase, so sexual selection would command ever greater importance in
evolution, reaching its zenith in human evolution. Darwin noted "He who admits the
principle of sexual selection will be led to the remarkable conclusion that the cerebral
system not only regulates most of the existing functions of the body, but has indirectly
influenced the progressive development [i.e. evolution] of various bodily structures and
of certain mental qualities." He did not attempt a one-way reduction of psychology to
biology, but saw psychology as a driving force in biological evolution.
11. Whereas Darwin's natural selection theory was widely accepted, his idea of sexual
selection through mate choice was almost universally rejected by Victorian biologists.
Alfred Wallace was a leading critic, suggesting that most male ornamentation was a
developmental side-effect of greater male energy and physiological exuberance.
Wallace's objections led mate choice theory to be viewed for the next hundred years as
Darwin's most embarrassing blunder. This sceptical view of mate choice was reinforced
by leading biologists of the early 20th century, including Thomas Hunt Morgan, Julian
Huxley, J.B.S. Haldane, and Ernst Mayr. They combined a group-selectionist, good-of-
the-species abhorrence of survival-reducing ornamentation with a Modernist machine
aesthetic (derived from the Bauhaus and other puritanical sects of socialism), which
viewed ornamentation as morally decadent, economically oppressive, and tasteless.
3
12. As a result, almost all of 20th century psychology, anthropology, neuroscience, and
the humanities developed without recognizing any role for mate choice in human mental
evolution. Instead, Freud's paleolithic fantasies dominated views of prehistoric sexuality,
and his theory of excess libido being sublimated into artistic creativity echoed Wallace's
surplus-energy arguments for ornamentation. This bias against mate choice theory
began to erode only in the 1970s, leading to a runaway revival of mate choice theory in
evolutionary biology, to the point that animal behavior journals are now dominated by
experiments on mate choice and sexual competition. Yet this revival has gone largely
unnoticed in mainstream psychology, neuroscience, and the social sciences, which still
view 'survival of the fittest' as evolution's bottom line, and which therefore have trouble
seeing any evolutionary rationale for those aspects of human nature most concerned
with self-ornamentation, display, status, ideology, fashion, and aesthetics.
III. CHAPTER 3: THE RUNAWAY BRAIN.
13. This chapter considers the possibility that runaway sexual selection drove the rapid
escalation of brain size characteristic of our lineage, but it ultimately rejects this 'runaway
brain' theory as neglecting the role of mutual mate choice, and failing to explain the
sexual equality in human brain size and intelligence.
14. Ronald Fisher (1930) suggested that sexual selection through mate choice could
lead to a positive-feedback process, in which ever-more- discriminating sexual
preferences become genetically linked to ever- more-extravagant sexual displays. The
runaway hypothesis was neglected for fifty years, until biologists developed
mathematical models in the early 1980s showing that Fisher was right. Heritable sexual
preferences will tend to become genetically correlated with the sexual ornaments that
they favor, and this genetic correlation gives the runaway process its evolutionary
momentum. The direction of runaway is so sensitive to initial evolutionary conditions that
it is very hard to predict which sexual ornaments will evolve in a lineage, but once
underway, runaway will tend to increase the complexity and magnitude of the favored
ornament to extremes. Runaway's unpredictability can explain why closely related
species can differ so dramatically in their ornamentation and courtship behavior.
15. In previous work (Miller, 1993), I suggested that human mental evolution was driven
by runaway sexual selection. If hominid females happened to develop a sexual
preference for creative intelligence, then males with more creative intelligence would
attract more sexual partners and would produce more offspring. Those offspring would
inherit both the taste for clever courtship and the capacity for producing it. Over many
generations, average creative intelligence in the lineage would increase rapidly, perhaps
explaining why brain size tripled in just two million years.
16. This 'runaway brain theory' could explain the speed of encephalization, the rarity of
creative intelligence across species, and people's propensity to show off their creativity
and intelligence in sexual courtship. However, the theory predicts large sex differences
in brain size and creative intelligence, and neuroanatomical and psychometric evidence
shows these differences are very small. Male human brains are only about 100 cubic
centimeters larger than female brains (after allometrically correcting for body size
differences), and there is no apparent sex difference in the g factor that underlies IQ test
performance. Males do account for a large proportion of public cultural displays in every
known society, such as the invention and dissemination of art, music, ideologies,
4
religions, philosophies, and science (Miller, 1999). But this cultural dimorphism might be
conflated with Holocene patriarchal cultural traditions.
17. The lack of sex differences in human mental capacities is a strong argument against
the runaway brain theory. This does not imply that sexual selection is irrelevant, only that
the choosy-female, displaying-male model assumed by Fisher (1930) is too simple for
the human case, in which both sexes are choosy (at least in establishing long-term
relationships), and both display their mental traits during courtship. If sexual selection
drove human mental evolution, it must have been a form of sexual selection that could
work given mutual mate choice.
IV. CHAPTER 4: A MIND FIT FOR MATING.
18. This chapter introduces the idea of sexual ornaments as costly, reliable indicators of
fitness, and argues that our most distinctive mental traits evolved through mutual mate
choice as fitness-indicators.
19. Many biologists believe sexual reproduction evolved as a way to minimize the
disruption caused by the copying errors and mutations intrinsic to DNA-based
reproduction. By extension, mate choice can be viewed as a method for enhancing this
anti-mutation effect, by favoring sexual partners who carry 'good genes' (i.e. genotypes
with a low number of deleterious mutations). Mate choice for good genes will tend to
focus on the observable traits of potential mates that best reveal their mutation load.
Therefore, traits that happen to reveal mutation load more accurately than other traits
(e.g. through more complex development that requires the interaction of more genes)
will tend to be favored in mate choice. Under sexual selection, such traits will tend to
grow ever larger, costlier, and more complex, so they function as ever more reliable
indicators of good genes. Insofar as genetic quality implies expected evolutionary
fitness, these sexually-selected traits could be called 'fitness indicators'.
20. Human brains make particularly good fitness indicators because their growth
depends on about half the genes in the genome, thereby summarizing a huge amount of
information about mutation load. Brains are also good indicators of nutritional state and
general health, because they have such high energetic costs, constituting only 2% of
body weight, but consuming over 25% of adult metabolic energy (60% in infancy).
Moreover, the more important brains became during primate evolution (for whatever
reason), the more incentive mate choice would have had to focus on specific indicators
of brain quality (i.e. mental fitness as distinct from physical fitness).
21. Fitness indicator theory raises evolutionary-genetic issues about the heritability of
fitness. Throughout the 1980s, many biologists were skeptical about 'good genes'
models of sexual selection because such models assumed fitness would remain
heritable across many generations. Yet Fisher's (1930) 'fundamental theorem of natural
selection' implied that selection should decrease genetic variance, driving disfavoured
alleles to extinction, thereby decreasing the heritability of fitness to zero at evolutionary
equilibrium. The 'heritability of fitness' debate continued today, but evidence is
accumulating that fitness remains heritable in many species much of the time, probably
through a combination of ever-changing selection pressures (e.g. geographic diversity of
habitats plus migration, and host-parasite co-evolution), and ever-recurring deleterious
mutations. The remainder of the book takes for granted the heritability of a general
5
'fitness factor' (analogous to, and superordinate to, the g factor) in humans, though much
more research needs to be done to demonstrate such a fitness factor (see Miller, in
press). Recent genetic analyses suggest that at least 1.6 harmful new mutations per
individual per generation have been arising in our lineage for the last several million
years (Eyre-Walker & Keightley, 1999). This probably exceeds the mutation rate that
natural selection could contain in the absence of sexual selection for genetic quality.
