Article

The Role of Inference Making and Other Language Skills in the Development of Narrative Listening Comprehension in 4-6-Year-Old Children

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

In this two-year longitudinal study, we sought to examine the developmental relationships among early narrative listening comprehension and language skills (i.e., vocabulary knowledge, sentence memory, and phonological awareness) and the roles of these factors in predicting narrative listening comprehension at the age of 6 years. We also sought to examine the role of inference-making skills as longitudinal and concurrent predictors of other language skills and listening comprehension from the age of 4 to 6 years. One hundred thirty Finnish-speaking children participated in the study. A theoretical model of the developmental relationships among the variables was proposed and the associations were analyzed by means of path analysis. Results showed that inference skills, assessed through picture-book viewing, made a significant and unique contribution to variation in later narrative listening comprehension. Inference skills also played an indirect role in narrative listening comprehension by making a significant contribution to vocabulary knowledge even after controlling for earlier vocabulary knowledge and sentence memory. Although vocabulary knowledge and sentence memory were related to concurrent narrative listening comprehension, they did not predict later listening comprehension over and above the autoregressor. The results are discussed in terms of the predictive validity and diagnostic sensitivity of inference skills assessments in listening comprehension. Implications for research and theory are also discussed.

No full-text available

Request Full-text Paper PDF

To read the full-text of this research,
you can request a copy directly from the authors.

... ικό πλαίσιο και η αναπαραγωγή τους απαιτεί την άμεση δική τους εμπλοκή (Licandro, 2016). Πρόσφατα, επίσης, ερευνητικά στοιχεία έχουν καταδείξει ισχυρή σχέση μεταξύ των γνωστικών λειτουργιών και των αφηγηματικών δεξιοτήτων, καθώς το αφηγηματικό επίπεδο των παιδιών αντικατοπτρίζει το γλωσσικό, γνωστικό και ευρύτερα κοινωνικό τους επίπεδο (Kiren, 2013. Lepola, Lynch, Laakkonen, Silvén, & Niemi, 2012. ...
... Εντούτοις, η επίδραση του φύλου δε θεωρείται δεδομένη, καθότι σε ορισμένες μελέτες διαφαίνεται μια διαφυλική διαφοροποίηση σε σχέση με τη δομή και το περιεχόμενο της αφήγησης (Κανέλου et al., 2016. Ross & Holmberg, 1990) ενώ σε άλλες όχι (Lepola, Lynch, Laakkonen, Silvén, & Niemi, 2012). ...
... Παρά ταύτα, οι ερευνήτριες καταληκτικά επισημαίνουν ότι μόνο ένα μικρό ποσοστό της μεταβλητότητας των τιμών, όπως και στη παρούσα μελέτη, ερμηνεύτηκε από το παράγοντα φύλο. Κατά συνέπεια, εκτός από το φύλο υπάρχουν και άλλοι γλωσσικοί και γνωστικοί παράγοντες οι οποίοι διαδραματίζουν σημαντικό ρόλο (Lepola et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Σκοπός της παρούσας εργασίας ήταν να εξετάσει την επίδραση ενός προγράμματος παρέμβασης για την ενίσχυση της κατανόησης του δομικού πλαισίου των ιστοριών σε παιδιά προσχολικής ηλικίας με στόχο την παραγωγή φανταστικών ιστοριών. Το δείγμα της μελέτης αποτέλεσαν 56 παιδιά ηλικίας 5- 6 ετών τα οποία φοιτούσαν σε 5 τάξεις νηπιαγωγείων της πόλης των Χανίων στην Κρήτη. Τα παιδιά του δείγματος χωρίστηκαν σε δύο ομάδες, μία πειραματική και μία ομάδα ελέγχου. Το πρόγραμμα εφαρμόστηκε μία φορά την εβδομάδα για 8 εβδομάδες. Στα παιδιά της πειραματικής ομάδας διαβάστηκαν 8 βιβλία με αφηγηματικά κείμενα. Σε κάθε συνεδρία, μετά την ανάγνωση του βιβλίου, εφαρμόστηκε μια πολυεπίπεδη στρατηγική μέσω της οποίας τα παιδιά εκπαιδεύτηκαν στην κατανόηση της δομής της ιστορίας και στην παραγωγή φανταστικών ιστοριών. Στα παιδιά της ομάδας ελέγχου διαβάστηκαν τα ίδια 8 βιβλία χωρίς να εφαρμοστεί κάποιο συγκεκριμένο πρόγραμμα ενίσχυσης του δομικού πλαισίου των ιστοριών. Τα αποτελέσματα έδειξαν ότι η στρατηγική που εφαρμόστηκε στο πρόγραμμα παρέμβασης βελτίωσε σημαντικά την ικανότητα των παιδιών της πειραματικής ομάδας να κατανοήσουν τα δομικά στοιχεία των ιστοριών και να παράγουν πιο οργανωμένες φανταστικές ιστορίες από ότι τα παιδιά της ομάδας ελέγχου. Επιπλέον, τη μεγαλύτερη βελτίωση στην παραγωγή φανταστικών ιστοριών την παρουσίασαν τα παιδιά που είχαν χαμηλό βαθμό επίδοσης στα αφηγηματικά κριτήρια. Τέλος, η βελτίωση των παιδιών διατηρήθηκε και μετά την είσοδό τους στο δημοτικό σχολείο. Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine the impact of an intervention program on kindergarten children’s understanding of story structural elements and their ability to narrate fictional stories. The sample consisted of 56 children aged 5-6 from five kindergarten classrooms in Chania, Crete, Greece. The sample was divided into one experimental and one control group. The intervention program lasted eight weeks. During the program eight illustrated story books were read to the whole class, one book a week. After the story book reading children in the experimental group were trained in narrating fictional stories using a multileveled instructional strategy. In the control group children were read the same eight story books without any specific program to enhance their narratives skills. The results showed that training children in narrating fictional stories helped them assimilate the story structure and apply it effectively when asked to create a fictional story. In addition, children in the experimental group had better performance in all narrative skills than those in the control group. The results showed that the biggest improvement in all narrative skills was among children having low pre-test scores in narrating fictional stories. Finally, children’s improved performance in narrating fictional stories remained after they entered primary school.
... These skills function at two distinct levels: foundational skills, encompassing lexical and syntactic knowledge, working memory, and attention, and higher-level skills such as inferential reasoning, theory of mind, and comprehension monitoring (Kim, 2016;Perfetti et al., 2005). Research findings from English-speaking children's reading comprehension support these theoretical models, demonstrating that higherlevel cognitive abilities contribute to children's text comprehension concurrently and predictively (Cain & Oakhill, 1999;Lepola et al., 2012;Strasser, K., & del Rio, F., 2014). Receptive vocabulary and syntactic knowledge significantly impact reading comprehension at a different level (Kim, 2016;Lepola et al., 2012). ...
