ArticlePDF Available

Materialism among Adolescents in Urban China

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Looks at attitudes of Chinese adolescents to materialism, including the effect of age on materialism and the influence of family and peers. Outlines the values of Chinese culture: thrift, respect for parents, group orientation, social harmony, good manners, face, and academic achievement; these values could impact both positively and negatively on endorsement of materialistic values. Points out that parental expectations of their children’s material success have increased since the one child per family policy. Finds that older adolescents were more materialistic than younger ones, that more materialistic adolescents tended to communicate more with their peers and less with their parents, and that television (which now reaches 92 per cent of households) has no effect because the Chinese government’s strict rules about TV programmes’ content requires them to reflect traditional values.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Materialism 1
Materialism among adolescents in urban China
Keywords: materialism--China—adolescents--survey
Dr. Kara Chan*
Professor
Department of Communication Studies
Hong Kong Baptist University
Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong
Fax: (852) 3411-7890
Telephone: (852) 3411-7836
E-mail: karachan@hkbu.edu.hk
Dr. Hongxia Zhang
Associate Professor
Guanghua School of Management
Peking University
hxzhang@gsm.pku.edu.cn
Iris C. Wang
MA Graduate
Guanghua School of Management
Peking University
Paper published in
Young Consumers
7(2), 64-77
YC 2006 adol mat.doc
Materialism 2
Advertising exposure and materialism among adolescents in urban China
Abstract
This study provides empirical data to test a proposed theoretical model on adolescents’
endorsement of materialistic values that is based on John’s (1999) model of consumer
socialization of children and Kasser, Rayan Couchman and Sheldon’s (2004) model of
materialistic value orientation. A survey of 730 adolescents aged 11 to 19 in Beijing was
conducted in March 2003 using Richins and Dawson’s (1992) materialism scale. Results
indicated that Chinese young people scored 3.9 on a 7-point scale of materialism. The
proposed model accounted for 15 percent of the variance of adolescents’ materialism scores.
Hierarchical regression analysis indicated that motives of advertising viewing and peer
communication about consumption were more important in predicting materialism than age
and family communication. Adolescents who reported high level of social motives for ad
viewing and adolescents who communicate frequently with peers about consumption were
more materialistic. Level of television viewing and gender had no effect on materialism when
other variables were controlled. (156 words)
Materialism 3
INTRODUCTION
One concern about consumer socialization is the undesirable influence of advertising on
young people’s preference for material goods as a means of achieving success, happiness, and
self-fulfillment. The adoption of materialistic values among young people will affect the balance
between private and public choices children make throughout life (Goldberg, Gorn, Peracchio &
Bomossy, 2003). Longitudinal studies among U.S. high school students from early 1970s to
1980s indicate a dramatic increase in private materialism as a life goal and a sharp decline in
emphasis on personal self-fulfillment (Easterlin & Crimmins, 1991). Materialism has been treated
as a negative value, connected to possessiveness, envy, lack of generosity, greed and jealousy
(Belk, 1983). Developmental psychologists, marketers, and policy makers are interested in
understanding how children develop materialistic values with age, and what factors are related to
the adoption of materialistic values.
With the adoption of the ‘Open Policy’ in the late 1970s, Mainland China regained its
entrepreneurship and consequently consumer culture had undergone rapid changes (Tse, Belk &
Zhou, 1989). The current study will help us to examine how adolescents in China endorse these
new materialist value orientations. Adolescents are defined as people aged 10 to 19 (World
Health Organization, 2005). There are 354 million young people under 18 in China, comprising
over one quarter of the total population. Due to the one-child policy, Chinese children and
adolescents have become the focal point of the family, exerting tremendous influence on
household purchases (McNeal & Yeh, 1997). Children and adolescents in China are
increasingly exposed to mass media for information and entertainment. A national survey of
3,284 grade 5-9 urban school children found that they spent the most time reading books and
watching television (Bu, 2001). A survey of grade 4-6 urban Chinese children found they used a
variety of information sources to learn about new products, including parents, retail outlets, and
Materialism 4
the mass media, and they considered television to be the most important medium (McNeal & Ji,
1999).
Television now reaches over 92 percent of China’s households (Bu, 2001). A typical
television schedule for the national television channel, China Central Television (CCTV) in
January 2003 shows that programming for children and adolescents consists of one to two hours
of cartoon entertainment in the morning, and one to two hours of entertainment after school. On
average the national channels carry about 9.5 hours and the local channels carry about one hour
of programs for children and adolescents every day. Of the ten hours of television programs for
young audiences each day, about 80 percent target elementary school children and 11 percent
target at secondary school students. CCTV launched a children and youth channel in December
2003 that carried 18 hours of broadcasting every day (Yu, 2004). Most of these television
programs were locally produced, with few imported programs for children and adolescents.
Children aged 12 and below liked to watch cartoons, children’s drama series, games shows and
variety shows. Adolescents, on the other hand, showed more interest in sports programs and
news programs (Bu, 1998). Adolescents reported that they watched television mainly for getting
news and for companionship (Bu, 2001).
A central factor in children’s consumer socialization comes from culture. In the case of
young children, the socialization process includes instilling in children the basic values and
modes of appropriate behavior consistent with the host culture. Many children acquire consumer
behavior norms by observing their parents and siblings, who function as role models.
Adolescents, however, were more likely to look to their peers for models of acceptable
consumption behaviors (John, 1999). Many scholars argue that mass consumption in China is
different from Western societies because of China’s long-standing values concerning families
and human relations (Zhao, 1997). Characteristics of the Chinese culture include a strong
cultivated habit of thrift, filial piety, group orientation, social harmony, good manners, face, and
Materialism 5
emphasis on academic achievements (Chan & McNeal, 2003a; Yau, 1988). Traditional Chinese
values suggest that they might impact both positively and negatively on the endorsement of
materialistic values. For example, values such as thrift and frugality will discourage the
possession of luxury goods while the value of face will encourage the owning of the same
material goods that other people have. Because of the one child per family policy, parents have
high expectation of their children. Born in the years of severe material shortage, Chinese parents
want to compensate for their lost past by providing a material-rich future to their second
generation (Zhao, 1996). Peer communication is important to Chinese adolescents because, in
the absence of siblings, they seek approval from peers.
At issue then, is whether Chinese children become more materialistic when they grow up.
Other questions include: how does television viewing and advertising exposure influence
adolescents’ materialistic values?; what are the factors influencing adolescents’ endorsement of
materialistic values? In the current study, adolescents’ endorsement of materialistic values is
viewed as a combination of a cognitive-psychological process of cognitive development as well
as a psychological response to insecurity and social learning.