22. Fitness indicators also raise reliability issues: what keeps low- fitness individuals
from cheating by displaying the high-quality ornament? Economists working on
'signalling theory' (a branch of game theory) showed in the 1960s that when there are
incentives for deception, signals of quality or intention must be costly in order to be
reliable. This point was independently applied to animal signalling, especially sexual
ornaments, by Amotz Zahavi (1975), with his 'handicap principle'. In his view, mate
preferences should only favor sexual ornaments that have such high marginal costs that
low-fitness pretenders could not afford to display a high-quality form of the ornament.
Sick, starving, inbred peacocks cannot afford to grow large tails, for example, and this
makes a large peacock tail as reliable fitness- indicator. If peacock tail size was
uncorrelated with general fitness, peahens would soon lose the sexual preference for
such an uninformative ornament. The handicap principle was vigorously debated for
twenty years in biology, in ignorance of the economic game theory work. Only recently
have biologists started to accept that prodigious, survival-reducing waste is a necessary
feature of most sexual ornaments, in order to keep them reliable as fitness indicators.
23. In some cases, however, ornaments can function as direct 'indexes' of fitness,
reliably revealing an individual's fitness without imposing high survival costs. Indexes
depend on the existence of some intrinsic genetic or developmental correlation between
fitness and ornament quality. Human intelligence is likely to be a fitness index, whereas
art, music, and humor are more likely to be fitness-indicating handicaps (unfortunately,
the book did not make this distinction very clear).
24. The book argues that our most distinctive psychological adaptations evolved mostly
through mutual sexual selection as fitness indicators. Of course, these species-unique
courtship adaptations are far out- numbered by the psychological adaptations for social
intelligence, foraging, predator avoidance, etc. shared with other primates. Yet we still
need some explanation of why small, efficient, ape-sized brains evolved into huge,
energy-hungry handicaps spewing out useless luxury behaviors such as flirtatious
conversation, music, and art.
V. CHAPTER V: ORNAMENTAL GENIUS.
25. This chapter examines how psychological biases (including sensory, perceptual,
cognitive, and emotional attunement to certain stimuli) influence mate choice and hence
the design of sexual ornaments. It then makes some analogies between courtship and
marketing, and between sexual selection and venture capital, and considers the possible
interactions during mental evolution between runaway sexual selection, sexual selection
for fitness indicators, and psychological biases.
26. Mate choice is mediated by the senses and the mind. Both include a large number of
'biases' or selective sensitivities that are evolutionarily contingent outcomes of their
design details, or of adaptation to certain environmental conditions. Biologists can
6
sometimes predict which stimuli will excite a perceptual system that evolved to detect
certain biologically relevant objects and events, but (as ethologists realized in the 1950s)
it is often hard to predict which artificial super-stimuli might excite the system even more.
Many sexual ornaments may have originated as accidental super-stimuli (arising from
novel mutations) that just happened to attract the attention of the opposite sex given how
their perceptual systems work. Since the super-stimulus sensitivities are often
evolutionarily contingent, the design of the sexual ornaments that they favor may be
evolutionarily contingent as well. This is yet another reason why sexual selection is an
unpredictable, diversifying process that rarely happens the same way twice.
27. As Darwin (1871) realized, the psychological biases affecting mate choice are not
restricted to low-level perception, but can include propensities for novelty-seeking and
pleasure-seeking. Given that primates are unusually neophilic and derive pleasure from
social interaction, these cognitive and motivational biases may have led mate choice to
favor more creative courtship with more social content during hominid evolution.
28. Any given sexual ornament, including the human mind, probably evolved through
some combination of runaway sexual selection, sexual selection for fitness indicators,
and psychological biases that happened to favor certain details of ornament design. The
interaction of these three sexual selection principles also helps to explain the
mechanisms of speciation, the proliferation of biodiversity, and the origins of evolutionary
innovations. Sexual selection works like venture capital, extending a line of reproductive
credit to potentially useful evolutionary innovations before they show any ecological
profitability. This may help to explain hominid encephalization before significant signs of
any cultural or technological progress. The obsession with survival selection is
analogous to the early 20th century corporate obsession with production as opposed to
marketing and advertising. Business had its 'marketing revolution' in the last half century;
it is time for evolutionary psychology to recognize that creative social behavior is to the
opposite sex what products are to consumers.
VI. CHAPTER 6: COURTSHIP IN THE PLEISTOCENE.
29. This chapter reconstructs Pleistocene hominid mating patterns using a variety of
evidence, and identifies the opportunities for mate choice to have influenced
reproductive success in small groups of hunter- gatherers.
30. Popular culture images of prehistory sustain the myth of rapacious, unchoosy cave-
men and helpless, unchoosy cave-women. Likewise, early primatology suggested that
'alpha males' attained the 'right to mate' with all females in a group. More recent studies
of human tribal societies and of other primates suggests these views are wrong, and that
both sexes exercise mate choice among chimpanzees, bonobos, and tribal humans,
which are all characterized by multi-male, multi-female social groups. Other evidence
(ranging from comparative morphology to evolutionary psychology) suggests that
hominids were not lifelong monogamists, but followed a pattern of serial social
monogamy (with relationships lasting a few days to a few years) augmented by extra-
pair copulations and some polygyny. Rapists would have been ostracized or killed, and
these costs would have usually exceeded the reproductive benefits of a single
copulation, given concealed ovulation.
7
31. It remains unclear how much paternal investment there was, as opposed to step-
parental investment functioning as a kind of altruistic courtship display. Data suggesting
that women favor self-confident, high-status men may reveal a preference for high
heritable fitness, rather than an expectation of long-term paternal investment. Given that
most individuals would have had their first child by their early 20s, and sought additional
mates thereafter, there must have been considerable overlap between parenting and
courtship -- stories told to children while within earshot of a potential mate may have
functioned more as courtship displays than as parental investment. Conversely, the mate
choices of adult females (i.e. mothers) may have been influenced by the preferences of
their children regarding which prospective step-father appeared kinder and more
entertaining. Given frequent interaction with kin, mate choice may have also worked as a
distributed decision-making process, taking into account the collective wisdom of
parents, siblings, and offspring. Equally, if individuals were choosing for heritable fitness,
kin of all ages would have had incentives to advertise their own fitness on behalf of their
fertile relatives, giving rise to various collective courtship rituals in which kin groups show
off to other kin groups. Even 'arranged marriages' impose sexual selection, if the parents
are using consistent mate choice criteria on behalf of their children (who may inherit the
same preferences).
32. A key section discusses a 'fitness matching' model of mutual choice for fitness
affordances, based on game theory models of 'two- sided matching'. The model posits a
mating market in which individuals assortatively mate for indicators of heritable fitness,
and those indicators evolve through an interaction between parental mate choice and
survival selection on offspring. Surprisingly, fitness matching can drive the evolution of
elaborate courtship behavior even under the limiting case of strict lifelong monogamy.
Fitness matching also maximizes the genetic variance in fitness in the next generation,
giving natural selection more raw material on which to work. The fitness matching
process automatically generates sexual equality in courtship adaptations. Such sexually-
selected adaptations with no sexual dimorphism have usually been dismissed as
'species recognition markers' by biologists, though they are displayed in the courtship of
many species. Fitness matching based on creative courtship behavior may have been
the key sexual selection process in human mental evolution.