... Research findings from English-speaking children's reading comprehension support these theoretical models, demonstrating that higherlevel cognitive abilities contribute to children's text comprehension concurrently and predictively (Cain & Oakhill, 1999;Lepola et al., 2012;Strasser, K., & del Rio, F., 2014). Receptive vocabulary and syntactic knowledge significantly impact reading comprehension at a different level (Kim, 2016;Lepola et al., 2012). ...
... In the absence of adequate support from grammatical knowledge, children in the DLD group may rely primarily on their vocabulary knowledge to understand the text. This reliance could potentially be further aided by their inferencing skills (Lepola,et al., 2012). This threshold account can also help explain the differences between our findings in the TD group and those of Fong and Ho (2017). ...
... Although the importance of listening narrative comprehension from a preschool age has been recognized as the best predictor of subsequent reading comprehension [5], most studies adopting a multicomponent approach focus on reading comprehension in schoolaged children [6,7]. However, recently this model has been partially tested for listening narrative comprehension in preschool children [8][9][10][11][12][13]. A growing body of research has investigated how monolingual children, from preschool to school age, use different language and cognitive skills in listening narrative comprehension [10][11][12][14][15][16]. ...
... Previous studies, see ref. [19], showed that receptive word knowledge in preschoolers accounted for 4% of unique variance in reading comprehension when they were in third grade. Several longitudinal studies found that measures of receptive word knowledge directly predicted reading and listening comprehension, over and above the autoregressor and other components, in primary school children [20][21][22][23][24]. Thus, it may be argued that vocabulary appears as a core language ability for successful listening narrative comprehension in preschool and school-aged children [13]. Additionally, Meara argued that vocabulary size is the fundamental competence for acquiring lexical competence and emphasized that children with larger vocabularies are more proficient language users, understand more and produce more complex oral narratives than children with smaller vocabularies [25]. ...
... These results have been confirmed by a recent study by Valentini and Serratrice [17] according to which vocabulary and morphological knowledge were the most significant predictors of English listening comprehension (L2) in bilingual children in the first two years of formal schooling. These skills specifically determined the children's listening comprehension but not their growth in listening narrative comprehension abilities over time, as is also the case for monolinguals [13]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Vocabulary is the key component of listening narrative comprehension, but its contribution has been scarcely investigated in bilingual children. This study aimed to examine (a) listening narrative comprehension and receptive vocabulary in L1 (Italian) and L2 (English) in preschool and first grade children; (b) the specific contribution of receptive vocabulary to listening narrative comprehension in both languages. Participants were 30 preschoolers and 32 first graders, who are all Italian children attending an international school in English. In both languages, receptive vocabulary was assessed through PPVT-R and listening narrative comprehension through TOR 3-8. The results showed that listening narrative comprehension was age appropriate in both languages but higher in L1. Lower vocabulary in L1 than L2 was found, and this difference is higher for preschoolers than for first grades; finally, two regressions performed on listening narrative comprehension in each language showed that only vocabulary in the same language accounted for listening narrative comprehension. Children obtain higher performance in L1; however, after a few years of L2 exposure in the educational context, language skills fall within the normal range, with some weakness in vocabulary. Vocabulary contribution to listening narrative comprehension is similar in both languages and specific for each.
... According to the results, statistically significant developmental differences were found in children's performance across all oral language tasks except for MLU-word and subordinate clauses, with the older children performing better than the younger age groups (Hipfner-Boucher et al. 2014;Lepola et al. 2012;Westerveld & Gillon 2010). Older children also tended to have higher performance across the number of sentences, number of different words and the number of total words than the younger ones. ...
... Boys and girls had similar narrative measurements in all the variables. This is in line with Hipfner-Boucheretal et al. (2014), Lepola et al. (2012), Fivush et al. (2012. Therefore, it is concluded that there are other variables that affect children's performance in the two narrative skills, such as language and cognitive skills (Allen et al., 2012;Bruner, 1986). ...
... Future studies have to measure gender differences as understanding oral language abilities between boys and girls could benefit clinicians and educators in identifying children with language impairment. Previous research data supports that gender differences in language skills are so small that they can be considered non-existent (Hipfner-Boucher et al., 2014;Hyde & Linn, 1988;Lepola et al., 2012). Specifically, Hyde and Linn (1988) analyzed 165 studies that evaluated the linguistic abilities of children and adults with various linguistic tasks. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: Oral narratives are crucial for evaluating youngsters since they reveal a plethora of information about young children's language development. This study aimed to investigate the development of narrative productivity, lexical diversity, syntactic complexity and in Cypriot-Greek-speaking children during preschool and primary school years. An additional aim was to measure the effect of the children’s gender in their performance on the measures of narratives. Methods: One hundred and twenty-nine typically developing Cypriot-Greek-speaking children were recruited for the study. Results: The results showed that the linguistic structure of narrations reflects a subtle developmental trend. As expected, older children tended to produce longer and syntactically more complex stories than the younger ones, which is in line with previous studies. There were significant differences in all productivity measures between the four-year-olds and those aged seven and ten years old. In addition, the results illustrated that there was not any significant effect of gender in the measures of narrative skills. Specifically, gender did not affect the children’s performance in the sample. Boys and girls had similar narrative measurements in all the variables. Conclusions: This study aims to provide initial evidence in retelling narrative development patterns on typically developing Cypriot-Greek speaking children that could be used by speech and language to compare children’s performance and detect language disorders.
... Successful passage comprehension relies on the ability to understand and recall the specific passage content and, typically, to provide correct responses afterward (Kim, 2016). Therefore, a passage comprehension task taps into different language domains, such as lexicon, grammar, phonology, and pragmatics, and require the integration of linguistic skills with previous experience and knowledge about the surrounding world to reach understanding (Bishop, 2014;Dochy et al., 1999;Lepola et al., 2012;Paul & Norbury, 2012). Bilingual children's knowledge and proficiency in the school language, the second language (L2), may be age inadequate due to different factors, that is, due to limited exposure to an L2 or due to a language disorder. ...
... When assessing passage comprehension in younger children, the narrative genre is usually chosen. Children are usually exposed to narratives from a very young age, both from watching children's television shows and from talking about daily events as well as through shared book reading, either at preschool or in the home environment (Bishop, 2014;Lepola et al., 2012). This indicates that the macrostructure of a narrative is familiar to children long before they start school. ...
... Passage comprehension performance requires skills in different language domains such as lexicon, grammar, and pragmatics. Linguistic skills have to be integrated with previous experience and knowledge about the surrounding world for the child to reach understanding (Bishop, 2014;Dochy et al., 1999;Lepola et al., 2012;Paul & Norbury, 2012). When the school language represents the L2, prior experience of, exposure to, and knowledge of that language may be reduced, which, in turn, may result in poorer understanding. ...
