ArticlePDF Available

Expectation and Dopamine Release: Mechanism of the Placebo Effect in Parkinson's Disease

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The power of placebos has long been recognized for improving numerous medical conditions such as Parkinson's disease (PD). Little is known, however, about the mechanism underlying the placebo effect. Using the ability of endogenous dopamine to compete for [11C]raclopride binding as measured by positron emission tomography, we provide in vivo evidence for substantial release of endogenous dopamine in the striatum of PD patients in response to placebo. Our findings indicate that the placebo effect in PD is powerful and is mediated through activation of the damaged nigrostriatal dopamine system.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Expectation and Dopamine
Release: Mechanism of the
Placebo Effect in Parkinson’s
Disease
Rau´l de la Fuente-Ferna´ndez,
1
Thomas J. Ruth,
2
Vesna Sossi,
2
Michael Schulzer,
1
Donald B. Calne,
1
A. Jon Stoessl
1
*
The power of placebos has long been recognized for improving numerous
medical conditions such as Parkinson’s disease (PD). Little is known, however,
about the mechanism underlying the placebo effect. Using the ability of en-
dogenous dopamine to compete for [
11
C]raclopride binding as measured by
positron emission tomography, we provide in vivo evidence for substantial
release of endogenous dopamine in the striatum of PD patients in response to
placebo. Our findings indicate that the placebo effect in PD is powerful and is
mediated through activation of the damaged nigrostriatal dopamine system.
The simple act of receiving any treatment
(active or not) may, in itself, be efficacious
because of expectation of benefit (1). This is
the placebo effect—a potential confounder in
assessing the efficacy of any therapeutic in-
tervention (2, 3). Placebo-controlled studies
were designed precisely to control for such an
effect (4). It has been assumed that the pla-
cebo response is not mediated directly
through any physical or chemical effect of
treatment (5). In Parkinson’s disease (PD),
the placebo effect can be prominent (6, 7 ).
We asked whether the placebo effect in
PD is produced by activation of the pathway
primarily damaged by degeneration [i.e., the
nigrostriatal dopaminergic system (8, 9)]. To
answer this question, we took advantage of
the ability of positron emission tomography
(PET) to estimate pharmacologically or be-
haviorally induced dopamine release based
on the competition between endogenous do-
pamine and [
11
C]raclopride (RAC) for bind-
ing to dopamine D
2
/D
3
receptors (1014).
We hypothesized that if the placebo effect is
mediated through the activation of the path-
way relevant to the disorder under study, we
should be able to detect placebo-induced re-
lease of endogenous dopamine in PD.
We examined the striatal RAC binding
potential of six patients with PD (group 1,
placebo group) under two conditions (15):
Condition 1, a placebo-controlled, blinded
study in which the patients did not know
when they were receiving placebo or active
drug (apomorphine) (16)—all patients re-
ceived both placebo and active drug; and
condition 2, an open study in the same pa-
tients without placebo.
We found a significant decrease in striatal
RAC binding potential [17% for the caudate
nucleus (range, 8 to 25%); 19% for the puta-
men (range, 8 to 28%); P 0.005 for both,
two-tailed paired t test] when the patients
received placebo compared with open base-
line observations (Table 1). This placebo-
induced change in RAC binding potential
was present in each patient and in each stri-
atal subregion, although it was greatest in the
posterolateral part of the putamen (Table 1).
The magnitude of the placebo response was
comparable to that of therapeutic doses of
levodopa (17 ), or apomorphine (see below)
(18). There were no differences in the striatal
RAC binding potential between this group of
patients when studied without placebo and a
second group of patients matched by age and
severity of parkinsonism studied exclusively
in an open fashion (group 2, open group) (15)
(Fig. 1).
These observations indicate that there is
placebo-induced release of endogenous dopa-
mine in the striatum (19). The estimated re-
lease of dopamine was greater in patients
who perceived placebo benefit than in those
who did not (20). This suggests a “dose-
dependent” relation between the release of
endogenous dopamine and the magnitude of
the placebo effect.
We next asked whether there might be an
interaction between the effects of the placebo
and the active drug (21). The placebo re-
sponse could synergistically enhance the ben-
efit of an active drug, in which case double-
blind, placebo-controlled studies would over-
estimate the active drug effect. Alternatively,
the placebo effect could mask (or decrease)
the specific effect of an active drug, which
would lead to the opposite conclusion in the
interpretation of a placebo-controlled study.
After adjusting for differences in “base-
line” RAC binding potential, we found no
significant differences in the response to apo-
morphine between the open group and the
placebo group (combining patients who per-
ceived a placebo effect and those who did
not) (22). However, the degree of apomor-
phine-induced change in RAC binding poten-
tial tended to be lower in patients who per-
ceived a placebo effect compared with those
who did not and with patients studied in an
open fashion (Fig. 2). We explored whether
this observation could reflect a floor effect in
the placebo group (i.e., whether the technique
was insensitive for further reductions in RAC
binding), but this did not appear to be the case
(Fig. 3) (23). We conclude that the placebo
response does not potentiate the effect of an
active drug. Indeed, our results suggest that in
some patients, most of the benefit obtained
from an active drug might derive from a
placebo effect.
