ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

This meta-analysis synthesized bullying prevention programs' effectiveness at increasing bystander intervention in bullying situations. Evidence from 12 school-based programs, involving 12,874 students, indicated that overall the programs were successful (Hedges's g = .20, 95% confidence interval [CI] = .11 to .29, p < .001), with larger effects for high school (HS) samples compared to kindergarten through eighth-grade (K-8) student samples (HS effect size [ES] = 0.43, K-8 ES = 0.14; p < .05). A secondary synthesis from eight of the studies that reported empathy for the victim revealed treatment effectiveness that was positive but not significantly different from zero (g = .05, 95% CI = .07 to .17, p = .45). Nevertheless, this meta-analysis indicated that programs increased bystander intervention both on a practical and statistically significant level. These results suggest that researchers and school administrators should consider implementing programs that focus on bystander intervention behavior supplementary to bullying prevention programs.
Content may be subject to copyright.
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying Prevention
Programs’ Effects on Bystander Intervention Behavior
Joshua R. Polanin
Loyola University Chicago
Dorothy L. Espelage
University of Illinois Urbana—Champaign
Therese D. Pigott
Loyola University Chicago
Abstract. This meta-analysis synthesized bullying prevention programs’ effec-
tiveness at increasing bystander intervention in bullying situations. Evidence
from 12 school-based programs, involving 12,874 students, indicated that overall
the programs were successful (Hedges’s g.20, 95% confidence interval [CI]
.11 to .29, p.001), with larger effects for high school (HS) samples compared
to kindergarten through eighth-grade (K-8) student samples (HS effect size [ES]
0.43, K-8 ES 0.14; p.05). A secondary synthesis from eight of the studies
that reported empathy for the victim revealed treatment effectiveness that was
positive but not significantly different from zero (g.05, 95% CI ⫽⫺.07 to .17,
p.45). Nevertheless, this meta-analysis indicated that programs increased
bystander intervention both on a practical and statistically significant level. These
results suggest that researchers and school administrators should consider imple-
menting programs that focus on bystander intervention behavior supplementary to
bullying prevention programs.
Bullying perpetration often occurs when
bystanders are present (Hawkins, Pepler, &
Craig, 2001; Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Bertz, &
King, 1982). In fact, some research has indi-
cated that more than 80% of the time an ob-
server witnesses victimization (O’Connell,
Pepler, & Craig, 1999). Despite the presence
of witnesses and bystanders, nearly 1 in 3
children report victimization by a bully in the
past 2 months (Frey, Hirschstein, Edstrom, &
Snell, 2009; Nansel et al., 2001; Wang, Ian-
notti, & Nansel, 2009). Consequentially, bul-
lying occurs with an audience of members
who play multiple roles (Salmavalli, Lager-
spetz, Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & Kaukianen,
1996) and often fail to intervene on behalf of
the victim with regularity. These bullying in-
cidents have lasting negative effects on the
bully, victim, and bystanders (Olweus, 2002;
Swearer, Espelage, Villancourt, & Hymel,
2010; Sweeting, Young, West, & Der, 2006;
Stevens, Oost, & Bourdeaudhuij, 2004).
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Joshua R. Polanin, Loyola University
Chicago, 820 N. Michigan Ave., Suite 1022C, Chicago, IL 60611; e-mail: jpolanin@luc.edu
Copyright 2012 by the National Association of School Psychologists, ISSN 0279-6015
School Psychology Review,
2012, Volume 41, No. 1, pp. 47–65
47
The past 20 years have seen a burgeon-
ing of bullying prevention programs (Fergu-
son, Miguel, Kilburn, & Sanchez, 2007; Ryan
& Smith, 2009; Ttofi & Farrington, 2009,
2011). Researchers, school administrators, and
teachers have used myriad designs, theories,
and techniques in an attempt to mitigate the
prevalence of bullying (Astor, Meyer, Ben-
benishty, Marachi, & Rosemond, 2005). Ttofi
and Farrington’s (2011) recent large-scale
meta-analysis of over 90 studies found that the
majority of these programs have been success-
ful at slowing the rate of bullying.
Although successful bullying programs
remain important accomplishments, Ttofi and
Farrington (2011) found that few programs
specifically target the behavior of bystanders
(i.e., an individual who witnesses bullying).
As such, prevention programs deemphasize a
population that constitutes between 60% and
70% of primary or secondary school students
(Glew, Fan, Katon, Rivara, & Kerntic, 2005;
Rivers, Poteat, Noret, & Ashurst, 2009). This
program oversight is unfortunate because ob-
servational research has found that when by-
standers intervene on behalf of the victim,
they successfully abate victimization more
than 50% of the time (Craig, Pepler, & Atlas,
2000; O’Connell et al., 1999).
Supported by the knowledge that by-
standers can successfully intervene on behalf
of the victim, a small amount of literature has
focused recently on increasing this behavior.
These programs explicitly emphasize the im-
portance of bystander intervention behavior
and measure this construct. Given these con-
ditions, the purpose of this meta-analysis is to
synthesize school-based bullying prevention
programs’ effectiveness to change bystander
intervention behavior. We also aggregated the
program’s influence on empathy for the victim
as a secondary synthesis because it has re-
ceived recent investigation (Gini, Albiero,
Benelli, & Altoe`, 2007). The following sum-
marizes the relevant literature, provides a
comprehensive examination of the synthesis
process and quantitative analysis outcomes,
and elucidates moderator analysis and publi-
cation bias results. Suggestions for future re-
search and policy are also provided.
Bullying in the Schools
Definition
Olweus (1973) first described bullying
as “mobbing” where a group or individual
teases or harasses another individual. As such,
early research focused solely on the physical
aspects of school environment (e.g., teacher–
student ratio), but found little connection to
perpetration or victimization (Swearer et al.,
2010). Recently, Frey et al. (2009) described
bullying as a social construct that disrupts
social connections among students. Ross and
Horner (2009) summarized the plethora of
definitions:
Common definitions of bullying involve re-
peated acts of aggression, intimidations, or
coercion against a victim who is weaker in
terms of physical size, psychological or so-
cial power, or other factors that result in a
notable power differential. (p. 748)
The bully construct has received much
research and hosts of definitions remain.
Taken together, the bully generally involves
an individual or group who incites physical or
emotional abuse on another individual or
group. Although other research exists on In-
ternet and workplace bullying (Mishna, Cook,
Saini, Wu, & MacFadden, 2010), this review
focuses on school bullying.
Prevalence and Negative Effects
School bullying is not a problem local to
the United States; rather, it is recognized
worldwide (Espelage & Swearer, 2003). Re-
ports from Europe to North America have
indicated that anywhere from 1% to 50% of
students had been bullied or victimized within
the last 2 months (Wang et al., 2009). Some
observations have shown that as many as 30%
of students were involved in bullying as either
the bully or victim (Frey et al., 2009). Hymel
and Swearer (2010) recently reported that 35%
of students indicated being bullied at least
once in the last 2 months with as many as 11%
of those sampled reported being bullied more
than 2 or 3 times in the last 2 months. More-
over, some research has found that bullying
roles remain relatively stable across time. In a
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
48
sample of 516 middle school students, Espel-
age, Bosworth, and Simon (2001) found that
individuals who perpetrated bullying contin-
ued to across multiple years. This finding has
been replicated from other settings and popu-
lations (McDougal, Hymel, & Vailliancourt,
2009; Scholte, Engels, Overbeek, Kemp, &
Haselager, 2007; Sourander, Helstela, Hele-
nius, & Piha, 2000).
Although bullying’s pervasiveness is
cause for concern, the negative consequences
related to bullying remain tantamount. Re-
searchers have indicated that bullying has
been linked to anger and misconduct (Bos-
worth, Espelage, & Simon, 1999), criminal or
delinquent behavior (Olweus, 2002), and sui-
cidal ideations (Kaltiala-Heino, Rimpela,
Marttunen, Rimpela, & Rantanen, 1999). Vic-
timization, on the other hand, has been linked
to poor physical health (Rigby, 1999), low
self-esteem (Rigby & Slee, 1993), depression
(Sweeting et al., 2006), anxiety (Craig, 1994),
and school avoidance (Kochenderfer & Ladd,
1996). Indeed, the negative effects of bullying
and being bullied are persistent and
problematic.
Bystander
Definition, Roles, and Negative Effects
A significant proportion of individuals
within school systems are considered individ-
uals who are bystanders of bullying (Glew et
al., 2005). Twemlow, Fonagy, and Sacco
(2004) defined a bystander as an individual
who lacks participation in bullying scenarios
as either the bully or victim. The bystander
may actively intervene to stop the bully, en-
courage the bully to continue, or view bullying
passively; bystanders can be either boys or
girls (Cowie, 2000; Smith, Twemlow, &
Hoover, 1998).
There are specific roles that the by-
stander can demonstrate. Some authors refer
to the bystander as a passerby, observer, wit-
ness, or participant (Salmivalli, Kaukianinen,
& Voeten, 2005; Twemlow et al., 2004); oth-
ers described roles in relation to sustaining or
preventing the bullying behavior such as rein-
forcer (e.g., laughing or seeing what is hap-
pening), assistant (e.g., follower of the bully),
defender (e.g., being supportive of the victim),
or outsider (e.g., remaining away from the
bullying situation; Salmivalli et al., 1996). For
the purposes of this review, we defined the
bystander generally as any student who wit-
nessed a bullying episode, with the operation-
alizing characteristic being the witnessing
presence of bullying, regardless of other
characteristics.
Despite these literature discordances,
few disagree about the adverse effects wit-
nessing bullying can have on the bystander.
Bystanders felt significantly more uncomfort-
able in bullying situations compared to bullies
(Stevens et al., 2004), and reported feelings of
anxiety and insecurity (Rigby & Slee, 1993).