Article
Purpose This study reports on the development of an auditory passage comprehension task for Swedish primary school children of cultural and linguistic diversity. It also reports on their performance on the task in quiet and in noise. Method Eighty-eight children aged 7–9 years and showing normal hearing participated. The children were divided into three groups based on presumed language exposure: 13 children were categorized as Swedish-speaking monolinguals, 19 children were categorized as simultaneous bilinguals, and 56 children were categorized as sequential bilinguals. No significant difference in working memory capacity was seen between the three language groups. Two passages and associated multiple-choice questions were developed. During development of the passage comprehension task, steps were taken to reduce the impact of culture-specific prior experience and knowledge on performance. This was achieved by using the story grammar principles, universal topics and plots, and simple language that avoided complex or unusual grammatical structures and words. Results The findings indicate no significant difference between the two passages and similar response distributions. Passage comprehension performance was significantly better in quiet than in noise, regardless of language exposure group. The monolinguals outperformed both simultaneous and sequential bilinguals in both listening conditions. Conclusions Because the task was designed to minimize the effect of cultural knowledge on auditory passage comprehension, this suggests that compared with monolinguals, both simultaneous and sequential bilinguals have a disadvantage in auditory passage comprehension. As expected, the findings demonstrate that noise has a negative effect on auditory passage comprehension. The magnitude of this effect does not relate to language exposure. The developed auditory passage comprehension task seems suitable for assessing auditory passage comprehension in primary school children of linguistic and cultural diversity.
... al. 2008;Roch and Hržica 2020). It has also been found that both receptive and expressive vocabulary are correlated with story retelling in preschool children (Jalongo and Sobolak 2011;Lepola et al. 2012). However, most of the previous studies have used mainly a single measure for assessing vocabulary, such as a multiple-choice task, or a word definition or word naming tasks. ...
... According to the results, statistically significant developmental differences were found in children's performance across all oral language tasks, with the older children performing better in comparison to the younger age groups (Hipfner-Boucher et al. 2014;Lepola et al. 2012;Westerveld and Gillon 2010). Older children also tended to have higher performance across all the story retelling microstructure and macrostructure criteria than the younger ones. ...
... In sum, the concurrent "reading" of the three models, demonstrates that the role of vocabulary skills for story retelling is critical and very stable across all the three age groups (Lepola et al. 2012). Additionally, a closer look at the three models together highlights that as the children get older a new language component is being added in the model as another contributor to their ability to retell a story, probably showing a developmental trend. ...
Article
Oral language and narrative skills constitute very critical factors for children’s academic performance and social competence. The aim of the present study was to investigate the developmental patterns of story retelling, as well as the relationship between oral language and story retelling in preschool and primary school children. Two hundred and thirty-seven Greek-speaking children (4–5, 5–6 and 6–7 years old) participated in the study. Vocabulary knowledge, phonological awareness, morphological awareness skills and pragmatics were examined through a standardized psychometric test. Story retelling was measured by inviting the children to listen to a story and then retell it. Children’s narratives were evaluated according to microstructure (use of conjunctions and lexical cohesion) and macrostructure (story grammar and temporal sequencing) criteria. The results showed that children performed better as they got older across all the oral language and story retelling tasks. Structural equation modeling revealed that vocabulary skills stand out as a stable predictor across all the three age groups. A new finding was also demonstrated, highlighting that morphological awareness, phonological awareness skills and pragmatics work together with vocabulary skills in diverse patterns at different points of a child’s development, in order to support his/her ability to retell a story.
... Narratives surround children from their earliest language experiences [1][2][3]. Young children experience narratives through shared book reading and participation in talks about daily events. Children as young as two to three years old develop a rich repertoire of knowledge about narratives and use that to narrate their needs, desires, plans, to understand, and respond to others' demands, requests, needs, and emotional reactions [4]. ...
... Results provide piecemeal evidence and scarce information about structural relations among language and cognitive skills involved [23]. We still have much to understand about how language and cognitive skills influence each other, and how they become integrated to produce successful narrative comprehension [3]. ...
... When children listen to a story, to understand adequately, they must be able to draw spontaneously appropriate inferences [53]. Inferential ability refers to the ability to integrate explicit contents with previous knowledge to derive meaning that is not explicitly stated in the text [3]. The ability to generate inferences has been found to contribute to young children's ability to understand literal as well as inferred meaning, leading to better listening texts comprehension [33,41,54]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Listening narrative comprehension, according to the theoretical framework of the multicomponent model for comprehension, involves numerous skills that interact dynamically between each other and have the potential to give rise to individual differences in comprehension. The purpose of the current work was to define a comprehensive and complete multicomponent model of listening narrative comprehension in preschool age. We investigated how variation in Length of Exposure to majority Language (i.e., how long children have been exposed to the Italian language), lower-order cognitive (WM, inhibitory control, attention shifting), language skills (receptive vocabulary, syntactic knowledge, rapid naming), and higher-order cognitive skills (inferences, TOM, knowledge of story-structure) are related to listening narrative comprehension in Italian of 111 preschool children (Mage = 61 months; SD = 6.8) growing in a monolingual or multilingual context. Structural equation modeling results showed that the model explained 60% variance in listening narrative comprehension in Italian of children aged four to six and predicted the outcome both through direct and mediated paths, coherently with the multicomponent model of comprehension.
... Este resultado es coincidente con una gran cantidad de investigaciones del área. La relación entre la edad y las medidas de inferencia y comprensión es coincidente con estudios propios (Barreyro et al., 2020), pero también con trabajos de diferentes países y en distintas lenguas (Ahmed et al., 2016;Cain & Oakhill, 2011;Fonseca et al., 2014;Kendeou et al., 2012;Lepola et al., 2012). El mismo patrón de la relación entre la edad y el vocabulario está en metaanálisis (Sterpin et al., 2021), como también en otro tipo de investigaciones que han utilizado diferentes medidas de vocabulario (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989;. ...
... El resultado es interesante, ya que la profundidad del vocabulario se vincula con el conocimiento que un sujeto tiene respecto del significado de las palabras, sus usos posibles y de relaciones conceptuales y esto guía y tiene incidencia en las habilidades de comprensión. Nuestros resultados son similares a los de otras investigaciones que muestran que el desarrollo del vocabulario acompaña y cumple un rol crucial en la habilidad de comprender un texto, ya sea escrito o narrado (Cain & Bignell, 2014;Currie & Cain, 2015;Daugaard et al., 2017;Kim, 2017;Lepola et al., 2012;Oakhill & Cain, 2018;Silva & Cain, 2015;Sterpin et al., 2021). También a los modelos teóricos de comprensión; por ejemplo, de acuerdo con el Modelo de Construcción-Integración de Kintsch (1998), todos los aspectos vinculados al conocimiento previo, en este caso el vocabulario, juegan un papel crítico en la comprensión del texto puesto que es vital para construir el modelo mental del texto, dado que entender el significado de las palabras y los conceptos permite establecer conexiones entre las diferentes partes del texto y relacionar lo leído con el conocimiento almacenado en la memoria. ...
