ArticlePDF Available

Recognizing faces across continents: The effect of within-race variations on the own-race bias in face recognition

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

People are better at recognizing faces of their own race than faces of other racial groups. This own-race bias (ORB) in face recognition manifests in some studies as a full crossover interaction between race of observer and race of face, but in others the interaction is accompanied by main effects or other complexities. We hypothesized that this may be due in part to unacknowledged within-race variation and the implicit assumption that the terms white and black describe perceptually homogeneous race categories. We therefore tested white and black South Africans on their recognition of black and white American faces and black and white South African faces. Our results showed the expected interaction, but only for South African faces. This finding supports explanations of the ORB that are premised on intergroup contact and perceptual experience and highlights the danger of assuming homogeneity of appearance within groups.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Previous studies have shown that people recognize
faces of their own race more accurately than they recog-
nize faces of other races, a finding that has become known
as the cross-race effect, or the own-race bias (Malpass &
Kravitz, 1969; see Meissner & Brigham, 2001, for a meta-
analytic review). A number of theoretical explanations for
this effect have been proposed (see Sporer, 2001, for a
review). One of the most widely accepted explanations
for this effect is that poorer recognition of other-race faces
may be rooted in the observer’s perceptual learning and
the amount of contact that he or she has had with people
of other races. An alternative explanation, proposed by
Levin (1996) and MacLin and Malpass (2001), suggests
that racial categorization occurs automatically and early
in the perceptual encounter with faces of another race,
taking attention away from individuating characteristics
of the face.
An own-race recognition bias typically manifests as a
disordinal (full crossover) interaction between race of ob-
server and race of face, so that observers from each group
show superior recognition performance on own-race over
other-race faces (although see Sporer, 2001). Most stud-
ies have reported a significant interaction between race of
observer and race of face that is accompanied by a signifi-
cant main effect either of race of participant or of race of
face (see, e.g., Chiroro & Valentine, 1995; Ng & Lindsay,
1994; Sporer, 1999). For example, Sporer (1999) found
that Turkish participants did not differ in their ability to
recognize Turkish and German faces, whereas German
participants recognized German faces better than they did
Turkish faces. Similarly, Walker and Hewstone (2006)
found an own-race bias among white U.K. residents in their
recognition of South Asian faces, but not for Asian people
living in the U.K. A reversal of the own-race bias effect
has also been reported in the literature, where participants
appeared to recognize other-race faces better than they did
own-race faces. For example, Wright, Boyd, and Tredoux
(2003) found that whereas white South African students
showed the expected own-race recognition advantage for
white faces, black South African students showed a sur-
prising recognition advantage for white faces.
Thus, although the cross-race effect has been con-
firmed in several individual studies, as well as in meta-
analyses (e.g., Antony, Copper, & Mullen, 1992; Both-
1089 Copyright 2008 Psychonomic Society, Inc.
Recognizing faces across continents:
The effect of within-race variations on
the own-race bias in face recognition
PATRICK M. CHIRORO
University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
COLIN G. TREDOUX
University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
STEPHANO RADAELLI
University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa
AND
CHRISTIAN A. MEISSNER
University of Texas, El Paso, Texas
People are better at recognizing faces of their own race than faces of other racial groups. This own-race bias
(ORB) in face recognition manifests in some studies as a full crossover interaction between race of observer and
race of face, but in others the interaction is accompanied by main effects or other complexities. We hypothesized
that this may be due in part to unacknowledged within-race variation and the implicit assumption that the terms
white and black describe perceptually homogeneous race categories. We therefore tested white and black South
Africans on their recognition of black and white American faces and black and white South African faces. Our
results showed the expected interaction, but only for South African faces. This finding supports explanations of
the ORB that are premised on intergroup contact and perceptual experience and highlights the danger of assum-
ing homogeneity of appearance within groups.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
2008, 15 (6), 1089-1092
doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1089
P. Chiroro, patrick.chiroro@gmail.com
1090 CHIRORO, TREDOUX, RADAELLI, AND MEISSNER
responding to poster invitations. Twenty-four were black South
Africans (M 21.05 years, SD 1.56) and 24 were white South
Africans (M 20.34 years, SD 1.67). An equal number of male
and female participants were included in each racial group. Each
participant received R20 (approximately $3) for his or her time.