This anxiety due to witnessing bullying has
been linked to aggressive retaliation (Musher-
Eizenman et al., 2004), and the fear of being
bullied often prevented bystanders from seek-
ing adult help (Unnever & Cornell, 2003). A
recent large-scale study conducted in the
United Kingdom found that compared to per-
petrators, bystanders were at elevated risk for
nonclinical outcomes (i.e., interpersonal sen-
sitivity), and compared to victims bystanders
were more likely to have elevated levels of
substance abuse (Rivers et al., 2009).
Evidence of the Bystanders’ Effects on
Bullying
Individuals who are bystanders remain
present more than 80% of bullying situations
(O’Connell et al., 1999), and therefore some
research has focused on a social-ecological
model of bullying prevention and intervention
(Frey et al., 2009; Swearer & Espelage, 2004).
The social presence and pervasiveness of the
bystander fosters myriad opportunities to in-
tervene. For example, bystanders supported
victims by reporting bullies to adults when
participating in a setting specifically designed
to change bullying behavior patterns through
bystanders (Sharp, Sellors, & Cowie, 1994).
Ross and Horner (2009) recently implemented
a school-wide bullying intervention program
that resulted in a decrease in reinforcing by-
stander behavior and bullying perpetration
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
49
overall. Moreover, interventions that focused
on dealing with conflict through peers instead
of direct interventions with adults led to pos-
itive effects (Cowie & Hutson, 2006), and an
individual’s willingness to intervene in bully-
ing situations was inversely related to the
amount of peer-group bullying perpetration
(Espelage, Green, & Polanin, 2011).
Bystander Intervention Program
Characteristics
To date, best practice guidelines to pro-
mote effective bystander intervention behav-
iors remain undefined because research find-
ings varied widely with regard to their imple-
mentation focuses and approaches. Several
mediums for interventions have been studied
to teach children about bystander behavior,
including classroom-based drama (Merrell,
2004), media such as videotaped reenactments
(McLaughlin, 2009; Schumacher, 2007), and
individualized computer-adaptive software to
track students’ progress within social scenar-
ios and provided feedback on effective by-
stander behavior (Evers, Prochaska, Van Mar-
ter, Johnson, & Prochaska, 2007). However,
all of these programs focus on bystander be-
havior perhaps, because there seems to be
some support for targeting peer-group behav-
iors to mitigate individual bullying (Salmivalli
et al., 1996). Peer-group interventions often
encourage bystander intervention (Andreou,
Didaskalou, & Vlachou, 2008; Frey et al.,
2009; Stevens et al., 2000) or enhance by-
stander empathy for the victim (Gini et al.,
2007; Nickerson, Mele, & Princiotta, 2008).
However, few studies have examined the ef-
fects of peer-group interventions on previctim
empathy (Merrell, Gueldner, Ross, & Isava,
2008) and there are resulting guidelines to
promote this behavior.
Previous Meta-Analyses on Bullying
Prevention Programs
A number of recent quantitative meta-
analyses (Ferguson et al., 2007; Ttofi & Far-
rington, 2009, 2011; Merrell et al., 2008;
Smith, Schneider, Smith, & Ananiadou, 2004)
and qualitative systematic reviews (Ryan &
Smith, 2009) have been conducted regarding
bullying and victimization intervention and
prevention programs. However, none of these
meta-analyses focused specifically on by-
stander intervention constructs. Merrell et al.’s
(2008) review included three studies that mea-
sured “intervene to stop bullying behavior,”
which resulted in a mean effect size of 0.17,
but this was a secondary analysis of a small
number of studies. Therefore, the goal of the
present study was to conduct a meta-analysis
that would directly address bystander inter-
vention behavior and empathy attitudes.
Given this goal, two primary research
questions are addressed:
1. What is the average treatment effect,
across the current literature, of bullying
prevention programs on bystander in-
tervention behavior?
2. What study characteristics produced the
largest treatment effect?
A secondary research question ad-
dressed bystander empathy for the victim:
3. What is the average treatment effect,
across the current literature, of bullying
prevention programs on bystander em-
pathy for the victim?
Method
Search Strategy
We used a comprehensive search to re-
trieve articles from the international research
literature within the last 30 years (1980
2010). We searched primarily five online da-
tabases: Dissertation Abstracts International,
Education Resources Information Center
(ERIC), PsycINFO, Medline, and Science Di-
rect. Combinations of the following terms
were used: “bystander or participant or de-
fender or other, ” “bully or victim, ” “school,
school program, or program, ” “prevention or
intervention, ” “aggression, ” and “not higher
education or not cyber-bully.” To ensure that
the identified studies focused on bystander
behavior as the primary goal, these terms were
searched in the abstract of the study. In addi-
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
50
tion, we searched the bibliographies of all
articles selected for relevant studies.
The search retrieved 360 total articles,
but only 83 were unique and compared to the
criteria listed in the next section. Of those 83,
53 were deemed irrelevant, 13 did not address
the intervene construct, 6 failed to include a
control group, and 1 was a repeat of a previous
study. Finally, we corresponded with a num-
ber of experts in the field to ensure inclusion
of all relevant articles. This correspondence
produced 1 relevant study. Hence, we re-
viewed and included 11 studies total.
Criteria for Considering Studies for
Review
The present study focused on school-
based interventions that emphasized changing
the bystander’s intervention behavior. To as-
sess the effects of these programs, we col-
lected peer-reviewed studies published or con-
ducted from 1980 to 2010, based solely within
a school system and intended purposefully to
modify bystander intervention behavior. Sub-
sequently, we excluded studies that focused on
changing bullying behaviors primarily and
collected a bystander measure only as a sec-
ondary procedure. The review included inter-
ventions from the United States and Europe,
but we limited inclusion to English-written
studies.
We reviewed studies that included par-
ticipants from the kindergarten through 12th-
grade population, but interventions with
school-aged children based outside the school
setting were excluded. In addition, we at-
tempted to collect studies that included “at-
risk” students and the general population, but
none of the studies distinguished between
these populations. It should be mentioned that
one study attempted to deconstruct the by-
stander into several types of bystanders to
observe treatment effects (Evers et al., 2007).
Although the deconstruction was informative,
it was the only study to implement such a
procedure. As such, for that study we used the
average intervention effects across all by-
stander types.
Furthermore, we included only studies
that used a treatment-control research design.
These designs included true experimental ran-
domly assigned groups, nonrandom quasi-ex-
perimental designs, and nonrandomly as-
signed matched group. We also included all
control group types; these included wait list,
treatment-as-usual, and “straw-man.” How-
ever, single-group pre/post-test (e.g., gain
scores) and cohort designs were excluded.
Outcomes
Studies must have included a bystander
intervention measure. We operationalized this
outcome as a measure that assessed the con-
tribution of the bystander to a bullying situa-
tion (Frey et al., 2005, 2009). Therefore, we
included studies that measured intention to
intervene, intention to stop bullying, direct
intervention, or conversely, difficulty in re-
sponding assertively to a bullying situation.
For example, Andreou et al. (2008) included
items that assessed students’ intention to in-
tervene on a 5-point Likert scale. We included
items that concerned students’ intention to
“seek teacher’s help,” “react against bullying,”
and “support the victims of bullying” (p. 241).
Table 1 provides the measures used, the
study’s stated construct, and the number of
items combined to create the measure.
In addition to the intervention outcome,
we collected results of the program’s effects
on changes in attitudes of empathy toward the
victim. We operationalized the empathy out-
come as a measure that indicated empathy for
the victim. For instance, Stevens et al. (2000)
used an empathy measure that included “feel-
ing sad about students who are bullied” and
“unpleasantness when another student is being
bullied” (p. 26). Measures that included items
similar to this construct constituted an empa-
thy scale. We should also mention that this
outcome constituted a secondary outcome
measure and thus was not a criterion for syn-
thesis inclusion. Studies that included a by-
stander intervention behavior measure but
failed to include a measure of empathy were
included.
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
51
Table 1
Characteristics of the Studies used in the Meta-Analysis
Study (DoP) Type Location
N
(%Male)
Grade
Setting Program Title
Program
Characteristics
Facilitator;
(Length)
Intervention
Measurement Design
IN ES
(95% CI)
EM ES
(95% CI)
Andreou
(2008)
J Greece 418
(60)
4
th
-6
th
Urban
Curriculum-
Based Anti-
Bullying
Awareness-building
(Awareness); Self-
reflection; Behavior
modification
(BehMod)
Teacher (1) Bystander
intention to
intervene
Q.01
(.20, .19)
.19
(.38, .01)
Evers (2007) J Multiple
US States
710
(41)
9
th
-12
th
Mixed
Build Respect BehMod;
Individualized
computer software;
Parent component
(Parent)
Computer
Based (2)
Passive
bystanding
Q .46
(.27, .64)
N.M.