Article
Full-text available
El propósito del presente trabajo consistió en estudiar el papel de la generación de inferencias y del conocimiento del vocabulario (vinculado a la dimensión de profundidad) en la comprensión literal de narraciones en niños preescolares de tres a seis años. Con este propósito se administró una prueba de vocabulario, se narraron tres cuentos a los niños, y se evaluó la generación de inferencias y la comprensión literal de las narraciones. Los resultados indicaron relaciones significativas entre el vocabulario, la generación de inferencias y la comprensión de las narraciones. Al llevar a cabo un análisis de regresión se observó un efecto de interacción entre el vocabulario y la generación de inferencias. Al analizar dicha interacción se observó que los niños con menor vocabulario dependían de sus habilidades de generación de inferencias para poder comprender, y que los niños con menor capacidad para generar inferencias dependían del conocimiento del vocabulario para lograr comprender la narración.
... During narration children use a more sophisticated, decontextualised language, utilising advanced vocabulary and syntax, since they are sharing a non-common experience removed from referents of time and environmental context (Miller 2016). Recent research has also shown a strong relationship between cognitive functions and narrative skills, as children's narrative level reflects their linguistic, cognitive and broader social level (Piasta et al. 2018;Lepola et al. 2012). Therefore, it is crucial to enhance children's narrative skills during their early years. ...
... Understanding is a complex mental process in which many factors such as learning motives, reading condition, children's activation, visual aids, organisation of the text and background knowledge activation of story schemata (the representations that a child has about the way that stories are shaped), play an important role in meaning construction (Lepola et al. 2012;Moschovaki and Meadows 2005). Following a sociocultural perspective, a child and a more competent adult or sibling engaged in a collaborative process whereby the more competent person provides a supportive structure facilitates children's learning of new skills through a form of apprenticeship (Rogoff 1990). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examined whether particular practices in story retelling improve children’s comprehension of story structure and enables them to further comment on the story content. Eighty-three (83) kindergarten children (M = 5.4) years old composed the experimental (N = 43) and the control (N = 40) groups. For 6 weeks, one time per week, six well-structured books were read to the experimental group as follows: Before reading there was a brief discussion, while after reading children were trained in story retelling. The intervention programme in retelling consisted of five levels, which began from total teacher assistance to the point where children were able to retell the story freely and completely. The same six books were simply read to the control group and after the reading the children discussed the interesting parts and made drawings based on the story. Children’s retellings were taped and analysed by a series of measures. Results confirmed the experimental group children’s achievements in almost all measurements indicating that the multilevel training programme in retelling enhanced children’s ability for deeper comprehension and further commentary.
... Working memory is a short-term memory system that supports the simultaneous processing of information, allowing the listener to retain previously received information while processing new input, and enabling the integration of both (Baddeley et al., 2020;Just & Carpenter, 1992). Regarding the relationship between comprehension and working memory, studies with individuals of different ages and across discourse modalities show that working memory is a reliable predictor of text comprehension (Currie & Cain, 2015;Currie & Muijselaar, 2019;Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005;Swanson, 2011) and listening comprehension in preschool children (Lepola et al., 2012;Strasser & Río, 2014). Studies conducted by Florit et al. (2009; demonstrated that short-term memory and working memory skills significantly contribute to oral comprehension in preschoolers aged 4 and 5, even after controlling for verbal abilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
El objetivo de este estudio fue analizar el papel del conocimiento de vocabulario, la memoria operativa y la velocidad de procesamiento en la comprensión de textos orales en niños hispanohablantes de 3 a 6 años. Un total de 165 niños (55.76% niñas; M = 4.64 años; DT = 1.07) participaron en el estudio. Los participantes escucharon tres textos narrativos y respondieron a preguntas sobre su contenido literal e inferencial, realizaron tareas de memoria operativa, una tarea de velocidad de procesamiento y una prueba de vocabulario. Los análisis de varianza revelaron diferencias en función de la edad en todas las tareas, con mejores desempeños en los mayores. Los análisis de correlación mostraron asociaciones entre comprensión, vocabulario y memoria operativa, sugiriendo que estas capacidades cognitivas contribuyen a la comprensión de narraciones orales. Los análisis de senderos indicaron que el impacto de la edad en la comprensión está mediado por el vocabulario y la memoria operativa verbal, mientras que la velocidad de procesamiento no intervino significativamente en la comprensión. Los resultados destacan la importancia de apoyar el desarrollo del vocabulario y las habilidades de memoria operativa en la educación temprana, ya que son fundamentales para mejorar las habilidades de comprensión. En conclusión, el estudio revela que las diferencias individuales en la comprensión oral están principalmente ligadas al conocimiento del vocabulario y a la capacidad de la memoria operativa, mientras que la velocidad de procesamiento desempeña un papel secundario.
... Working memory is a short-term memory system that supports the simultaneous processing of information, allowing the listener to retain previously received information while processing new input, and enabling the integration of both (Baddeley et al., 2020;Just & Carpenter, 1992). Regarding the relationship between comprehension and working memory, studies with individuals of different ages and across discourse modalities show that working memory is a reliable predictor of text comprehension (Currie & Cain, 2015;Currie & Muijselaar, 2019;Seigneuric & Ehrlich, 2005;Swanson, 2011) and listening comprehension in preschool children (Lepola et al., 2012;Strasser & Río, 2014). Studies conducted by Florit et al. (2009; demonstrated that short-term memory and working memory skills significantly contribute to oral comprehension in preschoolers aged 4 and 5, even after controlling for verbal abilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to analyze the role of vocabulary knowledge, working memory, and processing speed in the listening comprehension of oral texts in 3- to 6-year-old Spanish-speaking children. A total of 165 children (55.76% girls; M = 4.64 years; SD = 1.07) participated in the study. Participants listened to three narrative texts and answered questions about their literal and inferential content, performed working memory tasks, a processing speed task, and a vocabulary test. ANOVA results revealed significant age-related differences across all tasks, with older children performing better than younger ones. Correlation analyses showed associations between comprehension, vocabulary, and working memory; the results suggest that these cognitive abilities are key contributors to oral narrative comprehension. The path analyses fulfilled indicated that the impact of age on comprehension is mediated by vocabulary and verbal working memory, while processing speed did not significantly influence comprehension. The results highlight the importance of supporting vocabulary development and working memory skills in early childhood education, as these are critical to enhancing comprehension abilities. In conclusion, the study demonstrates that individual differences in oral comprehension are primarily driven by vocabulary knowledge and working memory capacity, while processing speed plays a secondary role.