VII. CHAPTER 7: BODIES OF EVIDENCE.
33. This chapter examines human morphological features that evolved under mate
choice, especially features that reveal mutual choice. These physical traits can work as
metaphors for sexually-selected mental traits, as both fitness indicators and aesthetic
ornaments resulting from runaway and perceptual biases.
34. Male human beards, penises, and upper-body muscles, and female breasts,
buttocks, and orgasmic capacities, show evidence of evolving through sexual selection.
There are also some monomorphic traits such as long head hair, hairless skin, highly
expressive faces, and full lips that function as sexual attractants. These fleshy parts
don't fossilize, and so have been somewhat neglected in human evolution theorizing.
However, all these traits are valued in modern mate choice, are displayed during
courtship, and are mostly unique to humans. Also, the dimorphic traits develop fully only
during puberty, in time for sexual attraction. These criteria also apply to many mental
traits.
8
35. The chapter concludes by discussing how sexual selection shaped the evolution of
physical and mental capacities for playing sports, which function as non-lethal domains
of sexual competition.
VIII. CHAPTER 8: ARTS OF SEDUCTION.
36. The last four chapters offer four case studies of uniquely human mental traits that
appear to have evolved under mate choice: art, morality, language, and creativity. This
chapter considers body ornamentation, art, and aesthetics.
37. Art is an easy example because no one has posited a credible survival function for
art, whereas it has obvious analogues to the sexually-selected visual displays of other
species such as peacocks and bowerbirds. Male bowerbirds construct large ornamental
bowers to attract females, decorating them with brightly colored flowers, pebbles, shells,
and feathers. Females inspect multiple bowers, choose the one they find most attractive,
and mate with its creator, raising her brood in a separate, simple nest of her own
construction. The bower is part of the bowerbird's 'extended phenotype' -- a genetically
evolved display constructed outside the body. Human aesthetic behavior also functions
as an extended phenotype, ranging from ochre pigments applied on as skin decoration,
to cave paintings and Venus figurines.
38. Human aesthetic preferences could have arisen (as mechanisms of mate choice for
good artisans) through runaway sexual selection, through sensory biases, and/or
through selection as fitness indicators. However, runaway can't make any predictions
about which particular preferences will evolve, and sensory bias theory doesn't work
very well here because other primates do not share the same aesthetic preferences as
humans, despite sharing almost identical visual systems. The idea that beautiful
artefacts carry information about the fitness of their makers makes more sense.
Thorstein Veblen (1899) and Franz Boas (1955) insisted that an artist's manifest
virtuosity (manual skill, access to rare resources, creativity, conscientiousness,
intelligence) is the major criterion of beauty in most cultures. Their view was eclipsed in
aesthetic theory by 20th century Modernism (which rejected the concepts of beauty and
virtuosity), but remains relevant to most popular culture, interior design, folk art, craft,
and fashion. This beauty-as-virtuosity theory also helps make sense of the hand axe,
which can be viewed as a sexually-selected feature of hominid extended phenotypes for
over 1.5 million years.
IX. CHAPTER 9: VIRTUES OF GOOD BREEDING.
39. This chapter examines our sexual abhorrence of selfishness, cheating, and lying,
and suggests that mate choice shaped our distinctive human capacities for sympathy,
kindness, sexual fidelity, moral leadership, magnanimity, romantic gift-giving, and
sportsmanship. It suggests that sexual selection explains much of human altruism that
cannot be explained through kin selection or reciprocal altruism, and tries to take
seriously David Buss's (1989) finding that 'kindness' was the top-ranked, most-desired
trait in a potential mate across all 37 cultures he studied.
40. All evolutionary theories of morality have to find a hidden genetic benefit to
apparently altruistic acts. Kin selection does it by pointing out that genetic benefits can
be spread across relatives by helping them, and reciprocal altruism theory does it by
9
pointing out that benefits to oneself can be spread across time, through repeated
interactions with trusted trading partners. These theories are good at explaining parental
solicitude, nepotism, economic prudence, and instincts for cheater-detection. However,
they leave most of human morality unexplained. Sexual selection provides a
complementary way of explaining how selfish genes can give rise to altruistic individuals.
41. Basically, the hidden genetic benefits of altruism could have been reproductive:
conspicuous magnanimity and other moral behaviors became sexually attractive
because they were good fitness indicators. Their reliability was guaranteed by the costs
of altruism, under the handicap principle. Only the fit could afford to be generous. Sexual
selection can favor almost any degree of generosity or heroism, despite their survival
costs, just as it can favor almost any length of peacock tail. Mate choice can work as a
moral filter from each generation to the next.
42. The evolution of big-game hunting provides one example of sexual selection for
magnanimity. Kristen Hawkes (1991) has argued that male hunting of large, dangerous
prey evolved not to 'feed one's family' (monogamous nuclear families being rare in the
Pleistocene), but to attract multiple female partners, who appreciated hunting ability as a
fitness indicator, and as a direct nutritional benefit to themselves and their offspring.
Anthropological data show that good hunters have more extra-pair copulations than poor
hunters.
43. The most complicated argument in the book concerns the evolution of moral displays
through an interaction between sexual selection for costly signals, and group selection
among alternative signalling equilibria. This relies on the recent evolutionary game
theory research on games with very large numbers of Nash equilibria. (At each
equilibrium, by definition, all individuals are acting rationally selfish -- the issue is which
equilibrium will be favored by evolution given competition between groups.) The
argument is too intricate to outline here, but it reaches the surprising conclusion that
evolution can favor precisely those sexual display equilibria that bring the highest group
benefits, without any conflict between individual-level selection and group-level selection
(see Boyd & Richerson, 1990). The relevance to human morality is that, given
competition between groups, sexual preferences will evolve to favor apparently altruistic
capacities for sympathy, magnanimity, group leadership, and punishment of cheaters --
rather than purely wasteful, non-altruistic displays such as the peacock's tail.
44. The chapter applies this logic to understand charitable behavior, male gift-giving
during courtship, the emotional capacities for sympathy and sexual fidelity, the sexual
aversion to psychopaths, the female preference for generous fathers and step-fathers,
and the capacity for sportsmanship (where cheating implies any behavior that lowers the
meritocratic correlation between a competitor's fitness and their success in the sport). It
concludes by urging evolutionary psychology to broaden its concept of human morality
from what Nietzsche called the Christian 'morality of the herd' (fairness, conscience,
equality, fidelity, altruism), to what he called the pagan virtues (e.g. bravery, beauty, skill,
leadership, stoicism, sacrifice, good manners).
X. CHAPTER 10: CYRANO AND SCHEHEREZADE.
45. This chapter addresses the evolution of language, emphasizing that if talking gives
away useful information to others, it raises the same evolutionary dilemma as altruism in
10
general -- a dilemma that, as with morality, can be resolved by reference to sexual
selection.
46. The debates over language's innateness and uniqueness have been resolved in
favor of Steven Pinker's (1994) view that language is a uniquely human, genetically-
based psychological adaptation that evolved for some set of biological functions. The
question remains: what functions? Most theories of language evolution do not identify
any specific selection pressures in favor of communication ability, and those that stress
survival benefits cannot explain why no other species evolved such a supposedly useful
ability. Moreover, most such theories fail to explain the selfish genetic benefits from the
apparently altruistic (information-giving) act of speaking, and fail to explain why people
compete to say things rather than to listen in group conversations.