Apparatus and Stimuli
Four sets of photographs were used: 24 black and 24 white U.S.
faces, and 24 black and 24 white South African faces. The South
African faces were collected by the second author, and the U.S. faces
by the fourth author. The four sets of faces were selected from larger
sets of stimuli on the basis of their (approximately equal) memora-
bility ratings, as provided by 16 independent raters of white South
African descent. (It should be noted that prior studies have demon-
strated strong correlations in interracial perceptions of memorabil-
ity; see, e.g., Meissner, Brigham, & Butz, 2005.) All the faces were
digitally removed from their backgrounds, using RealDraw Pro 3.1,
and were equated for light contrast, using GIMP 2.0. Two different
frontal views of each face in neutral pose were generated by apply-
ing a lighting and color transformation, one for use during study and
the other for use at test. SuperLab Pro v2.0 was used to control expo-
sure duration and interstimulus interval. The participants’ responses
were captured via a Cedrus RB560 response box. All the stimuli
were presented on a 15-in. color monitor connected to a Pentium 4
computer, with resolution set at 1,024 768 pixels. Color resolution
was set at 24 bits.
Design
The study utilized a 2 2 2 mixed design in which race of
participant (black vs. white) varied between participants and race of
face (black vs. white) and country of stimulus origin (South Africa
vs. United States) were manipulated within participants. Each face
served as a target and as a distractor in each of the conditions an
equal number of times.
Procedure
The participants were tested individually in a quiet room. During
study, each face was shown for 3 sec, with an interstimulus interval
of 1.5 sec. The participants were requested to remember the faces
presented to them. At test, the yes–no recognition procedure was
used. During study and at test, black and white faces were presented
randomly (i.e., mixed study and test lists were used). At test, presen-
tation of faces was response terminated; the participants responded
by pressing one of two keys on the Cedrus RB560 response box. The
keys were clearly marked “yes” (for faces seen previously) or “no”
(for new faces). Hits and false positives made by each participant on
each category of faces were recorded. The participants were thanked
and debriefed at the end of the experiment.
RESULTS
Hits and false positives were combined to form signal
detection measures of discrimination accuracy (d) and
response criterion (c), and a three-way split-plot factorial
ANOVA was performed on these measures. Of particular
interest in this study was the three-way interaction involv-
ing country of origin of faces, race of face, and race of
participant. This interaction was statistically significant
for d [F(1,46) 5.24, p .026, r
2
.10], but not for c
[F(1,46) 1.98, p .17, r
2
.04].
Figure 1 shows the mean d scores and suggests that the
two-way interaction of race of participant and race of face
was significant for the participants when they were tested
with South African faces, but not when they were tested
with U.S. faces. This was confirmed with follow-up tests
of each of the simple interaction effects [F(1,46) 21.37,
well, Brigham, & Malpass; 1989; Meissner & Brigham,
2001; Shapiro & Penrod, 1986), the results have varied
widely between studies. Some of this variation may be ac-
counted for by differences in the way the studies have been
conducted. For example, the meta-analysis by Meissner
and Brigham showed that the own-race bias is moderated
by type and nature of stimulus material used, encoding
time, and delay between study and test. These factors may
explain some of the differences between findings across
studies.
Ecological factors, such as variations in population
distribution and patterns of contact, could explain an ad-
ditional amount of the variance and may, in particular,
explain why interactions are often accompanied by main
effects. In habitats where minority groups are sparse, for
instance, it is feasible to expect that minority groups will
be provided far greater opportunity for learning cross-race
faces than will members of the majority, and that they will
show less recognition bias.
However, we suspect that an additional, largely unac-
knowledged factor—what might be called within-race
face variation—may explain a further amount of the ef-
fect variance.
In particular, the race categories white and black have
been assumed in the literature to be perceptually homo-
geneous, but this may be far from the truth. Faces that
are categorized in the U.S. as black may differ in many
ways from those similarly categorized as black in Africa
or South America, and this may be difficult to discern
without a “trained eye”—particularly difficult, we dare-
say, for cross-race recognition researchers, who tend to
be white Americans and Europeans. Even within each
continent, huge variations appear to exist in the facial
and bodily physiognomy of racial groups. For example,
the Maasai of Kenya look very different from the Zulu
people of South Africa, and both of those ethnic groups
share few similarities with typical black Americans. For
the Maasai or Zulu people, black U.S. faces may indeed be
unfamiliar and constitute an out-group stimulus set. From
a theoretical point of view, these within-race variations in
physiognomy are likely to be mapped onto differences in
intergroup contact, and this should show in face recogni-
tion performance.
Thus, a primary aim of the present experiment was to
test whether people of a common race will exhibit an own-
race bias that is independent of geographic region. To as-
sess this, white and black South Africans were presented
with black and white faces from both South Africa and
the United States. We anticipated that the black and white
South Africans would demonstrate an in-group face rec-
ognition advantage that is geographically specific. Thus,
the black South Africans were expected to show superior
recognition of black South African faces over all other
comparison groups.
METHOD
Participants
Forty-eight undergraduate students at a large, multiracial uni-
versity in South Africa volunteered to participate in the study by
IN-GROUP FACE RECOGNITION ADVANTAGE 1091
stimuli that has been used in several studies demonstrat-
ing an own-group recognition bias with U.S. participants
(e.g., Meissner et al., 2005). Given that this exact set of
study/test stimuli has not been used previously, however,
we concede that a replication of the present experiment
with U.S. participants would appear warranted.
