Content uploaded by Jeffrey D Long
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jeffrey D Long on Oct 07, 2016
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy
Curriculum-Based Measurement Oral Reading as an
indicator of reading achievement: A meta-analysis of
the correlational evidence
Amy L. Reschly
a,
⁎, Todd W. Busch
b
, Joseph Betts
c
,
Stanley L. Deno
d
, Jeffrey D. Long
d
a
University of Georgia, United States
b
University of St. Thomas, United States
c
Renaissance Learning, United States
d
University of Minnesota, United States
Received 17 February 2008; received in revised form 22 June 2009; accepted 11 July 2009
Abstract
This meta-analysis summarized the correlational evidence of the association between the CBM
Oral Reading measure (R-CBM) and other standardized measures of reading achievement for
students in grades 1–6. Potential moderating variables were also examined (source of criterion test,
administration format, grade level, length of time, and type of reading subtest score). Results
indicated a significant, strong overall correlation among R-CBM and other standardized tests of
reading achievement and differences in correlations as a function of source of test, administration
format, and reading subtest type. No differences in the magnitude of correlations were found across
grade levels. In addition, there was minimal evidence of publication bias. Results are discussed in
terms of existing literature and directions for future research.
© 2009 Society for the Study of School Psychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Curriculum Based Measurement; R-CBM; Reading achievement
In the 1970s, Deno and colleagues from the University of Minnesota set out to create a
set of measurement procedures that could be used to efficiently monitor student progress in
core educational skills and evaluate the effectiveness of instructional interventions, with the
Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
⁎Corresponding author. Department of Educational Psychology & Instructional Technology, University of
Georgia, Athens, GA, 30602, United States.
E-mail address: reschly@uga.edu (A.L. Reschly).
ACTION EDITOR: Randy Floyd.
0022-4405/$ - see front matter © 2009 Society for the Study of School Psychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2009.07.001
Author's personal copy
goal of accelerating student achievement. This work, referred to as Curriculum-Based
Measurement (CBM), resulted in measures that were amenable to frequent administrations,
sensitive to small changes in growth, inexpensive, and time efficient (Deno, 1985, 1992).
Thirty years of research provides evidence of the reliability and validity of these measures
as indicators of student achievement in reading, mathematics, and writing.
The data garnered from CBM measures have been used for a variety of purposes in
general, remedial, and special education. Research indicates that when CBM data are used
to monitor student performance and guide instructional modifications, student achievement
is raised (Fuchs, Deno, & Mirkin, 1984; Fuchs & Fuchs, 1986; Fuchs, Fuchs, Hamlett, &
Ferguson, 1992; Mirkin, Deno, Tindal, & Kuehnle, 1982; Stecker & Fuchs, 2000). In
addition to individual progress monitoring, CBM data have been used in Individual
Education Program (IEP) goals to evaluate the reintegration of special education students
into general education classes (Powell-Smith & Stewart, 1998; Shinn, Powell-Smith, Good,
& Baker, 1997), to create school- and district-level norms (Shinn, 1989), and for program
evaluation (Tindal, 1989). Measures have also been modified for use with exceptional
populations (Allinder & Eccarius, 1999; Morgan & Bradley-Johnson, 1995), translated into
other languages and countries (Kaminitz-Berkooza & Shapiro, 2005, Yeh, 1992), and used
with English Learners (e.g., Baker & Good, 1995; Wiley & Deno, 2005). Further, there
have been extensions of the CBM measurement philosophy to social, pre-academic skill
areas and content areas (e.g., Individual Growth and Development Indicators, Dynamic
Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills [DIBELS]; Espin, Shin, & Busch, 2005;
Greenwood, Dunn, Ward, & Luze, 2003; Kaminski & Good, 1996, 1998; Lembke, Foegen,
Whittaker, & Hampton, 2008; McConnell, McEvoy, & Priest, 2002) and age groups (i.e.,
infancy, preschool, kindergarten, and secondary levels).
Of all CBM measures and skill areas, the most widely researched and utilized in schools
across the U.S. is the oral reading or read-aloud measure (R-CBM).
1
For the R-CBM
measure, students are given a reading passage, typically one at their grade or instructional
level, and asked to read aloud for 1 min. At the end of 1 min, passages are scored for the
number of words read correctly. R-CBM, and its less frequently used CBM-reading
counterpart, MAZE, are general outcome measures (Fuchs & Deno, 1991). General
outcome measures are standardized, longitudinal assessments that use the same methods
and presumably equivalent content over time. Further, the measures may be used to
designate the performance desired from a student at the end of the monitoring period (Fuchs
& Fuchs, 1999). Therefore, an educator may set a long-range goal, typically the end of the
school year, and monitor student progress toward that goal using different reading passages
of presumably equivalent difficulty. These monitoring data are used to determine whether
the student is on-track to reach the specified goal and inform instructional efforts to
accelerate student performance. In the case of reading, progress toward this goal shows that
a student is overall becoming a better reader.
Scores from CBM measures in general, and R-CBM in particular, have been evaluated
according to traditional psychometric criteria for reliability and validity (Deno, 1992).
R-CBM scores have been shown to be technically adequate in terms of reliability and are
1
R-CBM is the term used throughout this article to describe data derived from the standard administration and
scoring of the CBM read-aloud measure.
428 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
moderately to highly correlated with scores from other standardized measures of reading
achievement (e.g., Marston, 1989). The psychometric properties of the scores of the R-CBM
measure, in combination withits ability to function as a general outcome measure and the ease
of administration, time efficiency, low cost, and frequency with which the measures may be
given, has led to use by educators across U.S.
Until recently, questions raised about CBM were largely academic in nature and
concerned philosophical issues and misunderstandings of its purpose (Shinn & Bamonto,
1998). In practice, the primary concerns expressed were the amount of time taken to assess
rather than instruct and the face validity of R-CBM as a measure of general reading
proficiency (i.e., whether data derived from scoring a student's passage reading for speed
and accuracy could possibly represent their overall reading skill; Foegen, Espin, Allinder,
& Markell, 2001; Wesson, Deno, & King, 1984; Yell, Deno, & Marston, 1992). However,
the zeitgeist created by increased accountability and high-stakes assessments stimulated the
use of R-CBM data for related purposes, such as screening to identify lower performing or
“at-risk”students (Deno et al., 2009), benchmarking (Good, Simmons, & Kame’enui,
2001; Shinn, 1989), predicting performance on high stakes assessments (e.g., Buck &
Torgesen, 2003; Crawford, Tindal, & Steiber, 2001; Hintze & Silberglitt, 2005;
McGlinchey & Hixson, 2004; Shapiro, Keller, Lutz, Santoro, & Hintze, 2006), and as
the basis for creating school- and district-wide improvement models (Deno et al., 2009). In
addition, CBM in general, and R-CBM in particular, have emerged as a cornerstone of
special education reform efforts (Grimes & Tilly, 1996; Marston, Muyskens, Lau, & Canter,
2003; Reschly & Bergstrom, 2009) and featured prominently in legislative initiatives such
as Reading First and the reauthorization of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act
[IDEA]. The raised profile of CBM and increased use of the R-CBM measure and other
CBM-like measures, such as the DIBELS, has spurred interest, criticism, and debate (e.g.,
Goodman, 2006; Kamii & Manning, 2005; Manzo, 2005a).
Some of the recent debate about R-CBM has revolved around the use of the tests.
In particular, whether or not these and similar tests (i.e., DIBELS) should have featured so
prominently in Reading First assessment plans (e.g., Glenn, 2007; Manzo, 2005b, 2007)
and the use of CBM and R-CBM in determining a students' response to intervention
and potential identification for a Learning Disability under the 2004 revision to IDEA (e.g.,
Naglieri & Crockett, 2005). Underlying the debate over the use of these measures in
Reading First is a larger question over whether R-CBM is a measure of Fluency (e.g.,
Samuels, 2007), one of the core elements of reading instruction outlined by the National
Research Panel (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [NICHHD],
2000). In the original work on R-CBM, the measure was referred to as read aloud or oral
reading; however, in recent years another term, oral reading fluency, has been used
to describe this measure, drawing the R-CBM measure into a larger debate over the nature
of reading development and fluency and assessment of this construct (Samuels, 2007).
The R-CBM measure is sometimes described as a fluency measure in that it is a time-
limited task on which performance is quantified in terms of both speed and accuracy.
The utility of R-CBM for making instructional decisions, however, is based on the many
and varied empirical relationships between R-CBM and other measures rather than the
validity of R-CBM as a measure of the construct of reading fluency, as described in theories
of reading (e.g., LaBerge & Samuels, 1974) or by the National Reading Panel (NICHHD,
429A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
2000). The debate over the use of R-CBM and CBM-like measures in Reading First and
IDEA and the nature of fluency aside, this article focuses on the empirical literature base of
the R-CBM measure and its associations with other standardized measures of reading
achievement.
There is considerable evidence supporting the use of R-CBM as a measure of general
reading proficiency and comprehension (Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, & Jenkins, 2001). However,
a number of questions remain. For instance, a concern often voiced by practitioners and
others is that R-CBM taps students' decoding skills rather than general reading
achievement or comprehension (Hamilton & Shinn, 2003), and there are conflicting
results regarding the consistency of the strength of the relationship between R-CBM and
other standardized measures of reading achievement across grades. For example, Jenkins
and Jewell (1993) found that the correlation coefficients between scores from R-CBM and
those from tests of comprehension and total reading decreased across grades two through
six. Similar findings were reported by Kranzler, Miller, and Jordan (1999). In contrast,
Hosp and Fuchs (2005) found stable, high correlation coefficients among R-CBM scores
and the Comprehension subtest scores of the Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests (WRMT-R;
Woodcock, 1987) across grades one through four; however, the same investigation reported
a small but significant decrease in the magnitude of correlations between R-CBM scores
and total reading scores on the WRMT-R in grade four when compared to grades one, two,
and three. Relatively few studies have examined R-CBM with middle and high school
students.
In addition to lingering questions related to the fitness of R-CBM as an indicator of
general reading proficiency and comprehension and the suggestion of grade-level declines
in the magnitude of the association between scores derived from R-CBM probes and other
standardized measures of reading achievement across elementary school, a number of other
questions warrant further attention. For example, R-CBM is increasingly used to predict
performance, sometimes across years, on state-derived high-stakes assessments and to
establish benchmarks for passing these tests. Further, it is recognized that there is a great
deal of variability in the difficulty and relative proficiency levels of various state tests
(Peterson & Hess, 2005) and very weak associations have been found between proficiency
levels on state tests and the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP; Linn,
2005).
In a related vein, over the last 30 years, external validity examinations of the relations
between R-CBM probe scores with scores derived from other standardized tests of reading
proficiency have shifted from individually administered achievement tests (e.g., Marston &
Deno, 1982) to those from group-administered achievement tests that are frequently used
for purposes of state-level, high-stakes assessment (e.g., Silberglitt & Hintze, 2005).
Correlation coefficients derived from R-CBM probe scores and scores from other
individually administered tests of reading proficiency may be higher due to similarity in
administration format or potentially higher quality test construction of individually
administered tests of reading proficiency. For example, different criteria have been
suggested for evaluating the psychometric evidence of inferences drawn from test scores
based on how the scores are used, with scores from screening and group tests requiring
lower minimum reliability and validity coefficients than those used for individual decision-
making (Salvia & Ysseldyke, 2007). Correlation coefficients between R-CBM probe scores
430 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
and scores derived from individual- and group-administered tests may also vary due to
potentially greater self-direction required to complete group-administered tests, which
would increase the likelihood that fatigue, inattention, and other variables may negatively
affect student performance.
An important task in accumulating validity evidence for inferences drawn from scores
on a particular test is determining the extent to which the scores function similarly for
various individuals and subgroups (American Educational Research Association, American
Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education, [AERA, APA,
NCME], 1999). In the case of R-CBM, one must ask whether correlation coefficients from
scores of R-CBM probes and other tests of reading achievement are similar across gender,
socioeconomic, racial–ethnic, and general and special education groups. Given the use of
the scores to establish benchmarks, predict future performance on high-stakes assessments,
and identify students in need of additional intervention, systematic over- or under-
prediction of performance for a particular subgroup of students would be problematic and
may indicate a need to have different prediction equations for various subgroups (e.g., one
for native English speakers, another for students who are English Learners). Studies of
predictive bias of R-CBM with respect to measures of reading comprehension and general
reading achievement have found disparate results with respect to bias in terms of racial–
ethnic group membership and home language (e.g., Hintze, Callahan, Matthews, Williams,
& Tobin, 2002; Klein & Jimerson, 2005; Kranzler et al., 1999).