Fonagy
(2009)
J Kansas 578
(46)
3
rd
-5
th
Rural
CAPSLE Psychiatric
consultation;
Awareness; Parent
Teacher (12) Helpful
bystanding
E .05
(.11, .22)
.23
(.40, .07)
Frey (2005) J Washington 913
(51)
3
rd
-6
th
Suburban
Steps to
Respect
Awareness; Social-
emotional skill-
building; Parent
Teacher (6) Bystander
responsibility
E .11
(.02, .24)
.18
(.05, .31)
Karna (2011) J Finland 8166
(50)
4
th
-6
th
Mixed
KiVa Awareness; Role-
playing; Modeling;
Parent
Teacher (12) Bystander
defending
E .14
(.10, .19)
.15
(.10, .19)
McLaughlin
(2009)
D Ohio 41
(41)
6
th
Urban Effective Bully
Prevention
Awareness; Modeling
with media
(ModMed)
Researcher
(1)
Bystander traits Q .21
(.42, .83)
.17
(.81, .46)
Menesini
(2003)
J Italy 293
(53)
6
th
-8
th
Urban
Befriending
intervention
Awareness; Enhanced
capacity;
Responsibility-
training
Not Reported
(12)
Defender traits Q .03
(.21, .26)
Not
Measured
(Table 1 continues)
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
52
Table 1 Continued
Study (DoP) Type Location
N
(%Male)
Grade
Setting Program Title
Program
Characteristics
Facilitator;
(Length)
Intervention
Measurement Design
IN ES
(95% CI)
EM ES
(95% CI)
Merrell
(2004)
D New York 56
(30)
9
th
Urban
5 W’s
Approach to
Bullying
Awareness; ModMed Researcher
(2)
Bystander
intention to
intervene
E .60
(1.17, 2.36)
Not
Measured
Schumacher
(2007)
M Pennsylvania 825
(42)
9
th
-12
th
Mixed
Bullying Video
Program
Awareness; ModMed Researcher
(1)
Bystander
intention to
intervene
E .43
(.29, .57)
.29
(.15, .43)
Stevens “A”
(2000)
J Belgium 301
(50*)
4
th
-8
th
Not
Reported
Anti-bullying
Intervention
Social-cognitive
training; ModMed
Teacher (1) Bystander
intention to
intervene
E .06
(.17, .29)
.05
(.28, .18)
Stevens “B”
(2000)
J Belgium 401
(50*)
9
th
-11
th
Not
Reported
Anti-bullying
Intervention
Social-cognitive
training; ModMed
Teacher (1) Bystander
intention to
intervene
E .39
(.19, .59)
.14
(.06, .33)
Whitaker
(2004)
B Texas 1763
(50)
5
th
Mixed
Expect Respect Awareness; Psycho-
education; Parent
Teacher (12) Bystander
intention to
intervene
E 0.25
(.15, .34)
Not
Measured
Notes: DoP Date of Publication; J Journal Article; D Dissertation; M Master’s Thesis; B Book Chapter; * imputed percent male; Parent Parent Training; Length
Time to Posttest in months; Q Quasi-experimental; E Experimental; T Treatment groups; C Control groups; IN ES Intervention Effect Size; EM ES Empathy Effect
Size.
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
53
As shown in Table 1, 10 of the 83
unique articles met the inclusion criteria and
were included in the meta-analysis. We dis-
continued the literature search on May 20,
2010, but added 1 article brought to our atten-
tion 3 months later, which brought the total
number to 11 studies.
Coding
Study details, appropriate program, and
sample information were coded directly into
an EXCEL (2010) database. This included
publication year, publication type, funding
provided, country of origin and publication,
program location, treatment and control sam-
ple characteristics (e.g., age, gender, race,
SES, disabilities), program characteristics
(e.g., length of time, intervention details), and
program facilitator. In addition, intervention
and empathy outcome measures were tran-
scribed. By coding directly into the EXCEL
database, we eliminated errors that might have
occurred during the normal transcription phase
(Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
The first author coded all 12 studies, but
one independent rater coded a randomly se-
lected portion of studies (5) for reliability pur-
poses. The two raters agreed 92% of the time.
The coders came to an agreement for all dis-
crepancies prior to completion.
Analysis
Quantitative synthesis, or meta-analysis,
is a statistical technique that combines related
research studies to estimate an overall treat-
ment effect (Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine,
2009; Glass, 1976; Hedges & Olkin, 1985).
Often, and in the case of the present review,
meta-analysis aggregates treatment effect
sizes to assess an intervention’s effectiveness.
The purpose of a meta-analysis, then, is to
generalize findings across multiple treatment
and setting types, participants, and times (Matt
& Cook, 2009). We conducted analyses using
SPSS (2010) and Comprehensive Meta-Anal-
ysis (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Roth-
stein, 2005) software.
Independent findings. A paramount
meta-analytic assumption is independence of
findings. Cooper (2010) discussed several oc-
currences that constitute nonindependence and
their effects on subsequent findings. Thus, we
conducted several common procedures to en-
sure independent findings. To ensure that only
one effect size was derived from each inter-
vention, we used only the first treatment out-
come reported for studies that reported multi-
ple post-treatment outcomes (see Andreou et
al., 2008). If studies implemented interven-
tions with two groups but only one control
group, we synthesized the treatment effects
prior to calculating the study effect size (see
McLaughlin, 2009). Finally, if one author im-
plemented an intervention and published mul-
tiple articles on the same sample, then we
reviewed only the first article published (see
Frey et al., 2005, 2009).
Effect size metrics. The majority of
effect sizes calculated used a continuous scale.
As such, the appropriate effect size metric was
the standardized mean difference (Equation
1):
dXG1XG2
Sp
(1)
where the numerator is the mean differ-
ence between treatment and control group
posttests, and the denominator is the pooled
standard deviation for the intervention and
comparison groups. Further, all dmetrics were
bias corrected using Hedges’s (1981) small
sample correction (g). This correction as well
as the sampling variance is represented by
Equations 2 and 3:
g
1
3
4N9
*d(2)
Varg
nG1nG2
nG1nG2
g2
2nG1nG2(3)
where Nis the total sample, dis the
original standardized mean difference, and n
G1
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
54
and n
G2
represent the treatment and control
group sample sizes, respectively.
In addition, we calculated logged odds
ratio effect sizes for two studies that used a
categorical outcome measure (Evers et al.,
2007; Merrell, 2004). Both measures were ob-
servations of treatment and control partici-
pants during free period times. The authors
observed how many times treatment children
intervened (or intended to intervene) in a bul-
lying situation compared to children in the
control group. Standard odds ratio calculations
were first used (Sanchez-Meca et al., 2003).
We then converted the logged odds ratio into
a standardized mean difference as outlined by
Lipsey & Wilson (2001) (Equations 4 and 5):
d
3*lnOR
(4)
SE
3*SElnOR
2
3(5)
where ln(OR) represents the original
logged odds ratio and SE
ln(OR)
represents the
original sampling variance.
Missing data. Only one of the studies
failed to provide appropriate descriptive sta-
tistics. Schumacher (2007) provided only a t
statistic, as well as sample sizes, that com-
pared the treatment and control groups. Lipsey
and Wilson (2001) provided an appropriate
conversion (Equation 6):
ESsm t
n1n2
n1n2
(6)
where trepresents the tstatistic the
study provided, n
1
represents the sample size
of the treatment group, and n
2
represents the
sample size of the control group.
Random effects model. We assumed
that the treatments were derived from a ran-
dom sample of the literature but lacked a com-
mon effect size. Given this assumption, Bo-
renstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein
(2010) posited that the random effects model
was most appropriate. We further assumed
that an underlying distribution of effect sizes
was plausible; thus our goal, given the random
effects framework, was to estimate the distri-
bution’s mean and confidence interval.
To estimate a random effects mean and
confidence interval, we calculated the
weighted treatment effect of each study. The
weighted effect estimation synthesized both
within-study error variance and common be-
tween-study variance. We represented this
weight calculation as Equation 7:
Wi1
ViT2(7)
where Wrepresents the ith study weight,
V
i
indicates the within-study error variance,
and T
2
represents the between-study variance.
We used these weights then to estimate the
combined treatment effect. This can be repre-
sented by Equation 8:
M
Wi*gi
Wi
(8)
where Mrepresents the combined effect,
W
i
represents the ith study weight, and g
i
indicates Hedges’s effect size gfor the ith
study. Further, we calculated confidence inter-
vals and pvalues by taking the square root of
the inverse of the sum of the weights.
Moderator analysis. A critical next
step to the investigation of effect size distri-
bution is moderator analysis. We started by
calculating the homogeneity statistic Q. This
statistic provided information about the distri-
bution of effect sizes, and a large test statistic
(i.e., rejecting the null hypothesis of study
homogeneity) indicated that moderator analy-
ses were appropriate (Raudenbush, 2009).
Given this statistical confirmation, we used
procedures analogous to analysis of variance
(ANOVA), where one attempts to model ef-
fect-size heterogeneity associated with cate-
gorical study-level variables. Further, because
of the small number of studies that constituted
the review, we calculated the variance compo-
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
55
nent across all groups rather than within
groups as is generally conducted (Hedges &
Vevea, 1998).
Lipsey (2009) discussed three types of
independent variables common to meta-ana-
lytic practice: extrinsic variables, method vari-
ables, and substantive variables. Extrinsic and
method variables relate to the study’s dissem-
ination (i.e., published or unpublished) or
methodological constraints (i.e., randomized
or nonrandomized). Substantive variables, on
the other hand, should be regarded as variables
of interest and generally include characteris-
tics of the population or treatment. For this
review, we coded substantive independent
variables to reflect the participant’s age, length
of treatment, and treatment type (e.g., individ-
ual or group).
Finally, we conducted a moderator anal-
ysis procedure analogous to regression,
weighted meta-regression. This statistical pro-
cedure allows for the simultaneous estimation
of study-level effects, but shares the problems
of typical regression (Cooper, 2010). For this
review, we modeled two independent vari-
ables simultaneously, treatment population
(categorical variable) and the percent of males
in the treatment group.
Sensitivity analysis. Two of the re-
viewed studies contributed a measure that
used an odds ratio. To ensure that study find-
ings were not biased by including these mea-
sures, we conducted a sensitivity analysis. The
analysis consisted of removing the 2 studies
(Evers et al., 2007; Merrell, 2004) and recal-
culating the weighted effect size. We hypoth-
esized that no difference would be found be-
tween the two types of measurements.
Publication bias. Publication bias re-
mained an important consideration during the
literature search and analyses. Rosenthal
(1979) introduced the “file-drawer problem,”
which stated that studies with small or nonsig-
nificant effect sizes tended to remain unpub-
lished. To combat this problem, we included
unpublished works from three dissertations
and one master’s thesis (Rothstein, Sutton, &
Borenstein, 2005).
