ArticlePDF Available

Cognitive effort during note taking

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Note taking is a complex activity that requires comprehension and selection of information and written production processes. Here we review the functions, abbreviation procedures, strategies, and working memory constraints of note taking with the aim of improving theoretical and practical understanding of the activity. The time urgency of selecting key points and recording them while comprehending new information at the same time places significant demands on the central executive and other components of working memory. Dual-and triple-task procedures allow the measurement of the momentary cognitive effort or executive attention allocated to note taking. Comparative data show that note taking demands more effort than reading or learning. However, it requires less effort than the creative written composition of an original text.
Content may be subject to copyright.
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY
Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Published online 17 December 2004 in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1086
Cognitive Effort during Note Taking
ANNIE PIOLAT
1
*, THIERRY OLIVE
2
and RONALD T. KELLOGG
3
1
University of Provence, France
2
CNRS and University of Poitiers, France
3
Saint Louis University, USA
SUMMARY
Note taking is a complex activity that requires comprehension and selection of information and
written production processes. Here we review the functions, abbreviation procedures, strategies, and
working memory constraints of note taking with the aim of improving theoretical and practical
understanding of the activity. The time urgency of selecting key points and recording them while
comprehending new information at the same time places significant demands on the central
executive and other components of working memory. Dual- and triple-task procedures allow the
measurement of the momentary cognitive effort or executive attention allocated to note taking.
Comparative data show that note taking demands more effort than reading or learning. However, it
requires less effort than the creative written composition of an original text. Copyright #2004 John
Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Note taking occurs in frequent and various everyday life situations. To make purchases, to
plan future events and activities, to study for examinations, to prepare a technical talk, to
design a model in an industry, and to record the minutes of work meetings are a few
examples. Furthermore, the reasons why individuals take notes are highly variable.
Despite the diversity of contexts and intentions, all note taking entails recording
information collected from one or several sources. Such a record constitutes a stable
external memory that is intended to help to plan a future activity, to learn, to think, or to
create. It is important to understand this common activity for both theoretical and practical
reasons. Although a large body of studies had investigated the different techniques that
note takers use and their effectiveness, surprisingly few have focused on the cognitive
processes underlying note taking.
For cognitive psychologists, it is important to study the mental operations that underlie
note taking in addition to studying the product itself, as linguists do. A cognitive analysis
is even more critical to understanding when it is recognized that note taking cannot be
equated to simply copying what is heard, observed or thought. On the contrary, in a large
majority of cases, note taking implies comprehension (van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983) and
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
*Correspondence to: Dr A. Piolat, Centre for Research on Psychology of Cognition, Language and Emotion,
Universite
´de Provence, 29 avenue Robert Schuman, 13621 Aix-en-Provence Cedex 1, France.
E-mail: annie.piolat@up.univ-aix.fr
Contract/grant sponsor: French ACI programme ‘Ecole et Sciences Cognitives’.
written production (Alamargot & Chanquoy, 2001) that is similar to original composition.
Note takers, as readers, must comprehend information and, as learners, try to store
information in long-term memory by writing it down. As writers, note takers must select
the information to record and format it in ways that differ from the source material. They
employ abbreviating operations, syntactical short-cuts, paraphrasing statements, and often
a physical formatting of the notes that differs from the linear text of written source material
(Piolat, 2001). Accordingly, the goal of a cognitive analysis is to specify the processes,
knowledge, and working memory resources that note takers activate when they produce
this unique kind of written product.
The present review will be organized as follows. We begin by characterizing the written
products (the notes) and the reasons for their production. We then focus on the central
attribute of note taking, namely, the speed with which both comprehension and production
processes must be carried out. This brings forward the important role of working memory
in note taking (Baddeley, 2000). Next, we describe the dual- and triple-task methods that
are fruitful for measuring the cognitive effort required by note taking (Olive, Kellogg, &
Piolat, 2002). We then report the findings of a series of studies that compare the cognitive
effort involved in note taking versus other language activities (reading comprehension,
verbal learning, and writing original texts). Finally, we examine how different contexts of
note taking affect the cognitive effort involved, including variations in the kind of source
material, the technique of note taking, the source medium, and the amount of information
contained in the source.
THE WHAT AND WHY OF NOTE TAKING
Functions and factors regulating note taking
Notes can be defined as short condensations of a source material that are generated by
writing them down while simultaneously listening, studying, or observing. Their function
is to gather information distributed in a lecture, a book or in any other situation that needs
to be remembered. In other words, notes are external memories whose content is more or
less explicit (Hartley, 1976; Kiewra, 1989; Kiewra & Frank, 1988; Kiewra, DuBois,
Christensen, Kim, & Lindberg, 1989).
The function of notes is not only to provide a record of information, however. Indeed,
even if note taking is frequently used in situations where transmission of information is
crucial, as in academic contexts (Armbruster, 2000; Canelos, Dwyer, Nichols, & Randall,
1984; Kiewra, 1987, 1988a, 1991; Piolat & Boch, 2004; Piolat, Roussey, & Barbier, 2003),
it is also frequent in everyday life and in many professional contexts (Hartley, 2002). In
these situations, manipulating and anticipating relevant information are crucial, as in
judging (Houdek Middendorf & Hoff Macan, 2002), problem solving (Cary & Carlson,
1999), and decision making (Castello & Monereo, 1999), including when the work is
collective (e.g. industrial design, preparation of an exhibition, etc.). In such cases, note
taking could facilitate an ongoing work.
In academic contexts (secondary school and university), after several years of practice,
students develop specific conceptions of the functions of note taking. These representa-
tions relate to the purposes of the notes (to record information, to concentrate, etc.), their
content and structure (abbreviating procedures, structure of information, etc.) as well as to
contextual factors (style of the teacher and how important information is signalled, kind of
292 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
course, etc.) that mediate note taking (Badger, White, Sutherland, & Haggis, 2001; Dunkel
& Davy, 1989; Van Meter, Yokoi, & Pressley, 1994). The students’ conceptions differ
widely, however, as a function of the kinds of academic courses for which they take notes
(Badger et al., 2001; Hadwin, Kirby, & Woodhouse, 1999).
For two decades, a few experimental studies have analysed different contextual factors
that affect note taking (for a review, see Piolat et al., 2003). When taking notes from lectures,
students are very attentive to a variety of signals given by the lecturer to control
comprehension (fluency, changes in prosody, notes on the blackboard, explicit instructions
for taking notes, etc.; see Isaacs, 1994; Titsworth, 2001). When taking notes from written
documents, typographic and linguistics marks present in the text (title and headings,
summarized statements, connectives, etc.) are used for selecting and structuring the
information (Rickards, Fajen, Sullivan, & Gillespie, 1997; Sanchez, Lorch, & Lorch, 2001).
Notes: Summarized products with different formats
Note taking is often realized under severe time pressure. To take notes quickly, it is
necessary to shorten and reduce information. Such constraints led to the invention of
stenography whose graphic traces and transcribed units are simplified compared with
alphabetical writings. But this technique is so scarcely mastered that note takers rarely use
stenography. They generally fashion their own method of note taking during their studies
or in their profession. This leads to much diversification in note-taking practices (Bretzing,
Kulhavy, & Caterino, 1987; Hartley & Davies, 1978).
Analyses of the structures of notes show that the techniques used when taking notes
affect three levels of language. First, abbreviating procedures may apply on lexical units
(namely spelling; see Figure 1). For instance, end truncation (writing down ‘poss.’ for
possibility’), conservation of the frame of consonants, and suffix contraction (writing
down ‘recog
ed
’ for ‘recognized’) are usual techniques of note taking (cf. Branca-Rosoff,
1998; Kiewra & Benton, 1988; Kiewra, Benton, & Levis, 1987; Lindberg-Risch &
Kiewra, 1990). It is also important to notice that the same note taker can use a variety
of techniques and that these techniques may be quite different from one individual to
another. Moreover, the variation with which a note taker uses these techniques is
astonishing. For example, the same word can be shortened in different ways within the
same note-taking task or from one note-taking task to another.
Despite this variety of techniques, some are automatized sufficiently to be transferred
from one language to another, as indicated by research on note taking in first and second
languages (Chaudron, Loschsky, & Cook, 1994; Clerehan, 1995). For instance, as Figure 1
illustrates, when the lexical structure of two languages is comparable, suffix contraction
can be used in the same way. In the two excerpts in Figure 1, the same student contracted a
suffix both in French (‘ind
elle
’ for ‘individuelle‘individual’ in English) and in English
(‘recog
ed
’ for ‘recognized’). By contrast, because Japanese and French are two languages
that are very differently structured, Japanese students have to discover and to learn new
techniques when they take notes in French (Barbier, Faraco, Piolat, Roussey, & Kida,
2003).
Second, syntax can be transformed by shortening statements. For this purpose, note
takers may adopt a telegraphic style to record the information. They may also use
substitutive techniques, with mathematical ( þ,¼), iconic ( ), Greek-alphabetic (ß)
symbols for example. Such symbols (dash, arrow, star) are also ways for increasing the
speed of note taking, as for example when they are used in lists (see Figure 2). They further
Note taking 293
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
avoid formatting the syntax of the source material that is heard or read (Branca-Rosoff,
1998; Barbier et al., 2003).