To date, reviews of the R-CBM literature have been narrative, rather than quantitative in
nature (Marston, 1989; Wayman, Wallace, Wiley, Ticha, & Espin, 2007) and a number of
potential moderating variables have yet to be systematically examined. The purpose of this
meta-analysis was to organize extant empirical results to obtain a quantification of the level
of linear relation between R-CBM scores and commonly used reading tests across
numerous published research endeavors. In addition, specific estimates were computed for
facets of the relationship that were thought to have a moderating effect on the magnitude of
the correlation coefficients. In this study, we examined the correlational evidence in the
form of Pearson's correlation coefficients (r) for students in grades 1–6 with R-CBM
administered according to standard CBM procedures in English prior to or concurrently
with norm-referenced, standardized tests of reading achievement. Moderating variables
were those related to students (grade-level, demographic characteristics) and criterion
measures (source of test, administration format, and length of time between R-CBM and the
administration of the reading criterion measures).
Method
Data collection
A systematic search of the literature was conducted to locate articles and technical
reports for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Dissertations and conference proceedings were
excluded because it was not possible to systematically access these sources. However,
technical reports, accessible via the Internet, were included. The latter was an important
consideration in that many correlations between R-CBM scores and state-specific
assessment scores were only available in technical report format.
431A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Key search terms were selected from those commonly used in the CBM reading
literature and entered into the electronic search engines ERIC, Education Full Text, and
PsychInfo. The number of hits for each term in ERIC, Education Full Text and PsychInfo,
respectively, were 958, 1201, 737 for read aloud; 170, 142, 226 for oral reading fluency;
2468, 713, 2381 for oral reading;205,92,314forCBM (205, 92, 314); 4, 7, 15 for R-CBM;0,
2, 0 for CBM-OR;3,3,4forCBM-R;and,35,23,72forDIBELS. Literature from the
inception of CBM in the late 1970s (Deno & Mirkin, 1977) through June of 2008 was
examined. As in other published meta-analyses (e.g., Swanson, Trainin, Denise, Necoechea,
&Hammill,2003), we also conducted a hand search of journals that frequently publish this
type of research (i.e., Journal of School Psychology,Journal of Special Education,Remedial
& Special Education,School Psychology Quarterly,andSchool Psychology Review)to
ensure the inclusion of articles that may have been missed by one of the search engines.
In addition, the published work of authors who frequently publish CBM research was
examined (D. Fuchs, L. Fuchs, Hintze, Marston, Shinn, Shapiro, Tindal, as well the authors
of this paper) to ensure the comprehensiveness of the literature search. Finally, the reference
lists of potential articles were scanned for additional citations. The use of these search terms
also led to the examination of several articles that were intervention rather than criterion
validity studies. These articles were included if correlations among R-CBM and other reading
measures were reported and other criteria for inclusion were met. The first and second authors
conducted the literature search and reviewed abstracts of articles identified in the keyword,
author, hand, and reference searches. Using these methods, 105 studies were selected for
additional review.
Three Internet sites that provided data used in this study were AIMSweb (http://www.
aimsweb.com), DIBELS (http://www.dibels.uoregon.edu) and a searchable database of
reports complied by the federally funded Research Institute on Progress Monitoring (RIPM;
http://www.progressmonitoring.org; Grant No. H324H030003) at the University of
Minnesota. Technical reports from the Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities
(1977–1983), the federally funded center in which much of the early validation work with
CBM measures was conducted, were available from the RIPM site. From these three sites,
78 reports were selected for additional review.
Evaluation of articles and reports
From these searches, the first and second authors created a list of potential articles and
reports and reviewed the full-text of each article or report for inclusion in the study. To be
retained in the meta-analysis, R-CBM probes had to be administered and scored according
to standard CBM criteria (e.g., instructions, scored for number of words read correctly in
1 min) in English prior to (across academic years) or within the same academic year as other
norm-referenced, standardized tests of reading achievement for students in grades one
through six. Studies were excluded if the grade level was above grade 6 (e.g., Fuchs, Fuchs,
& Maxwell, 1988), standardized achievement scores were used to predict R-CBM
performance across academic years (e.g., Alonso & Tindal, 2003), or R-CBM assessment
tasks were modified for use with special populations (e.g., Allinder & Eccarius, 1999), and
for technical reasons (i.e., the use of Grade Equivalent scores for the criterion measure as in
Pressley, Hilden, & Shankland, 2005 and the collapse of grade-level data into one score for
432 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
analysis as in Wilson, Schendel, & Ulman, 1992). Following these procedures, a total of 41
studies were retained for analysis (Table 1).
Coding
The coding of data was guided by a priori questions and possible moderating variables.
When more than one set of correlation coefficients was reported within a paper, those
specific to grade levels of interest and those for whole groups—rather than for only
educationally relevant subgroups—were entered into analyses. For example, in the Deno,
Mirkin, Chiang, and Lowry (1980) paper, correlation coefficients were reported separately
for general education students and those with learning disabilities as well as for the overall
group. Those for the overall group were entered for further analysis.
Data were coded into eight categories. The coding of two categories, correlation
coefficient and sample size associated with each respective coefficient, was straightforward.
These were drawn directly from articles and reports. The remaining six categories are
described below.
Source of test
Criterion measures were coded according to the dichotomy of state specific (e.g.,
Minnesota Comprehensive Assessment) or nationally normed, commercially available
(national) tests of student reading achievement (e.g., WRMT-R; Woodcock, 1987).
Administration format
Criterion measures were also coded according to how the tests were administered:
individually or to groups of students.
Type of criterion score
Correlation coefficients from criterion measures of reading achievement were coded
according to the type of score reported: Comprehension, Vocabulary, Word Identification,
Decoding, and Total Reading Score. These groupings came from the studies' authors
descriptions of the tests rather than an analysis of stimulus materials and response formats.
This rule was also true for the Total Reading Scores. Scores coded into this group were
typically composites or overall scores as delineated in the authors' test descriptions. A list
of tests and subtests, subtest groupings, and types of Total Reading Scores is presented in
Table 2. At the outset, our goal was to examine the association between R-CBM and
measures of achievement as a function of grade level, reading score type, and both grade
level and score type. However, there were insufficient data to examine the correlations
within grades for various score types.
Time
In general, the length of time between the administrations of two or more measures
affects the magnitude of the correlations between these measures. The majority of studies
completed to date have involved the administration of R-CBM and other standardized tests
of reading achievement within the same academic year; however, a few studies have
examined R-CBM and other tests across academic years. Correlations from studies retained
433A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1
Descriptive information for included studies.
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
Ardoin et al. (2004) Southeastern US N= 77 3rd graders in regular education
(35 females,42 males; 58% White,
40% African American, 2% other)
W Ind & Gp. Natl. .70, TR
.74, C
.42, C
.62, WI
.64, TR
.35, V
.58, C
.73, TR
.73, C
.41, C
.69, WI
.66, TR
.42, V
.60, C
Bain and Garlock
(1992)
2 elementary schools in
rural, western FL
Grade 1 N=66, Grade 2 N=198,
Grade 3 N=215
W Gp. Natl. .69, TR
.54, TR
.79, TRChapter 1 students
.79, TR
.70, TR
.74, TR
Baker et al. (2008) 16 school districts, 34 Reading
First schools in OR
Each cohort approx. 2,400 students,
69% FRL
W, A Gp. State & Natl. .72, TR
.82, TR
.63, TR
.72, TR
.72, TR
.79, TR
.80, TR
.58, TR
434 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
.63, TR
.63, TR
.65, TR
.68, TR
.67, TR
Baker and Good
(1995)
Rural district in WA State N=76 2nd grade students W Ind. Natl. .51, TR
.56, C
Barger (2003) NC N=38 3rd graders W Gp. State .73, TR
Buck and Torgesen
(2003)
13 schools from one FL district N= 1103 3rd grade students 49% female,
83% White, 7% African American,
6% Hispanic, 1% LEP, 19% Special
Education, 46% FRL
W Gp. State .70, TR
Colon and Kranzler
(2006)
North Central FL N=50 5th graders 44% male, 58%
Caucasian, 22% African American,
6% Asian, 4% Hispanic, 2% Native
American, 8% Other
W Ind. Natl. .741, C
.512, C
.805, TR
.813, C
.465, C
.832, TR
Crawford et al. (2001) Rural school district in
Western Oregon
N=51 students in both years of study
(2nd and 3rd grade) 95% White,
57% female
W, A Gp. State .60, TR
.66, TR
Deno et al.
(1980, 1982)
Study I: Suburban school
in St. Paul, MN
Study I: N= 33 students in
grades 1–5N=18 regular education
students (50% males)
N=15 students with Learning Disabilities
(73% males)
W Ind. & Gp. Natl. .87, WI
.82, C
.73, C
.76, C
.71, D
.78, C
Study III: Three urban schools
in Minneapolis, MN
Study III: N= 66 .80, C
43 regular education students;
23 students with Learning
Disabilities in grades 1–6
Fuchs et al. (1982) Midwestern urban elementary
school
N=30 English speaking students in
grades 1–6 randomly selected from
90 participated in a larger study
W Ind. Natl. .91, WI
.89, WI
(continued on next page)
435A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1 (continued)
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
Good et al. (2001) Urban district in the Pacific
Northwest (OR)
N= 364 3rd graders
6 elementary schools in the district: 37% to 63% FRL
10% minority, 18% at or below poverty
W Gp. State .67, TR
Hintze et al. (2002) Small urban school in the
Northeastern U.S.
N= 136 (Grade 2 N= 33, Grade 3 N= 31,
Grade 4 N= 34, Grade 5 N= 38)
W Ind. Natl. 65, C
49% male, 48% African American,
52% White
School demographics: 47% low income,
33% middle income, 20% high income
Hintze et al. (1997) Northeastern United States N= 57 (32 males, 25 females)
Grade 2 N= 19, Grade 3 N= 20, Grade 4 N=18
W Gp. Natl. .67, C
.66, C
One suburban elementary
school
86% received reading instruction in
regular education with no additional
assistance
82% Caucasian, 9% African-American,
4% Latino, 5% Asian
Hintze and
Silberglitt(2005)
North central United States N=1815 W, A Gp. State .49, TR
.58, TR
51% male, 3% Native American,
1% Asian Pacific Islander, 1% Hispanic,
1% Black not Hispanic, 94% White not
Hispanic, 5% Special Education, 30% FRL
.61. TR
7 elementary schools, 5 cohorts
of 1st grade students
.68, TR
.68, TR
.66, TR
.68 TR
.69, TR
Hosp and Fuchs
(2005)
Southeastern U.S. N= 310 W Ind. Natl. .71, D
Grade 1 (N= 74), Grade 2 (N= 81),
Grade 3 (N = 79), Grade 4 (N = 76)
.91, WI
Four elementary schools .79, C
57%, 56%, 53%, 53% male, respectively .86, TR
436 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Grades 1–4
41%, 37%, 56%, 49% African American
49%, 56% 39%, 50% White
11% , 7%, 5%, 1%
Other 5%, 9%, 11%, 12%
Special Education
.90, TR
.82, D
.88, WI
.83, C
.89, TR
FRL across the four schools: 81.8%, 42.4%,
41.2%, 34.4%
.91, TR
.82, D
.88, WI
.84, C
.87, TR
.91, TR
.72, D
.73, WI
.82, C
.78, TR
.83, TR
Jenkins and Jewell
(1993)
Two elementary schools in the
Pacific Northwest
N= 335
Grade 2 N= 47, Grade 3 N= 50,
Grade 4 N=66, Grade 5 N=47,
Grade 6 N= 125 33% FRL,
5% SpEd
W Gp. Natl. .83, TR
.88, TR
.86, TR
.73, TR
.67, TR
.86, C
.82, C
.86, C
.68, C
.63, C
.87, TR
.70, TR
.79, TR
.68, TR
.60, TR
.84, C
.67, C
.82, C
.64, C
(continued on next page)
437A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1 (continued)
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
.58, C
Ketterlin-Geller and
Tindal (2004)
Urban school district in the
Pacific Northwest
N= 1153 3rd graders
68% White, 5% Asian, 3% African American,
2% Native American, 9% Other, 51% female,
16% special education
W Gp. State .41, TR
Klein and Jimerson
(2005)
School district in Southern CA Cohort 2: Grade 1 (N= 473), Grade 2 (N= 642).