To avoid and interpret the overestima-
tion of the random-effects estimate, we used
the nonparametric trim and fill procedure (Du-
val & Tweedie, 2000) to assess the sensitivity
of results to publication bias. This procedure
estimates the number of publications theoret-
ically missing because of funnel plot asymme-
try, and then recalculates the random-effects
mean and confidence interval to include the
imputed missing studies. We also reported
Rosenthal’s fail-safe N (1979) and Egger,
Davey Smith, Schnieder, & Minder’s (1997)
regression coefficient.
Results
Meta-Analysis Literature
Table 1 provides characteristics for each
study included in the review. The studies re-
viewed include 7 published journal articles, 1
book chapter, and 3 unpublished papers (two
dissertations and one master’s thesis). Seven
of the 11 studies were conducted within the
United States and the other 4 were conducted
in Belgium, Finland, Greece, and Italy, respec-
tively. All studies were completed between
2000 and 2010.
One article contributed two effect sizes
because it included two mutually exclusive
interventions from two separate populations
(Stevens et al., 2000). Therefore, we synthe-
sized a total of 12 interventions. Each of
the 12 interventions included a treatment and
control group; 4 of the 12 programs used qua-
si-experimental design and the other 8 used a
randomized experimental design. A total
of 12,874 students participated in the 12
interventions.
Outcome Effect Sizes
Bystander intervention outcome. As
delineated in Lipsey and Wilson (2001), we
estimated the random effects weighted mean
by using Equation 8. The results revealed a
statistically significant positive weighted aver-
age (g.20, p.001, 95% CI .11 to .29).
In other words, the treatment increased by-
stander intervention behavior 20% of one stan-
dard deviation more than individuals in the
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
56
control group. Table 2 provided a forest plot of
the random-effects model’s relevant statistics.
Empathy outcome. Of the 12 inter-
ventions used to calculate the bystander inter-
vention outcome weighted average, 8 included
a measure on victim empathy. As previously
conducted, we used a random effects model to
estimate the weighted treatment mean. The
results revealed a very small, nonstatistically
significant result (g.05, p.38) with a
confidence interval that included zero (95%
CI ⫽⫺.07 to .17). The small number of
studies that included a measure of empathy
may not provide enough power to detect a
small effect. Therefore, the results of this anal-
ysis should be considered inconclusive (see
Table 3).
Moderator Analysis
We categorized the effect sizes into sev-
eral relevant groups and conducted the ran-
dom-effects ANOVA-like analysis (Table 4).
A statistically significant Qvalue indicated
appropriate heterogeneity between studies that
Table 3
Summary Statistics for Empathy Effect Sizes
Study Name Hedges’ g
Standard
Error
Lower
Limit
Upper
Limit Z-Value p-Value
Andreou .19 .10 .38 .01 1.90 .06
Fonagy .23 .09 .40 .07 2.75 .01
Frey .18 .07 .05 .31 2.72 .01
Karna .15 .02 .10 .19 6.62 .01
McLaughlin .17 .32 .80 .45 .05 .59
Schumacher .29 .07 .15 .43 4.13 .01
Stevens “A” .05 .12 .28 .18 .40 .69
Stevens “B” .14 .10 .06 .33 1.35 .18
Overall .05 .06 .07 .17 .74 .46
Table 2
Summary Statistics for Bystander Intervention Effect Sizes
Study Name Hedges’ g
Standard
Error
Lower
Limit
Upper
Limit Z-Value p-Value
Andreou .01 .10 .20 .19 .04 .97
Evers .46 .09 .27 .64 4.88 .01
Fonagy .05 .08 .11 .22 .62 .54
Frey .11 .07 .02 .24 1.68 .09
Karna .14 .02 .10 .19 6.51 .01
McLaughlin .21 .32 .42 .83 .65 .52
Mensini .03 .12 .21 .26 .22 .82
Merrell .60 .90 1.17 2.24 .66 .51
Schumacher .43 .07 .29 .57 6.08 .01
Stevens “A” .06 .12 .17 .29 .53 .60
Stevens “B” .39 .10 .19 .59 3.89 .01
Whitaker .25 .05 .15 .34 5.16 .01
Overall .20 .04 .11 .28 4.54 .01
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
57
measured the bystander intervention construct
(Q39.81, df 11, p.001; I
2
72.36).
However, because of the relatively small num-
ber of studies that included an empathy mea-
sure, we chose not to conduct moderator anal-
yses to protect against findings of chance.
The results of these analyses were de-
composed into substantive and methodologi-
cal characteristics. Samples that consisted of
high school students only generated a signifi-
cantly greater treatment effect (ES 0.43,
CI .33 to .52) compared to samples of
primary schools only (ES 0.14, CI .11 to
.18). We further assessed sample differences
by conducting an analysis to evaluate location
differences. The results revealed that pro-
grams located in the United States (US) did
not differ significantly from those located in
Europe (EU; US ES 0.26, EU ES 0.13,
p.17).
Another substantive moderator, treat-
ment length, failed to produce significantly
greater treatment effects (1–2 months
ES 0.31, 6 –12 months ES 0.16; p.09).
Similarly, treatments that included a parental
component (e.g., parent guides, parent training
sessions) failed to influence the treatment sig-
nificantly compared to programs without the
component (parent included ES 0.19, parent
excluded ES 0.20, p.92).
We also grouped the studies by who
facilitated the treatment programs. The teach-
Table 4
Moderator & Meta-regression Analysis of Bystander Intervention
Effect Sizes
Moderator K Hedges’ g95% C.I. p-value
Population
3
rd
–8
th
Grade 8 .14 .11, .18 .04
9
th
–12
th
Grade 4 .43 .33, .52
Location
United States 7 .26 .14, .38 .17
Europe 5 .13 .01, .26
Treatment length
1–2 months 5 .31 .16, .45 .09
6–12 months 7 .16 .06, .25
Parent Component
Yes 5 .19 .08, .31 .92
No 7 .20 .07, .34
Facilitator
a
Teacher 7 .15 .09, .22 .01
Other 4 .43 .30, .56
Assignment
Non-Random 4 .17 .01, .35 .74
Random 8 .21 .10, .31
Publication type
Peer-review 8 .16 .06, .25 .07
Non-peer review 4 .32 .17, .48
Meta-Regression S.E. Z-score p-value
Percent Male .01 .009 1.04 .15
HS (1 Yes) .25 .13 1.98 .02
p.05; a-Menesini et al. (2003) did not indicate the facilitator; HS High School sample.
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
58
ers implemented a significant portion of the
programs for 7 of the 12 programs. Four other
programs were facilitated by the researcher, a
counselor, or in one case (Evers et al., 2007),
with computer software. One study failed to
indicate who implemented the program (Me-
nesini, Codecasa, Benelli, & Cowie, 2003).
The results of this moderator analysis revealed
significantly greater treatment effects for pro-
grams that implemented the program with fa-
cilitators other than the teacher (teacher
ES 0.15, other ES 0.43, p.01). How-
ever, serious caution should be given to this
finding because two of the four programs that
used researchers as facilitators had the small-
est sample sizes, and therefore this could be a
reflection of imprecision or biased effects be-
cause of small samples (Levine, Asada, &
Carpenter, 2009).
Finally, we conducted moderator analy-
ses with two methodological groupings. We
first observed mean group differences between
randomly assigned (RA) and nonrandomly as-
signed treatments (NRA) groups. The results
indicated that there were no statistically sig-
nificant differences (RA ES 0.21, NRA
ES 0.17, p.74). We also estimated group
differences between peer-reviewed (PR) and
nonpeer-reviewed studies (NPR). The results
of this calculation revealed that nonpeer-re-
viewed studies did not produce a greater treat-
ment effect (PR ES 0.16, NPR ES 0.32,
p.07).
In addition to the ANOVA-like model-
ing, we conducted a weighted regression anal-
yses. This analysis allowed us to estimate the
effects of several predictors simultaneously.
We hypothesized that the percentage of males
in the treatment interventions and programs
conducted in a high school would be signifi-
cantly related to the treatment effect (i.e.,
Hedges’s g). The results of this analysis again
revealed that, after controlling for the percent-
age of males in the treatment group, high
school samples produced a greater treatment
effect compared to middle or elementary
school interventions (␤⫽0.25, Z1.98,
p.05). These results bolstered the previous
findings.
To ensure that the overall effect size was
not upwardly biased by including different
measurements, we conducted a sensitivity
analysis removing studies that used a dichot-
omous outcome. The results of the analysis
found that the overall weighted effect size
decreased slightly (original ES 0.20, mod-
ified ES 0.18). The overall effect remained
statistically and practically significant.
Publication Bias
We applied Duval and Tweedie’s (2000)
trim and fill procedure to address publication
bias. This procedure revealed that one nega-
tive result was missing from the bystander
intervention outcomes. However, the imputed
missing values would only slightly change the
overall fixed effect size (ES 0.20); more
important, it remained practically and statisti-
cally significant (see Figure 1). We also used
Rosenthal’s fail-safe N procedure; the results
of this calculations indicated that 236 null
studies would be required to result in a non-
significant finding. Egger’s regression inter-
cept coefficient calculation also produced non-
significant results (
0
0.57, p.26). Taken
together, we concluded that the review’s re-
sults were not affected significantly by publi-
cation bias.
Discussion
The purpose of this quantitative synthe-
sis was to examine the treatment effects of
bullying prevention programs on bystander in-
tervention behavior. Empathy for the victim
was also synthesized as a secondary outcome,
but was not of primary purpose for the current
review. In total, we reviewed 11 studies (12
effect sizes) from the United States and Eu-
rope that included 12,874 children.
Using meta-analytic techniques, the re-
sults revealed that the intervention behavior of
bystanders increased (i.e., bystanders indi-
cated greater intervention behavior in bullying
situations) compared to control groups (g
.20). The results of a secondary analysis
revealed that intervention programs did not
have a similar effect on empathy for the victim
(g.05), but this finding should be viewed as
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
59
inconclusive because of the small number of
studies that reported this outcome and its sec-
ondary nature.