The objectives of this study are:
a) to study whether Chinese adolescents endorse materialistic values;
b) to examine whether materialism changes with age during adolescence;
c) to examine the influences of family and peer communication, and media consumption on
adolescents’ materialistic value orientations.
The theoretical contribution of the current study is to enhance our understanding of
materialism and its relationship to demographic, socio-economic, and media factors. In addition,
educators and policy makers who are interested in social services marketing (for example,
encouraging young people to participate in volunteer activity) will benefit from this study. Much
research has been done on materialism and adolescents in the U.S. since the 1980s, but there are
Materialism 6
very few similar studies conducted in the Chinese context and the current study attempts to fill
this gap.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Theoretical framework and hypotheses
The current study adopts John’s (1999) model of consumer socialization of children and Kasser,
Ryan, Couchman and Sheldon’s (2004) model of materialistic value orientation. The former model
suggests that young people of different ages adopt different consumption motives and values. The
second model suggests that consumers (including adolescents) developed materialistic value
orientation through experience that induces feelings of insecurity and from exposure to materialistic
models and values. When individual’s psychological needs are not met, they will tend to move
toward materialism as a type of compensatory strategy to lessen the distressing effects of feelings of
insecurity. Individuals also learn to adopt materialistic values through social learning from family
members, peers, and the materialistic messages frequently found in television programs and
commercials (Kasser et al., 2004). Figure 1 shows the theoretical framework.
Age. Integrating Piaget’s theory of cognitive development (1970) and Selman’s theory of social
developments (1980), John (1999) proposes a model of consumer socialization. In the model,
consumer socialization is viewed as a developmental process that proceeds through different stages
as children mature into adult consumers. During the perceptual stage (ages 3-7), children are
characterized by a general orientation toward the immediate and readily observable perceptual
features of the marketplace. The value of possessions is based on surface features, such as having
more of something. During the analytical stage (ages 7-11), children are characterized by mastering
some consumer knowledge and skills. Concepts such as product categories or prices are thought of
in terms of functional or underlying dimensions. Children begin to understand the value of
possessions based on social meaning and significance. As the children enter the reflective stage
Materialism 7
(ages 11-16), they possess comprehensive knowledge about marketplace concepts such as branding
and pricing. They fully understand the value of possessions based on social meaning, significance,
and scarcity. Chan (2003) examined 246 Chinese children aged six to thirteen in Hong Kong and
found that they do not endorse strong materialistic values. Contrary to John’s (1999) model of
consumer socialization, even the youngest children aged six to seven demonstrated an
understanding of the value of possessions based on social significance. John’s (1999) model does
not specify whether materialism increases with age when children reach the reflective age. In the
current study, we hypothesize that age has positive impact on materialism (H1). This is because
older adolescents will more fully appreciate the value of possessions than younger adolescents.
Insecurity. According to Kasser et al.’s (2004) model, people in situations that do not
support the satisfaction of their basic psychological needs will adopt materialistic values as a
way of compensation.
Studies have found that the family environment has an impact on the endorsement of
materialistic values. Parental styles and practices that did not fully meet children’s needs are
associated with materialism (Kasser et al., 1995; Williams, Cox, Hedberg & Deci, 2000).
Adolescents who communicate less frequently with parents about consumption were more
materialistic (Moore & Moschis, 1981). Chinese parents with children aged 6 to 14 held negative
attitudes toward television advertising and children’s advertising. They perceived that advertising
was deceptive and annoying and they felt strongly that advertising should be banned from children’s
programming (Chan & McNeal, 2003b). Chinese parents often placed restrictions on the things
children should or should not buy. At the same time, parents often asked their children’s preference
when buying things for them. Parental communication patterns about consumption were similar for
parents with both younger and older children (Chan & McNeal, 2003a). Chinese parents’ negative
attitude toward advertising together with more communication within the family may reduce the
Materialism 8
level of children’s anxiety, and we therefore hypothesize that adolescents who communicate more
frequently with parents will be less materialistic (H2).
Communication outside the family also contributes to the difference in the level of
materialism among children. Studies showed that materialism is higher in children who
communicate with peers more frequently (Churchill & Moschis 1979; Moschis & Churchill, 1978)
and who are more susceptible to their influence (Achenreiner, 1997). We expect that children who
communicate frequently with peers may reflect their status of anxiety and the need for peer
approval. We therefore hypothesize that adolescents who communicate more frequently with peers
will be more materialistic (H3).
Exposure to materialistic models and values. According to Kasser et al.’s (2004) model,
materialistic values are frequently found in popular culture, the media and advertisements. People
exposed to materialistic models will be more likely to take on the materialistic values through
modeling (Bandura, 1971) and internalization of these values (Ryan & Connell, 1989). There is
evidence from empirical data that there is a positive correlation between exposure to television
and materialism, both in children and adolescents in Western societies (Buijzen & Valkenburg,
2003; Churchill & Moschis, 1979; Kapferer, 1986; Moschis & Moore, 1982) and Asian societies
including China, Hong Kong and South Korea (Chan, 2003; Cheung & Chan, 1996; Kwak,
Zinkhan & DeLorne, 2002; Yang & Ganahl, 2004). We therefore hypothesize that adolescents
who watch television more will be more materialistic (H4).
Another pervasive source of materialistic models is found in advertising messages.
Advertisements encourage consumption by using images of attractive and/or famous product
users, demonstrating social reward by using the products, and associating the products with
wealthy lifestyles (Kasser el at., 2004). There is empirical data that supports a positive
correlation between exposure to advertising and materialism (Buijzen & Valkenburg, 2003).
Moschis and Moore (1982) conceptualized advertising exposure according to both frequency of
Materialism 9
watching advertisements and reasons for watching. The motives for advertising viewing appear to
have two dimensions: seeking information about the products, and seeking images or talking
points. A longitudinal study of adolescents found that television advertising exposure had long-
term effects as well as short-term effects on adolescents’ adoption of materialistic values
(Moschis & Moore, 1982). We expect that watching advertising for different motives will have a
different impact on materialism. We hypothesize that adolescents who view advertising for
informative motives will be less materialistic (H5) while adolescents who view advertising for
social motives will be more materialistic (H6). This is because adolescents who watch ads for
social motives will pay more attention to the materialistic models and values shown in the
advertisements while adolescents watching ads for informative motives will pay less attention to
such models and values.
Gender has been included as factors in several studies but did not produce consistent results.
For example, male adolescents reported higher levels of materialism than female adolescents
(Achenreiner, 1997; Churchill & Moschis, 1979) while Chinese boys and girls in Hong Kong
reported similar levels of materialism (Chan, 2003). We therefore do not propose a hypothesis
about the impact of gender on materialism.