Grade 3 (N = 731) 73%, 64%, 65% Hispanic
(Grades 1 –3, respectively) 48%, 57%, 56% FRL
56%, 51%, 53% Spanish Home Language
W, A Gp. Natl. .84, TR
.74, TR
.81, TR
.80, TR
.77, TR
.77, TR
.68, TRCohort 3: Grade 1 (N= 759), Grade 2 (N= 600),
Grade 3 (N= 709) 74%, 72%, 74% Hispanic
(Grades 1 –3, respectively) 49%, 48%, 45% FRL
61%, 56%, 59% Spanish Home Language
Longitudinal: 1st grade R-CBM to 3rd grade
SAT-9: N= 401
District: 24% Caucasian, 71% Hispanic, 55% FRL,
56% Spanish Home Language
Kranzler et al.
(1998)
North Central FL N= 57 4th graders (28 males, 29 females) W Ind. Natl. .41, C
77% White, 19% African American, 4% Hispanic,
23% low income
Kranzler et al.
(1999)
Public elementary school in
north central FL
N= 326 general education students W Gp. Natl. .63, C
.52, CGrades 2–5(Ns: 84, 76, 94, 72, respectively)
.54, C
.51, C
69% Caucasian, 24% African American, 49% female,
English primary language of all students
Marston and Deno
(1982)
Minneapolis, MN N= 26 3rd grade students W Gp. Natl. .59, V
.84, WI
.88, C
438 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
.90, TR
.84, D
McGlinchey and
Hixson (2004)
7 years of data from 1
elementary school in urban MI.
One year all students in the
district included.
Total 4th grade Nacross years= 1, 362 W Gp. State .77, TR
Yr1N=139, Yr 2 N=68, Yr 3 N= 64, Yr 4
N= 843, Yr 5 N= 61, Yr 6 N= 73, Yr 7
N= 55, Yr 8 N=59
.69, TR
.74, TR
.63, TR
.49, TRSpEd 6%, 7%, 3%, 6%, 6%, 4%, 16%, 8%,
respectively Yr 1–8 .65, TR
.81, TRFRL 66%, 68%, 74%, 60%, 72%, 70%,
84%, 75% .76, TR
Female 45%, 41%, 48%, 48%, 48%, 48%,
51%, 56%
African-American 45%, 42%, 55%, 45%,
46%, 48%, 49%, 47%
Caucasian 54%, 58%, 44%, 48%, 48%,
44%, 47%, 47%
Hispanic 1%, 0% , 2%, 7%, 2%, 7%,
2%, 2%
American Indian, 0% , 0%, 0%, 0%, 4%,
1%, 2%, 3%
Asian American, 0%, 0%, 0%, 1%, 0%,
0%, 0%, 0%,
District: 52% non-Caucasian, 60% FRL
McIntosh, Graves,
and Gersten
(2007)
Large southern CA school
district
N= 59 for the longitudinal data W, A Ind. Natl. .51, C
Predominately ELs in ‘schools who serve the
poorest and lowest performing students in
the district’
.73, C
Riedel (2007) 26 schools in Memphis, TN
that received a Reading
Excellence Act grant
N= 1395 W, A Gp. Natl. .59, TR
.49, TR92% African American, 85% FRL, 55% female,
4% EL .67, TR
.54, TR
Roehrig, Petscher,
Nettles, Hudson,
and Torgesen
(2008)
Students in Reading First
schools in FL
N= 35,207 W Gp. State & Natl. .66, TR
.68, TR49% female, 36% White, 36% African American,
23% Latino, 3% multiracial, 1.5% Asian,
b1% Native American, 75% FRL, 17% SpEd,
.68, TR
.68, TR
(continued on next page)
439A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1 (continued)
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
12% EL, 3% Gifted .71, TR
.71, TR
.67, TR
.69, TR
.68, TR
.68, TR
.70, TR
.70, TR
Schilling, Carlisle,
Scott, and Zeng,
2007;Carlisle
et al., 2004
9 School districts, 49 schools,
in MI Reading First
N=2970 1st graders, N=2, 884 2nd graders,
N=3, 130 3rd graders
W, A Gp. Natl. .58, V
.58, D
.66, C
.66, TR
.63,V
.66, D
71, C
.71, TR
.62, V
.59, D
.70, C
71, TR
.63, V
.63, D
.74, C
.74, TR
.63 V
.64, D
.75. C
.74. TR
.61, V
440 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
.62. D
.67. C
.70, TR
.58, V
.61. D
.65. C
.67, TR
.56. V
.60. D
.63. C
.65. TR
Shapiro et al.
(2006)
2 school districts in Eastern PA District 1 N= 617 W Gp. State & Natl. .68, TR
Grade 3 N= 191, Grade 4 N= 213,
Grade 5 N= 213
.69, TR
.67, TR
32.8% low income, 8.4% LEP, 11.4% IEPs .65, TR
District 1, moderate size, urban
and suburban
.66, TRDistrict 2 N= 431
.67, TRGrade 3 N= 132, Grade 4 N= 133,
Grade 5 N= 166 .25, TR
64, TR6.3% low income, b1% LEP, 10.2% IEPs
.62, TR
District 2, Suburban .72, TR
.54, D
.67, V
.67, C
.71, TR
.53, D
.63, V
.67, C
.70, TR
.52, D
.64, V
.65, C
Shaw and Shaw
(2002)
CO N= 58 3rd graders W Gp. State .73, TR
.73, TR
(continued on next page)
441A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1 (continued)
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
.80, TR
Shinn et al. (1992) Mostly White public school
district in mid-size
northwestern city
N= 238 W Gp. Natl. .57, C
Grade 3 N= 114, Grade 5 N= 124 49% female;
96% received instruction in general education
.58, C
.69, D
.58, C
.60, C
.59, D
.60, C
.55, C
.49, D
.62, C
.54, C
.48, D
Sibley, Biwer, and
Hesch (2001)
IL Group 1: N= 112 5th graders, Group 2:
N= 114 6th graders
W, A Group State & Natl. .63, TR
.75, TR
.61, TRDistrict: 15% special education, Mostly white,
middle to upper middle class, 7% FRL,
4% minority
.62, TR
.65, TR
.76, TR
.76. TR
.71, TR
Silberglitt, Burns,
Madyun, and
Lail (2006)
Upper Midwest N= 5, 472 W Gp. State .68, TR
51.5% male, 2.3% Native American, 1.4% Asian,
1.0% Hispanic, 1.0% Black, 94.3% White
.65, TR
Grade 3 N= 3165, Grade 5 N= 3283 FRL range
across districts: 5.7% to 18.63%
7th and 8th grade data not included in table
Silberglitt and
Hintze (2005)
Five rural and suburban districts in
the Upper Midwest
N's range from 1441 to 2126 W, A Gp. State .47, TR
.57, TR
442 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Districts:53% male, 2.1% Native American,
1.4% Asian/Pacific Islander, .6% Hispanic, .
6% Black, 95.3% White, 26% FRL
.60, TR
.66, TR
.67, TR
.68, TR
.70, TR
.71, TR
Sofie and Riccio
(2002)
West Central AL N= 40 W Ind. Natl. .65, WI
20 students referred (ages 6–8) for
reading disability
.75, C
65% male, 90% White, 10% African American .64, D
20 students not referred with average achievement
40% male, 95% White, 5% African-American
Speece and Ritchey
(2005)
3 schools in a suburban district in
the mid-Atlantic states
N= 276 W Ind. Natl. .50, TR
2 cohorts of 1st grade students in 2 consecutive
academic years
57% male, 13% African American, 11% Asian
American, 55% European American,
16% Hispanic, 5% Multiracial, 1% Other
Stage and Jacobsen
(2001)
One elementary in Puget Sound N=173 4th graders W Gp. State .43, TR
54% male, 6% SpEd .43, TR
.44, TRDistrict: 90% European American, 5% Hispanic,
2% Native American, 1% African American,
15% FRL
Tindal and Marston
(1996)
N= 772 W Gp. Natl. .81, V
Grade 1 N= 198, Grade 2 N= 191, Grade 3
N= 183, Grade 4 N= 66, Grade 5 N= 68,
Grade 6 N=66
.73, V
.75, V
.30, C
.77, V43.5% White, 35.4% African American,
11% Asian, 7.4% Native American,
2.8% Hispanic
.20, C
.87, V
.63, C
.87, V
.68, C
.73, V
(continued on next page)
443A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 1 (continued)
Author Region and school type Sample characteristics
a
Time
b
Individual or
group adm.
Source
of test(s)
Included
correlations
(r) and score
type
.64, C
.75, V
.64, C
Uribe-Zarain (2007) 11 schools in DE that participated
in Reading First for 2 or more yrs.
N= 852 3rd grade students W Gp. State .52, TR
61% FRL, 52% female, 18% SpEd,
N6% LEP
VanDerHeyden,
Witt, and Naquin
(2003)
Rural community in southern LA N= 182 in 1st and 2nd grade, 42% male W Gp. Natl. .70, TR
School: 46% FRL, 85% Caucasian
Vander Meer, Lentz,
and Stollar (2005)
3 elementary schools in suburban
school district in southwest OH
N= 364 students W,A Gp State .65, TR
.63, TR3 of 5 schools from a suburban district
of 8800 students .65, TR
.65, TR
.612, TR
.61, TR
Wiley and Deno
(2005)
Urban elementary school in
St. Paul, MN
Grade 3 N= 36, Grade 5 N= 33 W Gp. State .71, TR
80%, 58% EL, 3rd and 5th grade,
respectively
.57, TR
Wilson (2005) AZ N= 241 3rd grade students W Gp. State .741, TR
Note. Table only includes data specific to grade levels included in the study.
a
EL = English Learner, FRL = Free or Reduced Lunch, IEP= Individualized Education Program, SpEd= Special Education.
b
W = measures administered within academic year, A = measures administered across academic years.
444 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 2
Tests, subtests, and groupings for analyses.
Comprehension tests and subtests Decoding subtests
CAT Reading Comprehension ITBS Word Analysis
DRP MAT-8 Sounds & Print
Gates–MacGintie Comprehension SAT Word Study
ITBS Reading Comprehension SAT Phonetic Analysis
MAT-6 Comprehension SAT-7 Word Study Skills
MAT-8 Comprehension SDRT Word Attack
PIAT Comprehension WJ-R Word Attack
SDRT Literal Comprehension WRMT Word Attack
SDRT Inferential Comprehension
SDRT Reading Comprehension Total scores
WJ-III Passage Comprehension Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills
WJ-R Passage Comprehension GRA+ DE
WJ III Reading Fluency
a
Gates–MacGintie Total Reading
WRMT Comprehension ITBS Total Score
WRMT-R Passage Comprehension MAT-6 Total Reading
SAT Comprehension MAT-8 Total Reading
SAT-7 Reading Comprehension NWEA Levels Tests (RIT Scores)
TerraNova —2nd ed. reading subtest
b
SAT Total Reading
SAT-9
Vocabulary subtests SAT-10
ITBS Vocabulary SDRT
CAT Vocabulary WJ-R BRS
MAT-8 Vocabulary K-TEA Reading Subtest
SAT Vocabulary WJ-III Broad Reading Cluster
WRMT-Revised Total Score
Word identification subtests WRMT-Revised Basic Skills
SAT Reading Words State tests: Arizona, Delaware,
WJ-R Word Identification Colorado, Illinois, Ohio, Oregon,
WJ III Letter Word Identification
WRMT Word Identification
WRMT-R Word Identification
Note.Tests were grouped according to what the type of skill score the test purported to produce and how authors
described the use of the particular test/subtest. Information regarding state tests of reading achievement may be
found at the respective state's Department of Education.