Nevertheless, this demonstration of a regional basis for
the recognition advantage for own-race faces supports ex-
planations that are premised on intergroup contact and
perceptual experience (Brigham & Malpass, 1985; Meiss-
ner & Brigham, 2001). Populations develop perceptual
expertise for faces that they regularly interact with, and
this is probably moderated by the perceived utility of the
interaction (Chiroro & Valentine, 1995; Malpass, 1990).
South African blacks and whites have a heritage of en-
forced segregation (Worden, 2000), and the presence of
an other-race recognition deficit occurs most likely as a
result of their lack of meaningful contact. They also show
a recognition deficit for faces from other geographic re-
gions, regardless of race.
In the end, we wonder whether it is perhaps time that
the concepts of race and, specifically, own-race bias be
retired from this literature. Race cannot be defined with
any precision (Malpass, 1993), and reliance on it may lead
to false conclusions, as demonstrated by the results from
this study. It may also lead to linguistic difficulties, since
recognition advantages have now been established for age
(Wright & Stroud, 2002) and sex (Rehnman & Herlitz,
2006). One solution may be to take the lead from Sporer’s
(2001) face-processing model and refer to the phenom-
enon as the in-group face recognition advantage.
AUTHOR NOTE
The research reported in this article was funded in part by a grant
awarded to the second author by the NRF in South Africa and to the third
author by the NSF in the United States. This financial assistance is grate-
p .001, r
2
.31; and F(1,46) 1.19, p .27, r
2
.03,
respectively]. Consistent with our predictions, U.S. faces
were treated as an out-group, with performance signifi-
cantly more accurate for South African faces than for U.S.
faces: A linear contrast testing this effect was significant
for both black [F(1,23) 11.13, p .003, r
2
.04] and
white [F(1,23) 13.34, p .002, r
2
.04] participants.
DISCUSSION
Face recognition researchers have investigated the own-
race recognition bias for almost 40 years, but few have
attempted to provide a definition of race. This is not sur-
prising, since the concept of race is notoriously unclear,
with most biologists asserting that it has no defensible
definition (e.g., Tobias, 1972; see also Sternberg, Grig-
orenko, & Kidd, 2005, who call it a “folk taxonomy”).
Specifically, if black (or white) is used to denote a race
of people, and if there is a true own-race bias in face rec-
ognition, black people living on one continent could be
expected to show a recognition advantage for black faces
from another continent. The results from our study refute
this, showing instead that the recognition advantage for
own-group faces is ethno-geographically specific: White
South Africans showed a recognition advantage for white
South African faces but not for white U.S. faces, and black
South Africans showed a similar recognition advantage
for black South African faces but not for black U.S. faces.
In fact, our results show that white and black South Afri-
cans exhibit a recognition advantage for their own ethnic
group over all three of the comparison groups.
A possible counterexplanation we wish to dismiss is
that the U.S. faces we used may themselves be somewhat
unusual and that U.S. participants might not show the
own-group recognition bias with these faces. The U.S.
faces used in this study were taken from a larger set of
2
1.5
1
d
0.5
0
South African Stimulus Materials
White Participants Black Participants
White faces Black faces
2
1.5
1
d
0.5
0
U.S. Stimulus Materials
White Participants Black Participants
White faces Black faces
Figure 1. Discrimination (d ) of South African and U.S. faces by South African observers. The t bars above each
column represent standard errors.
1092 CHIRORO, TREDOUX, RADAELLI, AND MEISSNER
own- and other-race faces: A dual-process approach. Applied Cogni-
tive Psychology, 19, 545-567.
Ng, W. J., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1994). Cross-racial facial recognition:
Failure of the contact hypothesis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-
ogy, 25, 217-232.
Rehnman, J., & Herlitz, A. (2006). Higher face recognition in
girls: Magnified by own-sex and own-ethnicity bias. Memory, 14,
289-296.
Shapiro, P. N., & Penrod, S. (1986). A meta-analysis of facial identifi-
cation studies. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 139-156.
Sporer, S. L. (1999, July). The own-race bias in Germany: Testing
the contact hypothesis with Turks and Germans. Paper presented at
the 4th European Conference on Psychology and Law of the U.S.
Psychology– Law Association and the European Psychology–Law
Association, Dublin.
Sporer, S. L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An inte-
gration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 7, 36-97.
Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Kidd, K. K. (2005). Intelli-
gence, race and genetics. American Psychologist, 60, 46-59.
Tobias, P. V. (1972). The meaning of race (2nd ed.). Johannesburg:
South African Institute of Race Relations.