The dopaminergic system is involved in
the regulation of several cognitive, behavior-
al, and sensorimotor functions, and particu-
larly in reward mechanisms (2428). Howev-
er, our experiments did not involve a direct
reward. We conclude that dopamine release
in the nigrostriatal system is linked to expec-
tation of a reward—in this case, the anticipa-
tion of therapeutic benefit (29, 30). All pa-
tients were familiar with the effect of an
active drug (levodopa), and such previous
experience may have enhanced their expec-
tation. We found that the level of expectation
may determine experience (20) —patients
who perceived a placebo effect had higher
release of dopamine than those who did not.
Our observations indicate that the placebo
effect in PD is mediated by an increase in the
1
Neurodegenerative Disorders Centre,
2
TRIUMF, Uni-
versity of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
V6T 2B5.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-
mail: jstoessl@interchange.ubc.ca
Table 1. Striatal RAC binding potential (mean SD) of PD patients (group 1) scanned at open baseline
and after receiving placebo (n 6).
Site Open baseline Placebo
Mean percent change
(range)
Head of caudate 1.964 0.221 1.638 0.230 16.6 (8.4–25.1)
Putamen
Rostral 2.398 0.342 1.976 0.321 17.6 (5.3–26.3)
Intermediate 2.621 0.438 2.142 0.389 18.2 (7.4–27.0)
Caudal 2.095 0.269 1.646 0.261 21.2 (8.8–32.6)
R EPORTS
10 AUGUST 2001 VOL 293 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1164
synaptic levels of dopamine in the striatum.
Expectation-related dopamine release might
be a common phenomenon in any medical
condition susceptible to the placebo effect.
PD patients receiving an active drug in the
context of a placebo-controlled study benefit
from the active drug being tested as well as
from the placebo effect. By contrast, in the
usual clinical practice setting, active drugs
may be devoid of placebo effect. We found
no evidence to suggest that the placebo effect
synergistically augments the action of active
drugs (in fact, a trend for the opposite was
observed), so positive conclusions derived
from placebo-controlled studies are not im-
pugned by our findings.
References and Notes
1. D. G. Altman, Practical Statistics for Medical Research
(Chapman & Hall, London, 1991), pp. 450 451.
2. H. K. Beecher, JAMA ( J. Am. Med. Assoc.) 159, 1602
(1955).
3. E. Ernst, K. L. Resch, Br. Med. J. 311, 551 (1995).
4. T. J. Kaptchuk, Lancet 351, 1722 (1998).
5. L. D. Fisher, G. van Belle, Biostatistics: A Methodology
for the Health Sciences (Wiley, New York, 1993), p.
22.
6. N. Shetty et al., Clin. Neuropharmacol. 22, 207
(1999).
7. C. G. Goetz, S. Leurgans, R. Raman, G. T. Stebbins,
Neurology 54, 710 (2000).
8. J. M. Fearnley, A. J. Lees, Brain 114, 2283 (1991).
9. S. J. Kish, K. S. Shannak, O. Hornykiewicz, N. Engl.
J. Med. 318, 876 (1988).
10. P. Seeman, H. C. Guan, H.B. Niznik, Synapse 3,96
(1989).
11. N. D. Volkow et al., Synapse 16, 255 (1994).
12. M. J. Koepp et al., Nature 393, 266 (1998).
13. A. J. Stoessl, T. J. Ruth, NeuroScience News 2,53
(1999).
14. M. Laruelle, J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 20, 423
(2000).
15. All PET scans were performed in three-dimensional
(3D) mode using an ECAT 953B/31 tomograph. We
obtained 16 sequential frames over 60 minutes,
starting at the time of injection of 5 mCi of [
11
C]ra-
clopride (mean SEM specific activity 4692
349 Ci/mmol at ligand injection). A time-integrated
image with 31 planes, each 3.37 mm thick, was made
from the emission data (from 30 to 60 minutes) for
each subject. The five axial planes in which the
striatum was best visualized were summed. On this
time- and spatially summed image, one circular re-
gion of interest (ROI) of 61.2 mm
2
was positioned on
the head of each caudate nucleus (Caud), and three
circular ROIs of the same size were placed without
overlap along the axis of each putamen (from rostral
to caudal putamen: P1, P2, and P3); ROI position was
adjusted to maximize the average radioactivity. The
ROIs were replicated on the spatially summed image
of each time frame. The background activity was
averaged from a single elliptical ROI (2107 mm
2
)
drawn over the cerebellum on the summed image of
two contiguous axial planes. The binding potential
(BP f NSB
max
/K
d
, where f NS is the free fraction of
tracer) was determined using a tissue input graphical
approach [J. Logan et al., J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab.