... Specifically, as discussed in Nicolopoulou and Richner (2007), boys and girls in their preschool years have a different apprehension of reality and therefore employ different narrative routes in the stories they produce (Buckner & Fivush, 1998;Nicolopoulou & Ilgaz, 2006). However, the effect of gender is not a given, as some studies indicate a difference in the structure and content of a narrative between the sexes (Ross & Holmberg, 1990), while others do not (Lepola, Lynch, Laakkonen, Silvén, & Niemi, 2012). Meier (2020) in her study tried to identify how preschoolers produce narratives and examined whether or not narrative skills develop differentially for girls than boys across time. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of an intervention program aimed to enhance pre-schoolers' ability to create fictional narratives. The sample consisted of 92 children, ages 4-6. The children from the treatment condition were trained through a multilevel intervention programme to produce original fictional stories. The intervention program lasted for eight weeks. During the intervention an instructional strategy was carried out on five levels (creating prior knowledge, discussion, modelling, monitoring the process and producing stories). The children in the control group after the book readings implemented in a discussion about the interesting parts of the stories. The results showed that the intervention programme significantly affected on pre-schoolers' ability to understand the structural elements of a story and to produce comprehensible and well organised fictional stories.
... 2016). Research evidence shows that vocabulary is significantly related to inference ability, listening comprehension, and reading comprehension (Lepola et al., 2012;Cain & Oakhill, 2014;Daugaard et al., 2017). In the constantly evolving field of education, the integration of games such as VSC could provide a potential avenue for transforming traditional classrooms into vibrant centres of active learning, thereby enriching students' engagement in vocabulary learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
for validation to ensure that they were appropriate for the topic and students' level of competency. They were also examined for face validity by an expert in test and measurement from the same university. The same test used as pre-test was reshuffled and used as post-test. The paired sample t-test was used to test the hypotheses at 0.05 level of significance. The findings of the study showed that Digital game, Vocabulary Spelling City (CSV), was very effective for teaching English vocabulary, and that gender is not a predictor of achievement with regard to the use of digital game in English vocabulary learning. Based on these findings, it was recommended, among others, that traditional classroom vocabulary instructions should be modified to provide contexts and examples directly applicable to the goals of vocabulary teaching using digital games, which may therefore promote a deeper understanding and retention of new concepts.
... Cabe también destacar que sería interesante realizar nuevos estudios que se enfoquen en la generación de inferencias causales durante la comprensión del discurso hablado por parte de estudiantes de distintas edades y niveles educativos. Es decir, realizar investigaciones que continúen explorando si la capacidad de generar inferencias causales se desarrolla, como ha sido hallado por investigaciones acerca del discurso escrito (Ackerman, Silver, & Glickman, 1990;Kendeou, Bohn-Gettler, White & van den Broek, 2008;Pérez López, 1990;van den Broek, 1997) e investigaciones preliminares acerca del discurso hablado (Kendeou, van den Broek, White, & Lynch, 2009;Lepola, Lynch, Laakonen, Silven & Niemi, 2012). Por último, la presentación de otros materiales (de distintas disciplinas, dis-"… se ha observado que las oraciones que dan lugar a la generación de una mayor cantidad de inferencias conectivas y de restablecimiento en la comprensión de materiales de discurso expositivo, facilitan el recuerdo y reconocimiento de enunciados…" tintos géneros, etc.), otras tareas (tales como rastreo de movimientos oculares, monitoreo de palabras, pensamiento en voz alta, detección de errores del habla) y la consideración del efecto de otras diferencias individuales (motivación, inteligencia) nos permitirán profundizar nuestra comprensión acerca del rol que la generación de estas inferencias tiene en la comprensión del discurso hablado. ...
Article
El objetivo de este artículo es hacer un recorrido por una selección de temas que han sido explorados en relación con la generación de inferencias causales en la comprensión del discurso escrito, a fin de presentar estudios preliminares que han comenzado a indagar acerca de su rol en la comprensión del discurso hablado. Con este fin, se presentan resultados de estudios que se han enfocado en la generación de inferencias consideradas necesarias y opcionales, haciendo énfasis en la generación de inferencias que involucran el establecimiento de conexiones causales entre enunciados en la comprensión del discurso escrito. A su vez, se presentan estudios que han comenzado a poner a prueba el rol de la generación de inferencias causales en la comprensión del discurso hablado.
... listening comprehension and have been found to be a unique predictor of later listening comprehension beyond vocabulary and prior listening comprehension skills(Lepola et al. 2012). It is of note, drawing inferences requires applying background knowledge (i.e. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This dissertation explores early (grade 1) reading acquisition of Ukrainian-Polish bilingual children from the psycholinguistic perspective. It focuses on identifying predictors of individual differences in emerging reading skills. Here, the bilingual learners are compared against their monolingual (Polish-speaking and Ukrainian- speaking) peers, to identify potential differences between the mono- and bilingual patterns of learning.
... Počúvanie naratívneho textu s porozumením je dôležitým predchodcom čítania naratívneho textu s porozumením (Lepola et al., 2012). 65 Deti, ktoré slabo porozumejú počúvanému textu, pravdepodobne slabo porozumejú aj čítanému textu (Stothard & Hulme, 1992;Cain et al., 2000). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Online: https://www.pulib.sk/web/kniznica/elpub/dokument/Liptakova3 _____________________________________________________________________________________ In: Liptáková, Ľ., ed. Výskum porozumenia textu v mladšom školskom veku. Prešov: Prešovská univerzita v Prešove, pp. 145–199. ISBN 978-80-555-3206-6 (print); 978-80-555-3268-4 (online) _____________________________________________________________________________________ V tejto kapitole predstavíme výsledky analýzy vytvárania inferencií pri počúvaní a pri čítaní naratívneho textu u žiakov 3. ročníka základnej školy. Snažíme sa teda zodpovedať sedem výskumných otázok deskriptívneho typu: (1) Ako participanti vytvárajú online inferencie pri počúvaní/čítaní naratívneho textu? (2) Ako participanti referujú o základnom porozumení počúvaného/čítaného naratívneho textu prostredníctvom metódy retellingu? (3) Ako participanti pri počúvaní/čítaní naratívneho textu vytvárajú offline inferencie koherentného a interogatívneho typu? (4) Akú úlohu zohráva pri vytváraní inferencií metóda odstupňovaných nápovedí? (5) Aké inferenčné profily detí možno pri počúvaní/čítaní naratívneho textu pozorovať? (6) Aké ďalšie komponenty porozumenia textu zohrávajú úlohu pri vytváraní inferencií? (7) Čo ukazuje porovnanie recepcie počúvaného a čítaného naratívneho textu _____________________________________________________________________________________ In this chapter, we will present the results of the analysis of the creation of inferences when listening and reading a narrative text by students of the 3rd grade of elementary school. Thus, we try to answer seven research questions of a descriptive type: (1) How do participants make online inferences while listening/reading a narrative text? (2) How do the participants report on the basic understanding of the listened/read narrative text through the retelling method? (3) How do participants create offline inferences of coherent and interrogative type while listening/reading a narrative text? (4) What role does the graded hint method play in making inferences? (5) What inferential profiles of children can be observed when listening/reading a narrative text? (6) What other components of text comprehension play a role in making inferences? (7) What does the comparison of the reception of the listened and read narrative text show?