47. Almost all complex acoustic signals in other species evolved as courtship displays
through sexual selection: frog croaks, bird song, whale song, etc. Human verbal
courtship (i.e. conversation between lovers) serves an analogous function, with mutual
advertisement of capacities for speaking, listening, thinking, remembering, story-telling,
and joke-making. Assuming they spoke three words a second for two hours a day during
courtship, with an average of three months of sex before a viable pregnancy, the
average hominid would have uttered a million words of verbal courtship before
reproducing. This million- word hurdle would have given prospective mates ample
opportunity to assess verbal ability, creativity, and intelligence, and to reject dumb,
boring mumblers. The importance of verbal courtship is exemplified by the legends of
Cyrano de Bergerac and Scheherezade, who provoked love through their poetic and
story-telling abilities.
48. In Turing's (1950) original 'imitation game', a male interrogator tries to determine
whether he is interacting via computer with a real woman, or a computer program that
imitates a woman. Turing assumed that verbal courtship (including capacities for making
jokes and composing poems) was the most challenging test for artificial intelligence, but
this sexual-selection aspect of the Turing test was stripped away by later AI researchers
as an irrelevant distraction.
49. Once language evolved, much more of our mental life became subject to sexual
selection. Verbal courtship could reveal whole new areas of mental functioning --
personality, intelligence, beliefs, desires, past experiences, future plans -- that are
hidden in the more physical courtship of other species. As language gave a clearer
window on the mind, the mind became more easily shaped by mate choice. This sexual
selection feedback loop between mate choice, language ability, and creative intelligence
was probably the mainspring of human mental evolution.
50. A detailed analysis of vocabulary size provides a quantitative case study of how
modern human language exceeds any plausible demands of survival and social
reciprocity. Our average 60,000 word vocabularies far exceed the 850-word vocabulary
of the artificial language 'Basic English', invented by I. A. Richards and C. K. Ogden in
the 1920s. Basic English, like most small-vocabulary pidgin languages, suffices for all
ordinary aspects of trade, cooperative work, and survival, including the exchange of
threats, promises, warnings, and news. Biology and astronomy textbooks have been
written in Basic English. This suggests most of our words are pragmatically redundant,
and must be serving some self-advertisement function. Since vocabulary size is highly
11
correlated with general intelligence, and is highly heritable, it appears to function as a
reliable indicator of heritable mental fitness. People are generally unaware of their
sexual preferences for large vocabularies (rare is the personal ad asking for a partner
who knows 50,000 useless synonyms), but assortative mating for vocabulary size is
higher than for almost any other mental trait.
XI. CHAPTER 11: THE WIT TO WOO.
51. The final chapter explores how evolution can favor benignly unpredictable behavior,
suggests hominids developed sexual preferences for interesting, funny mates over
boring mates, and proposes that these sexual preferences for unpredictable courtship
behavior drove the evolution of human creativity.
52. The earliest game theorists such as John von Neumann recognized that many
games require 'mixed strategies' -- strategies that randomize moves from one play to the
next. Unpredictability often brings strategic advantages. With the theory of 'protean
behavior', biologists independently developed the same principle in considering anti-
predator behavior: an unusual appearance and unpredictable evasion movements could
protect prey from being noticed and captured. The unpredictability of animal behavior
may reflect capacities for adaptively protean behavior, more than some side-effect of
neural noise or 'random error' (N. B. analysis of variance, psychology's favorite statistical
method, can't distinguish proteanism from noise).
53. With the evolution of 'Machiavellian intelligence' (the capacity for predicting and
manipulating the social behavior of conspecifics), primates would have been under
selection to be socially unpredictable to their reproductive competitors (Miller, 1997). The
unpredictability of primate aggression noticed by field researchers probably reflects a
mixed strategy. This sort of 'social proteanism' may have provided some genetic and
neurological foundations for human creativity, by endowing our brains with mechanisms
for randomizing social behaviors in general, which could be applied to the special case
of courtship, to produce amusingly unpredictable romantic behavior. Creativity could
have been favored initially as a reliable indicator of social proteanism ability, and of
youthfulness, insofar as juvenile primates tend to be more playful and unpredictable than
adults. In modern humans, creativity is also correlated highly with general intelligence,
and moderately with health and energy level; its heritability appears to be a side effect of
its correlation with intelligence, which is highly heritable.
54. Creative courtship may have also played upon neophilia, a fundamental attentional
and cognitive attraction to novelty. In terms of the psychological bias theory of sexual
selection, neophilia is just another bias that might influence mate choice. Darwin (1871)
argued that neophilia was an important factor in the diversification and rapid evolution of
bird song. Primate and human neophilia is especially strong, with boredom often cited as
a reason for terminating sexual relationships. Partners who offered more cognitive
variety and creativity in their relationships may have had longer, more reproductively
successful relationships -- as symbolized, again, by Scheherezade. A good sense of
humor is the most sexually attractive variety of creativity, and human mental evolution is
better imagined as a romantic comedy than as a story of disaster, warfare, predation,
and survival.
12
55. Sexual selection for creativity undermines some of the evolutionary epistemology
claims about the reliability of human knowledge. Whereas natural selection might tend to
favor minds with accurate, survival-enhancing world-models, sexual selection might
favor minds prone to inventing attractive, imaginative fantasies -- as long as fantasy-
invention ability remains a reliable fitness-indicator. (Brigham Young's religious visions
revealed his imagination and intelligence, and attracted his 27 wives, but that does not
guarantee the veracity of his belief that dead ancestors can be retroactively converted to
Mormonism.) Sexual selection rarely favors displays that include accurate
representations of the world -- peacock tail eye-spots catch attention by resembling
eyes, but they are not very good likenesses. Likewise for human ideologies: they may be
sexually attractive be revealing an individual's character, intelligence, and moral ideals,
but they need not be good representations of reality. Sexual selection can explain why
most people prefer fiction to non-fiction, religious myth to scientific evidence, and
political correctness to intellectual coherence.
XII. EPILOGUE.
56. The epilogue lists the theory's limitations, emphasizing that it is not a complete
theory of mental evolution, only a theory of the more distinctive, display-oriented human
capacities that have proven hard to explain in 'survival value' terms. It admits that my
ideas may have been unduly influenced in some cases by my being male, and calls for
further refinements by researchers of both sexes. Without committing the 'naturalistic
fallacy', it notes that debates about moral values and social ideals can and should be
informed by our concepts of human nature and human evolution -- especially debates
over educational policy, consumerism, political philosophy, and bioethics. It calls for the
conscious suppression of our instincts for discriminatory mate choice in social contexts
where such discrimination is inappropriate, and concludes with a hint about how sexual
selection may continue to shape human evolution in the future.
XIII. THE THEORY'S LIMITATIONS AND WEAKNESSES AND FURTHER
DIRECTIONS.
57. This final section goes beyond the book's contents somewhat to provide some
clarifications for Psycholoquy readers. First, the theory's limitations. 'The mating mind'
offers a snap-shot of a provisional theory under construction. It does not pretend to be a
complete account of human mental evolution, because it addresses only those
psychological adaptations manifest in sexual courtship. It accepts that natural selection
and other forms of social selection account for all the other adaptations for perception,
cognition, and movement, and all the other important domains of behavior such as
foraging, parenting, making friends, trading goods and services, and avoiding predators
and parasites. The theory does not apply to all the psychological adaptations that we
share with other vertebrates, mammals, primates, and apes. It does not address the
quandary of 'consciousness' or the dynamics of cultural change, though it might have
some bearing on these issues. It is best at explaining behaviors that young adults
display more than children or older people do, that men display more conspicuously than
women do, that high-fitness people display more than low-fitness people, that take lots
of energy, time, and skill, that reveal genetic quality, and that people cite as sexually
desirable traits. I leave it to others to discuss the existential and theological implications
of living in a lineage that directed its own evolution through its powers of mate choice.