These overall results mirrored the find-
ings of a previously conducted small synthe-
sis. Merrell et al. (2008) synthesized bullying
prevention programs to investigate the effects
of the programs on bullying perpetration, but
also included several secondary measures
(e.g., bystander intervention behavior, empa-
thy). The authors reported small but signifi-
cant treatment effects for bystander interven-
tion behavior (k3, g.17) and nonsignif-
icant negative effects with regard to empathy
(k3, g⫽⫺.10). Taken together, these
replications results provided evidence against
mono-operation bias and thus greater validity
(Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002).
Moderator analyses also revealed sev-
eral findings of interest. Results of both
ANOVA-like and weighted-regression analy-
ses revealed that the treatment effects were
greater for high school only samples. This is
somewhat surprising because some scholars
have postulated that bullying prevention pro-
grams are more effective for middle school-
aged children (Williford et al., 2011). These
results may indicate that bystander interven-
tion behavior is a developmental process and
programs may not influence younger students
as intended.
The purpose of meta-analytic research is
to generalize findings across populations,
treatments, outcomes, and designs (Matt &
Cook, 2009). Although this synthesis aggre-
gated a smaller number of studies, its findings
rendered generalizability of bullying preven-
tion program’s effects on the bystander inter-
vention construct. A few factors bolster this
belief.
The populations assessed varied across
ages, locations, and treatments. The largest
effects were found for high school only sam-
ples; however, an overall significant treatment
effect was also found for the total sample.
With regard to location, no significant differ-
ences were found between U.S. and European
samples. In addition, the studies employed a
wide variety of treatment programs that
proved efficacious. Moderator analyses also
suggested that study design, publication type,
and parental components produced similar
-2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0 .5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1.0
Standar d Error
Funnel Plot of Standard Error by Hedges’s g
Hedges’s g
Figure 1. Trim and fill funnel plot of intervention effect sizes. This figure
illustrates each effect size relative to its standard error; the shaded dot indicates
an imputed effect size.
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
60
findings across studies. These results consti-
tuted a test of effects holding “across pre-
sumed irrelevancies” (Shadish et al., 2002, p.
455) and increases the findings’ external va-
lidity. Further investigation and evaluation is
certainly required, but these results should
cautiously suggest program generalizability.
Limitations
Several limitations should be noted.
First, this meta-analysis included only 11 stud-
ies and 12 effect sizes. Although we took
precautions to ensure unbiased effect sizes and
findings, a great deal of caution should be used
when interpreting the findings. The findings
from a small collection of studies, no matter
the statistical technique or number of students
surveyed, should not enact immediate policy
and practical changes. This becomes espe-
cially clear when one considers the effect of
studies across time. Recent research in the
field of meta-analysis publication bias has in-
dicated that as programs increase in size and
fidelity, effect sizes tend to decrease (Trikali-
nos & Ioannidis, 2005). Therefore, we plan to
update these results periodically to observe the
effect of time.
Second, because of the nature of meta-
analyses, causal inferences should be stated
cautiously. Quantitative meta-analysis, al-
though statistically sophisticated and impor-
tant, remains essentially an observational
study (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001; Cooper &
Hedges, 2009). On the other hand, the sample
of studies we synthesized contained only those
that used a treatment and control group, and
these research designs constitute the most ef-
ficient measure of treatment effect (Shadish et
al., 2002). Therefore, the synthesis of these
results should partially reflect the nature of the
primary studies.
Third, because of the low number of
studies per factor, the ANOVA-like moderator
analyses generated relatively low statistical
power and the results should be interpreted
with caution (Hedges & Pigott, 2004). More-
over, the low number of studies per group
could easily capitalize on chance, or the
groupings may reflect some other unforeseen
variable. This becomes especially clear with
regard to the studies grouped by treatment
length. As alluded to previously, a majority of
the studies grouped in the 1–2 month category
were the smallest of studies. Smaller studies
tend to produce larger and more unstable ef-
fect sizes (Levine et al., 2009), and therefore
the grouping could reflect this phenomenon.
Fourth, although we made efforts to col-
lect all primary studies that focused on by-
stander intervention behavior, it is quite pos-
sible that studies failed to be included. New
material published postsearch, misspecified
search terms, or simple human error could
all cause inadvertent omission of extant lit-
erature. As such, we must temper our infer-
ences with regard to the extrapolation of this
information.
Implications for Future Policy and
Practice
This meta-analysis should help cau-
tiously to shift the emphasis of policy and
practice. The results of this meta-analysis re-
vealed two implications for policy. First, state
and national bullying legislation should imple-
ment and evaluate programs that address bul-
lying behaviors as a group process. Prevention
frameworks and programs that attempt to
abate bullying within schools are increasingly
emphasizing changes in school climate that
desist reinforcing bystander behavior or bul-
lying perpetration (Cohen, 2006). The results
of this study support these efforts to raise
awareness about the participant roles, to en-
courage active and prosocial behavior, and to
provide opportunities to role-play and practice
bystander intervention in vivo.
Second, the results of this meta-analysis
revealed that bullying prevention programs
might be effective at encouraging presocial
bystander intervention when the framework,
program, and/or curriculum explicitly target
bystander attitudes and behaviors. It is simply
not sufficient to only define prosocial by-
stander behaviors, such as walk away, get
help, or stand up to those engaged in bullying.
Policy must encourage the adoption of pro-
grams and interventions that shift attitudes
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
61
supportive of intervention (willingness to in-
tervene) and behaviors through a consistent
message about intervention and ample support
from adults and administrators.
Future Research
This research suggests important future
projects. First, primary research should focus
on designing programs, implementing change,
and measuring the bystander construct. As
mentioned previously, researchers should fo-
cus on changing the behavior of the bystander.
Second, further work is required to evaluate
the effects of bystander behavior on bullying.
This meta-analysis merely demonstrated that
explicitly stated bystander programs have the
ability to increase bystander intervention be-
havior. However, future research must con-
tinue to assess how bystanders implement
these processes and the direct effects on active
bullying. Third, the results from the empathy
review revealed inconclusive findings. Future
research is required to elucidate the effects of
prevention programs on this outcome. Finally,
continued quantitative syntheses that focus on
bystander behavior are required. Certainly
only 11 studies from the last 10 years cannot
accurately describe the scope of this issue.
Future meta-analyses should incorporate,
hopefully, new relevant literature.
Footnote
*Article used in meta-analysis.
References
*Andreou, E., Didaskalou, E., & Vlachou, A. (2008).
Outcomes of a curriculum-based anti-bullying inter-
vention program on students’ attitudes and behavior.
Emotional & Behavioural Difficulties, 13, 235–248.
Astor, R. A., Meyer, H. A., Benbenishty, R., Marachi, R.,
& Rosemond, M. (2005). School safety interventions:
Best practices and programs. Children and Schools,
27(1), 17.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Roth-
stein, H. R. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis,
Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Roth-
stein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect
and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Re-
search Synthesis Methods, 1, 97–111.
Bosworth, K., Espelage, D., & Simon, T. (1999). Factors
associated with bullying behavior in middle school
students. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 19,
341–352.
Cohen, J. (2006). Social, emotional, ethical, and academic
education: Creating a climate for learning, participa-
tion in democracy, and well-being. Harvard Educa-
tional Review, 76, 201–237.
Cooper, H. (2010). Research synthesis and meta-analysis
(4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Cooper, H., & Hedges, L.V. (2009). Potentials and limi-
tations. In H. Cooper, L.V. Hedges, & J. C. Valentine
(Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-
analysis (pp. 562–571). New York: Sage Publication.
Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The
handbook of research synthesis (2nd ed.). New York:
Russell Sage Foundation.
Cowie, H. (2000). Bystanding or standing by: Gender
issues in coping with bullying in English schools.
Aggressive Behavior, 26, 85–97.
Cowie, H., & Hutson, N. (2006). Peer support: A strategy
to help bystanders challenge school bullying. Pastoral
Care in Education, 23(2), 40 – 44.
Craig, W. M. (1994). The relationship among bullying,
victimization, depression, anxiety, and aggression in
elementary school children. Personal Individual Dif-
ferences, 24(1), 123.
Craig, W. M., Pepler, D., & Atlas, R. (2000). Observa-
tions of bullying in the playground and in the class-
room. School Psychology International, 21(2), 22–36.
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). A nonparametric “trim
and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in
meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical As-
sociation, 95(449), 89 –98.
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schnieder, M., & Minder, C.
(1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple,
graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629 – 634.
Espelage, D. L., Bosworth, K., & Simon, T. R. (2001).
Short-term stability and prospective correlates of bul-
lying in middle-school students: An examination of
potential demographic, psychosocial, and environmen-
tal factors. Violence and Victims, 16, 411– 426.
Espelage, D. L., & Swearer, S. M. (2003). Research on
school bullying and victimization: What have we
learned and where do we go from here? School Psy-
chology Review, 32, 365–385.
Espelage, D. L., Green, H. D., & Polanin, J. R. (in press).
Willingness to intervene in bullying episodes among
middle school students: Individual and peer-group in-
fluences. Journal of Early Adolescence.
*Evers, K., Prochaska, J., Van Marter, D., Johnson, J., &
Prochaska, J. (2007). Transtheoretical-based bullying
prevention effectiveness trials in middle schools and
high schools. [references]. Educational Research, 49,
397– 414.
Ferguson, C. J., Miguel, C. S., Kilburn, J. C., & Sanchez,
P. (2007). The effectiveness of school-based anti-bul-
lying programs: A meta-analytic review. Criminal Jus-
tice Review, 32, 401– 414.