Walker, P. M., & Hewstone, M. A. (2006). Perceptual discrimination
investigation of the own-race effect and intergroup experience. Ap-
plied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 461-475.
Worden, N. (2000). The making of modern South Africa. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Wright, D. B., Boyd, C. E., & Tredoux, C. G. (2003). Inter-racial
contact and the own race bias for face recognition in South Africa and
England. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 365-373.
Wright, D. B., & Stroud, J. N. (2002). Age differences in lineup
identification accuracy: People are better with their own age. Law &
Human Behavior, 26, 641-654.
(Manuscript received November 23, 2007;
revision accepted for publication June 3, 2008.)
fully acknowledged. Correspondence concerning this article should be
addressed to P. M. Chiroro, Department of Psychology, University of Pre-
toria, Pretoria 0002, South Africa (e-mail: patrick.chiroro@gmail.com).
REFERENCES
Antony, T., Copper, C., & Mullen, B. (1992). Cross-racial facial
identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality & Social
Psychology Bulletin, 18, 296-301.
Bothwell, R. K., Brigham, J. C., & Malpass, R. S. (1989). Cross-
racial identification. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 15,
19-25.
Brigham, J. C., & Malpass, R. S. (1985). The role of experience and
contact in the recognition of faces of own- and other-race persons.
Journal of Social Issues, 41, 139-155.
Chiroro, P., & Valentine, T. (1995). An investigation of the contact
hypothesis of the own-race bias in face recognition. Quarterly Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 879-894.
Levin, D. T. (1996). Classifying faces by race: The structure of face
categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
& Cognition, 22, 1364-1382.
MacLin, O. H., & Malpass, R. S. (2001). Racial categorization of faces:
The ambiguous race face effect. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law,
7, 98-118.
Malpass, R. S. (1990). An excursion into utilitarian analyses, with side
trips. Behavioral Science Research, 24, 1-15.
Malpass, R. S. (1993). “They all look alike to me.” In M. Merrens &
G. Brannigan (Eds.), The undaunted psychologist (pp. 74-88). New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Malpass, R. S., & Kravitz, J. (1969). Recognition for faces of own-
and other-race faces. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,
13, 330-334.
Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigat-
ing the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review.
Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 7, 3-35.
Meissner, C. A., Brigham, J. C., & Butz, D. A. (2005). Memory for
... Whether casually conversing with a new acquaintance, waving to the neighbourhood mail carrier, or witnessing a crime unfold, we encounter many individuals throughout the day who we may hope to later remember. When attempting to recognise faces, people often demonstrate an in-group face recognition advantage (Chiroro et al., 2008) such that when presented with faces of different races/ethnicities, learners generally better recognise faces of their own race compared with faces of other races or ethnicities. This effect has been coined the own-race bias (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969;Meissner & Brigham, 2001). ...
... To account for the ownrace bias, researchers have proposed perceptual expertise (Hills & Lewis, 2006) and social cognitive (Pauker et al., 2009;Sporer, 2001; see for hybrid models and a review) accounts. According to perceptual expertise accounts of the own-race bias, more frequent encounters with members of one's own race compared to members of other races facilitates the development of perceptual expertise when encoding and recognising own-race faces (Chiroro et al., 2008;Lucas et al., 2011;Rhodes et al., 1989Rhodes et al., , 2014. Thus, with increased exposure to same-race faces, people may become better able to identify these faces. ...
... Prior face recognition research has revealed that perceptual features indicating group membership like gender (Wright & Sladden, 2003), age (Rhodes & Anastasi, 2012), and species (Diamond & Carey, 1986) can influence one's ability to discriminate between studied and novel faces. A similar in-group face recognition advantage (Chiroro et al., 2008) has also been demonstrated for race/ethnicity whereby people better recognise same-race faces compared to other-race faces (Malpass & Kravitz, 1969;Meissner & Brigham, 2001; the own-race bias). ...
Article
Full-text available
People tend to better remember same-race faces relative to other-race faces (an “own-race” bias). We examined whether the own-race bias extends to associative memory, particularly in the identification and recall of information paired with faces. In Experiment 1, we presented white participants with own- and other-race faces which either appeared alone or accompanied by a label indicating whether the face was a “criminal” or a “victim”. Results revealed an own-race facial recognition advantage regardless of the presence of associative information. In Experiment 2, we again paired same- and other-race faces with either “criminal” or “victim” labels, but rather than a recognition test, participants were asked to identify whether each face had been presented as a criminal or a victim. White criminals were better categorised than Black criminals, but race did not influence the categorisation of victims. In Experiment 3, white participants were presented with same- and other-race faces and asked to remember where the person was from, their occupation, and a crime they committed. Results revealed a recall advantage for the associative information paired with same-race faces. Collectively, these findings suggest that the own-race bias extends to the categorisation and recall of information in associative memory.