13
58. One weakness of any theory that relies on sexual selection is that sexual selection
theory (like natural selection theory) is still very much under development, with debates
continuing about the heritability of fitness, the relative importance of indexes versus
handicaps, the dynamics of sexual selection given diverse preferences and multiple
ornaments, and the game-theoretic complexities of mating markets given mutual mate
choice. As the higher-level meta-theory of sexual selection develops, some of my mid-
level hypotheses about human mental evolution may turn out to be evolutionarily
implausible. Given the minimal sex differences in most human courtship abilities
discussed in the book, a high priority should be given to the development of better
models of sexual selection under mutual mate choice.
59. Another weakness concerns traits like language, that probably evolved through some
interplay of sexual selection and other forms of social selection (including group
selection between different evolutionary equilibria). Arguably, the theory over-
emphasizes the courtship functions of language and creative intelligence, while
neglecting their other fitness benefits in teaching children, making friends, helping kin,
trading services, making commitments, sustaining social norms, fighting other tribes, and
so forth. However, my intention was not to dismiss these other benefits, only to stress a
sexual-attraction benefit that had been neglected in previous theorizing.
60. A third weakness is the provisional nature of the reconstruction of hominid mating
patterns. This reconstruction is based on current data from archaeology, anthropology,
psychology, primatology, and genetics. However, today's data may be supplanted by
tomorrow's. Given the number of radical re-thinks that have occurred about prehistory
over the last several decades, it would be foolish to pretend that the current
reconstruction of Pleistocene mating is the last word on the subject. Nevertheless, the
requirements for sexual selection to influence the mind's evolution would remain valid
under a fairly wide range of mating patterns, because only perfectly random mating
would render mate choice irrelevant.
61. The book also has some pseudo-weaknesses that aren't really specific to this theory.
Evolutionary accounts of the most cherished human abilities are often criticized as
'genetic determinism' by those who wish to retain the mystique of the humanities and the
performing arts. Such criticisms are generic to all adaptationist accounts of human
creativity, and are especially inappropriate to this sexual selection account, which
emphasizes the feedback loop between psychology and biology via mate choice, and
sexual selection's ability to favor non-deterministic, unpredictable, creative forms of
courtship.
62. Another pseudo-weakness is that the theory only has patchy evidence in its support
at present. Any evolutionary theory that relies on existing data can be dismissed as a
'Just-so story' that just offers post-hoc interpretations of known facts; whereas any that
sticks its neck out and makes novel predictions not yet supported by evidence can be
dismissed as 'speculative'. Obviously, the best theories must rely on some evidence and
make some predictions, so may appear both post-hoc and speculative. That quandary is
generic to all scientific theories, from physics through psychology. The real issue is
testability (a broader, more realistic, Lakatosian version of Popper's falsifiability criterion),
and fortunately, the last two decades of biological research on animal mate choice have
produced ever-better methodologies for testing hypotheses about sexual selection,
which can also be applied to humans. Likewise for genetic and neuroscientific evidence
14
supporting the existence of these courtship adaptations: most senior scientists with
power to allocate time on the relevant lab equipment won't bother looking for such
evidence until someone points out the possible scientific benefits of doing so.
63. Much more empirical research is needed on mate choice for mental traits among
humans across diverse cultures, ranging from tribal hunter-gatherers to affluent
Westerners. We need more behavior- genetic work on the inheritance of mate
preferences and courtship abilities, and the heritability of variance in those adaptations.
We need more developmental psychology work on the emergence of these adaptations
over the life-course, and their facultative sensitivity to changes in personal mate value,
mating markets, and parental responsibilities. The hypothesis that there is a general
fitness factor (superordinate to both the g factor and general physical health) needs to
be tested with large, representative datasets that include psychometric, anthropometric,
health, social status, and other diverse measures of fitness-relevant traits. More data is
needed on the metabolic costs (e.g. glucose burn rates) of different mental activities, to
see whether courtship draws disproportionately on the costliest. Cognitive neuroscience
could try to localize courtship adaptations in the brain. Most importantly, we need much
better observations concerning real-life human courtship, including the measurable
aspects of courtship that influence mate choice, the reproductive (or at least sexual)
consequences of individual variation in those aspects, and the social-cognitive and
emotional mechanisms of falling in love.
REFERENCES
Boas, Franz (1955). Primitive art. New York: Dover
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1990). Group selection among alternative evolutionarily
stable strategies. J. Theoretical Biology, 145, 331-342.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate selection: Evolutionary hypotheses
tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1-49.
Cronin, H. (1991). The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual selection from Darwin
to today. Cambridge U. Press.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2 vols.). London:
John Murray. (Reprinted in 1981 by Princeton U. Press.)
Eyre-Walker, A., & Keightley, P. D. (1999). High genomic deleterious mutation rates in
hominids. Nature, 397, 344-346.
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of another hypothesis about men's foraging goals.
Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 29-54.
Miller, G. F. (1993). Evolution of the human brain through runaway sexual selection: The
mind as a protean courtship device. Ph.D. thesis, Psychology Department, Stanford
University.
15
Miller, G. F. (1997). Protean primates: The evolution of adaptive unpredictability in
competition and courtship. In A. Whiten & R. W. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence
II, pp. 312-340. Cambridge University Press.
Miller, G. F. (1999). Sexual selection for cultural displays. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, & C.
Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture, pp. 71- 91. Edinburgh U. Press.
Miller, G. F. (2000). Mental traits as fitness indicators: Expanding evolutionary
psychology's adaptationism. Annals of the. New York Academy of Sciences, 907.
Miller, G. F. (in press). Sexual selection for indicators of intelligence. For J. Goode (Ed.),
The nature of intelligence. Novartis Foundation Symposium 233. John Wiley, pp. 270-
275.
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. London: Allen Lane.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.
Turing, A. M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 433-460.
Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class. New York: Macmillan. Reprinted by
Dover.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. J. Theoretical Biology, 53,
16
... Some others suggest that artistic capacities may also have sexual value, enhancing mating attraction, mate retention, or competition for mates (see below). Art would then be a reproductive adaptation rather than a survival one (Darwin, 1871;Miller, 2011;Zahavi & Zahavi, 1999). ...
... While the decorations themselves have no direct survival value for the males, they function as an extended phenotype, signaling their fitness and aiding in attracting females (Kelley & Endler, 2012). Consequently, the trait of nest decoration is sexually selected for within the species (Driscoll, 2006;Miller, 2011). Similarly, while artistic works like paintings and sculptures may lack immediate practical use, they demand ingenuity, dexterity, and creativity-traits that can signal fitness (for a broader understanding of the phenomenon and other theoretical approaches, see Davis & Arnocky, 2022;Prum, 2012;Verpooten & Nelissen, 2012). ...