CAT = California Achievement Test (CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1985;www.ctb.com); Comprehensive Test of Basic
Skills (Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills, CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1983;www.ctb.com) DRP= Degrees of Reading
Power (Koslin, Koslin, Zeno & Ivens, 1989); Gates–MacGintie Total Reading (MacGintie, Kamons, Kowalski,
MacGintie, & McKay, 1978); GRA + DE (Williams, 2001); K-TEA=Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement
(Kaufman & Kaufman, 1985); ITBS = Iowa Tests of Basic Skills (www.education.uiowa.edu/itp/itbs/);
MAT = Metropolitan Achievement Test (Prescott, Balow, Hogan, & Farr, 1984; pearsonassess.com); PIAT:
Peabody Individual Achievement Test (Dunn & Markwardt, 1970); NWEA = Northwest Evaluation Association
(www.nwea.org); SAT = Stanford Achievement Test (Madden, Gardner, Rudman, Karlsen, & Merwin, 1973;
pearsonassess.com); SDRT = Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test (Karlsen, Madden, & Gardner, 1976); TerraNova
(CTB/McGraw-Hill, 2003;www.ctb.com); WJ-R = Woodcock Johnson–Revised (Woodcock & Mather, 1990); WJ
III = Woodcock Johnson III (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mathew, 2001); WRMT = Woodcock Reading Mastery Test
(Woodcock, 1973), WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (Woodcock, 1987).
a
In this case, the WJIII Reading Fluency test was placed in the comprehension test/subtest category due to the
nature of the task: students are required to make a semantic judgment on the accuracy of each test item in the
subtest.
b
Used by the school district as a comprehension measure (Riedel, 2007).
445A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
for analysis were coded according to length of time between the administration R-CBM and
other measures as follows: within an academic year, across two academic years, and across
more than two academic years.
Grade level
The grade in which R-CBM was administered required several codes. In addition to codes
for grades one through six, it was necessary to code combinations of grades (e.g., grades 1–3
and grades 4–6) as well as an “other”category when children from a broad combination of
grades (e.g. grades 1–5 or grades 1–6) were administered a single-level passage.
Sample characteristics
Initially, we had hoped to examine the association between R-CBM and other
standardized tests of reading achievement as a function of racial–ethnic background,
students receiving special education services, and those eligible and not eligible for Free or
Reduced Lunch subsidies. However, some studies reported demographic data for the school
or district in which the data were collected but not demographic data specific to the sample
included in the study, whereas others reported it for the sample but not specific to grade-
level, which was central to the purpose of the meta-analysis. In both of these cases, sample
characteristics specific to the students yielding the correlation coefficients would have been
inferred from these other data. It was decided that this analysis would not be appropriate
given the data that were available; however, descriptive information for the students
included in each study can be found in Table 1.
Interrater agreement
Initial coding was completed by the first author for all studies and correlations included
in analyses. Because more than one correlation coefficient was often reported within each
article or report, information associated with 73 correlations (25% of all correlation
coefficients in the study) was randomly selected by the second author to examine inter-rater
agreement. The second author coded this information independently of the first author and
calculated the inter-rater agreement for each coding category. Inter-rater agreement was
calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus
disagreements, and multiplying the result by 100 to obtain a percentage. Inter-rater agreement
across all coded categories met or exceeded 90%. Inter-rater agreement values across the seven
categories coded were 100% for source of test, administration format, and grade level
categories; 96% for length of time; 95% for R-CBM correlation coefficients; and 90% for
sample characteristics and type of criterion score. For categories where 100% agreement was
not reached, the two authors discussed the discrepant cases, and both independently recoded
the data. After the second round of recoding, all categories had inter-rater agreement of 100%.
Statistical analyses
Modeling and study artifacts
A random-effects meta-analysis model (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hedges & Vevea, 1998;
Hunter & Schmidt, 1990) was used to evaluate the study results. The random-effects model
446 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
has been recommended as a more appropriate approach to combining information across
studies in meta-analytic research than the traditional fixed effects model (Hunter &
Schmidt, 2000; National Research Council, 1992; Schmidt & Hunter, 2003). One of the
main reasons that the random-effects model was chosen over a fixed-effects model for this
research was that the latter assumes that correlations between the R-CBM scores and
criterion reading test scores are fixed in the population and thus constant. This assumption
seems implausible for the current analysis as numerous measures of R-CBM and criterion
reading measures were considered and one would expect the population effect size to
randomly vary from study to study.
The most obvious reason to question the assumption of a constant correlation in the
population for the present research was that a diversity of reading measures were examined,
which, while assumed to be measuring a similar construct of general reading skill, were not
all developed or intended to measure the same aspects of reading. For instance, different
state tests measure different standards in different ways and nationally developed tests have
different approaches to measuring reading. Therefore, it was difficult to assume that there
would be a fixed and constant correlation between all the different measures. With
numerous and diverse measures of reading, it seems very difficult to assume that all the tests
measure reading in the same way and with similar content to the same degree of precision
and with the same degree of validity. Therefore, a fixed correlation in the population was
regarded as an overly strict and tenuous assumption.
The use of a random-effects model allowed for heterogeneous variation underlying the
correlations between R-CBM scores and those from criterion tests of reading across studies
rather than assuming a single underlying validity as in the fixed effects model. Using this
model allowed for the correlations between R-CBM and criterion measures to differ across
studies, and should therefore capture the underlying variation in correlations that would result
from different R-CBM measures and different criterion measures used across the studies in the
sample. In addition, the random-effects model allows for generalizations beyond the studies
included in this analysis (Field, 2001), which was deemed important because of the numerous
R-CBM measures and the numerous tests of reading skills considered.
An important consideration for this study was effectively handling the study artifacts that
can affect the reported correlation estimates within studies (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). It was
suspected that three main artifacts would need to be addressed: sampling error, errors in
measurement of both R-CBM and criterion reading measures, and restriction of range.
However, during the compilation of studies and review of the reported materials, we were
unable to identify much of the necessary information to correct for unreliability and
restrictions of range. For instance, only 7 of 41 studies (17%) reported any information on
the reliability of the R-CBM measures used in the study (either on the study sample or from a
publisher). For the criterion measures, 61% of the studies (n= 25) did not report any
reliability information for the criterion measures, and 32% (n= 13) provided general
information from a technical manual or secondary source, such as Buros Mental Mea-
surements Yearbooks (e.g., Geisinger, Spies, Carlson, & Plake, 2007) or textbooks (e.g.,
Salvia & Ysseldyke, 2007). Only 17% (n= 7) of studies provided specific information
regarding any form of reliability for the particular subtests, grade levels, or both. One study
provided general information for one criterion measure and specific information for a second
criterion measure in the study.
447A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Unfortunately, a number of studies did not provide sufficient descriptive data to account
for potential restrictions in range on R-CBM or other standardized measures of reading
achievement. Twenty-nine percent (n=12) of the studies did not report means or standard
deviations for R-CBM, whereas 5% (n= 2) reported partial data (e.g., a mean but not a
standard deviation), and 15% (n= 6) of the studies reported data for means and standard
deviations for a particular subgroup of the study (e.g., at-risk students or general education
and special education students) rather than the overall sample. A similar pattern was found
for the other standardized measures of reading achievement. Only 7% (n=3) of studies
reported means and standard deviations for the population and study sample. Forty-two
percent (n= 17) did not report a mean or standard deviation for either the population or
sample, whereas another 42% (n= 17) reported means and standard deviations for the
sample but gave no information about the population. In some cases, the population values
were known or readily accessible (e.g., Woodcock–Johnson III; Woodcock, McGrew, &
Mathew, 2001), while others were more difficult to locate (e.g., state-specific tests). Seven
percent (n= 3) of studies gave partial information for the criterion population and sample
and 2% (n= 1) provided population information but did not report sample mean and
standard deviation.
Therefore, not enough information was reported in the studies to correct for these two
important artifacts, either at the individual study level or with respect to general artifact
distributions (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). Attempts to make corrections on the reported
correlations would have had to have been based on somewhat capricious “estimates”by the
authors or based on the potentially biased subsample of studies with the pertinent and
necessary information. It was decided not to correct for these artifacts, and report the lack of
consistency in reporting important statistics necessary for the evaluation of the accumulated
evidence of studies such as these, which was thought to be an important finding in itself.
The random effect model can be conceptualized as a hierarchical linear model (HLM)
and estimated using maximum likelihood methods (Raudenbush & Byrk, 2002; Raju and
Drasgow, 2003; Konstantopoulos & Hedges, 2004). Estimates of sampling error were dealt
with in the following explication of the random-effects model within the context of the
general HLM. The HLM model for meta-analysis is a two-level model with level 1 (L1)
modeling within-study variation and level 2 (L2) modeling between-study variation
(Raudenbush & Bryk , 2002). The two-level model is,
dj=dj+ejð1Þ
dj=g0+XsgsWsj +uj;ð2Þ
where d
j
is the observed effect size measure in the jth study, δ
j
is the population correlation,
and e
j
is the random sampling error. We assume e
j
is normally distributed with zero mean
and variance, V
j
(which is independent across studies). The L2 model of Eq. (2) indicates
that the study correlations might vary based on the moderator variables, W
sj
. Thus, the L2
model is a regression model in which the study correlation is the criterion and the moderator
variables are the predictors with regression coefficients, γ
s
, indicating the strength of
association. γ
0
is the intercept term and u
j
is the random L2 error, assumed to have zero
448 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
mean and non-zero variance, τ
2
. The HLM allows for differences in sampling error across
studies when assessing the between-study variability and moderator variable effects.
Transformation of correlation coefficients
Further specification of the model was needed, as the effect size of interest, the
correlation coefficient, can be treated in various ways (see Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). We
chose to transform the reported correlations from the Pearson product-moment metric to
Fischer's zmetric using the following transformation:
z=Oln 1+rðÞ=1rðÞ½;
where rindicates the observed correlation, and ln represents the natural logarithm.
The z-transformation approach was chosen for three reasons. First, given that it was
impossible to correct the reported correlations for study artifacts, any slight bias from the
use of the z-transformation was thought to be minimal. This decision was not meant to
imply that this transformation was more accurate than applying the corrections for study
artifacts, but it was only a practical consideration given the lack of evidence provided in the
studies to actually correct the correlations for the effects of two important artifacts. Second,
the transformation has the effect of making the distribution of correlations more normal,
which has advantages when pooling across studies and assessing publication bias (Duval &
Tweedie, 2000). Finally, the original intention of the z-transformation was to provide an
estimate of the standard error based solely on the sample size. Using this estimate of the
standard error allowed for estimating the regression parameters by fixing the L1 error term
for each study independently (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Each study sampling error was
estimated using the following formula for the standard error:
Vj=1=nj3
;
where V
j
is the L1 error variance, and n
j
is the sample size of the jth study.
Given the above rationale, d
j
=z
j
in the L1 model outlined above, the observed effect size
for the jth study was the z-transformed Pearson product-moment correlation, and the L1
variance was assumed known from the sample size of the study. All HLM statistical
analyses used the z-transformed correlations, but nearly all results (except publication bias)
are reported using Pearson rfor ease of interpretation.
Testing for moderating variables
The examination of moderator variable effects was performed in two stages. The first
stage used only the Total Reading Scores as the criterion reading measures. For this
analysis, the effects of the following moderating variables were investigated individually:
grade level (grades 1 to 5), administration type (individual vs. group) for only the national
tests, test type (state-specific test vs. national) for only the group-administered tests, and the
length of time between tests (within vs. across academic years). The analysis of grade level
was done by using the 3rd grade as the reference group and dummy coding 4 dichotomous
variables to reflect grades 1, 2, 4, and 5.
The investigation of administration type was restricted to national tests, as there were no
state-specific tests that were individually administered. Therefore, the investigation of the
449A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
moderating effect of state-specific and national tests was restricted to only the group
administered tests. It was thought to be inappropriate to investigate the effects of national
versus state tests without partitioning out the potentially more reliable individually
administered tests from the national tests. For the analysis of length of time between
assessments, all across-year coefficients were grouped. An a priori family-wise error rate
was set at α= .05. With the seven multiple tests outlined above, a Bonferroni corrected p-value
of α⁎=.007 was used for the individual test values.
The second stage used only the scores on the subtests measuring specific areas of
reading skill (e.g., Vocabulary and Comprehension). It was necessary to exclude Total
Reading Scores, which are typically composites of subtest scores, to ensure the correlation
coefficients were independent. For this analysis, the Comprehension subtests were used as
the reference group, and dummy coding was used for the other three domains: Vocabulary,
Word Identification, and Decoding. An a priori family-wise error rate was set at α= .05.