Third, the physical formatting of the notes may be quite different from the usual linear
formatting used when producing a text (for a synthesis, see Piolat, 2001; Piolat et al.,
2003). In general, the format of the notes (tabulation, inserts, etc.) exploits all the physical
space of a sheet in a non-linear way (see Figures 2 and 3). The formatting of the notes
written down on the paper is thus often near that of preparatory rough drafts of an essay
(Kellogg, 1988; Piolat, 1998, 1999; Piolat & Roussey, 1996). They can, however, look
very similar to a polished draft or a linear text (Slotte & Lonka, 2001).
Note-taking strategies use the physical space of the sheet to provide organizational cues
of the information that is recorded (for a synthesis, see Kiewra et al., 1989; Piolat, 2001).
When they take notes during their courses, a majority of students focus on the discourse of
the teacher that they will have to restore during their examination. So, they frequently
resort to a linear method of note taking that gives to their notes a traditional textual
appearance (Piolat & Boch, 2004; Van Meter et al., 1994). However, in some particular
educational settings (observation method for example) or in professional contexts (notes
taken during a meeting) note takers can voluntary pre-select information they wish to
integrate in their notes by following particular and well-defined note structures. For
example, as shown in Figure 3, the note taker has used a variant of the seven-question
method of note taking (Who? What? Where? When? How? Why? How many?).
Note taking and learning outcomes
Research on note taking has generally attempted to delineate the strategies and techniques
that lead to notes that are relevant and effective for knowledge acquisition (Baker &
Figure 1. An example of abbreviating procedures used by the same undergraduate student in first
language (French) and in second language (English)
294 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Lombardi, 1985; Barnett, Di Vesta, & Rogozinski, 1981; Kiewra et al., 1991; Kiewra,
DuBois, Christian, & McShane, 1988; Morgan, Lilley, & Boreham, 1988; Norton, 1981;
Norton & Hartley, 1986; Nye, Crooks, Powley, & Tripp, 1984; Peck & Hannafin, 1983;
Russell, Caris, Harris, & Hendricson, 1983; Slotte & Lonka, 1999; Spires, 1993).
However, notetakers must endeavour to learn how to note (Stahl, King, & Henk, 1991),
more especially because they often take notes with difficulty (Suristky, 1992).
The majority of the studies that investigate the effects of note-taking strategies on
learning have focused on the quality of the selection and the organization of the
information that is recorded (Boyle & Weishaar, 2001; Brown & Day, 1983; Dyer, Riley,
& Yekovitch, 1979; Einstein, Morris, & Smith, 1985; Horton, Lovitt, & Christensen, 1991;
Howe, 1974; Kiewra et al., 1987; King, 1992; Ladas, 1980; Nist & Hogrebe, 1987; Oakhill
& Davies, 1991; Piolat, in press; Smith & Tompkins, 1988; Thomas, 1978; Williams &
Eggert, 2002). These studies suggest that nearly all non-linear note-taking strategies (e.g.
with an outline or a matrix framework) benefit learning outcomes more than does the
linear recording of information, with graphs and concept maps especially fostering the
selection and organization of information. As a consequence, the remembering of
information is most effective with non-linear strategies (Dye, 2000; Fisher & Harris,
Figure 2. Use of abbreviating procedures (e.g. ‘Organizat,cr.’) and application of several list
effects with indentations for physical and linguistic formatting of notes in a second language
(English) by undergraduate students
Note taking 295
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
1974; Frank, 1984; Gruneberg & Mathieson, 1997; Robinson & Kiewra, 1995; Robinson,
Katayama, DuBois, & DeVaney, 1998; Slotte & Lonka, 2000; Titsworth & Kiewra, 2004).
Studies on the impact of note-taking strategies on recall and achievement in exams have
shown that students not only learn when they review their notes, but also while they take
their notes. The notes constitute an external memory that can be used later for studying and
other tasks (Kiewra, 1985a, 1985b; Kiewra, Benton, Kim, Risch, & Christensen, 1995;
Knigth & McKelvie, 1986; Laidlaw, Skok, & McLaughlin, 1993; Peters, 1972). Taking
notes themselves can also increase learning by fostering retention and connections of
information, as seen in the generation effect (NB: ‘The generation effects refers to the
finding that individuals retain materials that they have generated better than materials that
have been generated by others and given to them’ Foos, Mora, & Tkacz, 1994, 567).
Moreover, students also memorize during note taking, particularly when they engage in
deep comprehension of the source (Williams & Eggert, 2002).
All the studies mentioned above focus on the integration of information in long-term
memory, but fail to explore the role of working memory in note taking. Hartley and Davies
(1978) had considered that attentional capacity of note takers decreased as a function of
several factors such as the importance given to the course and to the information delivered.
Kiewra (1988b, 1989) mentioned the role of working memory in note taking, indicating
that quantity and quality of notes might be quite different according to the ‘working
memory skill’ of note takers. Scerbo, Warm, Dember, and Grasha (1992) analysed the
nature of the information that was noted and concluded that the attentional capacity of
note takers decreases throughout a course or lecture. Some other authors have considered
Figure 3. Notes recorded during the reading of a report in a professional context with the seven-
question method by a business woman
296 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
that the role of working memory when note taking is a means to decrease cognitive load
during reading (Yeung, Jin, & Sweller, 1997) or in problem solving (Cary & Carlson,
2001; Cohn, Cohn, & Bradley, 1995). The notes, as an external memory, thus support
retention in working memory of intermediate information or solutions that will be used for
comprehension or for elaborating a final solution. Researchers have neglected to study the
critical relation between working memory and note taking, given that taking notes
involves juggling comprehension and production processes under, at times, severe time
pressure.
MEASURING COGNITIVE EFFORT OF NOTE TAKING
Note taking: A high resource-consuming activity
Note takers have to deal with several problems related to the flow of information,
particularly when taking notes from lectures. When writing notes, the note taker is
constrained by the rate of speech of the lecturer: writing speed is about 0.2 to 0.3 words per
second whereas speaking speed is about 2 to 3 words per second (Foulin, 1995; A. Piolat,
unpublished phD thesis, 1982). In other words, note takers must maintain an active
representation of what they are hearing in order to get sufficient time to exploit and to
transcribe a portion, while being faced with a continuous updating of the message content
as it is spoken.
Even with written documents, note takers must also deal with a temporal pressure,
because their transcription remains slower than their reading. They cannot delay or slow
their writing time too much in order to still be able to maintain in working memory the
intermediate representations that result from comprehension. The temporal management
of information is, therefore, a special difficulty in note taking. Note takers must coordinate
the attention and storage demands of both comprehension and written production. They
are thus constrained by the limited resources of the central executive and storage
components of working memory (Baddeley, 1996).
Working memory, as defined by Baddeley (2000), plays a crucial role in all cognitive
activities that require the temporary storage and manipulation of information, whether the
individual accomplishing these activities is just learning or is highly skilled. Working
memory is engaged in comprehension (Daneman & Merikle, 1996) and in writing
(Kellogg, 1996; Levy & Ransdell, 2002; Olive, 2004). These two activities are also
elicited in note taking.
Although taking notes implies activating automatized processes specifically related to
comprehension (e.g. accessing the mental lexicon) and to writing (e.g. letter formation), it
also implies a set of anticipating and control mental operations (i.e. executive functions)
that are resource demanding. Multiple cognitive processes must be coordinated in rapid
succession for note taking to be successful. Such coordination involves the central
executive component of working memory (Baddeley, 1996, 2000). In note taking, these
controlled operations are for a large part conscious and are subject to a metacognitive
control (Castello & Monereo, 1999; Piolat & Bloch, 2004). In others words, note takers
may deliberately regulate their activity to simultaneously comprehend, evaluate, sort, and
write down the information that must be recorded.
To circumvent the limited capacity of working memory, note takers may choose
between two main strategies, with this choice being susceptible to change while taking
Note taking 297
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
notes (Piolat, 2001; Piolat et al., 2003). They may reduce their activity either to
comprehension (listening or reading and noting the less possible) or to transcription
(without processing the content of what is heard or read in order to be able to transcribe the
maximum of information). In the later case, the on-line verbatim transcription of speech
poses a serious psychomotor problem that note takers solve by giving up the transcription
of letters, words, part of sentences, even of whole sentences, by using abbreviating
procedures. Nevertheless, the use of such procedures is not always effective for resolving
the variation of rate between fast oral production and slow writing.
Even if, for some note takers, the abbreviating procedures do not require working
memory resources, the selection of information cannot be performed in an automatic way,
even when note takers have at their disposal procedures that markedly shorten their notes,
as for example, with concept maps (Piolat, 2001; Piolat & Boch, 2004). Thus, these
activities of comprehension and production of notes are, in large measure, deliberate
operations that require central executive resources.
The cognitive effort (Kahneman, 1973; Kellogg, 1987, 1988, 1994) that note taking
engages should vary according to different situational parameters such as the nature of the
information that is noted, the domain-knowledge of the note taker about the lecture or the
reading, etc. The assessment of the amount of cognitive resources that is required by a
complex cognitive activity may be carried out by several techniques according to the
nature of the task that is investigated. For our part, we elected the dual-task paradigm and
the triple-task techniques.