... Research Ganguli and Roy (2010) states that. Individual response mechanisms can be attributed to service quality using CMR theory (Sathye, 1999;Aladwani, 2006;(Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Laakkonen, E., Silvén, M., & Niemi, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
The development of banking services through electronic media was increasingly widespread along with the rapid growth of information technology. Technological developments through digital transformation in the banking system require banking services in Indonesia to switch to e-banking services. E-banking is a form of service innovation by utilizing technological developments. E-banking services that are currently developing rapidly are mobile banking and internet banking. The increase in mobile banking and internet banking transactions in Indonesia has increased by more than 300% from 2016 to August 2021. One of the banking companies that has the largest number of e-banking users in Indonesia is BCA. With the rapid development of e-banking services, it is necessary to evaluate the quality of e-banking services including reliability, privacy and security, website design and customer service and support. Therefore, this study aims to determine impact of the quality of BCA e-banking services on customer satisfaction (e-satisfaction) and loyalty (e-loyalty). The sample used in this study was 230 data taken by purposive sampling. The data obtained were analyzed using structural equation model analysis (SEM) with the help of the AMOS 24 application. The results showed that there was a significant impact between reliability, website design and customer service and support on e-satisfaction. Meanwhile privacy and security have a significant impact on e-loyalty. In addition, this study also found that there was a significant impact of e-satisfaction on e-loyalty.
... These connections enable the child to answer comprehension questions and reinforce the encoding of the story content in the child's long-term memory. As suggested by this and previous studies, the formation of conceptual connections and coherent representation is facilitated (among others) by assignment of correct context-based meanings to individual words (e.g., Lepola et al., 2020;Strasser & Río, 2014) and generation of extra-textual information implied by the explicit ideas mentioned in the story (e.g., Lepola et al., 2012;Tompkins et al., 2013). The present findings further suggest that kindergarteners have more difficulties executing these vital processes by themselves, and therefore benefit from external intervention, produced digitally in this case, which provides word meanings and inferential information required for coherence and comprehension (e.g., Kendeou et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Numerous studies have examined the positive and negative effects of various types of interactions that occur while children view electronic book (e-book) stories. However, the effects of the different types of interactions have not been compared, and more importantly, the optimal amount and reoccurrence of these interactions on children has not been explored. The present study was designed to fill this gap by examining the effect of amount, type and reoccurrence of e-book interactions on kindergarteners’ new word learning and story comprehension. To do so, 72 children aged 5–6 year-old viewed three picture e-book stories in one session read by digital narrators, and then repeated this procedure in a second session. Each story included a word clarification or a story elaboration type of interaction, which occurred at low (every two screens), medium (every screen), or high frequency (more than once on most screens) during viewing. Following each story, participants were asked to define target words and answer comprehension questions. Results showed that increasing the amount of interactions, whether word clarifications or elaborations, enhanced word learning and story comprehension following the second exposure to the e-book stories. These findings suggest that interruptions during e-book viewing to explain difficult words or expand the plot by inferential information, do not interfere with the narrative course and do not impair learning processes, at least not in the amounts of interactions examined in this study. These conclusions bear practical implications for the design of e-books, and more generally, for the interactive approach adults may adopt when reading a story to children.
... In addition, researchers have argued that the ability to draw inferences from spoken texts (i.e., shared bookreading) is a critical component for the development of listening comprehension (Lerväg et al., 2018). This line of research finds that preschoolers' inference-making abilities make a unique contribution above and beyond vocabulary to their later listening comprehension abilities (Florit et al., 2011;Lepola et al., 2012) and that weak inferencing skills are a significant source of reading comprehension difficulties (Cain & Oakhill, 2009;Oakhill & Cain, 2007). Given concerns for disparities in reading proficiency in elementary and middle grades, especially across income backgrounds (NCES, 2015), it has been argued that inference skills should be attended to at the earliest ages (van Kleeck, 2006;Wright, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study focuses on the relationship between teacher–text–child interactions during shared picturebook reading and its effect on children’s narrative comprehension. Using a micro-analytic approach, we studied nine large-group bookreading sessions of one experienced teacher and her classroom of 17 preschoolers from low-income backgrounds over a school year. We coded the bookreading interactions according to the level of analytic rigor demanded by the questions the teacher asked (literal, simple inferential, complex inferential), the children's responses to these questions (incorrect, adequate, correct), and the teacher's follow-up responses (evaluative, elaborative), including additional follow-up questions and prompts elicited by the children's responses. We examined whether picturebook difficulty (low, medium, high) was related to the demand level of the teacher's questions and the children's responses. We also examined whether children’s participation in these interactions was associated with children’s narrative comprehension, which was tested at the beginning and end of the school year. Overall, the teacher consistently asked a higher proportion of inferential (80%) than literal questions (20%). Medium-difficulty level books elicited more complex inferential questions than easier or harder books. Children correctly answered such questions from medium-difficulty books more often than easy or difficult ones. Increases in children's narrative comprehension from pretest to posttests were related to the children’s frequency of active participation and the accuracy of their responses. These results have implications for how picturebook choice and type of discussion can promote narrative comprehension among preschoolers from low-income backgrounds.
... With respect to meaningrelated responses, Silva and Cain (2015) showed among 5-6-year-olds how literal and inferential comprehension of a story mediated the effect of prior vocabulary to later reading comprehension. In addition, inference skills have been found to facilitate the development of children's memory for the narrative text (Lepola et al., 2012). Also, studies both among older and younger students, have shown that the amount and level of children's verbal participation affects their literacy skills. ...
Article
Research Findings: The present study examined children's listening comprehension during a two-semester-long dialogic reading. We explored the extent to which the development of children's listening comprehension was accounted for by initial listening comprehension and whether children's engagement and the amount and level of their verbal participation in shared reading contributed to their later listening comprehension. The opportunities provided by teachers, such as closed and open-ended questions were also examined. A total of 60 five-year-old children participated in 15 story groups. Children's listening comprehension was evaluated twice. Teachers rated the children's behavioral and cognitive engagement. Children's verbal participation and the teachers' questions were observed directly. A theoretical model involving direct and indirect pathways in the development of listening comprehension was analyzed. Path modeling showed that the children's cognitive engagement and the amount of verbal participation mediated the development of individual differences in listening comprehension. Additionally, teachers' open-ended questions contributed indirectly to listening comprehension via children's verbal participation. Practice or Policy: The findings highlight the benefits of children's active participation in discussions and talk-intensive reading aloud for their story comprehension.