With the three multiple tests outlined above, a Bonferroni corrected p-value of α⁎= .02 was
used for the individual test values.
Publication bias
Two methods were used to assess publication bias, a trim and fill analysis based on
funnel plots (Duval, 2005; Duval & Tweedie, 2000), and a file drawer analysis (Orwin,
1983; Rosenthal, 1979). An example of a funnel plot based on Fisher's ztransformation is
shown in Fig. 1. In the figure, precision in the form of the inverse of the standard error (1/
SE(z)) is plotted against Fisher's zfor the total score of the score type correlations (see
Fig. 1. Funnel plot of precision (1 / SE(z)) as a function of Fisher's zfor the score type of total score. Note: The
vertical dashed line is the mean zused as a basis for evaluating symmetry (see text).
450 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Table 3). The vertical dashed line is the mean value based on the 154 Fisher's zvalues.
Sampling variability is a function of precision, which gives rise to the funnel-like shape of
the points in Fig. 1 (wider at the bottom and narrower at the top). Publication bias is
indicated by asymmetry about the mean value and typically is a result of missing values in
the lower left of the funnel plot as low-valued non-significant correlations tend not to be
published (Begg & Berlin, 1988).
Trim and fill analysis uses an iterative algorithm that assesses the asymmetry in the
funnel plot (the trim part) and imputes the “missing”Fisher's zvalues to create a symmetric
funnel plot (the fill part). If no missing values are imputed, this is an indication of
statistically sufficient funnel plot symmetry and minimal publication bias. When missing
values are imputed, the mean zvalue and confidence interval are re-estimated based on the
observed and filled-in values. An important index is the difference in the mean zvalue for
the initial funnel plot and the mean zvalue for the filled-in funnel plot. Smaller differences
Table 3
Study correlations and publication bias results.
Main effect Initial analysis Trim and fill analysis File drawer
NP25 P50 P75 Mean
r
LCI UCI Mean
r
LCI UCI Missing
studies
Diff.
r
N
f
5N+10
All types 289 0.61 0.68 0.74 0.68 0.67 0.69 0.65 0.64 0.66 49 0.03 1649 1455
Grade
1 32 0.57 0.66 0.72 0.68 0.64 0.71 0.68 0.64 0.71 0 0 174 170
2 61 0.63 0.7 0.76 0.71 0.69 0.72 0.69 0.67 0.71 4 0.01 353 315
3 97 0.62 0.68 0.73 0.67 0.66 0.68 0.65 0.64 0.66 18 0.02 559 495
4 48 0.59 0.65 0.76 0.67 0.64 0.7 0.67 0.64 0.7 0 0 258 250
5 32 0.53 0.64 0.69 0.62 0.59 0.66 0.59 0.55 0.63 5 0.03 168 170
6 4 0.59 0.62 0.66 0.62 0.56 0.67 0.62 0.56 0.67 0 0 21 30
1–3 Comb 3 0.64 0.65 0.75 0.68 0.57 0.77 0.68 0.57 0.77 0 0 17 25
Other 12 0.68 0.77 0.86 0.77 0.72 0.82 0.76 0.7 0.81 1 0.01 77 70
Score type
Comprehension 72 0.58 0.66 0.74 0.67 0.64 0.69 0.64 0.62 0.66 8 0.02 414 370
Vocabulary 27 0.59 0.63 0.73 0.66 0.63 0.68 0.63 0.6 0.65 5 0.03 138 145
Word
identification
11 0.69 0.87 0.89 0.83 0.76 0.88 0.83 0.76 0.88 0 0 76 65
Decoding 25 0.52 0.61 0.7 0.61 0.59 0.63 0.61 0.58 0.63 1 0 128 135
Total score 154 0.64 0.68 0.74 0.69 0.68 0.69 0.65 0.64 0.66 37 0.04 893 780
Time between assessments
Within year 260 0.62 0.68 0.74 0.68 0.68 0.69 0.66 0.65 0.67 37 0.02 1499 1310
Across years 20 0.61 0.63 0.68 0.64 0.61 0.66 0.63 0.61 0.66 1 0.01 106 110
Across years
(2 or more yrs)
9 0.5 0.58 0.66 0.6 0.54 0.65 0.6 0.54 0.65 0 0 40 55
Type of test administration
Individual 48 0.69 0.77 0.86 0.77 0.73 0.81 0.76 0.72 0.8 3 0.01 302 250
Group 241 0.6 0.67 0.72 0.66 0.66 0.67 0.64 0.63 0.65 29 0.02 1374 1215
Test type
National 218 0.61 0.69 0.76 0.69 0.68 0.7 0.67 0.66 0.68 26 0.02 1252 1100
State-specific 71 0.61 0.66 0.69 0.64 0.63 0.66 0.63 0.62 0.65 12 0.01 401 365
Note. P25 = 2 5th Percentile , LCI = lower confid ence interval b ound, UCI = upper co nfidence inter val bound,
Diff. r=difference in mean r,N
f
=failsafe N.
451A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
indicate minimal publication bias whereas larger differences indicate greater bias. The trim
and fill method is based on symmetry assumptions, so that the funnel plot should be based
on Fisher's zrather than Pearson's r(Duval & Tweedie, 2000). In addition, one must
condition on the grouping variables, so that a trim and fill analysis will be performed for
each group defined in Table 3.
In a file drawer analysis, the focus is on the number of non-significant studies it takes to
nullify the overall effect (Rosenthal, 1979). More specifically, one estimates the number of
studies in which the null hypothesis, H
0
:ρ= 0, was not rejected that are required to render the
mean result in the meta-analysis to be statistically non-significant. This number is referred to
as the failsafe N, denoted as N
f
(Orwin, 1983). A large value of N
f
represents minimal bias as
this implies very many non-significant studies would have to be in the file drawer and thus,
missing from the meta-analysis. Conversely, a small N
f
represents greater bias as the meta-
analysis results are vulnerable to being nullified based on a few file drawer studies.
Results
Preliminary results and overall correlational results
A forest plot of all the correlations used in the meta-analysis may be found in Fig. 2. The
filled symbols are the values of Pearson's r, the vertical lines indicate the 95% confidence
intervals, and the dotted horizontal line is the overall mean. Additional information is
shown in Table 3 listed by study characteristics (main effects), including the total number of
correlations (N), the quartiles, and the mean with its associated confidence interval limits.
Fig. 2 shows the variability among the correlations, and Table 3 provide a general picture of
the results based on the study characteristics.
Fig. 2. Forest plot of all correlations. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the 95% confidence
interval, and the dashed horizontal line is the mean.
452 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
The weighted average and standard deviation (SD) were computed using the formula
found in Hunter and Schmidt (1990). The median correlation coefficient across all 289
coefficients reported in the studies was .68 with an interquartile range of .61 to .74 (see
Table 3). These results were very similar to the weighted average, .67, and SD, .06.
Variability was found across the levels of most of the main effects with the means and
medians quite similar in magnitude and the interquartile range and standard deviation
similarly wide. Most correlation coefficients tended to be in the .60 to .70 range with a few
outliers, such as .55 for across years validity with the criterion test being given 2 or more
years after the R-CBM measures, or a .75 on the individually administered tests.
Results reported in Table 3 are the Pearson product-moment correlation estimates from
the studies themselves and might differ from the results reported in the following evaluation
of main effects of possible moderating variables for two main reasons. First of all, the Fisher
ztransformation was used for the analysis of main effects, thus the transformation back to
the Pearson correlation metric might be slightly biased upwards (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).
Second, some main effects analyses were run on a subset of the total number of factors. For
example the comparison of nationally derived or state specific tests was only done at the
level of Total Reading Score and, thus, coefficients from subtests on the nationally available
tests would be found within the computations in Table 3, but not in the evaluation of main
effects.
Comparison of state-specific and national group-administered tests
A statistically significant estimate of the z-transformed correlation on the state-specific
tests was found (N= 70), γ
0
= 0.77, t(139) = 46.92, pb.001. This confirmed evidence that
R-CBM was a significant predictor of state-specific tests of reading standards. There was
also a significant positive increase related to the national tests (N= 71), γ
1
= 0.18, t(139) =
4.56, pb.001. The reliability of estimating the population z-transformed correlation
coefficient was found to be .81. Significant variation was also found between studies over
and above sampling error and the test type moderator variable, χ
2
(139) = 2668.84, pb.001.
Thus, the expected correlation coefficient with national tests of .74 was found to be
significantly higher than the correlation coefficient of .65 with state-specific tests. It should
be noted that the estimate for the national tests was based on about one-third of the total
number of coefficients noted in Table 3, which suggested that the ratio of subtests for the
national tests to Total Reading Scores was about 2:1 in the dataset. In addition, significant
variability between studies remained, suggesting that other potential moderating variables
might be found. A forest plot of the correlations and confidence intervals for this effect is
shown in Fig. 3.
Comparison of individual and group-administered national tests
A statistically significant estimate of the z-transformed correlation coefficient on the
individually administered tests was found (n= 13), γ
0
= 1.18, t(81) = 20.10, pb.001. This
confirmed evidence that R-CBM was a significant predictor of state-specific tests of
reading standards. There was also a significant negative estimate, reflecting a significant
decrease in the magnitude of the correlation, related to the group-administered tests
453A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
(N= 70), γ
1
=−0.29, t(139) = −4.59, pb.001. Significant variation was also found between
studies above both sampling error and the test type moderator variable, χ
2
(81) = 1571.53,
pb.001. Thus, the expected correlation coefficient of the individually administered tests of
.83 was significantly higher than the correlation coefficient of .71 with group administered
tests. In addition, significant variability between studies remained. One explanation for
these results is that individually administered tests may have higher reliability estimates
than group-administered tests, and therefore, due to the lack of correction for unreliability,
might have produced higher correlation coefficients. A forest plot of the correlations and
confidence intervals for this effect is shown in Fig. 4.
R-CBM and Total Reading Scores by grade
A statistically significant estimate of the z-transformed correlation coefficients for the
3rd grade students was found (n=57), γ
0
= 0.87, t(147) = 34.02, pb.001. This results
confirmed evidence that R-CBM was a significant predictor of third grade reading
outcomes. There were no statistically significant differences (all pN.007) for the first grade
(n= 22; p= .28), second grade (n= 34; p= .53), fourth grade (n= 24; p= .29), and fifth grade
Fig. 3. Forest plot of state/national main effect. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the 95%
confidence interval, and the dashed horizontal line is the mean.
454 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
(n= 15; p= .04) students. These results suggested that the magnitude of the correlation
between grades was not significantly different. The forest plot of the correlations and
confidence intervals for this effect is shown in x Fig. 5. Even though no significant
differences between grades were found, there was still significant between-studies
variation, χ
2
(147) = 3,026.64, pb.001. The expected correlation coefficient across grades 1
to 5 was .70, and significant variability suggested other potential moderating variables
might be found.
Length of time
A statistically significant estimate of the z-transformed correlation coefficient for within
an academic year was found (current grade) (n= 126), γ
0
= 0.88, t(152) = 51.60, pb.001.
This confirmed prior evidence that R-CBM was a significant predictor of reading skills
when the criterion test was taken within the same academic year. There was a statistically
significant difference for the across-academic-years (n= 28) estimates, γ
1
=−0.14, t(152) =
−3.58, p= .001. This negative estimate suggested that there was a significant decrease in
the magnitude of the correlations when the time span between R-CBM and criterion
Fig. 4. Forest plot of individual/group main effect. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the 95%
confidence interval, and the dashed horizontal line is the mean.
455A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Fig. 5. Forest plot of reading score by grade main effect. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the9 5%confid ence interval, and the dashed horizontalline is the mean.
456 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
measures were obtained, which comports to the common finding that correlations tend to
decrease in magnitude when the time span between measurement occasions increases.
Statistically significant variability was also found between studies, χ
2
(152) = 2,544.05,
pb.001. Thus, the expected correlation coefficient for the current grade was .71. A
significantly lower correlation coefficient of .63 (which included across-year tests taken at
least 1 year after the R-CBM) was found for all across-year studies. These results were
expected because correlation coefficients tend to decrease in magnitude as the time between
assessments increases. The forest plot of the correlations and confidence intervals for this
effect is presented in Fig. 6.
