ArticlePublisher preview available

Momentary Interruptions Can Derail the Train of Thought

American Psychological Association
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

We investigated the effect of short interruptions on performance of a task that required participants to maintain their place in a sequence of steps each with their own performance requirements. Interruptions averaging 4.4 s long tripled the rate of sequence errors on post-interruption trials relative to baseline trials. Interruptions averaging 2.8 s long-about the time to perform a step in the interrupted task-doubled the rate of sequence errors. Nonsequence errors showed no interruption effects, suggesting that global attentional processes were not disrupted. Response latencies showed smaller interruption effects than sequence errors, a difference we interpret in terms of high levels of interference generated by the primary task. The results are consistent with an account in which activation spreading from the focus of attention allows control processes to navigate task-relevant representations and in which momentary interruptions are disruptive because they shift the focus and thereby cut off the flow. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).
Momentary Interruptions Can Derail the Train of Thought
Erik M. Altmann
Michigan State University
J. Gregory Trafton
Naval Research Laboratory, Washington, District of Columbia
David Z. Hambrick
Michigan State University
We investigated the effect of short interruptions on performance of a task that required participants to
maintain their place in a sequence of steps each with their own performance requirements. Interruptions
averaging 4.4 s long tripled the rate of sequence errors on post-interruption trials relative to baseline
trials. Interruptions averaging 2.8 s long—about the time to perform a step in the interrupted task—
doubled the rate of sequence errors. Nonsequence errors showed no interruption effects, suggesting that
global attentional processes were not disrupted. Response latencies showed smaller interruption effects
than sequence errors, a difference we interpret in terms of high levels of interference generated by the
primary task. The results are consistent with an account in which activation spreading from the focus of
attention allows control processes to navigate task-relevant representations and in which momentary
interruptions are disruptive because they shift the focus and thereby cut off the flow.
Keywords: task interruption, procedural error, cognitive control, placekeeping
Understanding the mechanisms of cognitive control is one of the
main aims of cognitive psychology, with consequences for under-
standing human abilities and brain– behavior relationships in a
wide range of domains and populations. Cognitive control is
expressed in many ways, one of which involves selecting the next
thought to focus on when there are multiple options and when
interruptions or distractions can intervene. An example is a con-
versation, which generally follows a coherent thread. If an inter-
ruption occurs, such as an interlocutor’s cell phone ringing, the
thread may get lost, leading to a “Where were we?” moment (e.g.,
Trafton, Jacobs, & Harrison, 2012).
An important question for practical and theoretical reasons is
just how minimal such an interruption can be and still affect the
train of thought. For the cell phone example, the question is
whether the interlocutor need only attend to his or her phone for a
couple of seconds—to shut it off, for example, rather than to
actually take the call. In safety-critical contexts, the question is
whether it is safe to interrupt someone even for a few seconds in
the middle of a procedure without increasing the chance of error.
To operationalize “train of thought,” we used a task in which
subtasks— or what we call steps have to be performed in a
particular sequence, and correct performance depends on remem-
bering one’s place in this sequence. In behavioral research, se-
quential tasks like this arise mainly in two domains, one focused
on errors in sequential action selection, and the other on effects of
task interruption. In the error domain, the focus has been on
“routine” tasks (Norman & Shallice, 1986), like coffee-making
(Botvinick & Bylsma, 2005;Botvinick & Plaut, 2004;Cooper &
Shallice, 2000,2006), packing a lunch (Cooper, Schwartz, Yule, &
Shallice, 2005), or cleaning the house (Botvinick & Plaut, 2004).
However, routine tasks are generally performed with high accu-
racy, so the error data they generate are sparse. Reason (1990) used
diary methods to collect errors in routine tasks over long periods,
and others have focused on neurological patients for whom routine
tasks are difficult (Schwartz, Reed, Montgomery, Palmer, &
Mayer, 1991). In the task interruption domain, sequential tasks
with “post-completion” steps (Byrne & Bovair, 1997;Li, Bland-
ford, Cairns, & Young, 2008;Trafton, Altmann, & Ratwani, 2011)
generate elevated error rates on those steps, but by design those
steps are still a small proportion of the total. In a study of
interrupted routine performance, Botvinick and Bylsma (2005) had
to have their participants make 50 actual cups of coffee to generate
enough errors to analyze.