16, 834 (1996)]. Further details of the PET scan
protocol are reported elsewhere (17). We studied two
groups of PD patients, of six patients each, under two
different protocols as described below. Both groups
were matched by age and severity of parkinsonism as
measured by the Modified Columbia Scale (MCS)
[R. C. Duvoisin, in Monoamines noyaux gris centraux
et syndrome de Parkinson, J. de Ajuriaguerra, G. Gau-
thier, Eds. (Georg and Cie SA, Geneva, 1971), pp.
313–325]. Clinical details can be found on Science
Online at www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/293/
5532/1164/DC1. After being pretreated with domp-
eridone for 48 hours to prevent side effects, all
patients underwent three consecutive RAC PET scans
on the same day according to the following protocol:
(i) either baseline or placebo scan 12 to 18 hours
after withdrawal of medications; (ii) after subcutane-
ous injection of 0.03 mg of apomorphine per kilo-
gram of body weight; and (iii) after subcutaneous
injection of 0.06 mg/kg of apomorphine. The treat-
ment order was maintained constant for all patients.
Group 1 (the placebo group) was studied in a blind
fashion—patients did not know when they were
receiving placebo (subcutaneous injection of saline)
or apomorphine (all patients received all three treat-
ments). This group also received a fourth injection,
consisting of 0.12 mg/kg of apomorphine on the
same day, to explore the possibility of a floor effect
Fig. 1. Placebo-induced changes in RAC binding potential in the striatum ipsilateral (A) and
contralateral (B) to the more affected body side of patients with PD. The ROIs are on the head of
the caudate nucleus (Caud) and on the putamen, from rostral to caudal, P1, P2, P3 (15).
Comparisons were made between the group of patients studied in an open fashion (group 2, open
group; open bars) and the group of patients studied both with (solid bars) and without (hatched
bars) placebo intervention (group 1, placebo group). Within-subject placebo-induced changes in
RAC binding potential tended to be greater in the striatum contralateral to the more affected body
side (20%) than in the ipsilateral striatum (17%). The placebo group and the open group did not
differ in their baseline placebo-free RAC binding potential values [for the caudate nucleus, 1.96
0.22 (SD) versus 2.07 0.40, respectively; two-tailed t test, t 0.55 (df 10), P 0.59; for
the putamen, 2.37 0.34 versus 2.42 0.42, t 0.20 (df 10), P 0.84]. Error bars, SEM.
Fig. 2. Apomorphine-induced changes in RAC binding potential in the caudate nucleus (A) and
putamen (B) before (APO_0) and after (APO_1 0.03 mg/kg, and APO_2 0.06 mg/kg)
subcutaneous injection of apomorphine. Patients studied in an open fashion (open bars) had higher
RAC binding potential values than those included in the placebo group [independently of whether
they did not (hatched bars) or did (solid bars) perceive a placebo effect]. The decline in RAC binding
potential induced by an incremental dose of apomorphine tended to be less pronounced in patients
who perceived a placebo effect as compared with those who did not, and with patients studied in
an open fashion: interaction term (group apomorphine dose) evaluated by repeated measures
ANCOVA, F 4.66 (df 2, 9), P 0.041 for the caudate nucleus; F 3.40 (df 2, 9), P 0.079
for the putamen. Error bars, SEM.
Fig. 3. Linear regression plots for patients without (n 3; open symbols, thin lines) and with (n
3; solid symbols, thick lines) perceived placebo effect: (A) caudate and (B) putamen RAC binding
potential values against apomorphine dose (APO_dose). The four slopes were significantly different
from zero (P 0.01), but they did not differ significantly between patients with and without
perceived placebo effect (for the caudate nucleus, –3.2 versus –5.1, respectively, P 0.28; for the
putamen, –3.8 versus 6.5, P 0.15).
R EPORTS
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 293 10 AUGUST 2001 1165
(see below). Group 2 (open group) was studied in an
open fashion for comparison purposes (e.g., to investi-
gate the effect of novelty on dopamine release). Here,
recipients were scanned under all three conditions but
knew explicitly if they were receiving no medication or
which dose of apomorphine they were receiving at any
given time. The advantages of this design are threefold:
(i) It minimizes potential carry-over effects from the
active drug (apomorphine) (17). (ii) It helps maintain
the level of expectation throughout the study, which is
crucial to this experiment. For example, the occurrence
of apomorphine-induced side effects could “unblind”
the study. (iii) It maximizes the tolerability of the pro-
cedure. In total, there was a 2.5-hour interval between
scans (1-hour scan plus 1.5-hour break), sufficient to
allow for decay of radioactivity, as well as for dopamine
receptor recovery after apomorphine injection (16, 17).