... That is, features learned by the model do not linearly correspond to semantic features. In addition, evolutionary and socialcultural factors likely bias recognition of certain discrete features, such as emotional expression and skin or hair color (Chiroro et al., 2008)-factors that are likely unaccounted for in the GAN space. ...
Article
Visual recognition requires inferring the similarity between a perceived object and a mental target. However, a measure of similarity is difficult to determine when it comes to complex stimuli such as faces. Indeed, people may notice someone "looks like" a familiar face, but find it hard to describe on the basis of what features such a comparison is based. Previous work shows that the number of similar visual elements between a face pictogram and a memorized target correlates with the P300 amplitude in the visual evoked potential. Here, we redefine similarity as the distance inferred from a latent space learned using a state-of-the-art generative adversarial neural network (GAN). A rapid serial visual presentation experiment was conducted with oddball images generated at varying distances from the target to determine how P300 amplitude related to GAN-derived distances. The results showed that distance-to-target was monotonically related to the P300, showing perceptual identification was associated with smooth, drifting image similarity. Furthermore, regression modeling indicated that while the P3a and P3b sub-components had distinct responses in location, time, and amplitude, they were similarly related to target distance. The work demonstrates that the P300 indexes the distance between perceived and target image in smooth, natural, and complex visual stimuli and shows that GANs present a novel modeling methodology for studying the relationships between stimuli, perception, and recognition.
... Evidence of a different kind points to the contextual importance of the perceptual environment. For example, Chiroro et al. (2008) showed that White South African participants recognized White South Africans better than they did White Americans, suggesting contextually sensitive skills and knowledge. However, other studies suggest that the OGRB may be more dependent on social categorization processes than perceptual expertise. ...
Article
Full-text available
The own‐group recognition bias (OGRB) has been consistently linked to social contact in theoretical accounts. Indeed, social contact is assumed by most authors to underlie the perceptual expertise of out‐groups' faces. However, little is known empirically about how it might impact face‐processing strategies. We tested the proposition that social interaction would improve the face recognition performance of another group by modulating visual strategies for different face areas. In Experiment 1, we studied visual processes using an eye tracker during a person's first live encounter with a particular member of their own group (European) or an outgroup (African) to explore how increasing familiarity during a first interaction influences face‐processing strategies. In Experiments 2 and 3, we explored the effect of simulated intergroup contact on face recognition accuracy, while simultaneously studying the impact of contact on visual attention strategies that occur during recognition (Experiment 2) and encoding (Experiment 3). The results showed a strong OGRB and a difference in visual processes based on the ethnic group of the targets. Although a single interaction is not sufficient to reduce the OGRB, familiarization during a live interaction (Experiment 1) and virtual social contact (Experiment 2) had an impact on the visual strategies employed.
... In terms of potential limitations, we acknowledge that our Caucasian participants were from various countries. Because there are slight variations in the appearance of Caucasians from different regions (e.g., Northern Europeans/Australians/Germans or Southern Europeans/ Americans, Bowles et al., 2009;Chiroro et al., 2008;McKone et al., 2011McKone et al., , 2012, the face stimuli used in the CFMT or memory task in this study might not best match the 'own-race' group of all of our Caucasian participants. Nonetheless, it is likely that Caucasian participants identified those faces as from their own group, as the 'otherethnicity' effect is not always consistently observed (McKone et al., 2012;Sporer, 2001;Sporer et al., 2007), and a reliable ORE could be found in Caucasian participants who had relatively little experience with Asians, with the same version of the CFMT that might not most optimally matched their own ethnicity (e.g., Germans, Zhao et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has established a possible link between recognition performance, individuation experience, and implicit racial bias of other-race faces. However, it remains unclear how implicit racial bias might influence other-race face processing in observers with relatively extensive experience with the other race. Here we examined how recognition of other-race faces might be modulated by observers’ implicit racial bias, in addition to the effects of experience and face recognition ability. Caucasian participants in a culturally diverse city completed a memory task for Asian and Caucasian faces, an implicit association test, a questionnaire assessing experience with Asians and Caucasians, and a face recognition ability test. As expected, recognition performance for Asian faces was positively predicted by increased face recognition ability, and experience with Asians. More importantly, it was also negatively predicted by increased positive bias towards Asians, which was modulated by an interaction between face recognition ability and implicit bias, with the effect of implicit bias observed predominantly in observers with high face recognition ability. Moreover, the positions of the first two fixations when participants learned the other-race faces were affected by different factors, with the first fixation modulated by the effect of experience and the second fixation modulated by the interaction between implicit bias and face recognition ability. Taken together, these findings suggest the complexity in understanding the perceptual and socio-cognitive influences on the other-race effect, and that observers with high face recognition ability may more likely evaluate racial features involuntarily when recognizing other-race faces.