We therefore saw a need for a procedure that generates rich and
interpretable data on sequence errors, from healthy populations
performing under controlled laboratory conditions. To meet this
need, we developed a sequential task that combines a routine
sequencing component with relatively complex individual steps.
An important criterion was that every participant should effec-
tively be a trained expert on the step sequence, so that sequence
errors could be attributed to on-line control mechanisms rather
than lack of knowledge about the sequence. We therefore built the
This article was published Online First January 7, 2013.
Erik M. Altmann, Department of Psychology, Michigan State Univer-
sity; J. Gregory Trafton, Naval Research Laboratory, Washington, District
of Columbia; David Z. Hambrick, Department of Psychology, Michigan
State University.
This research was supported by grants from the Office of Naval Re-
search to Erik M. Altmann (N000140910093) and J. Gregory Trafton
(N0001412RX20082, N0001411WX30014). The authors thank Rick Coo-
per and Dave Plaut for their comments during the review process, Mark
Becker and Amber Markey for comments on earlier drafts, and Amber
Markey for collecting the data.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Erik M.
Altmann, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East
Lansing, MI 48824. E-mail: ema@msu.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General © 2013 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 143, No. 1, 215–226 0096-3445/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030986
215
... Studies have associated mobile phone addiction with procrastination, reduced social interaction, impulsivity, low self-esteem, and low academic performance [43]. Literature has suggested an indirect relationship between mobile phone addiction and learning engagement and that poor sleep quality, fatigue, and headache are the mediating factors through which the addiction affects students' learning [44]. Notably, a 3-second distraction (students trying to reach for their mobile phones) effectively disrupts students' attention when performing cognitive activities, such as learning [44]. ...
... Literature has suggested an indirect relationship between mobile phone addiction and learning engagement and that poor sleep quality, fatigue, and headache are the mediating factors through which the addiction affects students' learning [44]. Notably, a 3-second distraction (students trying to reach for their mobile phones) effectively disrupts students' attention when performing cognitive activities, such as learning [44]. The distraction is disadvantageous to both ongoing and subsequent cognitive tasks, leading to an increase in errors as the distraction period increases [42]. ...
... Although parents are yet to conclude the ongoing debate on consenting mobile phone usage among their children in schools [43][44][45][46], empirical evidence indicates that students who use their mobile phones in class take fewer notes [47] and have poor academic performances (overall) when compared to their counterparts [48,49]. Some studies have shown no significant difference in academic achievement (scores) between students using mobile phones and those not, although mobile phone users are reported to take longer to complete their class tests [50,51]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Students’ excessive reliance on mobile phones during English classes diminishes their learning engagement and understanding, which in turn affects their academic performance. Teachers’ prior observations at our selected university indicated that smartphone usage among English learners varied significantly across class groups and among individual students within groups. This study employed a mixed-tactics approach (i.e., interviews, classroom observations, and group discussions) to uncover influential factors (both environmental and psychological) shaping students’ behaviour of smartphone overuse during English learning hours. The key findings have underscored the impact of environmental variables, such as class size es that are un that are unmanageable) and examination policies (i.e., not having final exams at the end of the semester), alongside psychological factors like self-discipline (self-control) and motivation. Generally, this study highlights the need for targeted interventions (within the university) and enhanced classroom management strategies (by teachers) to alleviate or lessen digital distractions and bolster learning engagement. The study advocates for promoting digital wellness initiatives and collaborative efforts to support students’ English learning engagement while prioritizing well-being in educational contexts.
... Generally, a task interruption is defined as a situation in which an ongoing primary task is temporally suspended to perform a secondary task (e.g., Hirsch et al., 2024a). Numerous studies have shown that task interruptions have adverse effects on performance in the primary task (e.g., Altmann et al., 2014;Dodhia and Dismukes, 2009;see Werner et al., 2015, for a review). To examine these effects, many studies have focused on resumption costs (e.g., Altmann and Trafton, 2004;Blumberg et al., 2015;Monk et al., 2008;Trafton et al., 2003). ...