An additional open baseline scan was performed on
group 1 patients on a different day to obtain placebo-
free baseline values (interval between both sets of
scans, 1 to 4 months). All patients had been contacted
1 month before the scans, and details of the protocol in
which they were included were explained; they were
reminded of these details 3 days before the scans. We
avoided anticipation bias (e.g., patients’ knowledge of
the fact that the placebo effect can determine measur-
able changes in dopamine release might alter the re-
sults) by keeping the patients and the clinical staff
unaware of the purpose of the study. In all cases, care
was taken to optimize patient positioning in the scan-
ner. Motion within and between scans was minimized
by the use of a molded thermoplastic mask. All subjects
gave written informed consent. The study was approved
by the U.B.C. ethics committee.
16. S. T. Gancher, W. R. Woodward, B. Boucher, J. G. Nutt,
Ann. Neurol. 26, 232 (1989).
17. R. de la Fuente-Ferna´ndez et al., Ann. Neurol. 49, 298
(2001).
18. The placebo-induced change in striatal RAC binding
potential is much higher than the reported within
subject scan-rescan variation expected to occur with-
in subject for scan and rescan (mean, 5%) [N. D.
Volkow et al., J. Nucl. Med. 34, 609 (1993)]. The
administration of 0.03 and 0.06 mg/kg of apomor-
phine led to a 14% and 26% decrease, respectively, in
putamen RAC binding potential in the open group
(see Fig. 2).
19. The increasing rostrocaudal gradient of the placebo
effect ( Table 1) eliminates the possibility that the
results could be due to down-regulation of presyn-
aptic D
2
/D
3
receptors. Partial volume effects cannot
explain the gradient in BP
open baseline
–BP
placebo
re-
ported here. Other considerations supporting our in-
terpretation of the results can be found elsewhere
(17).
20. Because the clinical benefit from apomorphine lasts
typically about 1 hour (16), which is the duration of
RAC PET scans, no objective measurements on
changes in the clinical status after placebo or apo-
morphine injection were made (motor activity might
confound the assessment of changes in striatal RAC
binding potential). However, only half of the patients
reported placebo-induced clinical improvement
(comparable in magnitude to the clinical benefit ob-
tained when they were on their regular treatment
with levodopa). Although the number of subjects is
small, those patients who perceived the placebo ef-
fect (n 3) had higher changes in RAC binding
potential than those who did not (n 3) [for the
caudate nucleus, 22% versus 12%; for the putamen,
24% versus 14%; P 0.05 and P 0.01, respective-
ly, by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA)] (Fig. 2).
21. J. Kleijnen, A. J. M. de Craen, J. van Everdingen, L. Krol,
Lancet 344, 1347 (1994).
22. Repeated measures ANCOVA gave the following re-
sults: For the caudate nucleus, between-group differ-
ences, F 0.03 (df 1, 9), P 0.87; interaction
term (group apomorphine dose), F 0.09 (df 1,
10), P 0.77. For the putamen, between-group
differences, F 0.71 (df 1, 9), P 0.42; interaction
term, F 1.81 (df 1, 10), P 0.21. The power for
the interaction terms may not have been sufficient.
23. An apomorphine dose of 0.12 mg/kg led to a further
decrease in RAC binding potential in the placebo
group (Fig. 3). The total reduction in RAC binding
potential (compared with placebo-free baseline val-
ues) was 42% in the caudate nucleus (range, 19 to
59%) and 46% in the putamen (range, 24 to 60%).
24. R. A. Wise, Trends Neurosci. 3, 91 (1980).
25. H. C. Fibiger, A. G. Phillips, in Handbook of Physiology:
The Nervous System, vol. 4, Intrinsic Systems of the
Brain, V. B. Mountcastle, F. E. Bloom, S. R. Geiger, Eds.
(American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD,
1986), pp. 647– 675.
26. T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, Semin. Neurosci. 4, 119
(1992).
27. W. Schultz, J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1 (1998).
28. S. Ikemoto, J. Panksepp, Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 31,
6 (1999).
29. I. Kirsch, Ed., How Expectancies Shape Experience
(American Psychological Association, Washington,
DC, 1999).
30. J. M. Fish, Science 284, 914 (1999).
31. We thank J. McKenzie, S. Jivan, J. Leighton, T. Dobko,
and members of the UBC-TRIUMF PET team for
assistance with the scans. This study was funded by
the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Brit-
ish Columbia Health Research Foundation (R.F.-F. and
V.S.), the Pacific Parkinson’s Research Institute ( Van-
couver, B.C., Canada) (R.F.-F.), and a TRIUMF Life
Science grant. A.J.S. is supported by the Canada Re-
search Chairs program.
22 March 2001; accepted 5 June 2001
R EPORTS
10 AUGUST 2001 VOL 293 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1166
... The reward system is another likely neural underpinning of placebo analgesia since symptom reduction (decreased suffering) is a special case of reward. Furthermore, the expectation is closely related to the activation of tegmental or prefrontal dopaminergic neurons that project to the dorsal and ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (VS/NAc) [51][52][53]. Across studies, placebo analgesia increased fMRI responses [54,55], opioid [44,45,56], and dopamine [44] activities in the NAc during pain. Importantly, the isolated brain regions (e.g., NAc and PFC) in the reward system form the mesocortical (originates from the ventral tegmental area [VTA] and projects primarily to the frontal lobe, e.g., rostral anterior cingulate cortex [rACC]) and mesolimbic pathways (originates from the VTA and projects primarily to the ventral striatum, e.g., NAc) to encode expectancy effects on pain (i.e., placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia), and to explain individual differences of the magnitudes of placebo and nocebo effects [14]. ...