[FIGURE ONE ABOUT HERE]
Measures of materialism
Pollay (1986) summarizes the criticism made by scholars including Leiss (1976) and
Skolimowsk (1977) that advertising promotes materialism as the means to happiness, status
seeking, social stereotypes, short-sightedness, selfishness, preoccupation with sexuality, and
conformity.
Materialism 10
Children’s materialistic values have been identified in a number of studies, including
children aged four to five selecting playmates with or without a new toy (Goldberg & Gorn,
1978); motivation and enjoyment of collecting among first and fifth grade hobbyists (Baker &
Gentry, 1996); and surveys using materialism scales developed for children (Heerey, Hunt,
Lukey-Smith & Winter, 2002).
Belk (1984) conceptualized materialism as the importance a consumer attaches to worldly
possessions, and whether such possessions assume a central place in the consumer’s life. Belk’s
(1985) measure of materialism consisted of three personality traits (envy, nongenerosity, and
possessiveness) and has been used in a number of studies (O’Guinn & Faber, 1989; Wallendorf
& Arnould, 1988). Richins’s (1987) measure of materialism for adults consisted of seven items
like “It is important to have really nice things”, “The things I own give me a great deal of
pleasure”. The measure demonstrated low to moderate Cronbach alpha reliability (0.3 for China
and 0.5 to 0.7 for the other four countries) in a cross-cultural study of consumers and university
students in five countries including United States, Canada, Australia, Turkey, and China (Sirgy et
al., 1998). Richins and Dawson (1992) constructed the Material Values Scale consisting of three
factors including success, centrality, and happiness, producing a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.78.
METHOD
The data analyzed in this study were collected from a survey conducted in Beijing, China
from February to March 2003. The target population was grade 7 to 11 students. The city was
selected because it has a highly developed advertising industry. The advertising expenditure of
Beijing accounted for 15 percent of the national advertising expenditure in 2000. A draft
questionnaire was prepared based on previous studies (Moschis & Moore, 1982; Richins &
Dawson, 1992). Back-translation was used where one of the authors translated the questionnaire
from English to Chinese, and another author then translated it back into English to enhance
Materialism 11
translation equivalence. The questionnaire was pre-tested for clarity and accuracy by personally
interviewing six adolescents. The sample was a cluster design with a total of eleven secondary
schools selected (based on district and type of school) in urban Beijing. Nine schools offered
grade 7 to 12 programs while two were vocational training schools offering grade 10 to 12 only.
Adolescents studying in vocational training schools usually have lower academic scores than
adolescents studying in grammar schools, and are less likely to pursue further study in
universities. For each sampled school, one to two classes of students were invited to participate in
the survey. Altogether 788 close-ended structured questionnaires were distributed during regular
class meetings by one of the authors. Students were asked to fill in the questionnaire in the
absence of the teachers. The survey took about fifteen minutes to complete. A total of 730
questionnaires were collected. The overall response rate was 93 percent. The sample profile is
summarized in Table 1. All the respondents aged 11 to 13 and 87 percent of respondents aged 14
to 16 were studying in grammar schools; while 52 percent of respondents aged 17 to 19 were
studying in vocational training schools. The overall mean age was 14.9 years old.
[TABLE ONE ABOUT HERE]
Measures
The questionnaire consisted of fifteen statements on materialism, three statements on
communicating with parents about consumption, three statements on communicating with peer
about consumption, and seven statements on motivation of advertising viewing. Respondents
were asked to indicate on a seven-point scale the degree to which they agree with the statements
(1 = disagree very much, 7 = agree very much). Demographic and other variables such as
household income, number of hours of television viewing in weekdays and weekends, and
monthly personal expenses were also collected. Materialism was measured using Richins and
Dawson’s (1992) conceptualization of materialism as a mind-set of attitudes regarding
possessions as symbols of success, possessions occupying a central part of lives, and the belief
Materialism 12
that more possessions lead to more happiness. Eighteen items of Richins and Dawson’s (1992)
scale of materialism were used in the draft questionnaire.
Results from pretesting the questionnaire found that most of the respondents had difficulties
reporting attitudes toward luxury goods. Most of them reported that they had no experience in
buying luxury goods. As a result, three items in the centrality sub-scale that involved luxury
goods were deleted. Inter-item reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) for the remaining fifteen items was
0.66. It was similar to the Cronbach’s alpha value of 0.63 reported in a recent study of Chinese
college students (Yang & Ganahl, 2004). The mean formed the measure of materialism with
higher scores indicating stronger endorsement of materialistic values.
Communication with parents/peers about consumption was measured by asking
respondents to rate three items: “I always discuss advertisements with my parents/friends”, “I
always ask my parents/friends for purchase advice”, and “I always go shopping with my
parents/friends” on 7-point scales (1 = disagree strongly, 7 = agree strongly). These items were
from Moschis & Moore’s (1982) study. The inter-item reliability scores for family and peer
communication were 0.51 and 0.52 respectively. Advertising viewing for different motivations
was measured by asking respondents to rate seven statements such as “I watch advertisements to
find out how good a product is” on 7-point scales (1 = disagree strongly, 7 = agree strongly). The
seven statements were based on Moschis and Moore’s (1982) study. The inter-item reliability
was 0.74. Factor analysis generated a two-factor solution that accounted for 54 percent of the
total variance of advertising viewing. The two factors were labeled as “informative motives” (to
find out how good a product it is, to help decide what to buy, and to find out where to buy things
I want) and “social motives” (to impress others, to talk about with others, to learn about the in-
things to buy, and to see the images of the people that I wish to be). The inter-item reliability
measures were 0.67 and 0.63 respectively.
Materialism 13
Television viewing was recoded into three levels with nearly equal size based on the
number of hours of television viewing in a week (low = 0 - less than 9 hours, medium = 9 - 16
hours, high = more than 16 hours).
RESULTS
On average, the respondents watched television for 1.4 hours on a weekday and 3.7 on a
weekend. The mean hours of television viewing were 14.3 hours per week. Family
communication was positively skewed (skewness = 0.17) while peer communication was
negatively skewed (skewness = -0.09). The means of family communication and peer
communication about consumption were 4.1 (s.d. = 1.3) and 3.8 (s.d .= 1.3) respectively.
Adolescents in China more frequently communicated with family about consumption than with
peers (t = 6.0, df = 727, p < 0.001). The means of informative and social motives of advertising
viewing were 4.0 (s.d. = 1.4) and 3.6 (s.d. = 1.2). Adolescents in China were more likely to view
advertisements to satisfy information needs than social needs (t = 7.9, df = 729, p < 0.001).