... Regarding the effect of task-interruption duration, various studies showed that long task interruptions are more harmful for primarytask performance than short interruptions (e.g., Altmann et al., 2014;Altmann et al., 2017;Foroughi et al., 2016;Hodgetts and Jones, 2006). More specifically, the resumption of the primary task takes longer and is more error-prone after long compared to short task interruptions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Several studies showed that task interruptions at high mental workload moments are more harmful than task interruptions at low mental workload moments. In the present study, we used a theory-driven approach to define the mental workload during primary-task execution and to examine the effects of the interruption timing on primary-task performance. Methods Participants performed a primary task comprising a pre-defined sequence of six subtasks, with task interruptions occasionally occurring before the second, third, or fourth subtasks. Critically, the subtasks were organized either in two lag-2 repetition triplets or in two lag-2 switch triplets (e.g., ABA-CBC vs. CBA-CAB). This set-up allowed us to test two predictions about the effects of interruption timing on the resumption costs (i.e., the performance in subtasks following an interruption compared to the performance in the same subtask in non-interrupted primary tasks). First, we expected task interruptions before the fourth subtask being the less detrimental due to the presumed chunking of the six subtasks into two triplets. Second, in lag-2 switch triplets, task interruptions before the second and third subtasks were predicted to result in comparable resumption costs. In contrast, in lag-2 repetition triplets, task interruptions before the third subtask were hypothesized to be more disruptive than those before the second subtask. This is because the mental workload should be higher due to the need to overcome subtask inhibition. Results We found an interruption-timing effect with higher resumption costs for task interruptions occurring before the third subtask compared to interruptions before the second and the fourth subtasks. However, this effect did not differ across lag-2 repetition sequences and lag-2 switch sequences. Discussion These findings are discussed from a memory perspective and a context reconstruction perspective.
... Given the affordances of digital distractions like visual and auditory stimulating content in advertisements, as well as the rewards associated with engaging with such distractions (Bhargava and Velasquez 2021), we can expect such distractions will be looked at (attention diversion) and appraised favorably, at least initially. Thus, the focus of the individual will be diverted at least temporarily to the distractors (Altmann et al. 2014) and there will be delays in returning to and completing the task at hand. Hence, we propose: ...
... As per the proposed process model, digital distractions usually with stimulating content would attract attention and be appraised favorably such that people respond by focusing on them. For professional tasks that typically involve complex sub-tasks, it has been reported that even a momentary distraction can degrade performance (Altmann et al. 2014;Orhan et al. 2021). For most online tasks (both professional and leisure) that require some amount of concentration, digital distractions can lead to attention diversion at least temporarily. ...
Conference Paper
As online activities dominate our lives, digital distractions (e.g., advertisements) are suggested to divert individuals' attention away from tasks and impede task performance. Increased prevalence of these distractions has motivated research on interventions to alleviate their adverse effects. However, these studies typically lack models and empirical evidence of the effectiveness of the interventions. In this study, we develop a process model for digital distractions and explore the role of proposed blocking interventions. Through a pilot lab experiment, we examine the impact of both digital distractions and self-versus other-imposed blocking interventions on task accuracy and completion time. Preliminary results indicate that digital distractions impair task performance and increase completion time. Furthermore, we found self-deployed interventions to be more effective than other-deployed blocking interventions in mitigating these negative effects. We end with outlining our future research plans. This study advances our understanding of approaches for managing digital distractions and promoting productivity.
... The subtasks are usually forced-choice speeded categorisation tasks (e.g. UNRAVEL task by Altmann et al., 2014; WINDA task by Kopacz et al., 2019). In contrast to continuous tasks that require a continuous stream of actions over a longer period of time (e.g. car driving), procedural tasks comprise multiple discrete subtasks with a definite starting point (i.e. ...
... A well-known finding is that the disruptiveness of task interruptions on the performance in the primary task is stronger for long than for short interruptions (e.g. Altmann et al., 2014;Foroughi et al., 2016;Hodgetts & Jones, 2006). For interruptions in a time range up to 1 min, a study by Monk et al. (2008) provides evidence that interruption-duration effects are greatest for relatively short interruptions (i.e. up to 13 s). ...