Article
Previous cross‐cultural eye‐tracking studies examining face recognition discovered differences in the eye movement strategies that observers employ when perceiving faces. However, it is unclear (1) the degree to which this effect is fundamentally related to culture and (2) to what extent facial physiognomy can account for the differences in looking strategies when scanning own‐ and other‐race faces. In the current study, Malay, Chinese and Indian young adults who live in the same multiracial country performed a modified yes/no recognition task. Participants' recognition accuracy and eye movements were recorded while viewing muted face videos of own‐ and other‐race individuals. Behavioural results revealed a clear own‐race advantage in recognition memory, and eye‐tracking results showed that the three ethnic race groups adopted dissimilar fixation patterns when perceiving faces. Chinese participants preferentially attended more to the eyes than Indian participants did, while Indian participants made more and longer fixations on the nose than Malay participants did. In addition, we detected statistically significant, though subtle, differences in fixation patterns between the faces of the three races. These findings suggest that the racial differences in face‐scanning patterns may be attributed both to culture and to variations in facial physiognomy between races.
Article
The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) is used globally in emotion research. However, normative studies in diverse contexts do not consider the influence of education and socioeconomic status (SES) on picture ratings. We created the South African Affective Picture System (SA-APS) for use in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) by replacing some original IAPS images with pictures featuring more diverse groups of people and culturally appropriate stimuli. Healthy South African adults from higher and lower education/SES backgrounds (n = 80; n = 70 respectively) provided valence and arousal ratings for 340 images from the original IAPS and 340 images from the new SA-APS. Overall, their ratings of SA-APS images were better aligned with the US normative standards than their ratings of IAPS images, particularly with regard to valence. Those with higher SES/education rated IAPS images differently from those with lower SES/education (e.g., valence ratings of the latter were similar to US normative standards, whereas those of the former were more negative). Regression modelling indicated that sex and SES significantly predicted the current sample’s IAPS and SA-APS ratings (e.g., women and higher-SES participants rated high-arousal images as being significantly more arousing than men and lower-SES participants); hence, we created regression-based norms for both picture sets. These norms are especially useful in emotion research, because few studies emerge from LMICs, and few instruments account for substantial sociodemographic diversity. Extending the reach of tools such as the IAPS to LMICs can help ensure a more globally representative body of research in this field.
Chapter
Before one can understand a child’s capabilities as an eyewitness, one must first have insight into the memory mechanisms that account for these capabilities. There are three stages to memory (Pozzulo, J. D., The young eyewitness: How well do children and adolescents describe and identify perpetrators? American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14956-000, 2017). First is the process of encoding, which is when information is initially perceived and converted into memory. A person may be aware they are encoding new information, or they may not be (e.g., Yang et al., 2012). Second, memory must be stored. If the memory is rehearsed enough, it may be stored as a long-term memory, rather than only lasting as a short-term memory (Cowan, N., What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323–338, 2008). The final stage is retrieval, when memories are located and remembered by the individual. Generally, the process of remembering is viewed as constructive, rather than reproductive, meaning our minds do not accurately record everything like video recorders, and we cannot just play memories back in our minds exactly as they happened (Patihis, L., et al., False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(52), 20947–20952. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314373110, 2013). Rather, when retrieval occurs, our memories must be reconstructed, and this process is vulnerable to a number of influences, including leading questions and post-event information (e.g., Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M., Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. American Psychological Association, 1995; Schmidt, S., Autobiographical memories for the September 11th attacks: Reconstructive errors and emotional impairment of memory. Memory & Cognition, 32(3), 443–454. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195837, 2004).KeywordsMemoryEncodingStorageRetrievalFalse memoriesGistVerbatimFuzzy-trace theoryHenry OtgaarLanguageKnowledgeTheory of mind
Article
Individuals who are Hispanic or Latino make up a substantial portion of the U.S. and world population yet are vastly underrepresented as both participants and stimuli in the face-perception literature. Perceiving and recognizing faces are critical components of everyday social interactions, but cross-category effects (difficulty recognizing faces from other races or ethnicities) can hinder social interactions. Cross-category effects are the most commonly studied face-perception topic with these ethnic groups, but this empirical knowledge should be expanded via culturally relevant considerations. In this article, I describe (a) errors individuals display when categorizing target faces, (b) how social status influences identity and cross-category effects, (c) the potential impact of flexible and heterogeneous social identities on face processing, (d) a critical need for more developmental research, and (e) methodological expansions and generalizability concerns. Thus, I propose important directions for future studies to address these issues and advance knowledge in the field.