*Fonagy, P., Twemlow, S. W., Vernberg, E. M., Nelson,
J. M., Dill, E. J., Little, T. D., et al. (2009). A cluster
randomized controlled trial of child-focused psychiat-
ric consultation and a school systems-focused interven-
tion to reduce aggression. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 50, 607– 616.
Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Edstrom, L. V., & Snell,
J. L. (2009). Observed reductions in school bullying,
nonbullying aggression, and destructive bystander be-
havior: A longitudinal evaluation. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 101, 466 – 481.
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
62
*Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., Snell, J. L., Edstrom, L.,
Van Schoiack, L., MacKenzie, E. P., & Broderick,
C. J. (2005). Reducing playground bullying and sup-
porting beliefs: An experimental trial of the steps to
respect program. Developmental Psychology, 41,
479 – 491.
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary, and meta-anal-
ysis of research. Educational Researcher, 5(10), 3– 8.
Glew, G. M., Fan, M., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., &
Kermic, M. A. (2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjust-
ment, and academic performance in elementary school.
Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026 –1031.
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoe`, G. (2007).
Does empathy predict adolescents’ bullying and de-
fending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467– 476.
Hawkins, D. L., Pepler, D. J., Craig, W. M. (2001).
Naturalistic observations of peer interventions in bul-
lying. Social Development, 10, 543–555.
Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for glass’s es-
timator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of
Educational Statistics, 6, 107–128.
Hedges, L.V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for
meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2004). The power of
statistical tests for moderators in meta-analysis. Psy-
chological Methods, 9, 426 – 444.
Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed- and random-
effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Meth-
ods, 3, 486 –504.
Howell, D. C. (2010). Statistical methods for psychology
(7th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury.
Hymel, S., & Swearer, S. (2010). Bullying: An age-old
problem that needs new solutions. Education.com. Re-
trieved from http://www.education.com/reference/
article/bullying-about-power-and-abuse-of-power/
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpela, M., Marttunen, M., Rimpela,
A., & Rantanen, P. (1999). Bullying, depression, and
suicidal ideation in Finnish adolescents: School sur-
vey. British Medical Journal, 319, 348 –351.
*Karna, A., Voeten, M., Little, T. D., Poskiparta, E., Kal-
jonen, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). A large-scale evalu-
ation of the KiVa antibullying program: Grades 4 6.
Child Development, 82, 311–330.
Kochenderfer, B. J., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Peer victim-
ization: Manifestations and relations to school adjust-
ment in kindergarten. Journal of School Psychology,
34, 267–283.
Lagerspetz, K. J., Bjorkqvist, K., Bertz, M., & King, E.
(1982). Group aggression among school children in
three schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
23, 45–53.
Levine, T., Asada, K. J., & Carpenter, C. (2009). Sample
sizes and effect sizes are negatively correlated in meta-
analyses: Evidence and implications of a publication
bias against nonsignificant findings. Communication
Monographs, 76, 286 –302.
Lipsey, M. W. (2009). Identifying interesting variables
and analysis opportunities. In H. Cooper, L. V.
Hedges, & J. C. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of
research synthesis and meta-analysis (pp. 147–158).
New York: Sage Publication.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-
analysis (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Matt, G. E., & Cook, T. D. (2009). Threats to the validity
of generalized inferences. In H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges,
& J. C. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research
synthesis and meta-analysis (pp. 538 –557). New York:
Sage Publication.
McDougal, P., Hymel, S., & Vaillancourt, T. (2009).
What happens over time to those who bully and those
who are victimized? Education.com. Retrieved from
http://www.education.com/reference/article/Ref_
What_Happens_Over/
*McLaughlin, L. P. (2009). The effect of cognitive behav-
ioral therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy plus
media on the reduction of bullying and victimization
and the increase of empathy and bystander response in
a bully prevention program for urban sixth-grade stu-
dents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of
Toledo, Digital Dissertations.
*Menesini, E., Codecasa, E., Benelli, B., & Cowie, H.
(2003). Enhancing children’s responsibility to take ac-
tion against bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 1–14.
*Merrell, R. (2004). The impact of drama intervention
program on response of the bystander to bullying
situations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer-
sity of Rochester, Digital Dissertations.
Merrell, K. W., Gueldner, B. A., Ross, S. W., & Isava,
D. M. (2008). How effective are school bullying inter-
vention programs? A meta-analysis of intervention re-
search. School Psychology Quarterly,23, 26 – 42.
Mishna, F., Cook, C., Saini, M., Wu, M. J., & MacFadden,
R. (2010). Interventions to prevent and reduce cyber
abuse of youth. Research on Social Work Practice,
1(1), 1–10.
Musher-Eizeman, D. R., Boxer, P., Danner, S., Dubow,
E. F., Goldstein, S. E., & Herretick, D. M. I. (2004).
Social-cognitive mediators of the relation of environ-
mental and emotion regulation factors to children’s
aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 389 – 408.
Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J.,
Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying
behaviors among US youth: Prevalence and associa-
tion with psychosocial adjustment. Journal of Ameri-
can Medical Association, 285, 2094 –2100.
Nickerson, A. B., Mele, D., & Princiotta, D. (2008).
Attachment and empathy as predictors of roles as de-
fenders or outsiders in bullying interactions. Journal of
School Psychology, 46, 687–703.
O’Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer in-
volvement in bullying: Insights and challenges for
intervention. Journal of Adolescence,22, 437– 452.
Olweus, D. (1973). Personality and aggression. In J. K.
Cole & D. D. Jensen (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation (pp. 260 –321). Lincoln: University of Ne-
braska Press.
Olweus, D. (2002). Bullying at school. Malden, MA:
Blackwell Publishers.
Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). Analyzing effect sizes: Ran-
dom-effects models. In H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, &
J. C. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of research syn-
thesis and meta-analysis (pp. 295–314). New York:
Sage Publication.
Rigby, K. (1999). Peer victimization at school and the
health of secondary school students. The Journal of
School Psychology, 69, 95–104.
Rigby, K., & Slee, P. T. (1993). Dimensions of interper-
sonal relation among Australian children and implica-
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
63
tions for psychological well-being. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 133, 33– 42.
Rivers, I., Poteat, V. P., Noret, N., & Ashurst, N. (2009).
Observing bullying at school: The mental health im-
plications of witness status. School Psychology Quar-
terly, 24, 211–223.
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer problem” and
tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86,
638 – 641.
Ross, S. W., & Horner, R. H. (2009). Bullying prevention
in positive behavior support. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 42, 747–759.
Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (2005).
Publication bias in meta-analysis. West Sussex, Eng-
land: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Ryan, W., & Smith, D. J. (2009). Antibullying programs
in schools: How effective are evaluation practices?
Prevention Science, 10, 248 –262.
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman,
K., & Kaukianen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group
process: Participant roles and their relations to social
status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1–12.
Salmivalli, C., Kaukianinen, A., & Voeten, M. (2005).
Anti-bullying intervention: Implementation and out-
come. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75,
465.
Sanchez-Meca, J., Marin-Martinez, F., & Chacon-Mos-
coso, S. (2003). Effect-size indices for dichotomized
outcomes in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 8,
448 – 462.
Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Overbeek, G.,
Kemp, R. A. T., & Haselager, G. J. T. (2007). Stability
in bullying and victimization and its association with
social adjustment in childhood and adolescence. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 217–228.
*Schumacher, P. J. (2007). To what extent will a sixty-
second video on bullying produced by high school
students increase students’ awareness of bullying and
change their attitudes to reduce acceptance of bullying
in a high school environment? Unpublished master’s
thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Digital
Dissertations.
Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002).
Experimental and quasi-experimental designs. New
York: Houghton Mifflin.
Sharp, S., Sellors, A., & Cowie, H. (1994). Time to listen:
Setting up a peer counseling service to help tackle the
problem of bullying in schools. Pastoral Care Educa-
tion, 12, 3– 6.
Smith, J. D., Twemlow, S. W., & Hoover, D. W. (1999).
Bullies, victims, and bystanders: A method of in-
school intervention and possible parental contribu-
tions. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 30,
29 –37.
Smith, J. D., Schneider, B. H., Smith, P. K., & Ananiadou,
K. (2004). The effectiveness of whole-school antibul-
lying programs: A synthesis of evaluation research.
School Psychology Review,33, 547–560.
Sourander, A., Helstela, L., Helenius, H., & Piha, J.
(2000). Persistence of bullying from childhood to ad-
olescence—A longitudinal 8-year follow-up study.
Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 873– 881.
SPSS. (2010). Statistical package for social sciences,
Version 19.0.0. Chicago, IL: IBM.
Sutton, A. J. (2005). Evidence concerning the conse-
quences of publication and related biases. In H. R.
Rothstein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Pub-
lication bias in meta-analysis (pp. 175–192). West
Sussex, England: Wiley & Sons.
*Stevens, V., Oost, P. V., & Bourdeaudhuij, I. D. (2000).
The effects of an anti-bullying intervention programme
on peers’ attitudes and behaviour. Journal of Adoles-
cence, 23, 21–34.
Swearer, S. M., Espelage, D. L., Vaillancourt, T., &
Hymel, S. (2010). What can be done about school
bullying? Linking research to educational practice. Ed-
ucational Researcher, 39, 38 – 47.
Sweeting, H., Young, R., West, P., & Der, G. (2006). Peer
victimization and depression in early-mid adolescence:
A longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 76, 577–594.
Trikalinos, T. A., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Assessing
the evolution of effect sizes over time. In H. R. Roth-
stein, A. J. Sutton, & M. Borenstein (Eds.), Publication
bias in meta-analysis. West Sussex, England: John
Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2009). What works in
preventing bullying: Effective elements of anti-bully-
ing programmes. Journal of Aggression, 1, 13–24.
Ttofi, M. M., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Effectiveness of
school-based programs to reduce bullying: A system-
atic and meta-analytic review. Journal of Experimental
Criminology, 7, 27–56.