... Mobile phone use has been shown to limit its users' attention and appropriate timely decision-making, eventually affecting their psychological well-being (Salehan & Negahban, 2013). Altmann et al. (2014) found that interruptions as short as 2.8 s disrupted participants' concentration flow (several minutes of full, uninterrupted concentration) and increased errors in a serial cognitive task. Thus, smartphones with visual and auditory signals that alert the owner of incoming messages from social networks act as interrupters, disrupting the flow experience and negatively impacting productivity (Duke & Montag, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
In the digital age, mobile phones significantly impact human cognition and behavior. This experimental study examined the effects of passive mobile phone presence on attentional control in young adults aged 18–25. Participants were randomly assigned to a control (no phone) or an experimental group (phone present). Attention control was measured using the Attention Network Test (ANT). In contrast, smartphone nomophobia and addiction were measured with the Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q) and the Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS-SV). Contrary to previous literature, the presence of a mobile phone did not significantly distract participants or impair attentional performance. No significant relationship emerged between self-reported levels of distraction or nomophobia and actual attentional performance, although smartphone addiction seemed to have a weak effect on the errors made by those who performed in the presence of their mobile phone. Significant gender differences were found in terms of nomophobia, with women reporting higher levels than men. This study suggests that the relationship between mobile phone presence and attentional processes is more complex than previously hypothesized, bringing the existing literature under further consideration.
... The impact of interruptions on working memory may vary depending on multiple task characteristics. Firstly, tasks that are less cognitively demanding or cause less competition for working memory capacity are less affected by interruptions compared to tasks that are more complex or induce sudden shifts of attention (Altmann et al. 2014;Cades et al. 2007;Monk et al. 2008;Zickerick, Rösner, et al. 2021). Furthermore, certain task parameters are found to help mitigate the negative effects of interruptions on working memory. ...
Article
Full-text available
Interruptions are a common aspect of everyday working life, negatively affecting both task performance and long‐term psychological well‐being. However, research suggests that the effects of interruptions can be mitigated in several ways, such as the opportunity to anticipate the interruptions and preparation time. Here, we used a retrospective visual working memory task to investigate the effects of duration and flexible resumption after interruptions, with 28 participants (18–30 years old) attending the experiment. For this main task participants were required to remember the orientations of a set of coloured bars and retrieve one at the end of the trial in response to a retro‐cue. This task was sometimes interrupted with an arithmetic task that was presented before the retro‐cue. The period after the interruptions and the retro‐cue was either short (no additional time), long (additional 1000 ms), or self‐determined. Interruptions affected the main task performance irrespective of duration condition, but response times were shorter with the flexible condition. EEG analysis showed that having more time before resuming the interrupted task enabled stronger beta suppression which in turn modulated task performance, helping participants to safely disengage from the interrupting task, and refocus their attention back more efficiently. Further, flexibility in the timing of resumption provided additional benefits as seen in stronger oscillatory alpha and beta suppression to the retro‐cue, also being related to better task performance. These results demonstrate the important role of resumption time and individual flexibility in dealing with interruptions.
... Or conversely, could the absence of a mobile phone cause greater distraction or anxiety? Considering that attention is defined as the ability to concentrate, specifically, to exert conscious cognitive effort on a particular task or stimulus at any given moment 8 , and given that various studies suggest it is easy to lose focus due to short-term distractions such as messages, sounds, etc. 9,10 ; it is valid to consider how mobile phones could impact learner performance. This is not a new idea, and various studies have shown that mobile phone use negatively affects learners' attention, memory, and academic performance. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
This work presents the IMPROVE dataset, designed to evaluate the effects of mobile phone usage on learners during online education. The dataset not only assesses academic performance and subjective learner feedback but also captures biometric, behavioral, and physiological signals, providing a comprehensive analysis of the impact of mobile phone use on learning. Multimodal data were collected from 120 learners in three groups with different phone interaction levels. A setup involving 16 sensors was implemented to collect data that have proven to be effective indicators for understanding learner behavior and cognition, including electroencephalography waves, videos, eye tracker, etc. The dataset includes metadata from the processed videos like face bounding boxes, facial landmarks, and Euler angles for head pose estimation. In addition, learner performance data and self-reported forms are included. Phone usage events were labeled, covering both supervisor-triggered and uncontrolled events. A semi-manual re-labeling system, using head pose and eye tracker data, is proposed to improve labeling accuracy. Technical validation confirmed signal quality, with statistical analyses revealing biometric changes during phone use.
Article
Radiologists face a range of challenges to maximize the life-saving benefits of screening mammography, including pressure to maintain accuracy, manage heavy workloads, and minimize the risk of fatigue and burnout. This review provides targeted strategies to address these challenges and, ultimately, to improve interpretive performance of screening mammography. Workflow optimizations, including offline vs online and batched vs nonbatched interpretation, interrupted vs uninterrupted reading, and the importance of comparing current mammograms with prior examinations will be explored. Each strategy has strengths, weaknesses, and logistical challenges that must be tailored to the individual practice environment. Moreover, as breast radiologists contend with increasingly busy and hectic working conditions, practical solutions to protect reading environments and minimize distractions, such as the “sterile cockpit” approach, will be described. Additionally, breast radiologists are at greater risk for fatigue and burnout due to rising clinic volumes and an inadequate workforce. Optimizing the approach to reading screens is critical to helping breast imaging radiologists maintain and maximize the benefits of screening mammography, ensure the best outcomes for our patients, and maintain radiologist job satisfaction.