... More recently, some studies have opted for a combination of language measures (e.g., Lervåg et al., 2018) and this might also be a way towards capturing a more complex language comprehension profile. In addition, we have not included other independent measures previously shown to affect RC, such as verbal working memory (e.g., Lepola et al., 2012), and these could be incorporated into future studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reading comprehension (RC) is a multi-faceted construct but is often assessed with a single instrument. Previous research has highlighted that commonly used RC tests are only mildly correlated and vary in the skills they assess, including the differential contribution of oral language and decoding to children’s performance. Our study, framed within the Simple View of Reading model, examined the contribution of underlying component skills for multiple RC measures and evaluated whether the contribution of decoding and oral language skills changes according to the RC test used and developmental level. Two hundred Slovak-speaking children were assessed across two time points, using multiple RC tests and measures of decoding and oral language skills. The RC tests showed weak to moderate correlations, echoing findings from other languages. At the end of Year 1, the contribution of decoding and oral language to RC was similar in the Slovak transparent orthography. At the end of Year 2, the contribution of oral language had increased threefold, while decoding remained unchanged from Year 1. Crucially, there were also differences between the tests, with some more reliant on oral language. The results highlight the potential benefits of increasing understanding of the differential effects of the component skills in commonly used RC assessments as an aid to interpretation of children’s scores. Such an approach could not only identify children with poor scores, but also pinpoint where weaknesses lie in the underlying components so intervention targets could be formulated accordingly.
... Initially mobilised to understand oral language, inferential skills develop long before children learn to read (Filiatrault-Veilleux et al., 2016;Kendeou et al., 2008;Lepola et al., 2012) and contribute to vocabulary acquisition (Language and Reading Research Consortium, Currie, et al., 2019;Tomasello et al., 2007). The development of inferential skills based on listening language understanding will become also key to children's reading comprehension. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated young children's ability to draw psychological and nonpsychological inferences during reading comprehension. Whereas nonpsychological inferences require the retrieval of general background knowledge, psychological inferences rely on more contextualised knowledge relating to mental states. Based on several pretests, children, who were able to read fluently, aged 7-8 years (second graders; n = 42) and 8-9 years (third graders; n = 46) were assigned to either a skilled comprehenders group or a less skilled comprehenders group, based on their listening comprehension. They were then given short stories to read, followed by comprehension questions. Some questions required the drawing of psychological or nonpsychological inferences. Generalized linear mixed models revealed that (a) psychological inferences were more difficult to generate than nonpsychological inferences for all the children, skilled and less skilled comprehenders alike, (b) both types of inference skills (psychological and nonpsychological) were associated with similar improvements as grade level increased, and (c) less skilled comprehenders had greater difficulty than skilled comprehenders generating psychological and nonpsychological inferences. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
... These stages cover the age span from 2 -6 years of age, during which children go from the production of unconnected microstructures to complete narratives. By evaluating the retelling of stories by children aged 4 -6 years old, Hipfner-Boucher, Milburn, Weitzman, Greenberg, Pelletier and Girolametto (2014) [14] and Lepola et al. (2012) [15] realized that older children presented better narrative structure. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim The aims of this study is to explore the correlation between the Voice Disorder Index (VDI) score and acoustic and phonatory respiratory voice parameters in speech language pathology (SLP) students with and without self-perceived voice disorders. Methods The Voice Disorder Index (VDI) and the Lingwaves 3 “Voice Protocol” were administered to each participant in order to assess his/her self-perceived severity of his/her voice problem, as well as, acoustic and phonatory respiratory measurements. The correlation coefficients were used to evaluate and quantify the degree of a linear relationship between VDI scores and acoustic and phonatory respiratory voice measures in students with and without self-perceived voice disorders. Results Relatively strong, strong and strong positive correlations were revealed when comparing the VDI physical subscale with the Dysphonia Severity Index (r(11) = 0.56, P = 0.048), mean loudness when reading in English (r(11) = 0.70, P = 0.008) and mean loudness when reading in Greek (r(11) =0.69, P = 0.009) respectively. Conclusions SLP students may be more aware of the impact of their laryngeal discomfort and voice output characteristics on their daily activities that can predict certain acoustic parameters than the emotional and functional impact on their daily activities.
... In addition, we need to consider what is taught and how. Reading and language comprehension draw on a wide range of skills, both foundational lower-level language skills such as vocabulary and grammar and also higher-level knowledge and skills such as text structure, inference making, and comprehension monitoring (Cain et al., 2004;Kim, 2016;Lepola et al., 2012;Strasser & Río, 2014). Effective interventions need to influence children's processing of text for meaning (van den Broek et al., 2011); curricula and interventions that focus on just a single dimension, such as vocabulary, may be insufficient for developing the full range of knowledge and skills involved in constructing meaning from text, limiting the transfer of impacts to language and reading comprehension. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although substantial research has established how to teach word reading, the research base for teaching skills related to language and reading comprehension is more limited. We report a multistate experiment of a supplemental, whole-class, language-focused curriculum delivered in prekindergarten and kindergarten classrooms that was designed to improve children’s language comprehension and thereby support later reading comprehension. We randomly assigned 69 prekindergarten classrooms (n = 361 children) and 56 kindergarten classrooms (n = 328 children) to receive language-focused intervention or to a control condition. Children in intervention conditions experienced one of two instantiations of Let’s Know! (Let’s Know! Broad or Let’s Know! Deep) as implemented by their classroom teachers. Both instantiations provide four 30-min lessons per week of targeted instruction on key lower- and higher-level language skills over 25 weeks; the instantiations differ in the amount of practice afforded to particular skills. We measured children’s comprehension-related skills (target vocabulary, comprehension monitoring, understanding narrative text and story grammar, understanding expository text) via curriculum-aligned measures during the academic year and their vocabulary and language comprehension at pretest and posttest. Multilevel analyses showed similar effects for both instantiations, with Let’s Know! positively impacting some of the immediate tests of curriculum-aligned skills (vocabulary, comprehension monitoring, understanding of expository text) and also the posttest vocabulary outcome, but not standardized language comprehension outcomes; impacts on curriculum-aligned skills did not mediate effects on language comprehension outcomes. Results have implications for the Let’s Know! theory of change as well as continued research on supporting children’s language and comprehension skills.
... Although SLIC in autism has gained limited attention in the literature, ample research has documented inferential differences, suggesting that autistic children will also have difficulty processing context in idiomatic language (Freed & Cain, 2017;Kim, 2016;Lepola et al., 2012;Norbury, 2004;Potocki & Laval, 2019;Vulchanova et al., 2015). The literature concurs that linguistically supportive contexts facilitate idiom comprehension (Norbury, 2004;Potocki & Laval, 2019;Qualls et al., 2003); however, research considering the inferential abilities required to use supporting contexts in autism remains sparse. ...