Measuring cognitive effort with the dual-task paradigm
Cognitive effort refers to the fraction of limited attentional resources that are momentarily
allocated to a process (Kahneman, 1973; Tyler, Hertel, McCallum, & Ellis, 1979). Among
the experimental methods that are available, the dual-task technique has often been used
for evaluating the cognitive effort engaged in higher-order cognitive tasks, such as
comprehension or text production. In these tasks, participants are asked to perform
concurrently a primary task and a secondary probe task. For example, while composing a
text or taking notes, participants must react as fast as possible to tones (by pressing a
mouse button or by saying ‘stop’ to a microphone linked to a vocal key) that are
periodically distributed in a random interval (in general between 15 and 45 s). The central
executive of working memory must coordinate the concurrent tasks, focus attention when
each tone is detected, and select a motor response as instructed. Reaction time (RT) in this
dual-task situation is compared with a control condition when the probe is responded to as
a single task. The degree of interference in RT (IRT) caused by the primary task provides a
measure of the amount of cognitive effort devoted to composition or note taking. So, if the
composition of two texts whose content is quite different engages a comparable degree of
cognitive effort, the IRT should not differ. Conversely, if one task engages more cognitive
effort than another, then this should be revealed in their respective IRTs. In sum, the longer
the IRT, the more the primary task places demands on the central resources of working
memory.
Using this kind of secondary task, Kellogg (1986, 120; 1994, 17) compared the
cognitive effort of adults engaged in intentional and incidental learning of nonsense
syllables (Kellogg, 1986), text composition (Kellogg, 1994), reading comprehension
(Britton, Glynn, Meyer, & Penland, 1982), and game playing in chess (Britton & Tesser,
1982). As shown on Figure 4, undergraduates composing short texts in the laboratory
298 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
expend a high degree of cognitive effort, matched only by chess experts as they select a
move in the middle stages of a match. What is common to these two tasks is that they both
involve retrieval of large amounts of knowledge, conceptual planning, and the develop-
ment of solutions to the problems posed in each situation. In such activities, the
engagement of the individual in the task is critical and the cognitive effort allocated is
more substantial than when these operations are not needed, such as in reading, rote
learning or text copying (Olive & Kellogg, 2002). Although texts with complex sentences
demanded more effort than those with simple sentence structures, it is of interest that
reading syntactically complex texts, is still less demanding than writing.
Thus, it appears that the manipulation of information retrieved from long-term memory
and management of these processes in working memory (either in text composition or
when making a skilled move during a chess game) are highly demanding on the central
executive. In the remaining review, similar comparisons are made across different tasks. It
is recognized that these comparisons lead to inferences that must be tested in a single
experiment using statistical procedures. Even so, the comparisons suggest interesting
research questions that can be tested in future experiments.
Measuring cognitive effort with the triple task
The chronometric approach of mental functioning just discussed can be further extended
by analysing the cognitive effort associated with specific processes involved in note taking
Figure 4. Cognitive effort (in milliseconds) in different processing information tasks [(1) Olive &
Piolat, 2002; (2) Kellogg, 1986; (3) Britton et al., 1982; (4) Britton & Tesser, 1982; (5) Piolat, in
press, and (6) Piolat et al., 1996]. NB: the values of standard errors are not available
Note taking 299
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
or writing. This involves asking for an immediate directed introspection after each tone’s
detection. The participant categorizes his or her thoughts at the moment the tone occurred.
For example, in studying text composition, the participants indicated whether their
thoughts reflected planning ideas, translating ideas into text by generating sentences, or
reviewing ideas or text (Kellogg, 1987; Levy & Ransdell, 1995). With the triple task, then,
participants perform: (1) the primary task under investigation, (2) the secondary probe task
described above, and (3) a verbalization task that asks participants to label the process that
was interrupted by the probe. For this last task, participants have first been trained to
identify the cognitive processes activated at the moment based on immediate recollection
of their conscious thoughts at the time of the probe. In most of the writing studies, the three
major writing processes (i.e. planning, translating and reviewing) have been the focus of
the researcher and writers are thus trained to identify these processes. Obviously, different
processes can be the targets of the investigation.
The time course of the triple task is as follows: while writers compose their text, just
after having reacted as fast as possible to the auditory probes, they designate the process
interrupted by the probe, and then the task continues with several cycles of reaction to the
probes followed by directed introspection. Two variables are then analysed. First, the
verbalization data provide information about the processing time of the writing processes
and how this processing time changes throughout a writing session. The percentage of
times spent on a writing process constitutes an index of its processing time. Second, the
RTs to the probes allow one to study the allocation of cognitive resources to these
processes. Each RT to a probe is associated with an immediate, directed introspection
response and the latency of these RTs (in terms of a difference with a baseline RT)
estimates the cognitive effort given to the reported writing process. This method, then,
sheds light on how writers succeed or fail in managing the demands of writing by tracking
the time and effort devoted to specific processes.
The triple task uses directed introspection as opposed to the undirected introspection
obtained in think aloud protocols (Ericsson & Simon, 1980). Verbal protocols provide a
much richer record of the cognitive processes engaged in by the writer. However, thinking
out loud as one writes is particularly challenging given the heavy demands of the primary
task on working memory. When generating sentences, the think aloud protocol requires
speaking a sentence at the same time that it is written and the protocol is often nothing
more than a verbalization of the written sentence. Undirected verbalization is quite
informative with respect to planning and reviewing, but the continuous verbalization
requirement is more intrusive than training writers to identify one of three or four
categories (Olive et al., 2002; Piolat & Olive, 2000). A brief, categorical response
minimizes the disruption of the primary task and at the same time allows one to estimate
processing time and cognitive effort. Further, it allows participants to report nonverbal
processes, such as visual imagery in planning, without the cost of recoding into words as
required in a think aloud protocol (Piolat, Olive, Roussey, Thunin, & Ziegler, 1999). The
use of directed introspection has proven extremely valuable in other research areas, such
as the categories of ‘remember’ versus ‘know’ in studies of episodic memory (Gardiner &
Richardson-Klavehn, 2000).
The triple task may appear complex and disruptive of the primary task. Several
experiments have been carried out to test the impact of the probe task and directed
introspection on the primary task (Kellogg, 1987; Pe
´lissier & Piolat, 1999; Piolat, Kellogg,
& Farioli, 2001; Piolat, Roussey, Olive, & Farioli, 1996). The findings of these experi-
ments show that the functioning and performance of the participants to the primary task
300 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
are neither degraded nor altered. Moreover, processing time and cognitive effort of the
writing processes do not necessarily co-vary; for instance, although translating processes
are activated more often than planning and revising, the cognitive cost of translating is
usually smallest. The triple–task method is a powerful technique that highlights key
features of the strategies writers use when composing a text.
In sum, with such tools of mental chronometry and according to the goals of the study,
researchers can elect to focus on only the holistic cognitive effort engaged by a particular
task (e.g. by comparing note taking from a lecture or from a written document). For this
purpose, the dual-task technique with a secondary probe task suffices. The researcher
might, however, want to track the time course and cognitive effort of the cognitive
processes that underlie an activity. The triple-task technique offers a way to do so that is
un-intrusive and informative.
COGNITIVE EFFORT IN DIFFERENT SITUATIONS OF NOTE TAKING
Several complex cognitive processes involved in language processing activities are
simultaneously activated when taking notes (comprehension, writing, and learning). To
assess their respective cost, in terms of cognitive effort, findings from various studies are
compared in this section. Results are not presented in the following sections in a canonical
way for the following reasons. Firstly, our goal is to offer a synopsis of the different studies
that have evaluated cognitive effort during note taking and the detailed statistics of each
study are not presented here. Secondly, putting all these studies together is meant only to
present an overview of a large sample of results in order to suggest hypotheses, as was
done in Kellogg’s (1986) review of writing tools. Our main goal is to attempt to predict in
which way working memory is engaged depending on the writing activity (e.g. writing
versus note taking) and on the different contexts (e.g. note taking from a document versus
from a website).
Comparing note taking with comprehension, learning and writing
Taking notes implies comprehending either a written document or a lecture and recording
information by writing it down. So, we first compare noting from a lecture, reading a text
(to assess the cognitive effort of comprehension), and writing by longhand. Figure 4 shows
the results of four areas of enquiry in which cognitive effort was measured.
In a difficult intentional learning task, participants learned (psychology undergraduate
students) a list of nonsense syllables (Kellogg, 1986). In the reading study (Britton et al.,
1982), participants read a brief text. The note-taking situation asked participants
(psychology undergraduate students) to take notes from a 12-min lecture on literature
before filling a questionnaire (Piolat, in press; Roussey & Piolat, 2003). In the last study,
participants (psychology undergraduate students) composed an approximately one-page
long argumentative text (Piolat et al., 1996). As Figure 4 shows, the cognitive effort
engaged by note taking is greater than learning and comprehending (reading in Figure 4).
This indicates that taking notes demands more of the central executive than either learning
or comprehending alone, and supports the assumption that both of these activities are
engaged in note taking.