Article
Full-text available
The current research conducted a three-level meta-analysis with a total of 159 journal articles on the other-race bias in facial identification, which had been published between 1969 and 2021. The effect size analysis yielded moderate pooled effect sizes of the other-race bias on face identification—people showed higher hit rates and discriminability, lower false alarm rates, and more stringent criteria for own-race faces than for other-race faces. Results from the sensitivity analysis and publication bias analysis also supported the robustness of the other-race bias. In moderation analyses, participant race (White vs. non-White) and retention interval between the study and test phases produced stable moderating effects on estimates of the other-race bias. Despite an increase in racial diversity for decades in our society, the current meta-analysis still demonstrated robust effects of the other-race bias in facial identification, replicating findings from the previous meta-analyses.
Article
Full-text available
Global escalation of crime has necessitated the use of digital imagery to aid the identification of perpetrators. Forensic facial comparison (FFC) is increasingly employed, often relying on poor quality images. In the absence of standardized criteria, especially in terms of video recordings, verification of the methodology is needed. This paper addresses aspects of FFC, discussing relevant terminology, investigating the validity and reliability of the FISWG morphological feature list using a new South African database, and advising on standards for CCTV equipment. Suboptimal conditions, including poor resolution, unfavorable angle of incidence, color, and lighting, affected the accuracy of FFC. Morphological analysis of photographs, standard CCTV, and eye-level CCTV showed improved performance in a strict iteration analysis, but not when using analogue CCTV images. Therefore, both strict and lenient iterations should be conducted, but FFC must be abandoned when a strict iteration performs worse than a lenient one. This threshold ought to be applied to the specific CCTV equipment to determine its utility. Chance-corrected accuracy was the most representative measure of accuracy, as opposed to the commonly used hit rate. While the use of automated systems is increasing, trained human observer-based morphological analysis, using the FISWG feature list and an Analysis, Comparison, Evaluation, and Verification (ACE-V) approach, should be the primary method of facial comparison.
Article
Full-text available
The current article reviews the own-race bias (ORB) phenomenon in memory for human faces, the finding that own-race faces are better remembered when compared with memory for faces of another, less familiar race. Data were analyzed from 39 research articles, involving 91 independent samples and nearly 5,000 participants. Measures of hit and false alarm rates, and aggregate measures of discrimination accuracy and response criterion were examined, including an analysis of 8 study moderators. Several theoretical relationships were also assessed (i.e., the influence of racial attitudes and interracial contact). Overall, results indicated a "mirror effect" pattern in which own-race faces yielded a higher proportion of hits and a lower proportion of false alarms compared with other-race faces. Consistent with this effect, a significant ORB was also found in aggregate measures of discrimination accuracy and response criterion. The influence of perceptual learning and differentiation processes in the ORB are discussed, in addition to the practical implications of this phenomenon.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has reported that young adults are better at eyewitness face recognition than are older adults. However, these studies have used young adults as culprits and fillers. We explore how the relative ages of the witness and the culprit influence eyewitness accuracy in 2 experiments. In the first experiment, young (18–25 years old) and older (35–55 years old) adults each saw 4 crime videos. In 2 the culprit was a young adult and in 2 the culprit was an older adult. Participants were more accurate at identifying the culprit when viewing culprit present lineups comprising people of their own age: an “own age bias” analogous to the own race bias. In the 2nd experiment, using a similar procedure, young (18–33 years old) and older (40–55 years old) adults viewed both culprit present and culprit absent lineups. The results of the first experiment were replicated for the culprit present lineups. However, no own age bias was found for the culprit absent lineups. Implications for police procedures dealing with cross-generation identifications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Across a wide range of disciplines and phenomena, a particular theoret ical approach is at the center of attempts to understand human behavior. This theoretical approach, roughly characterized as Subjective Expected Utility (SEU) theory, figures prominently in formal and mathematical theo ries of behavior, as well as in intuitive and implicit analyses of behavior deriving from folk theory. A selection of SEU approaches is described. The SEU approach is then used to develop a theoretical analysis of cross-race social experience and its relation to the cross-racial face recognition phe nomenon.
Article
Full-text available
This article reviews the research on differential recognition for own-versus other-race faces. A meta-analysis of 14 samples revealed that the magnitude of the own-race bias is similar for both Black and White subjects, accounting for about 10o of the variance in recognition accuracy. There is a considerable consistency across studies, indicating that memory for own-race faces is superior to memory for other-race faces. Both Black and White subjects exhibited own-race bias in 79%0 of the samples reviewed.