Article
Background: Due to the emergence of smartphone addiction, as a 21th century phenomenon, investigating its subsequent negative effects is essential. Objective: The present study aims to test the predicting effect of smartphone addiction on musculoskeletal discomfort in hand/neck region as well as cognitive failures. Methods: A cross-sectional study was designed in which 533 smartphone users (60.2% females and 39.8% males; mean age: 35.9±11.0 years) participated. Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS), Cognitive Failures Questionnaires (CFQ), Neck Disability Index (NDI) and Cornell Hand Discomfort Questionnaire (CHDQ) were used for data collection. Age, gender and occupational use of electronic devices were considered as socio-demographic factors. Independent t-test, chi-square, and logistic regression were used for data analysis. Results: The overall prevalence of smartphone addiction was 37.9% among Iranian 20-61 years users. Addicted smartphone users were 3.3, 2.2, 2.9, and 2.8 times more likely to develop musculoskeletal discomfort in right hand, left hand, neck, and cognitive failures than non-addicted smartphone users. High occupational use of electronic devices predicted right-hand discomfort (by 2.6 times) as well as cognitive failures (by 3.4 times). Women were significantly more likely to develop all the studied outcomes. Age did not predict any of the studied outcomes. Conclusions: Addiction to smartphones can lead to a sharp rise in the prevalence of neck and hand discomfort, as well as cognitive failures, particularly among working age people. This is concerning issue that requires effective preventive and corrective measures. Developing behavioral approaches to address this problem can help reduce its impact on society.
Article
The authors develop and test a theoretical framework to examine the impact of app crashes on app engagement. The framework predicts that consumers increase engagement after encountering a single crash due to their need-for-closure and curiosity, yet reduce engagement after experiencing repeated and concentrated crashes, primarily because of frustration and perceived task unattainability; the recency of crashes moderates these effects. Field data analysis reveals that while a crash truncates a session and reduces content consumption, it increases page views in the following session. However, this increase in page views does not compensate for the loss during the crashed session. Frequent and more concentrated crashes curtail engagement. Three experiments in which crashes are exogenously manipulated in a different context support the validity and generalizability of these findings, confirm the proposed mediators, and demonstrate how to lessen the negative impact of repeated crashes with post-crash messages. The research adds new dimensions to the task pursuit literature and provides managers with a framework to quantify the economic impact of crashes, analyze content substitution behavior, and assess the bias of a transactional view of crash incidents. Additionally, it offers insights into targeted feature release to more tolerant users and strategic design of post-crash messages.
Article
Full-text available
Previous task-switching research raises a question concerning the role of episodic memory processes in cognitive control. The question is framed by the contrast between two procedures, explicit cuing and randomized runs, one of which presents a task cue perceptually on every trial and the other of which involves uncued trials. The present study compares performance across these procedures. Performance errors sensitive to errors in focusing on the correct task were higher under explicit-cuing conditions than under randomized-runs conditions, consistent with a high level of proactive interference from old task information. The results support an account in which control codes stored in episodic memory play an integral role in cognitive control, even under conditions in which all information needed for performance is perceptually available.
Article
Full-text available
This paper outlines a theoretical approach to the study of action in simple, routine activities, along with a coding system for describing and analysing performance. The approach is exemplified through a single case study involving a patient who developed a profound action disorder following rupture of a pericallosal artery aneurysm and concomitant anterior cerebral artery spasm. The Action Coding System provides a detailed, quantitative picture of this patient's action disorder as it was manifested in two routine tasks and as it recovered over time. Discussion focuses on the cognitive organisation and processing of simple, everyday tasks and suggests how disruption of performance may ensue when intentional control of action is compromised.