Table 2 summarizes adolescent’s response to the statements about materialism. They are
sorted in descending order of the mean scores. The statements that they agreed with most
indicated that Chinese adolescents like to own things that impress people. They reported that the
things they own are important to them. The statement with the lowest mean was ‘I usually buy
only the things I need’, indicating that Chinese adolescents were mainly buying the necessities.
The mean and the standard deviation for the materialism score was 3.9 and 0.7 respectively.
[TABLE TWO ABOUT HERE]
Materialism 14
Materialism and age
In order to test John’s (1999) model of consumer socialization, we analyzed results by age
group. In John’s (1999) model, children aged 11-16 belong to the reflective stage. John (1999)
did not describe consumer behavior beyond 16 years. In the current study, we assumed that
adolescents beyond 16 were early adults. For the purpose of data analysis, respondents were
categorized into three age groups, i.e. 11 to 13, 14 to 16, and 17 to 19, representing the early
reflective, late reflective stages of John’s (1999) model of consumer socialization, plus the early
adulthood stage. We split the reflective stage into two sub-stages as the school types were quite
different. This distinction was possible to make by a relatively large sample size in this age range.
Table 3 summarizes children’s materialism scores by age group. One-way ANOVA F-tests were
conducted to compare the means by age group.
Out of the fifteen statements, six statements yielded significant F-statistics between
respondents of different age groups. Duncan pairwise tests indicated that in most of the cases
with significant F-statistics, the age group 17 to 19 was the most materialistic. Older adolescents
were more likely to associate material possessions with success, to admire people with expensive
possessions, and to perceive they would be happier or better if they had more possessions. The
materialism scores for respondents in the age groups 11 to 13, 14 to 16, and 17 to 19 were 3.8,
3.9, and 4.1 respectively (F-statistic = 6.2, p < 0.001). Again, Duncan pairwise testing indicated
that respondents in the age group 17 to 19 were the most materialistic. There was no significant
difference in materialism scores between respondents in the other two age groups. There was no
significant difference among adolescents in early reflective and late reflective stages, and
respondents beyond age 16 were more materialistic.
[TABLE THREE ABOUT HERE]
Materialism 15
Testing the model
Hierarchical regression analysis is used to test the theoretical model discussed in Figure 1.
Table 4 summarizes the Pearson correlation coefficients between the materialism scores and the
predictors. The highest correlation coefficient was between informative and social motives of
advertising viewing. Family and peer communication about consumption were strongly
correlated. Peer communication about consumption correlated strongly with social motives of
advertising viewing.
[TABLE FOUR ABOUT HERE]
Predictors were entered into linear regression equations in four steps to predict adolescents’
materialism scores. In the first step, age group was entered to test John’s (1999) model and to
serve as a control. In the second step, communication with parents and peers about consumption
were entered. In the third step, television viewing, informative and social motives of advertising
viewing were entered. Table 5 shows the results of the hierarchical regression analysis and
standardized beta values of various predictors. Results indicated that all three steps had
significant F changes. This suggests that age, communication with family and friends, media
exposure and motives for advertising viewing are all useful in predicting adolescents’
endorsement of materialistic values. When these variables were controlled, there was no gender
difference in materialism scores. F-statistic of the model at the end of the third step (with all
predictors except sex) was 18.0 (df = 6, p < 0.001). R square of the model was 0.15 indicating
that the model accounted for 15 percent of the variance of adolescents’ materialism scores. As a
result, the model in Figure 1 was supported. According to the standardized beta values, social
motives for advertising viewing was the most important predictor, followed by peer
communication, age group, and family communication (negative correlates). Materialism scores
Materialism 16
were higher for those (a) most likely to view advertising for social motives, (b) those who
engaged in more peer communication about consumption, and (c) older adolescents, and (d) those
who engaged in less family communication about consumption. The regression model explained
fifteen percent of variation in adolescents’ materialism scores. As older adolescents were found
more materialistic, H1 was supported. Adolescents who communicate more frequently with
parents about consumption were less materialistic. Therefore, H2 was supported. Adolescents
who communicate more frequently with peers about consumption were more materialistic. As a
result, H3 was supported. Television viewing was found to have no impact on adolescents’
materialism. Therefore, H4 was not supported. Adolescents who view advertising for informative
motives were not less materialistic. As a result, H5 was not supported. Adolescents who view
advertising for social motives were more materialistic. Therefore, H6 was supported.
When sex was added to the regression model, the change in R square is 0.004 and was not
significant at the 0.05 level. In other words, when the predictors were controlled, female
respondents and male respondents did not have any significant difference in materialistic value
orientations.
[TABLE FIVE ABOUT HERE]
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
In the current study, older adolescents tended to be more materialistic than younger
adolescents. There are three possible explanations for this. First, according to John’s (1999)
model of consumer socialization, children at the reflective stage fully understand the value of
possessions based on social meaning, significance, and scarcity. The higher materialism scores
of older adolescents may simply reflect a stronger endorsement of these values. Secondly, the
sample profile accounted for some of these differences between the older and younger
Materialism 17
adolescents. Nearly half of the older adolescents were studying in vocational training schools that
prepare them to enter the workforce. They are going to enter a new circle of unfamiliar people
and may need possessions to reinforce self-identity. The third explanation is about exposure to
materialistic models and values. We think that older adolescents are more likely to encounter
people from a more diverse background, including ‘successful’ people with lots of luxury
possessions. We expect that they will be more likely to compare themselves to those with wealth,
and such comparison will encourage materialism.
Family communication was a negative predictor of the materialism score while peer
communication was a positive predictor. It indicates that the contents and effects of family and
peer communication are in the opposite direction. Parents are more likely to discourage
materialism while peers are more likely to enhance materialism. It can also mean that those
adolescents who scored higher in materialism tend to communicate less with their parents and
communicate more with their peers. As Chinese parents and adolescents have grown up in very
different economic environments, they would have much contrast in their attitudes toward
material possessions. In our focus group interviews with parents of elementary school children in
Beijing, parents reported that they often discourage endorsement of materialistic values at home.
Parents reported that when they discussed consumption with children, they always reminded
children “to live within means, to save as much as possible, and to refrain from buying luxury
goods”.
In the current study, television exposure was found to have no impact on materialism. CCTV
is owned by the state government and it has strict rules about the contents of television
programming. A recent content analysis of children’s television programs in China indicated that
97 percent of the programs are produced domestically and 3 percent are imported. Children’
television programs in China are highly educational and they reflect mainly traditional values of
collectivism, high power distance and masculine values (Chan & Chan 2004). Domestic
Materialism 18
television programs often present model children performing unselfish deeds for the nation
(Donald, 2002). We speculate that the prevalence of traditional values in television programs in
China may explain why spending more time on watching television does not trigger more
materialistic values.