Individual and group-administered reading subtest scores
A statistically significant estimate of the z-transformed correlation coefficient for the
Comprehension subtests (N= 72) was found, γ
0
= 0.80, t(131) = 31.01, pb.001. This result
indicated that R-CBM was a significant predictor of reading comprehension. There were no
statistically significant differences (pN.02) found for the Vocabulary (n= 27; p= .96) and
Fig. 6. Forest plot of length of time main effect. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the 95%
confidence interval, and the dashed horizontal line is the mean.
457A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Decoding (n= 25; p= .10) subtests, which indicated that R-CBM tended to correlate as
highly with these domains or reading skills as it did with Comprehension. However, there
was a statistically significant increase found for the Word Identification (n= 11) subtests,
γ
2
= 0.36, t(131) = 4.71, pb.001, which suggested that R-CBM tends to be more highly
correlated with isolated word reading skills than Comprehension, Vocabulary, and
Decoding measures. Statistically significant between studies-variation was also found,
χ
2
(131) = 1,109.19, pb.001. Thus, the expected correlation coefficient for Comprehen-
sion, Vocabulary, and Decoding subtests were all found to be .66 and was .82 with
Word Identification subtests. The forest plot of the correlations and confidence
intervals for this effect is presented in Fig. 7.
Publication bias
The results of the trim and fill and file drawer analysis are shown in the right-hand
columns of Table 3. All results are expressed in terms of Pearson's rfor ease of
interpretation (recall the trim and fill analysis is based on Fisher's z). For the trim and fill
analysis, the number of missing studies is 0 in a number of instances, indicating a relatively
symmetric funnel plot and evidence of minimal bias. For the cases in which the number of
missing studies is greater than 0, there is very little difference in the estimated mean r
between the initial and imputed funnel plots, with the largest difference being 0.04 for the
score type of total score. The small mean difference indicates that in general, the funnel
plots are relatively symmetric and represent minimal bias.
The failsafe N(N
f
) for the file drawer analysis is shown in the second to last column of
Table 3. There is some debate as to what criterion should be used to evaluate N
f
. A stringent
criterion indexes minimal bias as the case when N
f
N5N+10 (Mullen, Muellerleile, &
Bryant, 2001). Based on this criterion, there were seven instances in which N
f
b5N+ 10,
indicating potential bias. However, even in the most extreme case, which is Grade 6 (5N+
10 −N
f
= 9), N
f
was not much smaller than the cutoff criterion. A less stringent criterion
indexes minimal bias, as in the case when N
f
NN(McDaniel, Rothstein, & Whetzel, 2006).
Table 3 shows that in all instances N
f
NN, providing evidence of minimal bias.
Discussion
There are over three decades of research on the psychometric properties of CBM scores.
CBM, originally designed to evaluate the effectiveness of instruction and intervention with
individual students, is widely used in education for the purposes of screening, bench-
marking, goal-setting, and program evaluation, as well as individual progress monitoring
across general, remedial, and special education. One purpose of this study was to quanti-
tatively summarize the correlational evidence of the association between scores derived
from R-CBM, the most widely used and researched of the CBM measures, and scores from
other standardized measures of reading achievement. A second purpose was to examine
potential moderating variables of the correlation coefficients between R-CBM and criterion
measures of reading achievement as a function of characteristics of students (grade level,
demographics) and the criterion measures (test source, administration format, type of
score). The findings from this meta-analysis reveal a very clear picture. The association
458 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Fig. 7. Forest plot of type of reading score main effect. Note: Filled symbol is Pearson's r, vertical line is the 95% confidence interval, and the das hed horizontal line is the mean.
459A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
between scores from R-CBM probes and those derived from other standardized tests of
reading achievement is moderately high (weighted average r= .67), indicating R-CBM
scores function as reasonably good indicators of how well students are likely to perform
across a wide range of reading achievement tests. This finding is remarkable when one
considers the low cost in terms of resources, both time and financial, required to administer
and score R-CBM probes.
The relationship between R-CBM probe scores and scores from other tests of reading
achievement varied as a function of the source of test and administration format. The overall
correlation coefficient between R-CBM scores and those from individually administered
achievement tests was higher than that between R-CBM and group-administered
achievement tests. The higher correlation may be due to higher reliability and validity
coefficients found for scores derived from individually administered, nationally available
and normed tests of achievement, or it may reflect the similarity in administration format.
These results provide further support for the use of R-CBM with individual students.
As expected, significant differences were found among state and national group-
administered tests of student achievement, with a higher correlation among R-CBM and
national tests than with state-specific tests. One reason for this difference may be that the
national tests are developed to gauge general reading achievement whereas state-specific
tests are designed to assess specific grade-level standards. However, significant between-
study variability remained, indicating there are other moderating variables to be discovered.
One likely difference in the national and state-specific tests is the higher quality of
development, representativeness of the samples, and other technical characteristics of the
national tests. Given the varying difficulty levels and quality in the state-specific achieve-
ment tests (Peterson & Hess, 2005; Wallis & Steptoe, 2007), quality may account for this
between-study variability. Although lower than the correlation with national tests, the
association with state-specific tests was significant and moderately strong, which provides
additional support for the practice of using R-CBM in general education for screening and
identifying students at-risk for future low performance on state standards tests. However,
these results also highlight the need for tests with an accumulation of reliability and validity
evidence to support their use in high-stakes assessments and decisions about students,
educators, schools, and districts.
Another notable finding is that the range of correlations between R-CBM scores and
those of various reading subtests was relatively small. There were no differences among
score types, with the exception of Word Identification. Higher correlations were found
among scores from R-CBM probes and scores on subtests of Word Identification. The
magnitude of the difference between these correlation coefficients and those derived with
R-CBM probe scores and scores from other reading subtests might be a spurious result
given the relatively small number of Word Identification subtest correlations contained in
the meta-analysis (n= 11 vs. 25, 27, and 72 for Decoding, Vocabulary, and Comprehension,
respectively). Of these 11 correlations, eight were from the Woodcock Reading Mastery
Tests (original and revised versions; Woodcock, 1973, 1987), two were from the
Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Achievement (Woodcock et al., 2001) and one was from
the original Stanford Achievement test (Madden, Gardner, Rudman, Karlsen, & Merwin,
1973). The fact that no significant differences were evident in the correlations between
R-CBM and tests of Comprehension, Decoding, and Vocabulary adds credence to the ability
460 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
of R-CBM scoresto act as a general outcome measure of overall reading ability and is contrary
to one of the primary criticisms of R-CBM—that it appears to measure decoding rather than
comprehension or other reading skills (Hamilton & Shinn, 2003). Further, it is consistent with
conclusions by Fuchs and colleagues (2001) and other research suggesting that R-CBM is an
indicator of reading comprehension (Fuchs & Fuchs, 1986; Shinn, Good, Knutson, Tilly, &
Collins, 1992), as well as overall reading proficiency.
The question of how consistent the strength of the relationship is between scores from
R-CBM probes and scores from other standardized tests of reading achievement across
the elementary grades has not been entirely answered in the present study. Previous research
found disparate results with respect to this question (e.g., Hosp & Fuchs, 2005; Jenkins &
Jewell, 1993; Kranzler et al., 1999). One hypothesis from research that indicated a decline
in the correlation across grades is that there is a change in what the criterion achievement tests
measure as students progress through elementary school (Jenkins & Jewell, 1993). However,
our results do not indicate that a significant decline occurs in the criterion validity of R-CBM
across grades 1 through 6. We cannot be certain of our conclusion on this issue, however, in
that there were insufficient data to examine these correlations as a function of both grade-level
and type of score.
R-CBM is increasingly used to predict performance on high-stakes assessments. A
logical extension of this practice is to predict performance across years in order to inform
early intervention efforts. Early identification of those at risk for obtaining non-passing
scores on high stakes assessments is particularly desirable given the evidence that suggests
many reading difficulties could be prevented (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Torgesen,
2000) and the stability of reading difficulties from the second or third grade forward (Juel,
1988). Data from this study showed declines in the magnitude of the correlation across
years. However, predictions across two or more academic years were still significant and
moderately high, supporting the use of R-CBM as an indicator, or benchmark, of future
performance on high-stakes assessments.
We were unable to examine correlations between R-CBM scores and scores of other tests
of reading achievement as a function of student demographic characteristics. There appears
to be a great deal of variability in the samples of students with which R-CBM has been used
(Table 1). However, examinations of the extent to which scores from tests function similarly
for individuals of different backgrounds or groups is crucial to establishing the validity of
inferences drawn from any test (AERA et al., 1999) and the disparate results of studies that
examined predictive bias for R-CBM and tests of reading achievement and comprehension
do not allow firm conclusions to be drawn (e.g., Hintze et al., 2002; Klein & Jimerson, 2005;
Kranzler et al., 1999).
Finally, the potential effects of publication bias on the meta-analytic results were explored
through two separate analyses. A fill-and-trim analysis (Duval, 2005; Duval & Tweedie,
2000) was used to check for systematic publication bias based on funnel plots like the one
shown in Fig. 1. The results showed that average Pearson rvalues adjusted for asymmetry in
the funnel plots were very close to the original values. This provides evidence that the funnel
plots were reasonably symmetrical, which is indicative of minimal publication bias.
Furthermore, we conducted an analysis to address the “file drawer problem”that non-
statistically significant results tend not to be published (Rosenthal, 1979). The number of file
drawer studies was estimated and evaluated based on two different stringency criteria. The
461A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
results show that even when using the more stringent standard, there was no substantial
evidence of extensive bias. Taking the bias results collectively, we feel there is relatively
strong evidence our meta-analysis results are not unduly tainted by publication bias.
Limitations and future directions
It is necessary to note limitations of this study, as well as directions for future research. A
significant limitation was our inability to correct for errors in measurement of R-CBM
probes and other reading tests as well as restriction of range in scores. The data necessary to
make adjustments to correlation coefficients due to range restriction and instrument
unreliability were not available in most of the articles included in this meta-analysis. This
finding is disturbing in light of the fact that even the minimum requirement of reporting
means and standard deviations of the R-CBM instruments and other standardized tests of
reading achievement was missing in a substantial number of studies. In addition, the lack of
reporting of reliability in the samples used within the reported studies runs counter to recent
recommendations on technical reporting for research in psychology (Wilkinson & Task
Force on Statistical Inference, 1999). Reliability could have been computed for criterion
measures directly from the item responses. In addition, several studies included multiple
R-CBM passages, typically three, in each administration. An estimate of the reliability could
have been easily computed by taking the intercorrelations between passages or running a
simple factor analysis and computing the reliability directly. This lack of reliability estimates
for the diverse samples also precludes any attempts to conduct reliability generalizability
studies (Vacha-Haase, 1998).
Another limitation that reflects the current status of this literature is the varying number
of correlations included in each study; therefore, some studies and samples contributed
several correlations to the meta-analysis, whereas others may have only reported one or two
correlations. There is no way to correct for the uneven distribution of the number of
correlation coefficients across studies, but it should be noted in the interpretation of these
results. In addition, there are numerous potential moderating variables of the association
between R-CBM probe scores and scores from other standardized measures of reading
achievement. This study focused on characteristics of students (e.g., grade level) and the
criterion measures. Future research may also address other potential moderating variables
related to R-CBM, such as whether one or three passages were given, source of passages
(e.g., publishing company, curriculum), the psychometric aspects of different passage sets
(Betts, Pickart, & Heistad, 2009; Christ & Ardoin, 2009), and the use of specific grade-level
or universal passages, among other things. In addition, criterion measures were grouped
according to what the tests themselves purported to measure. Additional research may
examine the stimulus items on these tests and group them according to types of passages or
tasks that are used or forms of comprehension (e.g., literal, inferential). This type of analysis
in conjunction with examination of grade-level data may illuminate the decline across grade
levels observed in some studies.