In another experiment that used the triple-task technique (Ge
´rouit, Piolat, Roussey, &
Barbier, 2001), note takers had to categorize their activity after each reaction to a tone
Note taking 301
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
according to whether they were reading a booklet or recording information by writing it
down. Despite the reading task being highly effortful, judging from the large IRT observed
(590 ms), the note-taking task (661 ms) was still reliably more demanding. As this finding
shows, the writing activity of note taking seems to be more costly than that of
comprehension. Thus, taking notes consumes at least as many resources as composing a
text. Noting and writing both involve comprehension processes because both writers and
note takers build a conceptual mental representation of what they are composing— for the
former, and of what they are hearing for the latter. But they also activate other processes
to format their notes with the various physical and linguistic formatting procedures they
have at their disposal. Applying these procedures is more effortful than comprehending
(see reading in Figure 4). This finding is compatible with data from a comparison between
creative and non- creative activities (see next section, and text copying in Figure 4).
Comparing note taking with the processes involved in text composition
The analysis of the processes involved in note taking indicates that note taking is closer to
text composition than to graphic transcription (Piolat et al., 2003). This interpretation is
supported by data from the comparison between cognitive effort associated to note taking,
copying, and the three major cognitive processes (planning, translating and revising;
Piolat, in press; Roussey & Piolat, 2003) with data from an experiment that assessed
cognitive effort of the three major writing processes (Piolat et al., 1996) and with data
from an experiment in which the cognitive effort of copying was assessed (Olive & Piolat,
2002). In this last experiment, participants (psychology undergraduate students) had to
copy a text that they had just composed in one of the conditions.
As Figure 4 illustrates, note taking is not only transcribing (cf. copying). The cognitive
effort devoted to note taking is indeed reliably higher than the effort devoted to copying. In
other words, taking notes is not a simple graphic transcription of the information that is
carried out in the lecture or in the reading. Indeed, in addition to operations of
comprehension, when taking notes, note takers also select information, reformulate the
contents (abbreviations, telegraphic style, linguistic formatting of the notes), operations
which all require an effort that is greater than that required for a simple sequential
transcription. Finally, the low cognitive effort of copying (i.e. handwriting) also suggests
that gaining access to meaning, sorting and selecting information can be elicited
simultaneously with writing (more precisely, with translating and the associated motor
execution of handwriting).
The comparison between note taking and the three writing processes (planning,
translating and revising) first shows that planning the content of a text is the most effortful
process (Kellogg, 1994). Retrieving and organizing ideas during a text composition are
still more effortful than selecting the information that will be recorded. Searching a new
and ‘creative’ solution (i.e. the text written down) requires more resources than taking
notes, even if the notes often present content characteristics different from what has been
heard or read.
Comparing note taking from different sources and with different techniques
Note taking is used in very different situations that impose different information
processing demands. Note takers frequently extract information from a written document
302 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
(an article, a book) to learn its content, or in order to use this information later. By contrast
with note taking from a lecture, the pace at which note takers have to comprehend and
select information from a document is slower when they read. They can, therefore, activate
successively and not concurrently some of the operation needed to record the information
via handwriting. Techniques also vary substantially, with almost all note takers using a
personalized method for taking notes on a blank sheet. Some of them use pre-planned
methods that reduce the urgency in taking notes. The sheets are marked with a plan, such
as an outline, that guides the selection and recording of information from readings or
lectures.
Piolat (in press) has compared these different situations of note taking by psychology
undergraduate students (Roussey & Piolat, 2003). More precisely, she compared note
taking when listening to a lecture on literature with note taking while reading a transcript
of it, and she contrasted these two contexts with two methods: taking notes with one’s own
usual method or with a pre-planned method (Figure 5).
Not surprisingly, taking notes from reading requires less cognitive effort than from a
lecture. When listening, more operations are concurrently engaged and, thus, taking notes
from a lecture places more demands on working memory resources. In Piolat’s experiment
(in press), the note-taking method did not affect the amount of working memory resources
note takers engaged in the task. One possible interpretation is that the different methods
that can be used for taking notes only affect the temporal management of the different
activities that underlie note taking rather than their cognitive effort. This finding needs to
be replicated, however.
Comparing note taking from different mediums with different
amounts of information
Both the amount of information and its support can affect the cognitive effort of note
takers. The written documents that constitute the source of the note-taking activity can be
of various lengths (from a few pages to a book). Information can, moreover, be presented
on paper (books) or on a computer screen or notepad (hypertexts). In Figure 6 the findings
from two studies are presented. In one study, students took notes from a two-page written
document of 1680 words. Psychology undergraduate students were instructed that they
Figure 5. Mean cognitive effort (in milliseconds, and standard errors) when taking notes from a
lecture or while reading and with two methods (usual versus pre-planned; Piolat, in press; Roussey &
Piolat, 2003)
Note taking 303
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
had to take notes because they had then to complete a questionnaire (Piolat, in press;
Roussey & Piolat, 2003). In the other experiment, psychology undergraduate students
explored a large document (around 30 pages) on pollution that results from shipwrecks and
on specificities of maritime transport. Their task was to write an argumentation defending
the sea transport of dangerous products. Information was presented either in a booklet or
through a hypertext document (namely, a website; for a discussion of the differences
between the two types of documents, see Ge
´rouit et al., 2001).
As this comparison indicates (see Figure 6), taking notes from a short text is less
effortful than from a long document. In addition to activating text comprehension
processes, when taking notes from a large document, note takers also must generate a
large mental representation of the text (namely, a more complex mental model).
Furthermore, to perform the task well, they develop strategies for scanning the whole
text that are very different from the strategies developed for reading short texts. With short
text, the note takers generally adopt linear scanning strategies, while they do not for longer
texts.
Searching and sampling part of a text in order to find information to be recorded are
more effortful with a hypertext than with a paper document. Using computer technology to
manage information through the click of a mouse can actually increase cognitive effort,
judging from these results. It may be that the use of these technologies is less practised
than reading and handwriting. Similar results were obtained by Kellogg and Mueller
(1993), however, who found that writing by longhand was less effortful than using a word
processor even for skilled typists.
Note taking and linguistic skill
Note takers’ linguistic skill, and in particular their mastery of a language, is another factor
that might affect cognitive effort. Barbier and Piolat (M.-L. Barbier & A. Piolat, paper
presented at the Sig Writing Conference, Geneva, 2004) examined the notes produced by
undergraduate French students in their third year of English (as a foreign language). These
students were instructed to take notes from an 8-min lecture (an 840-word text was
recorded and then played) and they had then to summarize the text. They took notes first in
French (first language) from a lecture about enrollment in French universities and second
in English (second language) from a lecture about the general organization of studies in
French universities (test order was counterbalanced: half of the students passed L1 and
then L2, the other half L2 then L1).
Figure 6. Mean cognitive effort (in milliseconds) according to the size of the written source (two
pages versus 30 pages) and the medium (paper versus hypertext) [(1) Piolat, in press; Roussey &
Piolat, 2003; (2) Ge
´rouit et al., 2001]. NB: the values of standard errors are not available
304 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Producing a text (in our case summarizing it, see Figure 7) requires more executive
attention than taking notes in both languages. This finding supports the results reported in
Figure 4. Not surprisingly, both note taking and text composition in a second language
demands much more effort than in a first language.
Level of skill in a specific activity (as for example linguistic skill or domain knowledge,
cf. Olive, Piolat, & Roussey, 1997) does not explain all the observed inter-individual
differences. For instance, Piolat (in press) and Roussey and Piolat (2003) have shown that
working memory capacity assessed with the French adaptation of a reading span test
(Desmette, Hupet, Schelstraete, & Van der Linden, 1995)—favours the adoption of
different strategies of note taking and affects the cognitive effort that is engaged by note
takers.
CONCLUSION
We have argued that the time urgency of note taking imposes significant demands on the
limited resources of working memory. The problems encountered by the note taker can be
understood in terms of the major cognitive effort required in specific contexts or situations.
Note taking is a complex activity that involves interweaving both comprehension and
production processes. As with reading, writing and other complex activities, working
memory is a critical cognitive component and individual differences in performance can
be traced to how well the demands on working memory are managed. The findings
reviewed here show that the dual-task paradigm (and its variants) provides a relevant
window on the mental functioning of note takers. The effects of different situational
factors on the engagement of central executive of working memory can be fruitfully
studied.
It is important in future work to use the triple-task method to identify and investigate
more precisely all the information processing operations engaged in note taking.
Identifying when and how many times comprehension, selection and production processes
are activated, as well as how these processes are orchestrated constitutes an important
challenge to understanding the functional characteristics of note taking. Our objective here
was to delimit the basic process of note taking in a manner paralleling the written
composition of original texts to lay the foundation for decomposing the task and its mental
components further (Hayes & Flower, 1980; Kellogg, 1994).