Social motives of advertising viewing correlated positively with materialistic values while
informative motives of television advertising did not have correlation with materialistic values.
Among the seven predictors, watching advertising for social motives was the strongest predictor
of materialism. Advertisements are the most persuasive models of materialistic models and
values because advertisements show how products (material goods) are used to achieve different
goals. Adolescents who watched advertisements for social motives are actively seeking for
models of materialistic values. As a result, they are more likely to endorse materialistic values.
The current study found that Chinese adolescents in Mainland China do not endorse
materialistic values strongly. Their materialism scores are almost the same as that of college
students in Mainland China (Yang & Ganahl, 2004). Their materialism scores are lower than that
of adults in Mainland China (Sirgy et al., 1998). So, our data suggest that Chinese adolescents
and college students share similar level of materialism, and they become more materialistic when
they enter adulthood. Further longitudinal study is required to verify this result.
The current study did not find gender differences regarding materialistic values. Young
females were equally as materialistic as young males. This finding is similar to Chan’s (2003)
study of Chinese children in perceptual and analytical stages in Hong Kong. Previous suggestions
that boys are more materialistic come mainly from studies of adolescents in western societies. As
Chinese children’s television commercials contained higher percentages of male models and
spokespersons (Ji & McNeal, 2001), the finding was quite unexpected. It therefore provides
evidence of a lack of correlation between the gendered media contents and the impact on the
respondents’ endorsement of masculine values about success and happiness. The lack of a
Materialism 19
gender difference may also be attributed to the single-child policy and the lack of difference in
the patterns of family communication for boys and girls (Chan & McNeal, 2003a). Future
research should investigate why there is a lack of gender difference in materialistic values during
the reflective and early adulthood stages, and whether gender difference may evolve in late
adulthood.
To conclude, the current study shows the extent of endorsement of materialistic values of
adolescents in Mainland China and provides empirical support for a proposed theoretical model.
The study also indicates that a measurement of materialism developed in a western society can be
adapted to an acceptable level of inter-item reliability in a Chinese context. The current study
has two limitations. The first limitation is that Cronbach alpha coefficients for family and peer
communication were below 0.7 and efforts must be made to create scales with higher values. The
second limitation is that the study was conducted in Beijing city alone. Further research is
needed to investigate materialistic value orientations among adolescents in other cities and in
rural parts of China. Further study is also needed to compare materialism among adolescents and
adults to show whether there will be significant changes with important life events, such as
entering into the work force or forming a new family. For parents and educators, the study
indicates that, to safeguard adolescents from being materialistic, they can engage in more family
communication about consumption, and encourage adolescents to consume advertising for
information motives.
(5000 words for text)
Materialism 20
Figure 1 Theoretical framework
H1
H2
H3
H4
H5
H6
Social motives
of ad viewing
Family
communication
Informative
motives of ad
viewing
TV viewing
Peer
communication
Materialism
Age
Group
Insecurity
Exposure to
materialistic
models and values
Materialism 21
References:
Achenreiner, G.B. (1997) Materialism values and susceptibility to influence in children, in
Advances in Consumer Research, 24, eds. M. Brucks & D.J. MacInnis, Provo, UT:
Association for Consumer Research, pp.82-88.
Baker, S. M. & Gentry, J. W. (1996) Kids as collectors: a phenomenological study of first and
fifth graders, in Advances in Consumer Research, 23, eds. K. P. Corfman & J.G. Lynch,
Jr., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, pp.132-137.
Bandura, A. (1971) Social learning theory, Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Belk, R. W. (1983) Worldly possessions: Issues and criticisms, in Advances in Consumer
Research, 10, eds. R.P. Bagozzi, A.M. Tybout, Ann Arbor: Association for Consumer
Research, pp.514-519.
Belk, R. W. (1984) Three scales to measure constructs related to materialism: Reliability,
validity, and relationships to measures of happiness, in Advances in Consumer Research,
11, ed. T. Kinnear, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, pp.291-297.
Belk, R. W. (1985) Materialism: Trait aspects of living in the material world, Journal of
Consumer Research, 12 (December), pp.265-280.
Bu, W. (1998) Children Programming: Who's the Target? Journalism and Communication
(Xin wen yu chuan bo yan jiu), 5(2), pp.13-24. (In Chinese)
Bu, W. (2001) The Influence of Mass Media on Children (Da zhong chuan mei dui er tong de
ying xiang), Beijing: Xinhua Publishing House. (In Chinese)
Buijzen, M. & Valkenburg, P.M. (2003) The unintended effects of television advertising: A
parent-child survey, Communication Research, 30(5), pp.483-503.
Chan, K. (2003) Materialism among Chinese children in Hong Kong, International Journal of
Advertising and Marketing to Children, 4(4), pp.47-61.
Chan, K. & Chan, F. (2004) Advertising to children in China: A study of children’s TV media
environment, Korean Academy of Marketing Science Spring Conference Proceeding,
pp.55-73, Seoul: KAMS.
Chan, K. & McNeal, J. (2003a) Parent-child communications about consumption and
advertising in China, Journal of Consumer Marketing, 20(4), pp.317-332.
Chan, K. & McNeal, J. (2003b) Parental concern about television viewing and children’s
advertising in Mainland China, International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 15(2),
pp.151-166.
Cheung C.K. & Chan, C.F. (1996) Television viewing and mean world value in Hong Kong’s
adolescents, Social Behavior and Personality, 24(4), pp.351-364.
Churchill, G.A. & Moschis, G.P. (1979) Television and interpersonal influences on adolescent
consumer learning, Journal of Consumer Research, 6(June), pp.23-35.
Materialism 22
Donald, S.H. (2002) Crazy rabbits! Children’s media culture. In S.H. Donald, M. Keane & H.
Yin, (eds.), Media in China: Consumption, Content and Crisis, New York: Routledge
Curzon.
Easterlin, R. & Crimmins, E. (1991) Private materialism, personal self-fulfillment, family life,
and public interest: The nature, effects, and causes of recent changes in the values of
American youth, Public Opinion Quarterly, 55 (Winter), pp.499-533.
Goldberg, M.E. & Gorn, G.J. (1978) Some unintended consequences of TV advertising to
children, Journal of Consumer Research, 5 (June), pp.22-29.
Goldberg, M.E., Gorn, G.J., Peracchio, L. A. & Bomossy, G. (2003) Materialism among youth,
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(3), pp.278-288.