Twemlow, S. W., Fonagy, P., & Sacco, F. C. (2004). The
role of bystander in social architecture of bullying and
violence in schools and communities. Annals of the
New York Academy of Sciences,1036, 215–232.
Unnever, J. D., & Cornell, D. G. (2003). The culture of
bullying in middle school. Journal of School Violence,
2, 5–27.
Wang, J., Iannotti, R. J., & Nansel, T. R. (2009). School
bullying among adolescents in the United States: Phys-
ical, verbal, relational, cyber. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 45, 368 –375.
*Whitaker, D. J., Rosenbluth, B., Valle, L. A., & Sanchez,
E. (2004). Expect respect: A school-based intervention
to promote awareness and effective responses to bul-
lying and sexual harassment. In D. L. Espelage & S. M.
Swearer (Eds.), Bullying in American schools (1st ed.,
pp. 327–350). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Williford, A., Boulton, A., Noland, B., Little, T. D.,
Karna, A., & Salmivalli, C. (2011). Effects of the KiVa
anti-bullying program on adolescents’ depression, anx-
iety, and perception of peers. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology. Retrieved from http://www.spring-
erlink.com/content/ 2532380182216828/
Date Received: February 1, 2011
Date Accepted: September 7, 2011
Action Editor: Joseph Betts
School Psychology Review, 2012, Volume 41, No. 1
64
Joshua R. Polanin is a research methodology doctoral student at Loyola University
Chicago. His interests include methodological improvements to meta-analysis and hier-
archical linear modeling. He currently serves as the methodologist for a large, school-
based bullying prevention program and as the managing editor of the Campbell Collab-
oration’s Methods Group.
Dorothy L. Espelage is a professor in the Department of Educational Psychology at the
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Her research programs include investigations
of bullying, sexual harassment, and dating violence among adolescents for almost two
decades. She is engaged in a large randomized clinical trial of a school-based bullying
prevention program.
Therese D. Pigott is a professor at the School of Education, Loyola University Chicago.
Her research interests are statistical methods for meta-analysis and methods for handling
missing data in statistical analysis. She is a co-chair and editor of the Methods Group of
the Campbell Collaboration, an international organization that produces systematic re-
views of social interventions.
A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Bullying
65
Copyright of School Psychology Review is the property of National Association of School Psychologists and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
... Supportive family environments and community programs can offer essential resources for bullied youth, providing them with coping mechanisms and alternative social outlets (Holt et al., 2008). Furthermore, empowering youth to take an active stance against bullying, through peer-led initiatives and bystander intervention programs, has been shown to be effective in mitigating the incidence and impact of bullying behaviours (Polanin et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the pervasive issue of social ostracism and bullying among university students in Kazakhstan and Türkiye, underscored by Goffman’s (1963) stigma theory. It investigates how societal norms and categorizations contribute to the marginalisation of students based on various identities, leading to ‘spoiled identities’ and the amplification of these behaviours through digital media. Employing a multiple case research model within a qualitative methodological tradition, the study involves 10 students from Kazakhstan and 27 from Türkiye, utilising semi-structured interviews to collect data. The findings reveal that social ostracism and bullying are multifaceted phenomena deeply rooted in differences such as religious beliefs, political orientations, and socio-economic status. Key factors contributing to these issues include the influence of social media, lack of empathy, and prevailing social hierarchies. The effects on students’ academic, social, and emotional well-being are profound, leading to decreased engagement, isolation, and psychological distress. While some students are unaware of university support mechanisms, those who have accessed such services report positive outcomes, highlighting the importance of visibility and accessibility. The study calls for comprehensive strategies addressing both the immediate and underlying factors of social ostracism and bullying, advocating for policy development, supportive interventions, and a commitment to fostering inclusivity.
... The findings about the impact of these programs are promising. For example, a meta-analysis of studies of elementary through high school students showed small to medium significant effect sizes for increases in bystander helping to address bullying (Polanin et al., 2012) and reductions in relationship and sexual violence among middle through college-age participants (Jouriles et al., 2018;Park & Kim, 2023). A study of high school students found that program effects on reducing sexual violence perpetration were explained in part through increased bystander actions (Bush et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Research and practices focused on engaging bystander intervention to prevent violence have shown promise as part of multipronged strategies. It also faces challenges and mixed effects that hamper its effectiveness. Method: This perspective commentary critically evaluates what we have learned from the past 25 years of work. It provides a vision for four key areas that should be the focus of future work to enhance the effectiveness of this prevention approach. Results: Key areas are (a) the need to focus on strengths rather than risk factors, (b) the need to do more to address relational, (c) contextual factors for prevention, and (d) understanding bystander burden/consequences of actions. Conclusions: Each area is explored in relation to example research questions that need to be posed and answered and where prevention practitioners can continue to innovate.
... Researchers have found that mobilizing bystanders to intervene to stop bullying is an important part of bullying prevention (Polanin et al., 2012). Bullying decreases when bystanders intervene as "defenders" (Salmivalli et al., 2011); however, many students reported they lack the skills to intervene (Bauman et al., 2020) and only 20% reported using defending behavior when they witness bullying (Salmivalli et al., 2005). ...
... However, if the best prevention or intervention comes from within the class (cf. Polanin et al. 2012) the goal of interweaving outsiders and defenders as an anti-bullying group is still convincing. It still does not imply that outsiders are or can be directly socially intertwined with defenders as potential prosocial norm setters. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The issue of bullying persists by a lack of sufficient and sustained success in the implementation of intervention strategies and prevention initiatives. In the past decade, the results of evaluations were disconcerting, even though the designs of the programs were based closely or directly on the current state of research. To activate outsiders as part of the antibully group due to association with defenders didn’t work. Could the individualistic focus rather than a group-orientation have been overtaxed to search for explanations? Is it insufficient to analyze outsiders for their skills and abilities if perpetuating change in classroom is aimed for? Could Kurt Lewin’s perspective that “behavior is a function of person and context” offer insights for consideration? The present study identifies a key area for clarifying the mentioned, arising research question: the heterogeneity of the outsider role. Based on peer reports (PRQ) from German seventh to tenth graders (N=1631, recalculation of our data set), the potential heterogeneity of the outsider group is examined. The results provide a detailed insight into how outsiders can and do position themselves differently within the class, resulting in effects by heterogeneity by multi-layered division of the antibully group.
Article
The role of bystanders in bullying and cyberbullying prevention is crucial. Strategies must be tailored to address the shared and unique factors in online and offline contexts, ensuring that interventions create an environment where bystanders are empowered and feel responsible to act against bullying and cyberbullying. This meta-analysis examines and compares the effectiveness of interventions in enhancing bystander behaviors in bullying and cyberbullying scenarios. A comprehensive search was conducted using databases including PsycINFO, Medline, Sociological Abstracts, Social Service Abstracts, ERIC, and Scopus. Quasi-experimental and randomized controlled trials published before March 31, 2024 that reported that the effects of bystander interventions were included. The synthesis comprised 49 studies, reporting an overall random effect size (Cohen’s d) of 0.25. The results highlight the crucial roles of enhancing knowledge, self-efficacy, and coping skills. Subgroup analysis revealed that the effective bystander interventions include smaller sample sizes (<100 participants), shorter durations (<1 month), targeting college students, and utilizing offline intervention approaches and digital techniques. These findings emphasize the promising effects and tailored characteristics of bystander intervention programs in bullying and cyberbullying contexts. Our review identifies avenues for future research within educational settings to develop more effective bystander behavior interventions to reduce bullying and cyberbullying.
Article
Research The experience of obstetric violence is well documented. However, the impact on and experience of doulas and midwives who witness OV deserves more attention. The research questions that guided this study were: What types of harm are doulas and midwives witnessing, what factors influence their decision to intervene, and what impact does this experience have on their professional career and well-being? Methods This exploratory study used semi-structured qualitative interviews to understand how doulas and midwives navigate the experience of witnessing OV. The data analysis included reflexive thematic analysis. Feminist standpoint theory was employed to center the lived experiences of women and other individuals from marginalized groups in the research and in the knowledge acquisition process. Key contributions This study found that independent birth professionals like doulas and midwives are struggling with how to navigate their roles in the obstetric system. They find creative ways to advocate yet are often met with mistreatment themselves. This work is a call to action to the field of social work to engage in issues related to perinatal health and reproductive justice.
Preprint
Full-text available
Evaluations evidence that up to now outsiders can’t be targeted effectively to tackle bullying in schools. Neither promoting skills and abilities nor reducing fear aspects result in convincing improvements. Borrowing from the field of developmental psychology and supported by Darley and Latané’s findings on helping behavior, Eisenberg’s research proves personal distress as a source of freezing when empathetic instead of sympathetic responses take over. A linkage between observable outsider behavior and processual insights of bystanding is postulated and examined. 326 8th and 9th graders responded to the Participant Role Questionnaire, sociometric and related measures including the moral disengagement scale and underwent physiological testing to assess stress levels triggered by a 20-minute movie clip on bullying. The results extend existing knowledge on the behavioral level: For one in three, sociometric indexes imply a social detachment from class interactions by general outsiders. However, if outsiders are reported to be close to a bullying episode (situational outsiders), personal distress proves to be a central source to explain their behavior. Implications point to changes in how preventive actions should be adapted.
Article
Youth aggression is an international public health problem that has major consequences for victims and perpetrators. Given the social nature of aggression, understanding the situational role of peers is critical for informing effective prevention strategies. However, traditional research on situational peer influence has been limited by the neglect of the person–environment interaction and the failure to theorise the action decision making process. This article responds to these challenges by applying and developing situational action theory (SAT) to theorise the situational mechanisms and conditions of peer influence. The situational model of peer influence presented in this article moves beyond traditional rational choice approaches by integrating interdisciplinary research on situational peer effects with SAT's dual-process model. This model specifies how and why the presence of aggressive peers influences the action decision making process that leads to aggressive behaviour. This article also describes the Peer Relations and Social Behaviour (PEERS) space-time budget method which is a recent methodological innovation that can be used to test this theoretical model. It concludes by outlining the main practical implications for the prevention of youth aggression.