Article
Full-text available
Accusations of discriminatory treatment of minority persons in the criminal justice system create a need for policy and procedure development to create real and perceived equal treatment. A facial recognition deficit among law enforcement officers and witnesses for persons of another "race" contributes to unequal treatment of minority group members. This article demonstrates the other-race effect in an unusual context, reveals theoretical weaknesses, reveals the role of categorical processes in the phenomenon, and discusses policy implications. Experiment 1, based on feature and trait ratings, demonstrates that identical and racially ambiguous faces with different racial markers (hair) are perceived according to the marker. Experiment 2 demonstrates an other-race recognition effect using these faces. A feature acting as a racial marker can cause a face to be perceived and remembered differently. Other-race faces are perceived categorically, which drives the recognition process. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The current article reviews the own-race bias (ORB) phenomenon in memory for human faces, the finding that own-race faces are better remembered when compared with memory for faces of another, less familiar race. Data were analyzed from 39 research articles, involving 91 independent samples and nearly 5,000 participants. Measures of hit and false alarm rates, and aggregate measures of discrimination accuracy and response criterion were examined, including an analysis of 8 study moderators. Several theoretical relationships were also assessed (i.e., the influence of racial attitudes and interracial contact). Overall, results indicated a "mirror effect" pattern in which own-race faces yielded a higher proportion of hits and a lower proportion of false alarms compared with other-race faces. Consistent with this effect, a significant ORB was also found in aggregate measures of discrimination accuracy and response criterion. The influence of perceptual learning and differentiation processes in the ORB are discussed, in addition to the practical implications of this phenomenon. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Studies of the recognition of faces of an ethnic group different from one's own reveal a robust recognition deficit for faces of the respective out-group (cross-race effect or own-race bias) and a tendency to respond less cautiously with respect to out-group faces. Cross-national comparisons reveal that the cross-race effect appears to be larger among low-contact groups. Although exemplar-based models postulating a multidimensional face space are currently in vogue, some of the more traditional accounts (e.g., the contact hypothesis) should not be dismissed prematurely. An extended exemplar-based model that relates the out-group recognition deficit to the out-group homogeneity effect in social perception and judgment appears promising. An in-group/out-group model (IOM) of face processing is proposed that includes aspects of previous theories and derives new predictions (e.g., a cross-sex or cross-age effect). The IOM attempts to account for the out-group recognition deficit and the more lax response criterion with respect to out-group faces. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A meta-analytic integration of the cross-racialfacial identification effect is reported. The results indicate a significant, weak-to-moderate tendency for individuals to be more accurate in the recognition off aces of in-group members than those of out-group members. There is a trend for this effect to be stronger among White subjects than Black subjects. In addition, predictors exhibited different patterns for Black subjects and White subjects. Specifically, greater depth of processing engaged by experimental instructions in these studies strengthened the effect for White subjects and weakened it for Black subjects. Similarly, the cross-racial facial identification effect increased as a function of the duration of exposure to the target faces for White subjects but decreased as a function of duration of exposure for Black subjects. These results are explained in terms of recent theoretical developments concerning intergroup phenomena, particularly in terms of mechanisms of cognitive representations of in-groups and out-groups.
Article
Two studies were conducted to increase our knowledge of cross-race recognition of White and Oriental faces and to test the hypothesis that the "cross-race effect" (inferior facial recognition of other races) is due to lack of contact with the other race. In Experiment 1, White (n = 60) and Oriental (n = 60) university students in Canada attempted to recognize White and Oriental faces in a standard facial recognition paradigin. Although the cross-race effect was replicated for false alarms and d', neither perceived similarity nor self-rated contact predicted recognition accuracy. In Experiment 2, White (n = 92) and Oriental (n = 115) students from Singapore and Canada were tested. Contact with Whites and Orientals differed significantly for students in Singapore versus Canada but was not related to facial recognition even though the cross-race effect was replicated. On average, 6 predicted effects of the cross-race effect from the two experiments accounted for 10.83% of the variance, whereas the 18 predicted effects based on the contact hypothesis on average accounted for only 0.89% of the variance in facial recognition. The "contact hypothesis" is not a viable explanation of the results in studies of cross-race facial recognition. The cross-race effect remains unexplained.
Article
Although previous studies have demonstrated that faces of one's own race are recognized more accurately than are faces of other races, the theoretical basis of this effect is not clearly understood at present. The experiment reported in this paper tested the contact hypothesis of the own-race bias in face recognition using a cross-cultural design. Four groups of subjects were tested for their recognition of distinctive and typical own-race and other-race faces: (1) black Africans who had a high degree of contact with white faces, (2) black Africans who had little or no contact with white faces, (3) white Africans who had a high degree of contact with black faces, and (4) white Britons who had little contact with black faces. The results showed that although on the whole subjects recognized own-race faces more accurately and more confidently than they recognized other-race faces, the own-race bias in face recognition was significantly smaller among the high-contact subjects than it was among the low-contact subjects. Also, although high-contact black and white subjects showed significant main effects of distinctiveness in their recognition of faces of both races, low-contact black and white subjects showed significant main effects of distinctiveness only in their recognition of own-race faces. It is argued that these results support the contact hypothesis of the own-race bias in face recognition and Valentine's multidimensional space (MDS) framework of face encoding.This study was funded by a grant awarded to the first author by the Ford Foundation through the University of Zimbabwe. Grant No. 880/051.