Heerey, C., Hunt, S., Lukey-Smith, A. & Winter, S. (2002) Advertising to children and
materialism: A comparative study between England and Sweden. Exeter, The University
of Exeter: Unpublished final year undergraduate project.
Ji, M.F. & McNeal, J.U. (2001) How Chinese children’s commercials differ from those of the
United States: A Content Analysis. The Journal of Advertising, 30(3), pp.79-92.
John, D.R. (1999) Consumer socialization of children: A retrospective look at twenty-five
years of research, Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (December), pp.183-213.
Kapferer, J.N. (1986) A comparison of TV advertising and mothers’ influence on children’s
attitudes and values, in S. Ward, T. Robertson & R. Brown (eds.), Commercial
Television and European Children, Hants, England: Gower Publishing Company
Limited.
Kasser, T., Ryan, R.M., Couchman, C.E. & Sheldon, K. M. (2004) Materialistic values: Their
causes and consequences, in T. Kasser & A.D. Kanner (eds.), Psychology and Consumer
Culture, Washington, D.C.: American Psychology Association.
Kasser, T., Ryan R.M., Zax, M. & Sameroff, A.J. (1995) The relations of maternal and social
environments to late adolescents’ materialistic and prosocial values, Developmental
Psychology, 31, pp.907-914.
Kwak, H., Zinkhan, G. M. & DeLorme, D. E. (2002) Effects of Compulsive Buying
Tendencies on Attitudes Toward Advertising: The Moderating Role of Exposure to TV
Commercials and TV Shows, Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising,
24(2), pp.17-32.
Leiss, W. (1976) The Limits of Satisfaction, Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
McNeal, J.U. & Ji, M.F. (1999) Chinese children as consumers: An analysis of their new
product information sources, Journal of Consumer Marketing, 16(4), pp.345-364.
McNeal, J.U. & Yeh, C. H. (1997) Development of consumer behaviour patterns among
Chinese children, Journal of Consumer Marketing, 14(1), pp.45-59.
Materialism 23
Moore, R.L. & Moschis, G.P. (1981) The role of family communication in consumer learning,
Journal of Communications, 31 (Autumn), pp.42-51.
Moschis, G.P. & Churchill, G.A. Jr. (1978) Consumer socialization: A theoretical and
empirical analysis, Journal of Marketing Research, 15 (November), pp.599-609.
Moschis, G.P. & Moore, R.L. (1982) A longitudinal study of television advertising effects,
Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (December), pp.279-286.
O’Guinn, T.C. & Faber, R.J. (1989) Compulsive buying: A phenomenological exploration,
Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (September), pp.147-157.
Piaget, J. (1970) The stages of the intellectual development of the child. In P.H. Mussen (ed.),
Readings in Child Development and Psychology, pp291-298, New York: Harper and
Row.
Pollay, R.W. (1986) The distorted mirror: Reflections on the unintended consequences of
advertising, Journal of Marketing, 50(April), pp.18-36.
Richins, M. L. (1987) Media, materialism, and human happiness. Advances in Consumer
Research, (14), eds. M. Wallendorf & P. Anderson, Provo, UT: Association for
Consumer Research, pp.352-356.
Richins, M.L. & Dawson, D. (1992) A consumer values orientation for materialism and its
measurement: Scale development and validation, Journal of Consumer Research,
19(December), pp.303-316.
Ryan, R.M. & Connell, J.P. (1989) Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examining
reasons for acting in two domains, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
pp.749-761.
Selman, R.L. (1980) The Growth of Interpersonal Understanding, New York: Academic Press.
Sirgy, M.J., Lee, D., Kosenko, R., Meadow, H.L., Rantz, D., Cicic, M., Xi Jin, G., Yarsuvat,
D., Blenkhorn, D.L. & Wright, N. (1998) Does television viewership play a role in the
perception of quality of life?, Journal of Advertising, 27 (1), pp.125-142.
Skolimowski, H. (1977) The semantic environment in the age of advertising, in T.H. Ohlbren
& L.M. Berk (eds.), The New Languages: A Rhetorical Approach to the Mass Media and
Popular Culture, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 91-101.
Tse, D.K., Belk, R.W. & Zhou, N. (1989) Becoming a consumer society: A longitudinal and
cross cultural content analysis of print Ads from Hong Kong, The People’s Republic of
China, and Taiwan, Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), pp.457-472.
Wallendorf, M. & Arnould, E.J. (1988) My favorite things: A cross-cultural inquiry into object
attachment, possessiveness and social linkage, Journal of Consumer Research, 14
(March), pp.531-547.
Williams, G.C., Cox, E.M., Hedberg, V.A. & Deci, E.L. (2000) Extrinsic life goals and health
risk behaviors in adolescents, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, pp.1756-1771.
Materialism 24
World Health Organization. (2005) Strategic Directions or Improving the Health and
Development of Children and Adolescents, Retrieved 19th April, from
www.whqlibdoc.who.int/hq/2002/WHO_FCH_CAH_02.21.pdf
Yang, H. & Ganahl, D.J. (2004) A cross-cultural study between American and Chinese college
students regarding television viewing, materialism, beliefs and attitude toward
advertising. Paper presented at the Association for Educators in Journalism and Mass
Communication 2004 Convention, Advertising Division, Toronto, Canada, August 4-7.
Yau, O.H.M. (1988) Chinese cultural values: Their dimensions and marketing implications,
European Journal of Marketing, 22(5), pp.44-57.
Yu, P. (2004) Let the dream fly, Chinese Television, (1), pp.7-10. (In Chinese)
Zhao, B. (1996) The Little Emperors' Small Screen: Parental Control and Children's
Television Viewing, China. Media, Culture & Society, 18(4), pp.639-658.
Zhao, B. (1997) Consumerism, Confucianism, communism: Making sense of China today, New
Left Review, 222(Mar-Apr), pp.43-59.
... Students of universities are the appropriate population for this research, generally as students of universities have leisure time and some more money than students in other kinds of schools. 34,117 A structured questionnaire was dispersed to participants through lectures in the attendance of their teachers. Beforehand, the participants were requested to fill, they were briefed about the introduction of the research's variables. ...
... The findings of our research are related to existing literature. 10,34,[126][127][128] According to Hypothesis H1, university students in Saudi Arabia appear to be more likely to adopt compulsive shopping and materialistic behavior due to watching TV advertisements. People's perceptions are influenced by television. ...