Article
This paper examined British children's (8‐ to 10‐year‐olds) and adolescents' (13‐ to 15‐year‐olds, N = 340; Female N = 171, 50.3%) expectations, evaluations and reasoning about a bystander peer who challenges the social exclusion of an immigrant or non‐immigrant peer by a peer group of non‐immigrant students. Participants read a hypothetical scenario in which a peer was excluded from an afterschool club by the peer group. The scenarios were either intergroup or intragroup contexts. Participants' expectations of a peer bystander challenging the social exclusion by the peer group, their perception of how the peer group would evaluate the challenger, and their reasoning around their expectations were measured. Adolescents were less likely to expect a peer bystander to challenge exclusion compared to children. Participants' perceptions of how the group would evaluate the challenger were significantly lower in intergroup compared to intragroup contexts. In intergroup contexts, adolescents with low expectations of challenging favoured group dynamics and group repercussions reasoning over moral reasoning, while children did not use group repercussions reasoning.
Article
Full-text available
This Campbell Systematic Review examines the effectiveness of cyber abuse interventions in increasing internet safety knowledge and decreasing risky online behaviour. The review summarises findings from three studies: one conducted in Canada and the other two in the USA. The participants were middle school students in grades five to eight between the ages of 5‐19 who use the internet or cell phones. A total of 2,713 participants were included in the studies. Cyber abuse interventions and preventions are associated with an increase in internet safety knowledge. Despite the increase in knowledge, students who received the intervention did not become less likely to engage in inappropriate online behaviour, such as disclosing one's name, participating in open chat rooms, or emailing strangers. The three studies were evaluations of the following cyber abuse interventions: I‐SAFE cyber safety program, the missing cyber safety program, and the in‐school cyber bullying intervention (HAHASO). The I‐SAFE cyber safety had the largest effect on internet safety knowledge. Both the missing program and HAHASO suggests that intervention did not significantly change internet‐related safety attitudes or reduce the number of reported cyber bullying experiences. Given the low number of studies available for rigorous cyber abuse prevention and intervention evaluations, the evidence base for these conclusions is weak. Executive summary/Abstract BACKGROUND The Internet has created a new communication tool, particularly for young people whose use of e‐mail, websites, instant messaging, web cams, chat rooms, social networking sites and text messaging is exploding worldwide. While there are many benefits that result from electronic based communication, the Internet is, however, concurrently a potential site for abuse and victimization, whereby young people can fall victim to sexual perpetrators, stalkers, exploiters, and peers who bully online. Interventions regarding cyber abuse have been developed in response to a growing emphasis on protecting children and youth from online dangers. OBJECTIVES To examine the effectiveness of cyber abuse interventions in increasing Internet safety knowledge and decreasing risky online behaviour. SELECTION CRITERIA The scope of this review is experimental and quasi‐experimental prevention and intervention strategies that target children ages 5 to 19 years old and/or their parents, utilize a control group, and examine an outcome related to cyber abuse such as Internet safety knowledge, risky online behaviour, or exposure to inappropriate online content. SEARCH STRATEGY We searched the following databases : Psychological Abstracts (PsycINFO, PsycLIT, ClinPsyc‐clinical subset); MEDLINE; EMBASE; Database of reviews of effectiveness (DARE online); ChildData (child health and welfare); ASSIA (applied social sciences); Caredata (social work); Social Work Abstracts; Child Abuse, Child Welfare & Adoption; Cochrane Collaboration; C2‐SPECTR; Social Sciences Abstracts; Social Service Abstracts; Dissertation Abstracts International (DAI). We also handsearched Youth and Society; Journal of Interpersonal Violence; Annual Review of Sex Research; Computers in Human Behavior; Computers & Education; and Journal of Adolescent Health. Additionally, we contacted experts in the field and searched for grey literature. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS Two screeners reviewed abstracts and full‐text of all articles. Three articles met all inclusion criteria, and effect sizes and z‐tests were calculated for all relevant outcomes. MAIN RESULTS Significant z‐tests were found between pre‐and post‐test scores on measures related to Internet safety knowledge such as managing online risk and identifying online predators. Most z‐tests related to pre‐ and post‐ measures of risky online behaviour were not significant, including disclosing one's name, participating in open chat rooms, or emailing strangers. REVIEWERS’ CONCLUSIONS Results provide evidence that participation in psychoeducational Internet safety interventions is associated with an increase in Internet safety knowledge but is not significantly associated with a change in risky online behaviour. The need for further research in this field is highlighted.
Article
Bullying was investigated as a group process, a social phenomenon taking place in a school setting among 573 Finnish sixth-grade children (286 girls, 287 boys) aged 12–13 years. Different Participant Roles taken by individual children in the bullying process were examined and related to a) self-estimated behavior in bullying situations, b) social acceptance and social rejection, and c) belongingness to one of the five sociometric status groups (popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and average). The Participant Roles assigned to the subject were Victim, Bully, Reinforcer of the bully, Assistant of the bully, Defender of the victim, and Outsider. There were significant sex differences in the distribution of Participant Roles. Boys were more frequently in the roles of Bully, Reinforcer and Assistant, while the most frequent roles of the girls were those of Defender and Outsider. The subjects were moderately well aware of their Participant Roles, although they underestimated their participation in active bullying behavior and emphasized that they acted as Defenders and Outsiders. The sociometric status of the children was found to be connected to their Participant Roles. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Chapter
Quasi-experiments usually test the causal consequences of long-lasting treatments outside of the laboratory. But unlike “true” experiments where treatment assignment is at random, assignment in quasi-experiments is by self-selection or administrator judgment.
Article
Research synthesis relies on information reported in a selection of studies on a topic of interest. The purpose of this chapter is to examine the types of variables that can be coded from those studies and to outline the kinds of relationships that can be examined in the analysis of the resulting data. It identifies a range of analysis opportunities and endeavors to stimulate the synthesist's thinking about what data might be used in a research synthesis and what sorts of questions might be addressed.
Article
This chapter provides a nonstatistical way of summarizing many of the main points in the preceding chapters. In particular, it takes the major assumptions outlined and translates them from formal statistical notation into ordinary English. The emphasis is on expressing specific violations of formal meta-analytic assumptions as concretely labeled threats to valid inference. This explicitly integrates statistical approaches to meta-analysis with a falsificationist framework that stresses how secure knowledge depends on ruling out alternative interpretations. Thus, we aim to refocus readers' attention on the major rationales for research synthesis and the kinds of knowledge meta-analysts seek to achieve. The special promise of meta-analysis is to foster empirical knowledge about general associations, especially causal ones, that is more secure than what other methods typically warrant. In our view, no rationale for meta-analysis is more important than its ability to identify the realm of application of a knowledge claim - that is, identifying whether the association holds with specific populations of persons, settings, times and ways of varying the cause or measuring the effect; holds across different populations of people, settings, times, and ways of operationalizing a cause and effect; and can even be extrapolated to other populations of people, settings, times, causes, and effects than those studied to date. These are all generalization tasks that researchers face, perhaps no one more explicitly than the meta-analyst. It is easy to justify why we translate violated statistical assumptions into threats to validity, particularly threats to the validity of conclusions regarding the generality of an association. The past twenty-five years of meta-analytic practice have amply demonstrated that primary studies rarely present a census or even a random sample of the populations, universes, categories, classes, or entities (terms we use interchangeably) about which generalizations are sought. The salient exception is when random sampling occurs from some clearly designated universe, a procedure that does warrant valid generalization to the population from which the sample was drawn, usually a human population in the social sciences. But most surveys take place in decidedly restricted settings (a living room, for instance) and at a single time, and the relevant cause and effect constructs are measured without randomly selecting items. Moreover, many people are not interested in the population a particular random sample represents, but ask instead whether that same association hold with a different kind of person, in a different setting, at a different time, or with a different cause or effect. These questions concern generalization as extrapolation rather than representation (Cook 1990). How can we extrapolate from studied populations to populations with many, few, or even no overlapping attributes? The sad reality is that the general inferences meta-analysis seeks to provide cannot depend on formal sampling theory alone. Other warrants are also needed. This chapter assumes that ruling out threats to validity can serve as one such warrant. Doing so is not as simple or as elegant as sampling with known probability from a well-designated universe, but it is more flexible and has been used with success to justify how manipulations or measures are chosen to represent cause and effect constructs (that is, construct validity). If meta-analysis is to deal with generalization understood as both representation and extrapolation, we need ways of using a particular database to justify reasonable conclusions about what the available samples represent and how they can be used to extrapolate to other kinds of persons, settings, times, causes, and effects. This chapter is not the first to propose that a framework of validity threats allows us to probe the validity of research inferences when a fundamental statistical assumption has been violated. Donald Campbell introduced his internal validity threats for instances when primary studies lack random assignment, creating quasi-experimental design as a legitimate extension of the thinking R. A. Fisher had begun (Campbell 1957; Campbell and Stanley 1963). Similarly, this chapter seeks to identify threats to valid inferences about generalization that arise in metaanalyses, particularly those that follow from infrequent random sampling. Of course, Donald Campbell and Julian Stanley also had a list of threats to external validity, and these also have to do with generalization (1963). But their list was far from complete and was developed more with primary studies in mind than with research syntheses. The question this chapter asks is how one can proceed to justify claims about the generality of an association when the within-study selection of persons, settings, times, and measures is almost never random and when it is also not even reasonable to assume that the available sample of studies is itself unbiased. This chapter proposes a threats-to-validity approach rooted in a theory of construct validity as one way to throw provisional light on how to justify general inferences.