... Apart from placebo analgesia, the reward system is also implicated in placebo effects in Parkinson's disease. An early PET study showed that placebos trigger the release of endogenous dopamine in the striatum [51]. Later studies reconfirmed the involvement of dopamine release in the dorsal and ventral striatum in placebo effects in Parkinson's disease [57]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Placebo and nocebo effects are salubrious benefits and negative outcomes attributable to non-specific symbolic components. Leveraging advanced experimental and analytical approaches, recent studies have elucidated complicated neural mechanisms that may serve as a solid basis for harnessing the powerful self-healing and self-harming capacities and applying these findings to improve medical practice and minimize the unintended exacerbation of symptoms in medical practice. We review advances in employing psychosocial, pharmacological, and neuromodulation approaches to modulate/harness placebo and nocebo effects. While these approaches show promising potential, translating these research findings into clinical settings still requires careful methodological, technical, and ethical considerations.
... Placebo-controlled trials also showed that placebo helped in treating PD [55]. The funding of federal grants for the specific study of placebo effects in Parkinson's disease is, in itself, of historical significance [56]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In 1817, James Parkinson first coined the term Paralysis Agitans (An Essay on the Shaking Palsy), Jean-Marie Charcot was the first to coin term Parkinson's disease (PD). Three most common and obvious symptoms in patients with PD are tremor, rigidity, and bradykinesia. A multidisciplinary team involving neurologists, primary care practitioners, nurses, physical therapists, social workers is used to diagnose PD. Nonpharmacological and pharmacological treatment is given to the patient. However, this disease demands more clinical translational and prognostic research, identifying biomarkers that can help in early diagnosis of the disease and on developing future disease-modifying interventions.
... We also showed beneficial effects in both severe and mild patients. Cognitive functions of AD patients were improved by 29% in the CMA group, whereas they were improved only by 14% in the placebo group after 84 days, consistent with a placebo effect that is seen in other AD clinical trials [50,65,66]. The improvement of cognitive function was supported by positive alterations in cortical thickness and maintenance of the hippocampal subfield volumes in the CMA group, while no cortical thickness difference but significant volume decline was found in the placebo group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is associated with metabolic abnormalities linked to critical elements of neurodegeneration. We recently administered combined metabolic activators (CMA) to the AD rat model and observed that CMA improves the AD-associated histological parameters in the animals. CMA promotes mitochondrial fatty acid uptake from the cytosol, facilitates fatty acid oxidation in the mitochondria, and alleviates oxidative stress. Methods Here, we designed a randomised, double-blinded, placebo-controlled phase-II clinical trial and studied the effect of CMA administration on the global metabolism of AD patients. One-dose CMA included 12.35 g L -serine (61.75%), 1 g nicotinamide riboside (5%), 2.55 g N-acetyl- L -cysteine (12.75%), and 3.73 g L -carnitine tartrate (18.65%). AD patients received one dose of CMA or placebo daily during the first 28 days and twice daily between day 28 and day 84. The primary endpoint was the difference in the cognitive function and daily living activity scores between the placebo and the treatment arms. The secondary aim of this study was to evaluate the safety and tolerability of CMA. A comprehensive plasma metabolome and proteome analysis was also performed to evaluate the efficacy of the CMA in AD patients. Results We showed a significant decrease of AD Assessment Scale-cognitive subscale (ADAS-Cog) score on day 84 vs day 0 ( P = 0.00001, 29% improvement) in the CMA group. Moreover, there was a significant decline ( P = 0.0073) in ADAS-Cog scores (improvement of cognitive functions) in the CMA compared to the placebo group in patients with higher ADAS-Cog scores. Improved cognitive functions in AD patients were supported by the relevant alterations in the hippocampal volumes and cortical thickness based on imaging analysis. Moreover, the plasma levels of proteins and metabolites associated with NAD + and glutathione metabolism were significantly improved after CMA treatment. Conclusion Our results indicate that treatment of AD patients with CMA can lead to enhanced cognitive functions and improved clinical parameters associated with phenomics, metabolomics, proteomics and imaging analysis. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov NCT04044131 Registered 17 July 2019, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04044131
... We also showed beneficial effects in both severe and mild patients. Cognitive functions of AD patients were improved by 29% in the CMA group, whereas they were improved only by 14% in the placebo group after 84 days, consistent with a placebo effect that is seen in other AD clinical trials [50,65,66]. The improvement of cognitive function was supported by positive alterations in cortical thickness and maintenance of the hippocampal subfield volumes in the CMA group, while no cortical thickness difference but significant volume decline was found in the placebo group. ...