... That may be inferred from the fact that the present research results are comparable to those of investigations done in other cultures. 10,34 Despite the country's strong Islamic traditions, globalization has led to buyer attitudes in Saudi Arabia comparable to elsewhere. Following the research results, Saudi Arabian youth share the same materialism as young people in other industrialized countries. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This study aims to examine the influence of social media sites and television advertisements on compulsive shopping behavior, and whether this influence is mediated by materialism in the university students of Saudi Arabia. Methods Data was collected from 487 students at Saudi universities. AMOS and Structural equation modeling (SEM) were utilized to examine the data. The research supports the Hypothesis that adolescents who are more materialistic are more prone than less materialistic adolescents to engage in compulsive shopping behavior. Results The findings were consistent with other research, suggesting that the same remains true in the culture of Saudi Arabia. The research’s findings show that television advertisements and the use of social media sites positively related to compulsive shopping behavior among university students, and materialism mediated the relationship between television advertisements and social media sites. Conclusion The research emphasizes the significance of comprehending the materialistic attitude and consumption choices of adolescents and offers crucial information for scholars, decision-makers, and management of top companies.
... 19 Bhatia, S., Hendricks, S., và Bhatia, S. (1993) nhấn 20 mạnh rằng nếu muốn giới trẻ giảm tải việc hút thuốc 21 thì chúng phải được khuyến khích học hỏi nhiều giá 22 trị xã hội tốt đẹp hơn thay vì chỉ cung cấp thông tin 23 tiêu cực về mối nguy hiểm sức khỏe do hút thuốc gây 24 nên 4 . Các kết quả này nhấn mạnh tầm quan trọng của 25 các giá trị xã hội tích cực mà trẻ em hay thanh thiếu 26 niên cần có, ví dụ như đức tính như khoan dung, sẵn 27 sàng chấp nhận cảm xúc và niềm tin của người khác 28 (những người không giống với chúng ta), một trong 29 những đức tính quan trọng của con người và là tài sản 30 quý giá của thế hệ trẻ 5 . 31 Một nghiên cứu gần đây cho thấy rằng các yếu tố bên 32 trong của thanh thiếu niên Gen Z bao gồm yếu tố gia 33 đình, thành tích học tập và các giá trị văn hóa có ảnh 34 hưởng mạnh mẽ đến các quyết định của cuộc đời, 35 chẳng hạn việc lựa chọn chuyên ngành đại học, trong 36 khi yếu tố bên ngoài như chất lượng giáo dục lại có tác 37 động không đáng kể 6 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Nowadays, many studies indicate that parents play a crucial role in transmitting social values to their children, shaping certain standards for decision-making in their offspring. Drawing on the secondary data from the World Values Survery conducted from 2017 to 2022, this article extracts data from several countries in Southeast Asia (including Indonesia, Malaysia, Myanmar, Thailand, Singapore, and Vietnam) regarding the social values, which children should be encouraged to learn from their families. The processed data reveals the top 5 values selected by respondents are: 1) good manners, 2) responsibility, 3) tolerance and respect for others people, 4) independence, and 5) hard work. Furthermore, by using the Chi-Square technique, this study compares evaluations based on several socio-demographic characteristics. The results show that national factors differ statistically significantly across all 11 measured aspects, while living area and marital status only respond to 8 out of 11 items, and the number of children in the family shows the least difference in distribution. The findings from this article provide a robust foundation for developing population development policies, particularly targeting children in Southeast Asia in general and Vietnam in particular. Additionally, the article suggests avenues for Vietnamese’s researches major in family and gender to explore leveraging data from the World Values Survey organization to analyze social value issues at the national and regional levels.
... As a result, a new media emerged named "morning TV show" which has been considered the prime driver of social consumption, excessive buying, material possession, and reduced life satisfaction among young adults. Despite the extensive research on social media excessive usage and its consequences on children, adolescents, and youngsters in developed nations (Moretti, 2011, Pham et al., 2012, Mueller et al., 2011, Fitzmaurice & Comegys, 2006, Manolis & Roberts, 2012, Chan et al., 2006, Tal-Or and Hershman-Shitrit, 2015, to the best of our knowledge, no previous study has discussed the increasing effects of morning TV show consumption on millennial Muslim females in the Pakistan during the COVID-19. In filling this gap, the current research contributes to the existing literature. ...
Article
Full-text available
The primary aim of this research is to examine what makes millennial Muslim females more materialistic and less satisfied with their lives in Pakistan during the COVID-19 pandemic. In today’s world, investigating the underlying mechanism of the exponential increase in Muslim female materialism tendency is considered a worthwhile problem. Therefore, the current research develops a theoretical model based on the stress–strain model. It uses the framework to test the impact of morning TV show consumption on Muslim females’ life satisfaction, social consumption, and compulsiveness through the mediation of materialism in COVID-19. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used. The data was collected through the mall intercept survey method from 720 millennial Muslim females. During COVID-19, the study findings revealed that high viewing of morning TV shows appears to be a significant determinant that leads to high materialism, which results in highly negative outcomes (i.e., compulsive buying, social consumption, and less satisfaction). Moreover, the results found that materialism mediated the relationship between morning TV show consumption and three studied outcomes.
... Older adolescents might display higher levels of materialism due to increased social pressures, exposure to consumer culture, and the desire for status symbols that signify adulthood and independence. High school students may prioritize owning the latest smartphone or designer clothing to fit in with their peers or to express their social identity, more so than younger adolescents who might be less influenced by such external factors (Chan et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study delves into the intricate relationship between narrative engagement in Chinese dramas and adolescents' interest in Chinese food culture, as well as their engagement in social activities. It examines the hypothesis of no significant linear relationship between narrative engagement and various aspects of adolescent behavior and preferences. Employing the Narrative Engagement Scale to measure narrative engagement and a set of questions derived from The Quality-of-Life Scale to gauge interest in Chinese food culture, the research utilized purposive sampling targeting adolescents in Karnataka. Results indicated positive correlations between narrative engagement and interest in Chinese food culture, social engagement, personal development, and recreational activities. Linear regression analyses revealed significant predictive power of narrative engagement on these variables.
... In addition to advertisements, materialistic content like sharing the last purchases and exchanging information about brand products on social media is ubiquitous (Chu et al., 2013;Pellegrino et al., 2022). Empirical evidence for social modeling as a cause of developing materialism, as proposed by Kasser et al. (2004), can be derived from two longitudinal studies that show that frequent and prolonged exposure to materialistic content increases dispositional materialism in the long run (Chan et al., 2006;Opree et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, social media such as YouTube, TikTok, and Instagram have become an essential part of the everyday lives of children and young adults. Integrating elements of these social media into higher education may have the potential to enhance situational intrinsic learning motivation through the emotional design and proximity to students' lives, but this also poses the risk of fostering a situational materialistic focus due to the ubiquitous materialistic content on especially Instagram, undermining situational intrinsic learning motivation. In the present study, we examined if the primary use of Instagram is associated with higher materialism and how exposure to Instagram-framed pictures influences situational intrinsic learning motivation. The current study conducted an online experiment. Participants (N = 148) were randomly assigned to one of three groups after they rated items about general and problematic social media use and materialism. In the first two groups, participants were asked to rate the pleasantness of luxury or nature Instagram-framed pictures. A third group received no pictures. Afterwards, the situational intrinsic learning motivation was assessed through a mock working task. The findings prove that people who (primarily) use Instagram tend to be more materialistic than people who (primarily) use another social medium and that exposure to Instagram-framed pictures neither positively nor negatively influenced situational intrinsic learning motivation but moderated the relationship between problematic social media use and situational intrinsic learning motivation. Limitations, implications, and future directions for social media use inhigher education are discussed.