... It is perhaps surprising that, despite many publications in this area (e.g., DeLecce et al., 2022;Karamihalev, 2013;Miller, 2001Miller, , 2011Novaes & Natividade, 2023;Varella et al., 2022;Voland & Grammer, 2003), there is a lack of research explicitly testing the adaptive role of practicing art and its impact on an artist's potential reproductive success. Nonetheless, there is limited evidence that suggests (at least in Western communities) that more creative individuals or artists have more sexual partners (Beaussart et al., 2012;Clegg et al., 2011;Lange & Euler, 2014) and that women perceive artistically talented men (not necessarily artists) as more desirable partners (Clegg et al., 2008;Kaufman et al., 2016;Varella et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Human fascination with art has deep evolutionary roots, yet its role remains a puzzle for evolutionary theory. Although its widespread presence across cultures suggests a potential adaptive function, determining its evolutionary origins requires more comprehensive evidence beyond mere universality or assumed survival benefits. This paper introduces and tests the Collector Hypothesis, which suggests that artworks serve as indicators of collectors’ surplus wealth and social status, offering greater benefits to collectors than to artists in mating and reproductive contexts. Our study among Indigenous Papuan communities provides preliminary support for the Collector Hypothesis, indicating that, compared to artists, collectors are perceived as having higher social status and greater attractiveness to women. These findings provide unique insights into Papuan communities and contribute to the ongoing discussion about art’s adaptive significance of art by suggesting that artistic capacities may benefit not only creators but also those who accumulate and display art. Further research in diverse cultural contexts is needed for a comprehensive understanding of this interplay.
... Some approaches assume that art can be characterised by aesthetics such as beauty, talent, and originality (Dutton, 2009;Miller, 2000). Developing computational approaches for quantifying and predicting the values of concepts such as aesthetics is difficult for art because the importance and originality (and therefore also our perception) of art greatly depend on its historical context (Cetinic & She, 2022). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis research art behaviors and their expression when artists work with ai image generators There are several different ways to look at art. In general, art is (or perhaps used to be) considered exclusively human. We expect it to encapsulate our emotions and express our experiences, which we deem human-specific (Chatterjee, 2014). Our ancestors were already engaging in artistic manifestations long before society as we know it existed. Where does art originate? Is it really exclusively human? Some approaches to the study of art argue that art is defined by the individual receiving it as such. This is the Reception theory. In this framework, the context the audience grants to a work made by AI determines the surrounding sentiments (Gombrich, 1995). One can also approach art as any work considered art by institutions. According to this theory, institutions (such as prestigious museums) control what we see as art (Dickie, 1974). both the institutional and the receptive point of view, art is defined as a kind of label that can be applied to any work. Unfortunately, audiences can be divided, and institutions can disagree. Cognitive science is now developing a more contemporary perspective on art by approaching art by the process. In other words, in cognitive theory, an object is art when it is made as such. With the current technological advancements of AI image generators, the most significant difference between AI-generated work and man-made work is the process. Therefore, the cognitive approach could be a good asset for separating AI and human creativity. This will be the focus of this thesis. Could cognitive theory shed new light on AI and create a distinction between human creativity and AI-generated images? What the art-making process entails exactly is a subject that still needs much research and has only recently gained more focus academically. For example, by considering art as a behaviour. This brings us to the main question: What are art behaviours from a cognitive science perspective, and are these behaviours present when producing works with generative image AI? To properly answer this, I interviewed artists to see what aspects of art behaviours they express when working with AI versus when working with their preferred traditional media. This way, the expression of art behaviour and their extent can be seen by comparing the two interviews. This could provide new insights into what differentiates human-made works from AI-generated images. In order to do this, I will first answer the following sub-questions: 1. What are art behaviours? 2. Can AI-generated images be seen as art from a cognitive science approach? 3. What type of art behaviours are present in the experience of an artist when generating images? Chapter 1 explains what art behaviours are exactly, where they originate, and discusses the Artification theory. I propose the following aspects that are inherent to art behaviour: - a feeling of expressing a part of yourself or your experiences through imagination - a lack of functional goals or purpose. - a feeling of emotional involvement - a conflicting process where the end product differs from the original aim - possibly, a feeling of originality and value I will ask the interviewees about these aspects to see if they express art behaviour when working with AI. In Chapter 2, I will discuss how (generative image) AI works and how different studies of art perceive AI. Do they consider generated AI images creative? Here, I explain that AI images are not perceived equally to human works, and that the cognitive approach quickly dismisses AI as ‘never creative’ because it lacks the process. However, artists might experience art behaviours when working with generative AI. If there is a clear difference between showing art behaviour when working with AI works and with traditional works, this could explain the anthropocentric bias surrounding AI works. Chapter 3 will present the research's methodology/operationalisation and bring together chapters 1 and 2. I will try to answer the question: What type of art behaviours are present in an artist's experience when generating images? For this, I interviewed three participants twice: once while working with AI (Dall-E2) and once while working with traditional media. I conclude that most aspects of art behaviours discussed tend to be more present when working with the preferred medium than when working with AI. If anything, AI shows us that the study of art is far from dichotomous and still has anthropo-centrically biased tendencies. With this thesis, I aim to find aspects of art behaviour that are permanent, essential, or characteristic attributes of art behaviours and to what degree they are expressed when working with AI. I aim to contribute new insights into the ongoing discussion regarding AI from a critical and new standpoint. Eventually, I hope to add a contemporary perspective to the infamous discussion of what we call art and whether this is inherently human.
... Altruism is a well-established example of intersexual competition, evolved through sexual selection (Arnocky et al., 2017;Farrelly, 2011). Sexual selection and altruism are observed in heterosexual contexts, with theories being developed, describing how altruism is an evolved trait, used as a mating signal (Miller, 2000). This is supported by research showing that those who display altruistic behaviours are seen as desirable when evaluating a potential mate (Farrelly, 2011;Phillips et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
The role of altruism in mate choice is a well-researched area in evolutionary psychology. Same-sex sexual attraction (SSSA) is an under-researched area and is not covered in research exploring the role of altruism in mate choice. To begin addressing this gap, we explored the effect of sexual orientation on preferences for altruism in mate choice. We recruited heterosexual and homosexual participants (N = 321) to complete two scales assessing attractiveness of a potential partner who completed altruistic actions and secondly, participants were asked to rate how altruistic they are on an individual basis. There was no significant effect of sexual orientation on the desirability of altruism in a potential mate, demonstrating that those who act altruistic are desirable, regardless of sexual orientation. However, gender and self-rated altruism were found to have a statistically significant effect on the desirability of a potential mate. Limitations and future directions of research are also discussed.
... One discussed option is that an altruistic individual has a high genetic quality that can be inherited by the offspring (e.g. Farrelly, 2011;Miller, 2000Miller, , 2007Oda et al., 2014;Van Vugt & Iredale, 2013), yet there is a lack of evidence for this. Instead, there is some indirect support that parental care is the trait being signalled (e.g. ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies in humans have shown that altruism may influence mate choice decisions, highlighting its potential role as a sexual signal. Whether using altruism as a signal is an adaptation, however, remains largely unresolved. In this review, we aim to investigate the adaptive nature of altruism as a sexual signal by constructing a comprehensive nomological network of evidence. Drawing upon interdisciplinary scientific modes of evidence (psychology, medicine, physiology, genetics, phylogenetics, hunter-gatherer studies, cross-cultural studies, and theoretical studies), our analysis reveals an extensive breadth of evidence across the various disciplines supporting the hypothesis that altruism is an adaptive sexual signal. However, only the psychological evidence demonstrates substantial depth. Other fields exhibit a lack of variation in research methods or a minimal amount of studies, limiting the strength of their respective contributions. Despite these limitations, there is still sufficient support for the nomological network. Based on the evidence of the various disciplines, we conclude that there is sufficient support that altruism as a sexual signal is an adaptation. We provide considerations for future research that would expand the scope of the evidentiary depth across the various disciplines, which would strengthen our understanding of the role of sexual selection in human altruism.
... According to e.g. Miller (2000) and Franks and Rigby (2005: 208), human mate selection often involves the display of cognitive abilities through a creative, extravagant use of language (see also "decorative" language, in the sense of Haiman 2013). Ideophones certainly show such extravagance. ...