Figure 7. Mean cognitive effort (in milliseconds, and standard errors) by undergraduate students
when taking notes or when summarizing a lecture after having taken the notes in first language
(French) and second language (English) (Barbier & Piolat, in press)
Note taking 305
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
It also remains for future research to relate cognitive effort (that most often results from
strategic adaptation) with behavioural outcomes, such as the quality of the notes taken or
the performance or the time required to complete the task. However, in contrast to texts
written for public consumption, notes are generally a private product intended to be
meaningful only to the note taker. It may be more difficult to adopt criteria of quality that
are agreed upon as valid for note taking in comparison to written texts.
However, because processing time (or frequency) of the processes involved in note
taking can also be assessed with the triple-task method, the effect of the different methods
(or formats) and strategies (for instance focusing essentially on comprehension on
recording of information) of note taking can be investigated. Moreover, inter-individual
characteristics of note takers, already visible in the notes, may be discovered. Perhaps
there are individual ‘writing signatures’ for note taking similar to those observed already
in full texts’ compositions (Levy & Ransdell, 1995; van Waes & Schellen, 2003).
In text composition, writers have to find a balance between the quality of the text they
want to reach and the cognitive cost of the activity (Kellogg, 1994; Olive & Piolat, 2003).
In particular, this balance is sensitive to the level of expertise (domain knowledge,
linguistic skill). By analogy with text composition, evidence for such a trade-off between
procedures in note taking, namely the nature of the produced notes, and the requirements
in cognitive resources should appear a promising way for a better understanding of note
taking and of its learning outcomes.
The observations reviewed here indicate that, from a cognitive perspective, note taking
cannot be conceived of as only a simple abbreviated transcription of information that is
heard or read. Rather, on the contrary, it is an activity that strongly depends on the central
executive functions of working memory to manage comprehension, selection, and
production processes concurrently. Indeed, the severe time pressure of note taking requires
that information is both quickly comprehended and recorded in written form. It is a unique
kind of written activity that cumulates both the inherent difficulties of comprehending a
message and of producing a new written product. Yet, it differs in many of its
characteristics from the usual linear and conventionally presented written texts.
AUTHORS’ NOTE
Annie Piolat, Centre PsyCLE, University of Provence (France). Thierry Olive, Laboratory
Language and Cognition, CNRS and University of Poitiers (France). Ronald T. Kellogg,
Department of Psychology, Saint Louis University (Missouri, USA).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank Professor J. Hartley and an anonymous reviewer for their suggestions to improve
the manuscript.
The article was supported by a grant from the French ACI programme ‘Ecole et
Sciences Cognitives’.
REFERENCES
Alamargot, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2001). Through the models of writing. In G. Rijlaarsdam (Series
Ed.), Studies in writing (Vol. 9). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
306 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Armbruster, B. B. (2000). Taking notes from lectures. In R. F. Flippo, & D. C. Caverly (Eds.),
Handbook of college reading and study strategy research (pp. 175–199). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 49A, 5–28.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423.
Badger, R., White, G., Sutherland, P., & Haggis, T. (2001). Note perfect: an investigation of how
students view taking notes in lectures. System, 29(3), 405–417.
Baker, L., & Lombardi, B. R. (1985). Students’ lecture notes and their relation to test performance.
Teaching of Psychology, 12, 28–32.
Barbier, M.-L., & Piolat, A. (in press). L1 and L2 cognitive effort of note taking and writing. In
L. Allal, & B. Schneuwly (Eds.). Proceedings of the Special Interest Group on Writing. Geneva,
Switzerland.
Barbier, M.-L., Faraco, M., Piolat, A., Roussey, J.-Y., & Kida, T. (2003). Comparaison de la prise de
notes d’e
´tudiants japonais et espagnols dans leur langue native et en franc¸ais L2 [Comparing note
taking between Japanese and Spanish students in L1 and L2]. Retrieved from May 20, 2004, from
Arob@se 7, 1-2 [http://www.arobase.to/v7/].
Barnett, J. E., Di Vesta, F. J., & Rogozinski, J. T. (1981). What is learned in note-taking? Journal
of Educational Psychology, 73, 181–192.
Boyle, J. R., & Weishaar, M. (2001). The effects of strategic note taking on the recall and
comprehension of lecture information for high school students with learning disabilities. Learning
Disabilities: Research & Practice, 16(3), 133–141.
Branca-Rosoff, S. (1998). Abre
´viations et ico
ˆnes dans les prises de notes des e
´tudiants. [Abbrevia-
tions and icons in note taking by students] In M. Bilger, K. dan den Eynde, & F. Gadet (Eds.),
Analyse linguistique et approches de l’oral. Recueil d’e
´tudes offert en hommage a
`Claire-
Blanche-Benveniste [Speech analysis. Festchrift to Claire Blanche-Benveniste] (pp. 286–299).
Leuven-Paris: Peeters.
Bretzing, B. H., Kulhavy, R. W., & Caterino, L. C. (1987). Note taking by junior high students.
Journal of Educational Research, 80, 359–362.
Britton, B. K., & Tesser, A. (1982). Effects of prior knowledge on use of cognitive capacity in three
complex cognitive tasks. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 421–436.
Britton, B. K., Glynn, S. M., Meyer, B. J. F., & Penland, M. J. (1982). Effects of text structure on use
of cognitive capacity during reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 51–61.
Brown, A. L., & Day, J. D. (1983). Macrorules for summarizing texts: the development of expertise.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 1–14.
Canelos, J., Dwyer, F., Nichols, B., & Randall, T. W. (1984). The note taking strategy of directed
overt activity for improving learning on three types of intellectual tasks. Journal of Instructional
Psychology, 11(3), 139–148.
Cary, M., & Carlson, R. A. (1999). External support and the development of problem-solving routines.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 1053–1070.
Cary, M., & Carlson, R. A. (2001). Distributing working memory resources during problem solving.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 836–848.
Castello, M., & Monereo, C. (1999). Strategic knowledge in note-taking: a study in high education.
Infancia y Aprendizaje, 88, 25–42.
Chaudron, C., Loschky, L., & Cook, J. (1994). Second language listening comprehension and lecture
note-taking. In J. Flowerdew (Ed.), Academic listening: Research perspectives (pp. 75–92).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clerehan, R. (1995). Taking it down: note taking practices of L1 and L2 students. English for specific
purposes, 14(2), 137–157.
Cohn, E., Cohn, S., & Bradley, J. J. (1995). Note-taking, working memory, and learning in principles
of economics. Research in Economic Education, 26(4), 291–307.
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: a meta-
analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 422–433.
Desmette, D., Hupet, M., Schelstraete, M. A., & Van der Linden, M. (1995). Adaptation en langue
franc¸aise du ‘reading span test’ de Daneman et Carpenter [French adaptation of Daneman &
Carpenter’ reading span test]. L’Anne
´e Psychologique, 95, 459–482.
Note taking 307
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Dunkel, P. A., & Davy, S. (1989). The heuristic of lecture note taking: perceptions of American and
international students regarding the value and practice of note taking. English for Specific
Purposes, 8(1), 33–50.
Dye, G. A. (2000). Graphic Organizers to the Rescue! Helping Students Linkand Remember
Information. Teaching Exceptional Children, 32(3), 72–76.
Dyer, J., Riley, J., & Yekovitch, F. (1979). An analysis of three study skills: note-taking,
summarizing and rereading. Journal of Educational Research, 73(1), 3–7.
Einstein, G. O., Morris, J., & Smith, S. (1985). Note-taking, individual differences, and memory for
lecture information. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(5), 522–532.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychological Review, 87, 215–251.
Fisher, J. L., & Harris, M. B. (1974). Effects of note taking preference and types of notes taken on
memory. Psychological Reports, 35, 384–385.
Foos, P. W., Mora, J. J., & Tkacz, S. (1994). Student study techniques and the generation effect.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(4), 567–576.
Foulin, J. N. (1995). Pauses et debits: les indicateurs temporels de la production e
´crite
[Pauses and fluency: chronometrics indices of writing production]. L’Anne
´e Psychologique, 95,
483–504.
Frank, B. M. (1984). Effects of filed independence-dependence and study technique on learning from
a lecture. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 669–678.
Gardner, J. M., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2000). Remembering and knowing. In E. Tulving, &
F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 229–244). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Ge
´rouit, C., Piolat, A., Roussey, J.-Y., & Barbier, M. L. (2001). Cou
ˆt attentionnel de la recherche
d’informations par des adultes sur hypertexte et sur document papier. In M. Mojahid, & J. Virbel
(Eds.), Actes du 4Colloque International sur le Document Electronique (pp. 201–215). Paris:
Europia production.
Gruneberg, M. M., & Mathieson, M. (1997). The perceived value of minds maps (spider diagrams)
as learning and memory aids. Cognitive Technology, 2(1), 21–24.
Hadwin, A. F., Kirby, J. R., & Woodhouse, R. A. (1999). Individual differences in note-taking,
summarization and learning from lectures. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 45(1),
1–17.
Hartley, J. (1976). Lecture handouts and student note taking. Programmed Learning and Educa-
tional Technology, 13, 58–64.
Hartley, J. (2002). Note taking in non academic settings: a review. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16,
559–574. DOI: 10.1002/acp.814
Hartley, J., & Davies, I. K. (1978). Notetaking: a critical review. Programmed Learning and
Educational Technology, 15, 207–224.