... These pharmacological effects of apomorphine in the 3D iDopas/iAstros co-culture were not observed in the corresponding 2D monolayer iDopas/ iAstros coculture. We also observed a reduction in the released dopamine neurotransmitter at synapse from its basal level after treating with apomorphine in the 3D model, which is also in agreement with the findings in human studies by positional emission tomography 60 where they showed that apomorphine inhibited the presynaptic dopamine levels. Many animal behavioral studies have established that optical manipulation creates perception without sensory input by showing whisker movement 61 , creating fear memory 62 , increasing social interaction 63 and other physiological effects. ...
Article
Full-text available
Three-dimensional (3D) organotypic models that capture native-like physiological features of tissues are being pursued as clinically predictive assays for therapeutics development. A range of these models are being developed to mimic brain morphology, physiology, and pathology of neurological diseases. Biofabrication of 3D gel-based cellular systems is emerging as a versatile technology to produce spatially and cell-type tailored, physiologically complex and native-like tissue models. Here we produce 3D fibrin gel-based functional neural co-culture models with human-iPSC differentiated dopaminergic or glutamatergic neurons and astrocytes. We further introduce genetically encoded fluorescence biosensors and optogenetics activation for real time functional measurements of intracellular calcium and levels of dopamine and glutamate neurotransmitters, in a high-throughput compatible plate format. We use pharmacological perturbations to demonstrate that the drug responses of 3D gel-based neural models are like those expected from in-vivo data, and in some cases, in contrast to those observed in the equivalent 2D neural models. Fibrin gel-based 3D co-culture models with human-iPSC differentiated dopaminergic or glutamatergic neurons and astrocytes are shown to be functional using biosensors and can be scaled up for high-throughput assays.
Article
Background/Aims Whole body vibration could benefit functional mobility in people with Parkinson's disease. A preliminary study was undertaken to analyse the acute effect of whole body vibration on unobstructed walking and obstacle circumvention in people with Parkinson's disease. Methods People with Parkinson's disease and typically healthy individuals as matched controls were divided into four groups with nine individuals in each: experimental or placebo for people with Parkinson's disease and experimental or placebo for the control group. The participants were evaluated in two different gait conditions: unobstructed walking and obstacle circumvention. Then the participants undertook a session of whole body vibration on a KIKOS P201 lateral vibratory platform in two positions: feet shoulder-width apart, and feet shoulder-width apart with slightly flexed knees. The participants were re-evaluated after this session. Results After whole body vibration, those in the experimental Parkinson's disease group had a reduced co-contraction of the tibialis anterior and the gastrocnemius lateralis muscles during unobstructed walking, whereas the co-contraction of the tibialis anterior and the gastrocnemius lateralis muscles increased in the experimental control group. In addition, those in the experimental control group had reduced stride duration in unobstructed walking and in obstacle circumvention. After the placebo intervention, the co-contraction of the tibialis anterior and the gastrocnemius lateralis muscles increased in all conditions and stride duration was reduced in unobstructed walking. Conclusions Although whole body vibration had no acute efficiency on gait (unobstructed walking and obstacle circumvention), it can improve other components, such as strength, which could be important for people with Parkinson's disease.
Chapter
Health-related cognitive expectations have been shown to induce changes in neurophysiological and molecular pathways that result in either positive clinical outcomes (placebo effect or healing) or negative outcomes (worsening or nocebo effect) depending on the direction of beliefs (positive or negative). This chapter examines the theoretical premises and scientific evidence underlying the placebo/nocebo effects. Positive and negative cognitive expectations, verbal suggestions, open versus hidden treatments, clinical environmental stimuli, and healing-inducing conditions within the quality of the nurse-patient relationship on patient outcomes are discussed mainly from the findings of experimental studies. Healing-inducing nursing strategies to enhance symptom relief and optimize treatment efficacy and drug adherence are suggested.KeywordsCognitive expectationsHealth-related beliefs for a clinical benefitHealing-inducing strategiesNocebo-reducing clinical strategiesInnate healing processes
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is associated with metabolic abnormalities linked to critical elements of neurodegeneration. We recently administrated Combined Metabolic Activators (CMA) to the AD rat model and observed that administration of CMA activated the mitochondrial functions and eventually improved the AD-associated histological parameters in the animals. CMA consists of NAD + and glutathione precursors and includes L-serine, nicotinamide riboside, N-acetyl-L-cysteine, and L-carnitine tartrate. Methods Here, we designed a randomised, double-blinded, placebo-controlled phase-II trial and studied the effect of CMA administration on the global metabolism of AD patients. The primary endpoint was on the difference in cognitive and daily living activity scores between the placebo and the treatment arms. The secondary aim of this study was to evaluate the safety and tolerability of CMA. We also performed a comprehensive human plasma metabolome and proteome analysis. Results Based on our analysis, we showed a significant decrease of ADAS-Cog scores on Day 84 vs Day 0 (p = 0.00001, 29% improvement) in the CMA group. Moreover, there was a significant enhancement (p = 0.0073) in ADAS-Cog scores between CMA and placebo groups in patients with higher ADAS-Cog scores. Improved cognitive functions were endorsed with relevant hippocampal volumes and cortical thickness alterations. Moreover, the plasma levels of proteins and metabolites associated with NAD + and glutathione metabolism are significantly improved after treatment. Conclusion In conclusion, our results show that treating AD patients with CMA leads to enhanced cognitive functions associated with the improved metabolome, proteome and structural neuroimaging parameters, suggesting a role for such a therapeutic regimen in treating patients, especially with severe AD. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov NCT04044131 Registered 17 July 2019, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04044131