Chapter
This chapter is part of a larger project, which purports to establish that language evolution involved a complex gene-culture feedback loop among several emerging phenomena that reinforced each other, including (i) advent of cognitive modernity (crucially entailing cross-modal thinking, aka cognitive fluidity); (ii) the beginnings of grammar; (iii) the beginnings of pragmatics; and (iv) the beginnings of self-domestication (henceforth, HSD), or, more neutrally, the management of aggression, including a trend towards prosociality (Benítez-Burraco et al., 2021). Here we focus on the linguistic aspects and details of this proposal, i.e. on the nature and uses of this early language which was an essential component of the feedback loop. In our proposal (Progovac and Benítez-Burraco, 2019), these early uses of language were mostly expressive, rather than propositional, and they were characterized by simple, rudimentary grammars, correlating with crude pragmatic abilities, including crude persuasive abilities. In this respect, our proposal advocates pragmatic motivations for the emergence and entrenchment of early grammars, as well as for later complexification of grammars, with grammatical and pragmatic abilities also engaged in a mutually reinforcing feedback loop. Here we consider expressive language on the side of social interaction (insult and swearing in the context of conflict management), but also on the side of cognition (i.e. cross-modal thinking, here understood as the ability to merge basic perceptive and conceptual primitives, with the aim of vividly construing how the world is). We also discuss the neurobiological mechanisms by which these early expressive uses of language, entangled in a feedback loop with human self-domestication, would have contributed to the gradual enhancement of cross-modality, the gradual complexification of grammars, the gradual emergence of sophisticated uses of language, and the gradual suppression of reactive aggression.
... En este sentido, la competencia literaria sería una señal de fitness que jugaría un rol en la selección de pareja. En esta vertiente, que hace de la competencia literaria un activo para la selección sexual se encuentra la propuesta de Geoffrey Miller (2000), a la que se añade posteriormente Denis Dutton (2010, 209 y ss.). Dutton recurre tanto a la teoría de las señales de costo elevado (Zahavi y Zahavi 1999) como al análisis del lujo desarrollado por Thorstein Veblen en su Teoría de la clase ociosa para sostener que la competencia literaria constituye una señal fiable, por su coste, de la valía de su emisor en el contexto de la selección sexual. ...
Article
Full-text available
A partir de 2009, el darwinismo literario adquirió un creciente reconocimiento en las universidades estadounidenses y en países de habla inglesa. Esta nueva corriente de teoría y crítica literaria apenas ha sido recibida en España. Por una parte, este artículo expone sus principales presupuestos teóricos y ofrece una crítica del empleo espurio que realiza de la teoría de la evolución de Darwin. Por otra, ofrece una explicación de su nacimiento y auge poniéndola en relación con los conflictos normativos que desató la querella por el canon en la década de los noventa. Por último, se plantea una defensa de los abordajes naturalistas de los hechos literarios que no incurran en el reduccionismo funcionalista que lastra el darwinismo literario.
... Altruism is a well-established example of intersexual competition, evolved through sexual selection (Farrelly, 2011;Arnocky et al., 2017). Sexual selection and altruism are observed in heterosexual contexts, with theories being developed, describing how altruism is an evolved trait, used as a mating signal (Miller, 2000). This is supported by research in that those who display altruistic behaviours are seen as desirable when evaluating a potential mate (Phillips et al., 2008;Farrelly, 2011). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The role of altruism in mate choice is a well-researched area in evolutionary psychology. Same-Sex Sexual Attraction (SSSA) is an under-researched area and is not covered in research exploring the role of altruism in mate choice. This study explored the effect of sexual orientation on preferences for altruism in mate choice. We recruited heterosexual and homosexual participants (N=321) to complete two scales assessing attractiveness of a potential partner who completed altruistic actions and secondly, participants were asked to rate how altruistic they are on an individual basis. There was no significant interaction of sexual orientation on the desirability of altruism in a potential mate, demonstrating that those who act altruistic are desirable, regardless of sexual orientation. However, gender and self-rated altruism were found to have a statistically significant effect on the desirability of a potential mate. Limitations and future directions of research are also discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The study aims to uncover the relationship and influence between strategic physiognomy through its dimensions (empowerment, inspiration, deep understanding) and the reputation of the organization through (creativity, social responsibility, service quality), through the interactive role of human resource management practices through (recruitment, employment, planning Human Resources, Development and Training). The of Methodology Study:-The study problem was identified in several questions centered around the nature of the role represented by the correlation and influence between the study variables, and for this, a hypothetical outline of the study was developed that expresses three main hypotheses, and in order to ensure the validity of the hypotheses, all of them underwent multiple tests to ensure their validity. The study used a questionnaire as a way to obtain data.The Approach of Study:-he study used the descriptive and analytical method as the method of the study. The study population identified (520) workers at Al-Kafeel Hospital in the Karbala Governorate. The study applied a random sample to select (175) employees and achieved the sample.A percentage (33%) of the total study population. The data were analyzed using a set of statistical methods (factor analysis, Kendall correlation factor, simple and multiple analysis factor, and Kruskal-Wills analysis) and the results were extracted using the computer program (pct v.23 SPSS).The of Conclusions Study:-The ability of Al-Kafeel Hospital employeesat their various job levels has led to the promotion of strategic firadness, which represents a major factor in successful organizations across multiple professional specialties, and helps the organization's management to think dynamically and take a closer look at the wise issues that affect the daily management of the organization, and thus have a role in Achieving the organizational reputation that builds a positive image in the imagination of the stakeholders dealing with the organization, as it guarantees continuity and survival through the interactive role of human resource management practices represented by the interest of the top management in attracting workers with distinguished skills and experiences according to clear and solid plans as well assetting development and training programs. The study to a set of conclusions demonstrated the existence of an impact of empowerment and a deep understanding only among the variables of strategic firadit in achieving the reputation of the organization and there is no effect of the rest of the variables, which means confirming the need to attend human resource management practices represented in recruitment, recruitment, human resource planning, development and training as well as the decisions of administrative leaders The organization researched as the cornerstone of the ng These practices and their interactive role in influencing the behavior and decisions of the hospital. The researched organization must crystallize its work behaviors according to the strategic variables of Al-Firaasa as one of the most important qualifications necessary to predict levels of improvement in indicators and achieve the reputation of the organization.Key Word: Strategic physiognomy, empowerment, inspiration, deep understanding, reputation of the organization, creativity, social responsibility, service quality, human resource management practices, recruitment and recruitment, human resource planning, development and training
Article
A tanulmány a humor és a nevetés sokrétű szerepét vizsgálja antropológiai kontextusban, feltárva, hogy ezek a jelenségek miként tárják fel a társadalmi struktúrákat és a kulturális gyakorlatokat. A humor, mint az emberi tapasztalat univerzális aspektusa, betekintést nyújt egy közösség mélyebb működési módjaiba, tükrözve a kulturális konstrukciók és az emberi sokféleség összetett kölcsönhatását.