Hayes, R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. W. Gregg,
& E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Horton, S. V., Lovitt, T. C., & Christensen, C. C. (1991). Note taking from textbooks: effects of a
columnar format on three categories of secondary students. Exceptionality, 2, 19–40.
Houdek Middendorf, C., & Hoff Macan, T. (2002). Note taking in the employment interview: effects
on recall and judgments. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(2), 293–303.
Howe, M. J. A. (1974). The utility of taking notes as an aid to learning. Educational Research, 16,
222–227.
Isaacs, G. (1994). Lecturing practices and note taking purposes. Studies in Higher Education, 19(2),
203–216.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Kellogg, R. T. (1986). Designing idea processors for document composition. Behavior Research
Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 18, 118–128.
Kellogg, R. T. (1987). Effects of topic knowledge on the allocation of processing time and cognitive
effort to writing processes. Memory and Cognition, 15, 256–266.
Kellogg, R. T. (1988). Attentional overload and writing performance: effects of rough draft and
outline strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14(2),
355–365.
Kellogg, R. T. (1994). The psychology of writing. New York: Oxford University Press.
308 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Kellogg, R. T. (1996). A model of working memory in writing. In M. C. Levy, & S. E. Ransdell
(Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 57–
71). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.
Kellogg, R. T., & Mueller, S. (1993). Performance amplification and process restructuring in
computer-based writing. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 39, 33–49.
Kiewra, K. A. (1985a). Investigating note taking and review: a depth of processing alternative.
Educational Psychologist, 20, 23–32.
Kiewra, K. A. (1985b). Learning from a lecture: an investigation of note taking, review and
attendance at a lecture. Human Learning, 4, 73–77.
Kiewra, K. A. (1987). Note taking and review: the research and its implications. Journal of
Instructional Science, 16, 233–249.
Kiewra, K. A. (1988a). Cognitive aspects of autonomous note taking: control processes, learning
strategies, and prior knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 23, 39–56.
Kiewra, K. A. (1988b). Comparing of self-questioning, summarizing, and note taking-review as
strategies for learning from lectures. American Educational Research Journal, 29(2), 303–323.
Kiewra, K. A. (1989). A review of note-taking: the encoding storage paradigm and beyond.
Educational Psychology Review, 1, 147–172.
Kiewra, K. A. (1991). Aids to lecture learning. Educational Psychologist, 26, 37–53.
Kiewra, K. A., & Benton, S. L. (1988). The relationship between information-processing ability and
note-taking. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 13, 33–44.
Kiewra, K. A., & Frank, B. M. (1988). Encoding and external storage effects of personal lecture
notes, skeletal notes, and detailed notes for field-independent and field-dependent learners.
Journal of Educational Research, 81, 143–148.
Kiewra, K. A., Benton, S. L., & Levis, L. B. (1987). Qualitative aspects of note taking and their
relationship with information-processing ability and academic achievement. Journal of Instruc-
tional Psychology, 14(3), 110–117.
Kiewra, K. A., DuBois, N. F., Christian, D., & McShane, A. (1988). Providing study notes: a
comparison of the three types of notes for review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 595–
597.
Kiewra, K. A., DuBois, N. F., Christensen, M., Kim, S., & Lindberg, N. (1989). A more equitable
account of the note taking functions in learning from lecture and from text. Instructional Science,
18(3), 217–232.
Kiewra, K. A., DuBois, N. F., Christian, D., McShane, A., Meyerhoffer, M., & Roskelley, D. (1991).
Note taking functions and techniques. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 240–245.
Kiewra, K. A., Benton, S. L., Kim, S., Risch, N., & Christensen, M. (1995). Effects of note-taking
format and study technique on recall and relational performance. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 20, 172–187.
King, A. (1992). Comparison of self-questioning, summarizing, and note-taking-review as strategies
for learning from lectures. American Educational Research Journal, 29(2), 303–323.
Knigth, L. J., & McKelvie, S. J. (1986). Effects of attendance, note taking and review on memory for
lecture: encoding versus external storage function of notes. Canadian Journal of Behavioral
Science, 18, 52–61.
Ladas, H. (1980). Note taking on lectures: an information-processing approach. Educational
Psychology, 15(1), 44–53.
Laidlaw, E. N., Skok, R. L., & McLaughlin, T. F. (1993). The effects of note taking and self-
questioning on quiz performance. Science Education, 77(1), 75–82.
Levy, C. M., & Ransdell, S. E. (1995). Is writing as difficult as it seems? Memory & Cognition, 23(6),
767–779.
Levy, C. M., & Ransdell, S. E. (2002). Writing with concurrent memory loads. In G. Rijlaarsdam
(Series Ed.), Studies in Writing & T. Olive, & C. M. Levy (Eds.), Studies in Writing:
Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 9–29). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Lindberg-Risch, N., & Kiewra, K. A. (1990). Content and form variations in note taking: effects
among junior high students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(6), 355–357.
Morgan, C. H., Lilley, J. D., & Boreham, N. C. (1988). Learning from lectures: the effect of
varying the detail in lecture handouts on note taking and recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2,
115–122.
Note taking 309
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Nist, S. L., & Hogrebe, M. C. (1987). The role of underlining and annotating in remembering textual
information. Reading Research and Instruction, 27, 12–25.
Norton, L. S. (1981). The effects of note taking and subsequent use on long term recall. Programmed
Learning and Educational Technology, 18(1), 16–22.
Norton, L. S., & Hartley, J. (1986). What factors contribute to good examination marks: the role of
note taking in subsequent examination performance. Higher Education, 15, 355–371.
Nye, P. A., Crooks, T. J., Powley, M., & Tripp, G. (1984). Student note taking related to university
examination performance. Higher Education, 13, 85–97.
Oakhill, J., & Davies, A. M. (1991). The effects of test expectancy on quality of note taking and
recall of text at different times of day. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 179–189.
Olive, T. (2004). Working memory in writing: empirical evidence from the dual-task-technique.
European Psychologist, 9(1), 32–42.
Olive, T., & Kellogg, R. T. (2002). Concurrent activation of high- and low-level writing processes.
Memory & Cognition, 30(4), 594–600.
Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2002). Suppressing Visual Feedback in written composition: effects on
Processing Demands and Coordination of the Writing. International Journal of Psychology, 37(4),
209–218.
Olive, T., & Piolat, A. (2003). Activation des processus re
´dactionnels et qualite
´des textes [Writing
processes and texts quality]. Le Langage et L’Homme, 38(2), 191–206.
Olive, T., Piolat, A., & Roussey, J.-Y. (1997). Effort cognitif et mobilisation des processus: Effet de
l’habilete
´re
´dactionnelle et du niveau de connaissances [Cognitive effort and time processing of
the writing processes: effect of writing skills and level of knowledge]. In D. Mellier, & A. Vom
Hofe (Eds.), Attention et contro
ˆle cognitif: Me
´canismes, de
´veloppement des habilete
´set
pathologies [Attention and control: mechanisms, development and pathologies] (pp. 71–85).
Rouen: P.U.R.
Olive, T., Kellogg, R. T., & Piolat, A. (2002). The triple task technique for studying the processus of
writing: why and How? In G. Rijlaarsdam (Series Ed.), Studies in Writing & T. Olive, & C. M.
Levy (Vol. Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 31–59). Dordrecht:
Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Peck, K., & Hannafin, M. (1983). The effects of note taking pretraining and the recording notes on
the retention of aural instruction. Journal of Educational Research, 75(2), 100–107.
Pe
´lissier, A., & Piolat, A. (1999). Are writing processes activated sequentially or in parallel? The
way to approprietly use Kellogg’s three task method. In E. Espe
´ret, & L. Tolchinsky (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 1998 European Writing Conference: writing and Learning to Write at the dawn
of the 21st Century (pp. 71–76). Poitiers: MSHS.
Peters, D. L. (1972). Effect of the note taking and rate of presentation on short-term objective test
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 276–280.
Piolat, A. (1998). Evaluation and Assessment of Written Texts. The encyclopedia of language and
education, language testing and assessment (Vol. 7, pp. 189–198). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
Piolat, A. (1999). Planning and text quality among undergraduate students: findings and questions. In
M. Torrance, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Knowing what to write: Conceptual processes in text
production (pp. 121–136). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
Piolat, A. (2001). La prise de notes [Note Taking]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Piolat, A. (in press). Effects of note taking technique and working-memory span on cognitive effort.
In G. Rijlaarsdam (Series Ed.), Studies in Writing, & D. Galbraith, M. Torrance, & L. van Waes
(Vol. Eds.), Recent developments in writing process research (Vol. 1: Basic processes and word-
level effects). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Piolat, A., & Boch, F. (2004). Apprendre en notant et apprendre a
`noter. In E. Gentaz, & P. Dessus
[Learning by taking notes and learning to take notes] (Eds.), Comprendre les apprentissages.
Psychologie cognitive et e
´ducation [Understanding learning. Cognitive psychology and educa-
tion] (pp. 133–152) Paris: Dunod.
Piolat, A., & Olive, T. (2000). Comment e
´tudier le cou
ˆtetlede
´roulement de la re
´daction de textes?