Finally, future research may also address other limitations to this research, such as
empirical examinations of bias for students of varying socioeconomic, language, and
racial–ethnic backgrounds. Another important area of research will examine the use of
CBM with students who are non-native English speakers. Evidence is accumulating that
462 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
scores from R-CBM are a valid indicator of reading achievement and sensitive to growth
for English Learner (EL) students in English (Baker & Good, 1995; Kung, 2007; Ramirez
& Shapiro, 2006; Wiley & Deno, 2005). Given the growing EL population in schools
across the U.S. (U.S. Department of Education, 2006), increased accountability for the
performance of EL students through initiative and legislations such as No Child Left
Behind, and poor educational outcomes for students who are not native English speakers
(e.g., Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2007; Perie, Grigg, &
Donahue, 2005), research examining whether these measures are reliable, valid, sensitive to
growth, and of high utility when used in conjunction with a Problem-Solving Model (Deno,
2005) with this population is needed.
Conclusion
CBM was designed to provide educators with a set of tasks that were reliable, valid, low-
cost, and time-efficient indicators of student achievement in core academic areas. In
reading, there is remarkable consistency in the relationship between R-CBM and other
standardized measures of reading achievement across decades, samples, and various
achievement tests. These results are extraordinary when one considers the brevity,
availability, and low-cost of R-CBM. Educators should have great confidence in their use of
R-CBM as an indicator of students' overall reading achievement.
References
2
Allinder, R. M., & Eccarius, M. A. (1999). Exploring the technical adequacy of Curriculum-Based Measurement in
Reading for children who use manually coded English. Exceptional Children,65, 271−283.
Alonso, J., & Tindal, G. (2003). Analysis of reading fluency and comprehension measures for sixth grade students
(Tech. Rep. No. 24). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.
American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on
Measurement in Education. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC:
AERA.
*Ardoin, S. P., Witt, J. C., Suldo, S. M., Connell, J. E., Koenig, J. L., Resetar, J. L., et al. (2004). Examining the
incremental benefits of administering a MAZE and three versus one curriculum-based measurement reading
probes when conducting universal screening. School Psychology Review,33, 218−233.
*Bain, S. K., & Garlock, J. W. (1992). Cross-validation of criterion-related validity for CBM reading passages.
Diagnostique,17, 202−208.
*Baker, S. K., & Good, R. (1995). Curriculum-Based Measurement of English reading with bilingual Hispanic
students: A validation study with 2nd grade students. School Psychology Review,24, 561−578.
*Baker, S. K., Smolkowski, K., Katz, R., Fien, H., Seeley, J. R., Kame’enui, E. J., et al. (2008). Reading fluency as
a predictor of reading proficiency in low-performing, high-poverty schools. School Psychology Review,37,
18−37.
*Barger, J. (2003). Comparing the DIBELS oral reading fluency indicator and the North Carolina end of grade
reading assessment (Tech. Rep.). Asheville, NC: North Carolina Teacher Academy.
Begg, C. B., & Berlin, J. A. (1988). Publication bias: a problem in interpreting medical data. Journal of the Royal
Statistical Society,151, 419−463.
Betts, J., Pickart, M., & Heistad, D. (2009). An investigation of the psychometric evidence of CBM-R passage
equivalence: Utility of readability statistics and equating for alternate forms. Journal of School Psychology,47,
1−17.
2
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
463A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
*Buck, J., & Torgesen, J. (2003). The relationship between performance on a measure of oral reading fluency and
performance on the Florida Comprehensive Assessment Test (FCRR Tech. Rep. No. 1). Tallahassee, FL: Florida
State University.
*Carlisle, J. F., Schilling, S. G., Scott, S. E., & Zeng, J. (2004). Do fluency measures predict reading achievement?
Results from the 2002-2003 school year in Michigan's reading first schools (Tech. Rep. No. 1, Evaluation of
Reading First in Michigan). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.
Christ, T., & Ardoin, S. (2009). Curriculum-based measurement of reading: Passage equivalence and selection.
Journal of School Psychology,47,55−75.
*Colon, E. P., & Kranzler, J. H. (2006). Effect of instructions on Curriculum Based Measurement of Reading.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,24, 318−328.
*Crawford, L., Tindal, G., & Steiber, S. (2001). Using oral reading rate to predict student performance on statewide
assessment tests. Educational Assessment,7, 303−323.
CTB/McGraw-Hill. (1983). Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills. Monterey, CA: Author.
CTB/McGraw-Hill. (1985). California Achievement Test. Monterey, CA: Author.
CTB/McGraw-Hill. (2003). TerraNova second edition: California Achievement Tests technical report. Monterey,
CA: Author.
Deno, S. L. (1985). Curriculum-based measurement: The emerging alternative. Exceptional Children,52,
219−232.
Deno, S. L. (1992). The nature and development of curriculum-based measurement. Preventing School Failure,
36,5−10.
Deno, S. L. (2005). Problem-solving assessment with Curriculum-based Measurement (CBM). In R. Brown-
Chidsey (Ed.), Problem-solving based assessment for educational intervention New York: Guilford Press.
Deno, S. L., & Mirkin, P. K. (1977). Data-based program modification: A manual. Reston, VA: Council for
Exceptional Children.
*Deno, S. L., Mirkin, P., & Chiang, B. (1982). Identifying valid measures of reading. Exceptional Children,49,
36−45.
*Deno, S. L., Mirkin, P., Chiang, B., & Lowry, L. (1980). Relationships among simple measures of reading and
performance on standardized achievement tests. Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities Tech. Rep. No.
20 Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.
Deno, S., Reschly, A. L., Lembke, E., Magnussen, D., Callender, S., Windram, H., et al. (2009). A school-wide
model for progress monitoring. Psychology in the Schools,46,44−55.
Dunn, L. M., & Markwardt, F. C. (1970). Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: American
Guidance Service.
Duval, S. J. (2005). The “trim and fill”method. In H. R. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication
bias in meta analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments (pp. 127−144). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Duval, S. J., & Tweedie, R. L. (2000). A non-parametric “trim and fill”method of accounting for publication bias
in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association,95,89−98.
Espin, C. A., Shin, J., & Busch, T. W. (2005). Curriculum-based measurement in the content areas: Vocabulary-
matching as an indicator of social studies learning. Journal of Learning Disabilities,38, 353−363.
Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. (2007). America's children: Key national indicators of
well-being, 2007.Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office Retrieved on September 6, 2007 from www.childstats.gov
Field, A. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo Comparison of fixed- and random-
effects methods. Psychological Methods,6, 161−180.
Foegen, A., Espin, C. A., Allinder, R. M., & Markell, M. A. (2001). Translating research into practice: Preservice
teachers' beliefs about Curriculum-Based Measurement. Journal of Special Education,34, 226−236.
Fuchs, L. S., & Deno, S. L. (1991). Paradigmatic distinctions between instructionally relevant measurement
models. Exceptional Children,57, 488−500.
*Fuchs, L. S., Deno, S. L., & Marston, D. (1982). Use of aggregation to improve the reliability of simple direct
measure of academic performance,Vol. IRLD-RR-94. (pp. 33): University of Minnesota, Minneapolis Institute
for Research on Learning Disabilities.
Fuchs, L. S., Deno, S. L., & Mirkin, P. K. (1984). Effects of frequent curriculum-based measurement and
evaluation on pedagogy, student achievement, and student awareness of learning. American Educational
Research Journal,21, 449−460.
464 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (1986). Effects of systematic formative evaluation on student achievement. Exceptional
Children,53, 199−207.
Fuchs, L. S., & Fuchs, D. (1999). Monitoring student progress toward the development of reading competence:
A review of three forms of classroom based assessment. School Psychology Review,28, 659−671.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., & Ferguson, C. (1992). Effects of expert system consultation within
curriculum-based measurement using a reading maze task. Exceptional Children,58, 436−450.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading
competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading,5, 239−256.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal reading comprehension measures.
Remedial and Special Education,9,20−29.
Geisinger, K. F., Spies, R. A., Carlson, J. F., & Plake, B. S. (Eds.). (2007). The seventeenth mental measurements
yearbook Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
Glenn, D. (2007). Consultants in reading program had conflicts, report says. Chronicle of Higher Education,53
(29), A16.
*Good, R. H., Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2001). The importance of decision-making utility of a
continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third grade high-stakes outcomes.
Scientific Studies of Reading,5, 257−288.
Goodman, K. S. (2006). The truth about DIBELS: What it is, what it does. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Greenwood, C. R., Dunn, S., Ward, S. M., & Luze, G. J. (2003). The Early Communication Indicator (ECI) for infants
and toddlers: What it is, where it's been, and where it needs to go. The Behavior Analyst Today,3,383−388.
Grimes, J., & Tilly, W. D. (1996). Policy and process: Means to lasting educational change. School Psychology
Review,25, 465−476.
Hamilton, C., & Shinn, M. R. (2003). Characteristics of word callers: An investigation of the accuracy of teachers'
judgments of reading comprehension and oral reading skills. School Psychology Review,32, 228−240.
Hedges, L., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.
Hedges, L., & Vevea, J. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods,3,
486−504.
*Hintze, J., Callahan, J., Matthews, W., Williams, S., & Tobin, K. (2002). Oral reading fluency and prediction of
reading comprehension in African American and Caucasian elementary school children. School Psychology
Review,31,540−554.
*Hintze, J. M., Shapiro, E. S., Conte, K. L., & Beslie, I. M. (1997). Oral reading fluency and authentic reading
material: Criterion validity of the technical features of CBM survey-level assessment. School Psychology
Review,26, 535−553.
*Hintze, J. M., & Silberglitt, B. (2005). A longitudinal examination of the diagnostic accuracy and predictive
validity of R-CBM and high stakes testing. School Psychology Review,34, 372−386.
*Hosp, M. K., & Fuchs, L. S. (2005). Using CBM as an indicator of decoding, word reading, and comprehension:
Do the relations change with grade? School Psychology Review,34,9−26.
Hunter, J., & Schmidt, F. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Hunter, J., & Schmidt, F. (2000). Fixed effects vs. random effects meta-analysis models: Implications for
cumulative research knowledge. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,8, 275−292.
*Jenkins, J. R., & Jewell, M. (1993). Examining the validity of two measures for formative teaching: Reading
aloud and maze. Exceptional Children,59, 421−432.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades.
Journal of Educational Psychology,80, 437−447.
Kamii, C., & Manning, M. (2005). Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS): A tool for
evaluating student learning? Journal of Research in Childhood Education,20,75−90.
Kaminitz-Berkooza, I., & Shapiro, E. S. (2005). The applicability of Curriculum-Based Measurement to measure
reading in Hebrew. School Psychology International,26, 494−519.
Kaminski, R. A., & Good, R. H. (1996). Towards a technology for assessing basic early literacy skills. School
Psychology Review,25, 215−227.
Kaminski, R. A., & Good, R. H. (1998). Assessing early literacy skills in a problem-solving model: Dynamic
Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills. In M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Advanced applications of curriculum-based
measurement (pp. 113−142). New York: Guilford Press.
465A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Karlsen, B., Madden, R., & Gardner, E. F. (1976). Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test. San Antonio, TX:
Psychological Corporation.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1985). Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Service.
*Ketterlin-Geller, L. R., & Tindal, G. (2004). Analysis of reading fluency and comprehension measures for 3rd
grade students (Tech. Rep. No. 22). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.
*Klein, J. R., & Jimerson, S. R. (2005). Examining ethnic, gender, language, and socioeconomic bias in oral
reading fluency scores among Caucasian and Hispanic students. School Psychology Quarterly,20,23−50.
Konstantopoulos, S., & Hedges, L. (2004). Meta-analysis. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative
methodology for the social sciences (pp. 281−297). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publishing.
Koslin, B. L., Koslin, S., Zeno, S. M., & Ivens, S. H. (1989). The degrees of Reading Power Test: Primary and
standard forms. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.
*Kranzler, J. H., Brownell, M. T., & Miller, M. D. (1998). The construct validity of Curriculum-Based
Measurement of Reading: An empirical test of a plausible rival hypothesis. Journal of School Psychology,36,
399−415.
*Kranzler, J. H., Miller, M. D., & Jordan, L. (1999). An examination of racial/ethnic and gender bias on
curriculum-based measurement of reading. School Psychology Quarterly,14, 327−342.
Kung, S., (2007). Predicting the success on a state standards test for culturally and linguistically diverse students
using curriculum-based oral reading Measures. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis.
LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive
Psychology,6, 293−323.
Lembke, E. S., Foegen, A., Whittaker, T. A., & Hampton, D. (2008). Establishing technically adequate measures of
progress in early numeracy. Assessment for Effective Intervention,33, 206−214.