... Consumers' compulsive behavior can be defined as "intense, chronic, repetitive and uncontrollable impulse to purchase, erected by certain events lead by psychological, social and monetary consequences (Flight & Sacramento, 2015;Japutra et al., 2019;Laque et al., 2022). In marketing psychology, consumers' compulsive behavior gained consideration by researchers (Chan et al., 2006). Literature has grouped the tendencies and sustaining psychological factors that cause compulsiveness (Desarbo & Edwards, 1996;Yi, 2013). ...
Article
The study focuses on the exploration of the consumption intentions of Chinese food consumers and the sojourners living in China. Further, the study adopts the mediating role of trend affinity in the quest to examine the sojourner's intentions to consume Chinese cuisine. The data is collected from the sojourners living in China and from the local Chinese consumers with the help of a questionnaire-based survey. The survey provides valid 235 and 268 responses from sojourners and domestic consumers respectively. The study uses Smart PLS-SEM analysis for results estimation. The optimum stimulation level (OSL) and hedonic values are affirmed to be the preferred predictors of native consumer intentions. By contrast, OSL, hedonic, and utilitarian values are the preferred predictors for sojourners. Trend affinity mediates the relationship between the independent variables and consumption intention. The relationship of OSL, hedonic, and utilitarian values on the intentions of consumers varies. The current study establishes the role of trend affinity in Chinese cuisine purchase intentions, which is a new dimension for the literature. Moreover, the study compares the behavior between sojourners and native Chinese consumers residing in mainland China.
... In Pakistan, young adults have a strong desire for material possessions. Pakistani youth, similar to other countries, are attracted to possessions and friendships because they have a significant link between peers and materialism (Chan, Zhang, & Wang, 2006). Hence, the results confirm adults who are more involved socially experience pressure to comply with the individuals of the peer group and indulge in CBB. ...
Article
Full-text available
The rationale of the study is to explore the impact of enhancement motive (EM), social motive (SM), coping motive (CM), and materialism (MAT) over compulsive buying behavior (CBB) and also to determine the relationship among them. CB disorder is an alarming dysfunctionality that requires investigation; therefore, the paper aims to shed light on the prevalence of CBB and its contributing factors. The study also included an investigation of the impact of demographic factors such as age, marital status, level of education, level of income, and employment status over CBB. The research included a sample size of 515 participants, and data was gathered using an online survey instrument. The data-gathering tool used in this study is a standardized, pre-tested, and structured questionnaire. Pearson correlation and multiple regression were applied. Results indicated a positive and significant impact of EM, CM, SM, and MAT over CBB. Whereas, from among the demographic factors, only age turned out to be a significant determinant of CBB and tends to have a negative relationship with CBB, that is, the act of CBB decreases with an increase in age.
Chapter
This chapter seeks to understand the participants’ reasons for beautifying themselves and how the findings relate to postfeminist discourse regarding the themes of women’s individualism, choice, and empowerment. This analysis demonstrates how young Chinese women employ postfeminist logic when they discuss their experiences engaging in beauty practices. The data shows several key postfeminist sensibilities, such as participants perceiving themselves as freely choosing subjects, liberation from male objectification, and gaining pleasure from beauty practices. Discussion about how young Chinese women’s pleasure of beautification is constructed through three different stages, including the consumption of beauty products, the process of beautification, and the end results of beautification. The analysis seeks to avoid a traditional binary approach, which argues between pleasure and oppression. Instead, it illustrates the complexity of young women’s experiences of pleasure regarding beautification in relation to consumerism, aesthetics, skills, career development, and sisterhood bonding.
Article
Bu çalışmanın amacı; ekonomik ve sosyal yönden yıkıcı etkileri olan kompulsif satın alma davranışına etki eden unsurlardan; reklamlara yönelik tutum, reklamlarda ünlü kullanımının ve aile etkisinin materyalist tutuma etkisini belirlemektir. Bu doğrultuda 18 yaş üzeri 387 tüketiciye çevirim içi yoluyla anket yöntemi uygulanmıştır. Anket verileri SPSS 25 programıyla analiz edilmiş ve modelde oluşturulan hipotezler test edilmiştir. Analiz sonucunda tüketicilerin reklamlara ve reklamlara yönelik tutumlarının materyalizm ve kompulsif satın alma tercihlerine etki ettiği, aile etkisinin ise materyalizm ve kompulsif satın alma tercihlerine etki etmediği anlaşılmış ve materyalist tutumun kompulsif satın alma niyetinin belirleyicilerinden olduğu anlaşılmıştır. Ayrıca yaş değişkeninin, kompulsif satın alma niyeti değişkeni üzerinde anlamlı bir etkiye sahip olduğu, gelir durumunun ve eğitim düzeyinin ise kompulsif satın alma niyetinde etkisinin olmadığı anlaşılmıştır. Materyalist eğilim, marka bilinci, lüks ve konfor arayışı, sembolik tüketim vb. davranışların temelini oluşturarak karar verme ve satın almalara önemli ölçüde yön vermektedir. Araştırmada da ortaya konduğu üzere bu ürünlerin reklamlarda çıkması, reklamlarında da ünlülerin kullanılması bu eğilimi daha da arttırmaktadır.
Article
Full-text available
A benchmark study was conducted to examine Mainland Chinese parents' attitudes toward advertising and parental mediation of television viewing. A survey of 1,665 parents of elementary school children aged six to fourteen in Beijing, Nanjing and Chengdu was conducted December 2001 to March 2002. Results indicated Chinese parents hold negative attitudes toward television advertising in general and children's advertising specifically. The negative attitudes result mainly from the perception that advertising is deceptive and annoying. Parents feel strongly that advertising should be banned on children's programming. Ninety-eight percent of parents exercise some control over the contents and time of television viewing. Despite a low level of co-viewing and discussion of television commercials with children, Chinese parents perceived that they have great influence on their children's attitudes toward advertising.
Conference Paper
Full-text available