La me
´thode de la triple ta
ˆche: Un bilan me
´thodologique [How to study the cost and the unfolding
of writing? Theoretical constraints and research methods]. L’Anne
´e Psychologique, 100, 465–502.
Piolat, A., & Roussey, J.-Y. (1996). Students’ drafting strategies and text quality. Learning and
Instruction, 61, 111–129.
310 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Piolat, A., Roussey, J.-Y., Olive, T., & Farioli, F. (1996). Charge mentale et mobilisation des
processus re
´dactionnels: examen de la proce
´dure de Kellogg [Mental load and time processing of
writing: test of Kellogg’s procedure]. Psychologie franc¸ aise, 41-4, 339–354.
Piolat, A., Olive, T., Roussey, J.-Y., Thunin, O., & Ziegler, J. C. (1999). Scriptkell: a tool for
measuring cognitive effort and time processing in writing and other complex cognitive activities.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computer, 31, 113–121.
Piolat, A., Kellogg, R. T., & Farioli, F. (2001). The triple task technique for studying writing
processes: on which task is attention focused ? Current Psychology Letters. Brain, Behavior and
Cognition, 4, 67–83.
Piolat, A., Roussey, J.-Y., & Barbier, M. L. (2003). Mesure de l’effort cognitif: Pourquoi est–il
opportun de comparer la prise de notes a
`la re
´daction, l’apprentissage et la lecture de divers
documents? [Measuring cognitive effort: why is it relevant to compare note taking, writing,
learning and reading] Arob@se, 1-2, 118–140 [http://www.arobase.to]. [20 May 2004].
Rickards, J. P., Fajen, B. R., Sullivan, J. F., & Gillespie, G. (1997). Signaling, Note-taking, and field
independence-dependence in text comprehension and recall. Journal of Educational Psychology,
89, 508–517.
Robinson, D. H., & Kiewra, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: graphic organizers are superior to
outlines in improving learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 455–467.
Robinson, D. H., Katayama, A. D., DuBois, N. F., & DeVaney, T. (1998). Interactive effects of
graphic organizers and delayed review in concept acquisition. The Journal of Experimental
Education, 67, 17–31.
Roussey, J.-Y., & Piolat, A. (2003). Prendre des notes et apprendre. Effet du mode d’acce
`sa
`
l’information et de la me
´thode de prise de notes [Taking notes and learning. Effects of the access
mode to information and to the note taking procedure]. Arob@se 7, 1-2. [ http://www.arobase.to/
somm.html]. [20 May 2004].
Russell, I. J., Caris, T. N., Harris, G. D., & Hendricson, W. D. (1983). Effects of three types of
lectures notes on medical student achievement. Journal of Medical Education, 58, 627–636.
Sanchez, R. P., Lorch, E. P., & Lorch, R. F. (2001). Effects of headings on text processing strategies.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(3), 418–428.
Scerbo, M. W., Warm, J. S., Dember, W. N., & Grasha, A. F. (1992). The role of time and cueing in a
college lecture. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17, 312–328.
Slotte, V., & Lonka, K. (1999). Review and process effects of spontaneous note taking on text
comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24(1), 1–20.
Slotte, V., Lonka, K. (2000). Spontaneous concept maps aiding the understanding of scientific
concepts. International Journal of Sciences and Education, 21, 515–531.
Slotte, V., & Lonka, K. (2001). Note taking and essay writing. In G. Rijlaarsdam (Series Ed.), &
P. Tynja
¨la
¨, L. Mason, & K. Lonka (volume Eds.), Studies in writing, writing as a learning tool:
Integrating theory and practice (Vol. 7, pp. 131–141). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Smith, P., & Tompkins, G. E. (1988). Structured note taking: a new strategy for content area readers.
Journal of Reading, 32, 46–53.
Spires, H. A. (1993). Learning from a lecture: effects of comprehension monitoring. Reading
Research & Instruction, 32(2), 19–30.
Stahl, N. A., King, J. R., & Henk, W. A. (1991). Enhancing students’ note taking through training
and evaluation. Journal of Reading, 34(8), 614–622.
Suritsky, S. K. (1992). Note taking approaches and specific aeras of difficulty reported by university
students with learning disabilities. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 10(1), 3–
10.
Thomas, G. S. (1978). Use of student’s notes and lecture summaries as studies guides for recall.
Journal of Educational Research, 71, 316–319.
Titsworth, B. S. (2001). The effects of teacher immediacy, use of organizational lecture cues, and
students’ note taking on cognitive learning. Communication Education, 50(4), 283–297.
Titsworth, B. S., & Kiewra, K. A. (2004). Spoken organizational lecture cues and student note taking
as facilitators of student learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(4), 447–461.
Tyler, S. W., Hertel, P. T., McCallum, M. C., & Ellis, H. C. (1979). Cognitive effort and memory.
Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning & Memory, 5, 607–617.
van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic
Press.
Note taking 311
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
Van Meter, P., Yokoi, L., & Pressley, M. (1994). College students’ theory of note taking derived from
their perceptions of note-taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 323–338.
van Waes, L., & Schellens, P. J. (2003). Writing profiles: the effect of the writing mode on pausing
and revision patterns of experienced writers. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 829–853.
Williams, R. L., & Eggert, A. (2002). Note taking predictors of test performance. Teaching of
Psychology, 29(3), 234–237.
Yeung, A. S., Jin, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). Cognitive load and learner expertise: split attention and
redundancy effects in reading with explanatory notes. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23,
1–21.
312 A. Piolat et al.
Copyright #2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 19: 291–312 (2005)
... Storage refers to the written record of notes produced in this way, which can be reviewed by the students at a later point in time as a supplement to their imperfect memories. Despite the benefits of encoding on students' long-term recall of course content, the process of writing down information while listening to a lecture and attempting to understand it is a cognitively demanding proposition, one which can ultimately overwhelm working memory (Jansen et al., 2017;Piolat et al., 2005). Meanwhile, numerous studies have shown that access to highly complete notes, that is, notes that contain a high proportion of relevant information from a course lecture, correlates positively with students' ability to recall information (Einstein et al., 1985;Raver & Maydosz, 2010). ...
... Therefore, the external storage of information that students create through note-taking may be incomplete, impeding their ability to review course information at a later time. Moreover, the process of taking notes may also come with the significant cognitive cost of attempting to listen, write and comprehend simultaneously, which may be burdensome enough to overload learners' working memories in some cases (Jansen et al., 2017;Piolat et al., 2005). Therefore, the subsections that follow will examine two alternative forms of note-taking for reducing the amount of encoding students engage in while increasing their access to quality storage: collaborative note-taking and instructorprovided notes. ...
... As shown in the figure, the amount of encoding effort on the part of the learner varies greatly in individual note-taking, and the amount of effort students expend in encoding the notes corresponds to the quality of the storage produced. However, prior work has shown that even high amounts of effort by an individual to encode lead to lower quality of storage of notes (Fanguy et al., 2023), as individual students tend to take rather porous notes (Jansen et al., 2017;Piolat et al., 2005). Collaborative note-takers tend to produce notes with higher storage quality compared to those of individual note-takers, and each member of the note-taking group has to expend less effort to encode (Fanguy et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Note-taking is generally regarded as an effective learning strategy, but it is also cognitively demanding. Students often omit salient information in their notes due to the burden of trying to listen while writing down what they hear. Two alternative forms to traditional note-taking are online collaborative note-taking and instructor-provided notes. The former strategy allows students to work together online in small groups to share the burden of note-taking as well as the benefits of the collaborative document they produce together. The latter strategy involves the course instructor providing students with a complete set of notes, freeing students cognitively to listen and make connections with the lecture content. However, research on these two approaches remains sparse, and thus far, no study has compared their effects on learning performance. Therefore, the present study compared the learning performances of 161 students divided into two treatment conditions within a flipped learning environment: one where students took collaborative notes and another where students received notes provided by the instructor. Quiz scores showed no differences between the two conditions, but measures of both group and individual writing quality were significantly higher in the online collaborative note-taking condition than with the instructor-provided notes. Implications for practice or policy: To enhance students’ recall, instructors should provide notes to students. To bolster students’ ability to collaborate, instructors should have students take online collaborative notes. To enable students to improve and apply a skill, such as writing, instructors should have students take online collaborative notes.
... Note-taking is an essential activity for learning, as it creates external storage of information for future access, helps students encode the provided information [26], and helps students stay focused [24]. As notes are commonly taken while processing new information, note-taking is a complex cognitive activity, requiring students to perform multiple steps to understand, select relevant information, and process them into notes [36]. Also, as note-taking is often performed during time-sensitive events, such as lectures or meetings, there is an urgency to record information before it is lost. ...
... Also, as note-taking is often performed during time-sensitive events, such as lectures or meetings, there is an urgency to record information before it is lost. As such, being able to take notes quickly is a high priority [1,36]. Digital note-taking has become more prevalent due to the increasing availability of tablets and laptops and allows digital storage of notes for easy access [1]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Students often take digital notes during live lectures, but current methods can be slow when capturing information from lecture slides or the instructor's speech, and require them to focus on their devices, leading to distractions and missing important details. This paper explores supporting live lecture note-taking with mixed reality (MR) to quickly capture lecture information and take notes while staying engaged with the lecture. A survey and interviews with university students revealed common note-taking behaviors and challenges to inform the design. We present MaRginalia to provide digital note-taking with a stylus tablet and MR headset. Students can take notes with an MR representation of the tablet, lecture slides, and audio transcript without looking down at their device. When preferred, students can also perform detailed interactions by looking at the physical tablet. We demonstrate the feasibility and usefulness of MaRginalia and MR-based note-taking in a user study with 12 students.