Article
Full-text available
M. G. Berman, J. Jonides, and R. L. Lewis (2009) adapted the recent-probes task to investigate the causes of forgetting in short-term memory. In 7 experiments, they studied the persistence of memory traces by assessing the level of proactive interference generated by previous-trial items over a range of intertrial intervals. None of the experiments found a reduction in proactive interference over time, which they interpreted as evidence against time-based decay. However, it is possible that decay actually occurred over a shorter time period than was tested in this study, wherein the shortest decay interval was 3,300 ms. By reducing the time scale, the 2 experiments reported in the current commentary revealed a sharp decrease in proactive interference over time, with this reduction reaching a plateau in less than 3 s. This pattern suggests that decay operates in the early stages, whereas subsequent forgetting is likely to be due to interference.
Article
Winner of the 2012 Heineken Prize for Cognitive Science, awarded to an individual who has done outstanding work in cognitive science. The prize draws attention to the importance of studying the cognitive functions of human and animals, a relatively new branch of science. The winners of the 2012 Heineken Prizes are chosen by independent juries appointed by the Royal Academy. The prizes are funded by the Dr. H.P. Heineken Foundation and the Alfred Heineken Fondsen Foundation. The award is given by the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences endowed by Heineken Foundation. Professor Duncan is receiving the Dr. A.H. Heineken Prize for Cognitive Science for his remarkable innovative, multidisciplinary research into the relationships between psychology, behaviour and intelligence on the one hand and neural processes on the other. His concepts have become a cornerstone of cognitive neuroscience. Selected as a Choice Outstanding Academic Title for 2011 in the Psychology category. Human intelligence is among the most powerful forces on earth. It builds sprawling cities, vast cornfields, coffee plantations, and complex microchips; it takes us from the atom to the limits of the universe. Understanding how brains build intelligence is among the most fascinating challenges of modern science. How does the biological brain, a collection of billions of cells, enable us to do things no other species can do? In this book John Duncan, a scientist who has spent thirty years studying the human brain, offers an adventure story-the story of the hunt for basic principles of human intelligence, behavior, and thought. Using results drawn from classical studies of intelligence testing; from attempts to build computers that think; from studies of how minds change after brain damage; from modern discoveries of brain imaging; and from groundbreaking recent research, Duncan synthesizes often difficult-to-understand information into a book that will delight scientific and popular readers alike. He explains how brains break down problems into useful, solvable parts and then assemble these parts into the complex mental programs of human thought and action. Moving from the foundations of psychology, artificial intelligence, and neuroscience to the most current scientific thinking, How Intelligence Happens is for all those curious to understand how their own mind works.
Article
For decades, the fundamental processes underlying memory and attention have been understood within an "information processing" framework in which information passes from one processing stage to another, leading eventually to a response. More recently, however, the attempt to build a general theoretical framework for information processing has been largely supplanted in favor of two more recent approaches: mathematical models of processing and direct investigations of brain function. This book reconciles theoretical conflicts in the literature to present an important, analytical update of the traditional information-processing approach by modifying it to incorporate the last few decades of research on memory, attention, and brain functioning. Throughout, the book cogently considers and ultimately refutes recent challenges to the fundamental assumption of the existence of special short-term memory and selective attention faculties. It also draws a key distinction between memory processes operating inside and outside of the focus of attention. The book hopes to foster an understanding of how memory and attention operate together, and how both functions are produced by brain processes.
Article
We built and evaluated a predictive model for resuming after an interruption. Two different experiments were run. The first experiment showed that people used a transactive memory process, relying on another person to keep track of where they were after being interrupted while retelling a story. A memory for goals model was built using the ACT-R/E cognitive architecture that matched the cognitive and behavioral aspects of the experiment. In a second experiment, the memory for goals model was put on an embodied robot that listened to a story being told. When the human storyteller attempted to resume the story after an interruption, the robot used the memory for goals model to determine if the person had forgotten the last thing that was said. If the model predicted that the person was having trouble remembering the last thing said, the robot offered a suggestion on where to resume. Signal detection analyses showed that the model accurately predicted when the person needed help.
Article
Performance on measures of working memory (WM) capacity predicts performance on a wide range of real-world cognitive tasks. I review the idea that WM capacity (a) is separable from short-term memory, (b) is an important component of general fluid intelligence, and (c) represents a domain-free limitation in ability to control attention. Studies show that individual differences in WM capacity are reflected in performance on antisaccade, Stroop, and dichotic-listening tasks. WM capacity, or executive attention, is most important under conditions in which interference leads to retrieval of response tendencies that conflict with the current task.