Article
Full-text available
الغرض تهدف الدراسة إلى الكشف عن العلاقة والتأثير بين الفراسة الإستراتيجية من خلال أبعادها (التمكين، الإلهام، الفهم العميق ) وسمعة المنظمة من خلال (الإبداع ، المسؤولية الاجتماعية ,جودة الخدمة)، من خلال الدور التفاعلي لممارسات إدارة الموارد البشرية من خلال (الاستقطاب والتوظيف، تخطيط الموارد البشرية، التنمية والتدريب). منهجية الدراسة حددت مشكلة الدراسة في عدة تساؤلات تمحورت حول طبيعة الدور المتمثل بالارتباط والتأثير بين متغيرات الدراسة، ولأجل ذلك وضع مخطط فرضي للدراسة يعبر عن ثلاث فرضيات رئيسة، ولأجل التأكد من صحة الفرضيات خضعت جميعها لاختبارات متعددة للتأكد من مدى صحتها . وقد استعملت الدراسة الاستبانه وسيلة للحصول على البيانات . منهج الدراسة استعملت الدراسة المنهج الوصفي التحليلي كمنهج للدراسة . وتحدد مجتمع الدراسة (520) من العاملين في مستشفى الكفيل في محافظة كربلاء ، وطبقت الدراسة العينة العشوائية في اختيار (175) موظفا وحققت العينة نسبة (33%) من مجتمع الدراسة الكلي . وقد تم تحليل البيانات باستخدام مجموعة من الأساليب الإحصائية ( التحليل العاملي، معامل ارتباط كندال، معامل التحليل البسيط والمتعدد، وتحليل كروسكال وايلز ) واستخرجت النتائج باستخدام البرنامج الحاسوبي (pct v.23 SPSS). استنتاجات الدراسة : أدت قدرة العاملين في مستشفى الكفيل بمختلف مستوياتهم الوظيفية إلى تعزيز الفراسة الإستراتيجية التي تمثل عامل رئيس في المنظمات الناجحة عبر تخصصات مهنية متعددة، وتساعد إدارة المنظمة على التفكير بشكل ديناميكي وأخذ نظرة فاحصة على القضايا الحكيمة التي تؤثر على الإدارة اليومية للمنظمة، وبالتالي يكون لها دور في تحقيق السمعة التنظيمية التي تبني صورة إيجابية في مخيلة أصحاب المصلحة المتعاملين مع المنظمة حيث يضمن لها الاستمرارية والبقاء من خلال الدور التفاعلي لممارسات إدارة الموارد البشرية المتمثلة باهتمام الإدارة العليا على جذب العاملين من ذوي المهارات والخبرات المتميزة وفق خطط واضحة ورصينة وكذلك وضع برامج للتنمية والتدريب، وتوصلت الدراسة إلى مجموعة من الاستنتاجات أثبتت وجود تأثير للتمكين والفهم العميق فقط من بين متغيرات الفراسة الإستراتيجية في تحقيق سمعة المنظمة ولا يوجد تأثير لبقية المتغيرات، مما يعني تأكيد الحاجة إلى حضور ممارسات إدارة الموارد البشرية المتمثلة بالاستقطاب والتوظيف وتخطيط الموارد البشرية والتنمية والتدريب وكذلك قرارات القيادات الإدارية في المنظمة المبحوثة بوصفها حجر الزاوية في نجاح تلك الممارسات ودورها التفاعلي في التأثير على سلوكيات وقرارات المستشفى. وعلى المنظمة المبحوثة أن تبلور سلوكيات عملها وفقاً لمتغيرات الفراسة الإستراتيجية بوصفها أحد أهم المؤهلات الضرورية للتنبؤ بمستويات التحسين في مؤشرات وتحقيق سمعة المنظمة.
Article
Full-text available
Many important models of the evolution of social behavior have more than one evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS). Examples include co-ordination games, contests, mutualism, reciprocity, and sexual selection. Here we show that when there are multiple evolutionarily stable strategies, selection among groups can cause the spread of the strategy that has the lowest extinction rate or highest probability of contributing to the colonization of empty habitats, and that this may occur even when groups are usually very large, migration rates are substantial, and “extinction” entails only the disruption of the group and the dispersal of its members. The main requirements are: (1) individuals drawn from a single surviving group make up a sufficiently large fraction newly formed groups, and (2) the processes increasing the frequency of successful strategies within groups are strong compared to rate of migration among groups. The latter condition suggests that this form of group selection will be particularly important when behavioral variation is culturally acquired.
Article
Full-text available
It is suggested that characters which develop through mate preference confer handicaps on the selected individuals in their survival. These handicaps are of use to the selecting sex since they test the quality of the mate. The size of characters selected in this way serve as marks of quality. The understanding that a handicap, which tests for quality, can evolve as a consequence of its advantage to the individual, may provide an explanation for many puzzling evolutionary problems. Such an interpretation may provide an alternative to other hypotheses which assumed complicated selective mechanisms, such as group selection or kin selection, which do not act directly on the individual.
Article
Full-text available
Costly signaling theory (CST) offers an explanation of generosity and collective action that contrasts sharply with explanations based on conditional reciprocity. This makes it particularly relevant to situations involving widespread unconditional provisioning of collective goods. We provide a preliminary application of CST to ethnographic data on turtle hunting and public feasting among the Meriam of Torres Strait, Australia. Turtle hunting appears to meet the key conditions specified in CST: it is (1) an honest signal of underlying abilities such as strength, risk-taking, skill, and leadership; (2) costly in ways not subject to reciprocation; (3) an effective means of broadcasting signals, since the collective good (a feast) attracts a large audience; and (4) seems to provide benefits to signalers (turtle hunters) as well as recipients (audience). We conclude with some suggestions as to the broader implications of this research, and the costly signaling paradigm in general, for understanding collective action and generosity in human social groups.
Chapter
I propose to consider the question, “Can machines think?”♣ This should begin with definitions of the meaning of the terms “machine” and “think”. The definitions might be framed so as to reflect so far as possible the normal use of the words, but this attitude is dangerous. If the meaning of the words “machine” and “think” are to be found by examining how they are commonly used it is difficult to escape the conclusion that the meaning and the answer to the question, “Can machines think?” is to be sought in a statistical survey such as a Gallup poll.
Article
It is widely assumed that among hunter-gatherers, men work to provision their families. However, men may have more to gain by giving food to a wide range of companions who treat them favorably in return. If so, and if some resources better serve this end, men's foraging behavior should vary accordingly. Aspects of this hypothesis are tested on observations of food acquisition and sharing among Ache foragers of Eastern Paraguay. Previous analysis showed that different Ache food types were differently shared. Resources shared most widely were game animals. Further analysis and additional data presented here suggest a causal association between the wide sharing of game and the fact that men hunt and women do not. Data show that men preferentially target resources in both hunting and gathering which are more widely shared, resources more likely to be consumed outside their own nuclear families. These results have implications for 1) the identification of male reproductive trade-offs in human societies, 2) the view that families are units of common interest integrated by the sexual division of labor, 3) current reconstructions of the evolution of foraging and food sharing among early hominids, and 4) assessments of the role of risk and reciprocity in hunter-gatherer foraging strategies.
Article
It has been suggested that humans may suffer a high genomic deleterious mutation rate. Here we test this hypothesis by applying a variant of a molecular approach to estimate the deleterious mutation rate in hominids from the level of selective constraint in DNA sequences. Under conservative assumptions, we estimate that an average of 4.2 amino-acid-altering mutations per diploid per generation have occurred in the human lineage since humans separated from chimpanzees. Of these mutations, we estimate that at least 38% have been eliminated by natural selection, indicating that there have been more than 1.6 new deleterious mutations per diploid genome per generation. Thus, the deleterious mutation rate specific to protein-coding sequences alone is close to the upper limit tolerable by a species such as humans that has a low reproductive rate, indicating that the effects of deleterious mutations may have combined synergistically. Furthermore, the level of selective constraint in hominid protein-coding sequences is atypically low. A large number of slightly deleterious mutations may therefore have become fixed in hominid lineages.