... • Design Goal 2: Help people manage the cognitive load of creating dialogue maps during the conversation (D2). Creating dialogue maps can be a cognitively demanding process due to the complex visual structure it requires [20,69]. While encouraging users to collaboratively create dialogue maps, the system should also reduce the extra cognitive effort brought by the collaborative map creation. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Video meeting platforms display conversations linearly through transcripts or summaries. However, ideas during a meeting do not emerge linearly. We leverage LLMs to create dialogue maps in real time to help people visually structure and connect ideas. Balancing the need to reduce the cognitive load on users during the conversation while giving them sufficient control when using AI, we explore two system variants that encompass different levels of AI assistance. In Human-Map, AI generates summaries of conversations as nodes, and users create dialogue maps with the nodes. In AI-Map, AI produces dialogue maps where users can make edits. We ran a within-subject experiment with ten pairs of users, comparing the two MeetMap variants and a baseline. Users preferred MeetMap over traditional methods for taking notes, which aligned better with their mental models of conversations. Users liked the ease of use for AI-Map due to the low effort demands and appreciated the hands-on opportunity in Human-Map for sense-making.
... Actively attending to aspects of source information during an event (e.g., by noting down information) has been shown to enhance source memory (Lindsay 2007). However, as note taking can be cognitively demanding (e.g., Piolat, Olive, and Kellogg 2005) and may divide attention, the aim of the current study was to focus participants' attention on the cognitive tasks of thinking about questions and remembering information. It is acknowledged that, in operational settings, interviewers may take notes during interviews and may be available during subsequent interviews to aid interviewers recall. ...
Article
Full-text available
Investigative interviewers are often required to accurately remember information that has been provided by different people. This can be at the scene of an event or during follow‐up investigations in interview rooms. Interviewers must accurately monitor the source of information to differentiate between witnesses' accounts and to assess what information is novel and what has been corroborated by others or by physical evidence. The current research examined the effects of cognitive load on memory and source monitoring accuracy for information provided by multiple witnesses. Participants, under conditions of high cognitive load (HCL) where load was induced via interviewer‐relevant tasks (e.g., formulating questions) or no cognitive load (NCL), watched five mock‐witnesses' accounts of the same crime. Each witness provided several details of the crime that were unique to their individual account. When asked about account details, and which witness had provided each detail, mock‐interviewers' memory accuracy was lower in the HCL condition than the NCL condition. There was no difference between cognitive load conditions for source monitoring accuracy, which was poor regardless of condition.
Article
In today’s digital landscape, social media platforms, particularly Facebook, play a pivotal role in shaping consumer-brand interactions, especially in high-interest sectors such as the smartphone market in Romania. This study investigates which message strategies are associated with higher levels of user engagement on the Facebook pages of two leading smartphone brands, Samsung and Xiaomi. The research explores the dynamics between messaging strategies (informational and transformational) and user engagement, providing insights into the digital advertising strategies adopted by these brands during the crucial holiday shopping season. The findings suggest that the informational dimension, notably ration message strategy, generated different types of engagement. In particular, ration led to higher levels of sharing than ego. Furthermore, regarding reactions, ration resulted in greater engagement than ego, routine and sensory. This research contributes to understanding digital consumer engagement and provides valuable insights for marketers seeking to optimize social media strategies in the competitive smartphone market.
Article
In generally the reading comprehension is a very difficult task for students in all the stages of study, especially when they are reading text in a foreign language, the EFL reading” (English as foreign language) is one of the most common research topics in the learning field. This study aimed to investigate the effect of teaching EFL reading comprehension to Japanese students through Kit-Build concept mapping (KB-mapping) method. In doing so, we have conducted an experiment to compare the student comprehension with KB-mapping and Scratch-Build Concept Mapping (SB-mapping) Methods. In doing so, a comprehension test and delayed comprehension test (two weeks after using) were used. By comparing the results of the Comprehension Test (CT) scores and the Delayed Comprehension Test scores (DCT) for the both conditions groups, we found that the using of KB-mapping had the same effects of SB-mapping in the understanding the text just after the using, but KB-mapping had better effects in the recalling and remembering the text after a while.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates academic reading difficulties and coping strategies among undergraduate English as Foreign Language (EFL) students in the English Education Study Program at Pattimura University, Indonesia. Using a mixed-methods case study design, data were collected from 40 undergraduate students through questionnaires and group interviews. Quantitative analysis revealed three primary challenges: understanding specialist vocabulary (M=3.8), taking brief and relevant notes (M=3.8), and working out meanings of difficult words (M=4.0). Qualitative findings identified four main coping strategies: technology utilization, systematic note-taking, text rereading, and mind mapping techniques. The results indicate that while students face significant vocabulary-related challenges in academic reading, they have developed sophisticated strategies to overcome these difficulties. This study contributes to understanding EFL students' academic reading experiences in Indonesian higher education contexts and provides insights for developing targeted pedagogical interventions.
Chapter
Full-text available
A collection of original papers by researchers working in the field which comprehensively addresses the area of second language academic listening. This collection of original papers comprehensively addresses the area of second language academic listening. The papers are grouped under five broad headings. The first section provides an overview of research relevant to second language lecture comprehension. The second analyses aspects of the cognitive processes involved in listening comprehension. In the third section, the object of the comprehension process is examined, and in the fourth, ethnographic approaches are explored by extending the concept of listening comprehension to place it in the wider context of 'the culture of learning'. In the final section, the theory of second language listening comprehension is related to practical pedagogic concerns. Each section is preceded by an accessible introduction and the book as a whole provides detailed coverage of important aspects of academic listening phenomena.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter concentrates on the relationship between the quality of note taking during writing and subsequent outcomes — as measured by the content and quality of essay-type answers. During their research project the authors studied the notes of more than 1200 students in order to answer the questions: What kinds of note taking activities are related to sensible and well-written answers in a difficult examination? How note taking can serve more effectively writing as a learning tool? The results demonstrated that writing summary notes has an effect on both learning the content knowledge and on the linguistic features of the resulting essays. Yet, several procedural aspects of writing play crucial roles in the learning process. Spontaneous note taking affects learning and writing operations utilising distinct types of mental representations. This depends on the type of notes taken, the nature of the writing tasks, and the writing conditions — such as notes-present or notes-absent. Overall, these results raise the need to pay more attention to the ability to write notes in one’s own words, argue purposefully and to monitor the coherence and accuracy of meaning. These skills are especially important in learning by writing.
Article
In this study, we determined the relation between full-semester notetaking and test performance variables in a large undergraduate human development course. Although the notetaking framework equally weighted readings and lectures, notetaking over lectures (particularly accurate notetaking) proved more promising in accounting for test performance measures than did notetaking over reading materials.
Article
This study represents an extension of previous research on interfering and facilitating variables affecting the encoding function of note taking by adding several review conditions. Ninety-six subjects were assigned to five note-taking and three non-note-taking groups. Subjects heard a 16-minute segmented lecture and recalled lecture content 48 hours later. The review of notes neutralized both the interfering and facilitating effects on recall of note-taking manipulations reported in previous research. Review of student-generated notes resulted in better recall than did review of lecturer-generated summaries.
Article
The effects of instruction in notetaking, the recording of notes, and sex of the notetaker on the retention of aurally presented information were examined. Subjects were 43 male and 61 female sixth grade students, randomly assigned to four experimental conditions: instructed note- takers, uninstructed notetakers, instructed non-notetakers, and uninstructed non-notetakers. Notetaking instruction consisted of a videotaped presentation designed to train students in paying attention, selecting main ideas, maintaining pace with the rate of instruction, and personalizing presented information. All students then heard a brief prose passage, while either taking or not taking notes in accordance with their experimental group assignments. Notes were collected from notetaking groups, and were not returned for review. Assessments of recall of key information were conducted immediately, 5 days, and 30 days after the presentation of the instructional passage. Significant interactions were found between instruction in notetaking and the recording of notes, sex of student and notetaking, and notetaking and time. The findings indicated that the effects of notetaking may be conditional, depending on factors such as intelligence and verbal ability, and that additional efforts toward identifying such conditions are warranted.
Article
College students read a 2,000-word text with or without taking notes. Afterwards, one-half of the learners wrote passage summaries without referring to the text, while the remainder completed a spatial relations task. Subjects then either reread the original passage or engaged in placebo work. All subjects subsequently took both a 30- item immediate test and a seven-day delayed test composed of equal numbers of factual and idea questions. Both notetaking and rereading strategies improved recall; however, these effects were nonadditive. Participants recalled more substantive information, but rereading increased factual recall. The time and conditional probability analyses supported these findings. The authors concluded that the presence of the original text was essential to further learning.