Linn, R. L. (2005). Test-based educational accountability in the era of No Child Left Behind. (Tech. Rep.).
University of California Los Angeles,: Center for the Study of Evaluation, National Center for Research on
Evaluation, Standards, and Student Testing (CRESST).
MacGintie, W. H., Kamons, J., Kowalski, R. L., MacGintie, R. K., & McKay, T. (1978). Gates-MacGintie Reading
Tests, 2nd Ed. Chicago: Riverside Publishing.
Madden, R., Gardner, E. R., Rudman, H., Karlsen, B., & Merwin, J. C. (1973). Stanford Achievement Test. New
York: Harcourt Brace.
Manzo, K. K. (2005). National clout of DIBELS test draws scrutiny. Education Week,25(5), 1−12.
Manzo, K. K. (2005). States pressed to refashion Reading First grant designs. Education Week,25(2), 1−25.
Manzo, K. K. (2007). Ed. Department allowed singling out of ‘Reading First’products. Education Week,26(26), 13.
*Marston, D. (1989). A curriculum based measurement approach to assessing academic performance: What is it
and why do it. In M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Curriculum Based Measurement: Assessing Special Children (pp. 18−78).
New York: Guilford.
*Marston, D., & Deno, S. L. (1982). Implementation of direct and repeated measurement in the school setting,
Vol. IRLD-RR-106. (pp. 45): University of Minnesota, Minneapolis Institute for Research on Learning Disabilities.
Marston, D., Muyskens, P., Lau, M., & Canter, A. (2003). Problem-solving model for decision making with high-
incidence disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,18, 187−200.
McConnell, S. R., McEvoy, M. A., & Priest, J. S. (2002). “Growing”measures for monitoring progress in early
childhood education: A research and development proves for Individual Growth and Development Indicators.
Assessment for Effective Intervention,27,3−14.
McDaniel, M. A., Rothstein, H. R., & Whetzel, D. L. (2006). Publication bias: A case study of four test vendors.
Personnel Psychology,59, 927−953.
*McGlinchey, M. T., & Hixson, M. D. (2004). Using Curriculum Based Measurement to predict performance on
state assessments in reading. School Psychology Review,33, 193−203.
*McIntosh, A. S., Graves, A., & Gersten, R. (2007). The effects of response to intervention on literacy
development in multiple-language settings. Learning Disability Quarterly,30, 197−212.
Mirkin, P. K., Deno, S. L., Tindal, G., & Kuehnle,K. (1982). Frequency ofmeasurement and data utilization strategies
as factors in standardized behavioral assessment of academic skill. Journal of Behavioral Assessment,4,361−370.
Morgan, S. K., & Bradley-Johnson, S. (1995). Technical adequacy of curriculum-based measurement for Braille
readers. School Psychology Review,24,94−103.
466 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Mullen, B., Muellerleile, P., & Bryant, B. (2001). Cumulative meta-analysis: A consideration of indicators of
sufficiency and stability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,27, 1450−1462.
Naglieri, J. A., & Crockett, D. P. (2005). Response to Intervention (RTI): Is it a scientifically proven method?
Communiqué,34(2), 38−39.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching
children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications
for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
National Research Council. (1992). Combining information: Statistical issues and opportunities for research.
Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences Press.
Orwin, R. F. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics,8, 157−159.
Perie, M., Grigg, W. S., & Donahue, P. L. (2005). The Nation's Report Card: Reading 2005 (NCES 2006–451). U.S.
Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington,
D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Peterson, P. E., & Hess, F. M. (2005). Johnny Can Read…in some states. Education Next,5,52−53.
Powell-Smith, K. A., & Stewart, L. H. (1998). The use of curriculum-based measurement on the reintegration of
students with mild disabilities. In M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Advanced applications of curriculum-based measurement
(pp. 254−307). New York: Guilford Press.
Prescott, G. A., Balow, I. H., Hogan, T. P., & Farr, R. C. (1984). Metropolitan Achievement Tests (Mat-6). San
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Pressley, M., Hilden, K., & Shankland, R. (2005). An evaluation of end-grade-3 Dynamic Indicators of Basic
Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS): Speed reading without comprehension, predicting little (Tech. Rep.). East
Lansing, MI: Michigan State University, Literacy Achievement Research Center.
Raju, N., & Drasgow, F. (2003). Maximum likelihood estimation in validity generalization. In K. Murphy (Ed.),
Validity generalization: A critical review (pp. 263−286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ramirez, R. D., & Shapiro, E. S. (2006). CBM and the evaluation of reading skills of Spanish-speaking English
Language Learners in bilingual education classrooms. School Psychology Review,35, 356−369.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods,
2nd Ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Reschly, D. J., & Bergstrom, M. K. (2009). Response to intervention. In T. B. Gutkin & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), The
handbook of school psychology (pp. 434−460)., 4th Ed. New York: Wiley.
*Riedel, B. W. (2007). The relation between DIBELS, reading comprehension, and vocabulary in urban first-grade
students. Reading Research Quarterly,42, 546−567.
*Roehrig, A. D., Petscher, Y., Nettles, S. M., Hudson, R. F., & Torgesen, J. K. (2008). Accuracy of the DIBELS
oral reading fluency measure for predicting third grade reading comprehension outcomes. Journal of School
Psychology,46, 343−366.
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem”and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin,86,
638−641.
Salvia, J., & Ysseldyke, J. (2007). Assessment in special and inclusive education, 10th Ed. Boston: Houghton-
Mifflin.
Samuels, S. J. (2007). The DIBELS tests: Is speed of barking at print what we mean by reading fluency? Reading
Research Quarterly,42, 563−566.
*Schilling, S. G., Carlisle, J. F., Scott, S. E., & Zeng, J. (2007). Are fluency measures accurate predictors of reading
achievement? Elementary School Journal,107, 429−448.
Schmidt, F., & Hunter, J. (2003). History, development, evolution, and impact of validity generalization and meta-
analysis methods, 1975–2001. In K. Murphy (Ed.), Validity generalization: A critical review (pp. 31−65).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
*Shapiro, E. S., Keller, M. A., Lutz, J. G., Santoro, L. E., & Hintze, J. M. (2006). Curriculum based measures and
performance on state assessment and standardized tests: Reading and math performance in Pennsylvania.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,24,19−35.
*Shaw, R., & Shaw, D. (2002). DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency-Based Indicators of Third Grade Reading Skills for
Colorado State Assessment Program (CSAP) (Tech. Rep.). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.
Shinn, M. R. (1989). Identifying and defining academic problems: CBM screening and eligibility procedures. In
M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Curriculum Based Measurement: Assessing special children (pp. 90−129). New York:
Guilford Press.
467A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Shinn, M. R., & Bamonto, S. (1998). Advanced applications of Curriculum-Based Measurement: “Big ideas”and
avoiding confusion. In M. R. Shinn (Ed.), Advanced applications of Curriculum-Based Measurement (pp. 1−31).
New York: Guilford Press.
*Shinn, M. R., Good, R. H., Knutson, N., Tilly, W. D., & Collins, V. L. (1992). Curriculum-based measurement
reading fluency: A confirmatory analysis of its relation to reading. School Psychology Review,21, 459−479.
Shinn, M. R., Powell-Smith, K. A., Good, R. H., & Baker, S. (1997). The effects of reintegration into general
education reading instruction for students with mild disabilities. Exceptional Children,64,59−80.
*Sibley, D., Biwer, D., & Hesch, A. (2001). Establishing Curriculum-Based Measurement Oral Reading Fluency
performance standards to predict success on local and state tests of reading achievement (Tech Rep). Arlington
Heights, IL: Arlington Heights School District 25.
*Silberglitt, B., Burns, M. K., Madyun, N. H., & Lail, K. E. (2006). Relationship of reading fluency assessment data with
state accountability test scores: A longitudinal comparison of grade levels. Psychology in the Schools,43,527−535.
*Silberglitt, B., & Hintze, J. M. (2005). Formative assessment using CBM-R cut scores to track progress toward
success on state mandated achievement tests. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment,23, 304−325.
Snow, C. E., Burns, S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
*Sofie, C. A., & Riccio, C. A. (2002). A comparison of multiple methods for the identification of children with
reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities,35, 234−244.
*Speece, D. L., & Ritchey, K. D. (2005). A longitudinal study of the development of oral reading fluency in young
children at risk for reading failure. Journal of Learning Disabilities,38, 387−399.
*Stage, S. A., & Jacobsen, M. D. (2001). Predicting student success on a state-mandated performance-based
assessment using oral reading fluency. School Psychology Review,30, 407−419.
Stecker, P. M., & Fuchs, L. S. (2000). Effecting superior achievement using curriculum-based measurement: The
importance of individual progress monitoring. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,15, 128−134.
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Denise, M., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D. (2003). Rapid Naming,
Phonological Awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlation evidence. Review of Educational
Research,73, 407−440.
Tindal, G. (1989). Evaluating the effectiveness of educational programs at the systems level using curriculum-based
measurement. In M. Shinn (Ed.), Curriculum-based assessment: Assessing special children (pp. 202−238).
New York: Guilford Press.
*Tindal, G., & Marston, D. (1996). Technical adequacy of alternative reading measures as performance
assessments.Exceptionality,6, 201−230.
Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem
of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,15,55−64.
*Uribe-Zarain, X. (2007, February). Relationship between performance on DIBELS Oral Reading Fluency and
performance on the Reading DSTP Year 2005–2006. Newark, DE: Delaware Education Research &
Development Center, University of Delaware.
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2006). Public Elementary and Secondary
Students, Staff, Schools and School Districts: School Year 2003–04 (NCES 2006-307). Retrieved from http://
nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=96 on September 6, 2007.
Vacha-Haase, T. (1998). Reliability generalization: Exploring variance in measurement error affecting score
reliability across studies. Educational and Psychological Measurement,58,6−20.
*VanDerHeyden, A. M., Witt, J. C., & Naquin, G. (2003). Development and validation of a process for screening
referrals to special education. School Psychology Review,32, 204−227.
*Vander Meer, C. D., Lentz, F. E., & Stollar, S. (2005). The relationship between Oral Reading Fluency and Ohio
Proficiency Testing in Reading (Tech. Rep.). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.
Wallis, C., & Steptoe, S. (2007, May 24). How to fix No Child Left Behind. Time Retrieved January 28, 2009, from
http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1625192-1,00.html
Wayman, M., Wallace, T., Wiley, H. I., Ticha, R., & Espin, C. A. (2007). Literature synthesis on Curriculum-Based
Measurement in reading. Journal of Special Education,41,85−120.
Wesson, C., Deno, S. L., & King, R. (1984). Direct and frequent measurement of student performance: If it's good
for us, why don't we do it? Learning Disability Quarterly,7,45−48.
*Wiley, H. I., & Deno, S. L. (2005). Oral reading and maze measures as predictors of success for English learners
on a state standards assessment. Remedial and Special Education,26, 207−214.
468 A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469
Author's personal copy
Wilkinson, L., & Task Force on Statistical Inference (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines
and explanations. American Psychologist,54, 594−604.
Williams, K. T. (2001). Technical manual: Group Reading Assessment and Diagnostic Evaluation. Circle Pines,
MN: American Guidance Service.
*Wilson, J. (2005). The relationship of Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) Oral Reading
Fluency to performance on Arizona Instrument to Measure Standards (AIMS). Technical Report Tempe, AZ:
Tempe School District No. 3.
Wilson, M., Schendel, J. M., & Ulman, J. E. (1992). Curriculum-based measures, teachers' ratings, and group
achievement scores: Alternative screening measures. Journal of School Psychology,30,59−76.
Woodcock, R. W. (1973). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests, Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Services Inc.
Woodcock, R. W., & Mather, N. (1990). Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achievement - Revised. Allen, TX: DLM
Teaching Resources.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mathew, N. (2001). Woodcock–Johnson III Test of Achievement. Itasca, IL:
Riverside Publishing.
Yeh, C. (1992). The use of passage reading measures to assess reading proficiency of Chinese elementary school
students. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Minnesota.
Yell, M., Deno, S. L., & Marston, D. E. (1992). Barriers to implementing Curriculum-Based Measurement.
Diagnostique,18,99−105.
469A.L. Reschly et al. / Journal of School Psychology 47 (2009) 427–469