Article
Background Monotherapy with monoamine oxidase B (MAO-B) inhibitors enhances the level of endogenous dopamine in treatment for Parkinson’s disease (PD) and provides some benefits. Certain neuropsychiatric functions are also regulated by central dopaminergic activity.AimTo investigate the relationship of the efficacy of monotherapy with MAO-B inhibitors on motor symptoms in PD with baseline cognitive function.Patients and methodsOutcomes were examined for 27 consecutive drug-naïve PD patients who received initial treatment with a MAO-B inhibitor (selegiline: 11, rasagiline: 16). Selegiline was titrated to an optimal dose. The dose of rasagiline was fixed at 1 mg/day. Motor symptoms were assessed using the Movement Disorder Society-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III before treatment and after the efficacy reached a plateau within 19 weeks after drug initiation, and the % improvement in motor symptoms was calculated. Pre-treatment cognitive function was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB). Correlations of % improvement in motor symptoms and baseline cognitive assessments were examined using Spearman correlation coefficients and multiple regression analysis.ResultsIn all patients, the mean % improvement in motor symptoms was 46.5% (range 0–83.3%). Spearman correlation coefficients showed the % improvement in motor symptoms was correlated with FAB (r = 0.631, p < 0.001). In multiple regression analysis with patient background factors as independent variables, only FAB was associated with improvement in motor symptoms in the MAO-B group.Conclusion Better FAB scores predict a significant improvement in motor symptoms with treatment with MAO-B inhibitors, suggesting high activity of endogenous dopamine.
Article
Full-text available
Studies addressing behavioral functions of dopamine (DA) in the nucleus accumbens septi (NAS) are reviewed. A role of NAS DA in reward has long been suggested. However, some investigators have questioned the role of NAS DA in rewarding effects because of its role in aversive contexts. As findings supporting the role of NAS DA in mediating aversively motivated behaviors accumulate, it is necessary to accommodate such data for understanding the role of NAS DA in behavior. The aim of the present paper is to provide a unifying interpretation that can account for the functions of NAS DA in a variety of behavioral contexts: (1) its role in appetitive behavioral arousal, (2) its role as a facilitator as well as an inducer of reward processes, and (3) its presently undefined role in aversive contexts. The present analysis suggests that NAS DA plays an important role in sensorimotor integrations that facilitate flexible approach responses. Flexible approach responses are contrasted with fixed instrumental approach responses (habits), which may involve the nigro-striatal DA system more than the meso-accumbens DA system. Functional properties of NAS DA transmission are considered in two stages: unconditioned behavioral invigoration effects and incentive learning effects. (1) When organisms are presented with salient stimuli (e.g., novel stimuli and incentive stimuli), NAS DA is released and invigorates flexible approach responses (invigoration effects). (2) When proximal exteroceptive receptors are stimulated by unconditioned stimuli, NAS DA is released and enables stimulus representations to acquire incentive properties within specific environmental context. It is important to make a distinction that NAS DA is a critical component for the conditional formation of incentive representations but not the retrieval of incentive stimuli or behavioral expressions based on over-learned incentive responses (i.e., habits). Nor is NAS DA essential for the cognitive perception of environmental stimuli. Therefore, even without normal NAS DA transmission, the habit response system still allows animals to perform instrumental responses given that the tasks take place in fixed environment. Such a role of NAS DA as an incentive-property constructor is not limited to appetitive contexts but also aversive contexts. This dual action of NAS DA in invigoration and incentive learning may explain the rewarding effects of NAS DA as well as other effects of NAS DA in a variety of contexts including avoidance and unconditioned/conditioned increases in open-field locomotor activity. Particularly, the present hypothesis offers the following interpretation for the finding that both conditioned and unconditioned aversive stimuli stimulate DA release in the NAS: NAS DA invigorates approach responses toward `safety'. Moreover, NAS DA modulates incentive properties of the environment so that organisms emit approach responses toward `safety' (i.e., avoidance responses) when animals later encounter similar environmental contexts. There may be no obligatory relationship between NAS DA release and positive subjective effects, even though these systems probably interact with other brain systems which can mediate such effects. The present conceptual framework may be valuable in understanding the dynamic interplay of NAS DA neurochemistry and behavior, both normal and pathophysiological.
Article
Animals will work for a reward consisting of electrical stimulation at certain specific sites within the brain. James Olds termed these ‘pleasure centers’, but both the ‘pleasure’ and the ‘center’ aspects of the hypothesis were subsequently criticised, even by Olds himself. However, as Roy Wise describes here, recent work suggests that, with some minor modifications, the concept still provides a useful experimental paradigm.
Article
Placebos have doubtless been used for centuries by wise physicians as well as by quacks, but it is only recently that recognition of an enquiring kind has been given the clinical circumstance where the use of this tool is essential "... to distinguish pharmacological effects from the effects of suggestion, and... to obtain an unbiased assessment of the result of experiment." It is interesting that Pepper could say as recently as 10 years ago "apparently there has never been a paper published discussing [primarily] the important subject of the placebo." In 1953 Gaddum1 said: Such tablets are sometimes called placebos, but it is better to call them dummies. According to the Shorter Oxford Dictionary the word placebo has been used since 1811 to mean a medicine given more to please than to benefit the patient. Dummy tablets are not particularly noted for the pleasure which they give to their recipients.
Article
Motor fluctuations are a major disabling complication in the treatment of Parkinson's disease. To investigate whether such oscillations in mobility can be attributed to changes in the synaptic levels of dopamine, we studied prospectively patients in the early stages of Parkinson's disease with a follow-up after at least 3 years of levodopa treatment. At baseline, 3 positron emission tomography (PET) scans using [11C]raclopride before and after (1 hour and 4 hours) orally administered levodopa were performed on the same day for each patient. Patients who developed “wearing-off” fluctuations during the follow-up period had a different pattern of levodopa-induced changes in [11C]raclopride binding potential (BP) from that observed in patients who were still stable by the end of the follow-up. Thus, 1 hour post-levodopa the estimated increase in the synaptic level of dopamine was 3 times higher in fluctuators than in stable responders. By contrast, only stable responders maintained increased levels of synaptic dopamine in the PET scan performed after 4 hours. These results indicate that fluctuations in the synaptic concentration of dopamine precede clinically apparent “wearing-off” phenomena. The rapid increase in synaptic levels of dopamine observed in fluctuators suggests that increased dopamine turnover might play a relevant role in levodopa-related motor complications. Ann Neurol 2001;49:298–303
Article
Manipulations of dopamine levels in the dorsal and ventral striatum are shown to affect the activation of behaviour in distinct, yet parallel ways, which depend upon the nature of the neocortical and limbic input to these structures. Whereas dopamine in the dorsal striatum contributes to the sensorimotor co-ordination of consummatory behaviour and the development of a ‘response set’ in motor preparatory processes for skilled responses, dopamine in the ventral striatum influences the impact of reward-related stimuli on appetitive aspects of behaviour. The circumstances under which the striatal dopamine projections are normally active to effect these functions are defined by studies which attempt to correlate firing in single units or neurochemical indices of dopamine activity with environmental conditions, internal states and behaviour.
Article
The micro-architecture of the substantia nigra was studied in control cases of varying age and patients with parkinsonism. A single 7 mu section stained with haematoxylin and eosin was examined at a specific level within the caudal nigra using strict criteria. The pars compacta was divided into a ventral and a dorsal tier, and each tier was further subdivided into 3 regions. In 36 control cases there was a linear fallout of pigmented neurons with advancing age in the pars compacta of the caudal substantia nigra at a rate of 4.7% per decade. Regionally, the lateral ventral tier was relatively spared (2.1% loss per decade) compared with the medial ventral tier (5.4%) and the dorsal tier (6.9%). In 20 Parkinson's disease (PD) cases of varying disease duration there was an exponential loss of pigmented neurons with a 45% loss in the first decade. Regionally, the pattern was opposite to ageing. Loss was greatest in the lateral ventral tier (average loss 91%) followed by the medial ventral tier (71%) and the dorsal tier (56%). The presymptomatic phase of PD from the onset of neuronal loss was estimated to be about 5 yrs. This phase is represented by incidental Lewy body cases: individuals who die without clinical signs of PD or dementia, but who are found to have Lewy bodies at post-mortem. In 7 cases cell loss was confined to the lateral ventral tier (average loss 52%) congruent with the lateral ventral selectivity of symptomatic PD. It was calculated that at the onset of symptoms there was a 68% cell loss in the lateral ventral tier and a 48% loss in the caudal nigra as a whole. The regional selectivity of PD is relatively specific. In 15 cases of striatonigral degeneration the distribution of cell loss was similar, but the loss in the dorsal tier was greater than PD by 21%. In 14 cases of Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome (SRO) there was no predilection for the lateral ventral tier, but a tendency to involve the medial nigra and spare the lateral. These findings suggest that age-related attrition of pigmented nigral cells is not an important factor in the pathogenesis of PD.