Article
Purpose The aim of this review is to illuminate the connection between inferential skills and spoken language idiom comprehension (SLIC) with a focus on autism. Idioms are frequently occurring figurative expressions, such as feeling blue, on cloud nine, and all tied up, that have literal and nonliteral meanings. Method In this review article, an overview of SLIC is provided, highlighting the importance of inferential skills and other pertinent factors and theories contributing to idiom acquisition in autistic children. The search criteria used included peer-reviewed journal articles from 1982 to 2021 to synthesize both seminal and current research on this topic. Though some articles selected for this review did not focus on idioms in spoken language, they were deemed necessary for analyzing various aspects impacting idiom development and comprehension. Results Research shows that figurative language differences are a hallmark feature of autism, causing idioms to be interpreted literally. Occurring in everyday spoken and written language, idiom comprehension is essential for adequate communication in daily interactions. Poor inferential abilities directly impact SLIC and have negative implications on social and academic outcomes. Several factors contributing to SLIC have been highlighted in the literature, including transparency (semantic analyzability), familiarity, and context. Conclusions The comparable skills underpinning inferential comprehension and SLIC raise the argument that SLIC is incumbent upon intact inferential abilities. As such, autistic children will benefit from utilizing inference from context to deduce idiomatic meanings. Future directions for research and recommendations for improving SLIC in clinical practice are offered.
... Thus, social-emotional processes are expected to bidirectionally relate to reading and writing (Katzir et al., 2018). Finally, interactive relations are also hypothesized between component skills (e.g., vocabulary and syntactic knowledge and inference; Lepola et al., 2012;vocabulary and morphology;Kieffer & Lesaux, 2012). ...
Article
This article presents the application of the interactive dynamic literacy (IDL) model (Kim, 2020a) toward understanding difficulties in learning to read and write. According to the IDL model, reading and writing are part of communicative acts that draw on largely shared processes and skills as well as unique processes and skills. As such, reading and writing are dissociable but interdependent systems that have hierarchical, interactive, and dynamic relations. These key tenets of the IDL model are applied to the disruption of reading and writing development to explain co-occurrence of reading–writing difficulties using a single framework. The following hypotheses are presented: (a) co-occurrence between word reading and spelling and handwriting difficulties; (b) co-occurrence of dyslexia with written composition difficulties; (c) cooccurrence between reading comprehension and written composition difficulties; (d) co-occurrence of language difficulties with reading difficulties and writing difficulties; (e) co-occurrence of reading, writing, and language difficulties with weak domain-general skills or executive functions such as working memory and attentional control (including attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]); and (f) multiple pathways for reading and writing difficulties. Implications are discussed.
... Thus, social-emotional processes are expected to bidirectionally relate to reading and writing (Katzir et al., 2018). Finally, interactive relations are also hypothesized between component skills (e.g., vocabulary and syntactic knowledge and inference; Lepola et al., 2012;vocabulary and morphology;Kieffer & Lesaux, 2012). ...
Article
This article presents the application of the interactive dynamic literacy (IDL) model (Kim, 2020a) toward understanding difficulties in learning to read and write. According to the IDL model, reading and writing are part of communicative acts that draw on largely shared processes and skills as well as unique processes and skills. As such, reading and writing are dissociable but interdependent systems that have hierarchical, interactive, and dynamic relations. These key tenets of the IDL model are applied to the disruption of reading and writing development to explain co-occurrence of reading–writing difficulties using a single framework. The following hypotheses are presented: (a) co-occurrence between word reading and spelling and handwriting difficulties; (b) co-occurrence of dyslexia with written composition difficulties; (c) co-occurrence between reading comprehension and written composition difficulties; (d) co-occurrence of language difficulties with reading difficulties and writing difficulties; (e) co-occurrence of reading, writing, and language difficulties with weak domain-general skills or executive functions such as working memory and attentional control (including attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]); and (f) multiple pathways for reading and writing difficulties. Implications are discussed.
... For example, social-emotional aspects about writing are expected to develop in interactive manner with writing; so does content/topic knowledge with writing particularly in an advanced phase such as the knowledge-transforming stage by Kellogg (1996). Other component skills are also expected to have interactive relations, such as vocabulary and grammatical knowledge and their relations with inferencing skills (Currie & Cain, 2015;Kim, 2017;Lepola et al., 2012), and morphological awareness with vocabulary and grammatical knowledge (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2012;McBride-Chang et al., 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Within the context of the Direct and Indirect Effects Model of Writing (Kim & Park, 2019), we examined a dynamic relations hypothesis, which contends that the relations of component skills, including reading comprehension, to written composition vary as a function of dimensions of written composition. Specifically, we investigated (a) whether higher-order cognitive skills (i.e., inference, perspective taking, and monitoring) are differentially related to three dimensions of written composition—writing quality, writing productivity, and correctness in writing; (b) whether reading comprehension is differentially related to the three dimensions of written composition after accounting for oral language, cognition, and transcription skills, and whether reading comprehension mediates the relations of discourse oral language and lexical literacy to the three dimensions of written composition; and (c) whether total effects of oral language, cognition, transcription, and reading comprehension vary for the three dimensions of written composition. Structural equation model results from 350 English-speaking second graders showed that higher-order cognitive skills were differentially related to the three dimensions of written composition. Reading comprehension was related only to writing quality, but not to writing productivity or correctness in writing, and reading comprehension differentially mediated the relations of discourse oral language and lexical literacy to writing quality. Total effects of language, cognition, transcription, and reading comprehension varied largely for the three dimensions of written composition. These results support the dynamic relation hypothesis, role of reading in writing, and the importance of accounting for dimensions of written composition in a theoretical model of writing.
... However, the results are likely due to shared variance of vocabulary with inference (r = 0.50) and decoding (r = 0.44, see Table 2). The moderate relations of vocabulary with inference and decoding are in line with previous work (e.g., for inference, see Lepola et al., 2012;Tompkins et al., 2013;Currie and Cain, 2015;Kim, 2016Kim, , 2017for decoding, see Ouellette, 2006;Ricketts et al., 2007). Vocabulary learning requires deriving or inferring meaning from context using meaning cues, and inferencing unstated meaning in a text relies on knowledge of vocabulary words (Currie and Cain, 2015;Kim, 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
We examined the relations of inference, vocabulary, decoding, short-term memory, and attentional control to reading comprehension and mathematics performance for first-grade students in the US (N = 83). The students were composed of 75% Hispanics, 15% Whites, and 6% Asian Americans. Students' performance on mathematics and reading comprehension were very strongly related (r = 0.88). Results from path analysis showed that inference (0.27 ≤ s ≤ 0.38) was independently and positively related to both reading comprehension and mathematics performance after accounting for short-term memory, attentional control, decoding, and vocabulary. Decoding was independently related to reading comprehension, but not mathematics, whereas vocabulary was independently related to mathematics, but not to reading comprehension. Attentional control was directly related to mathematics, and indirectly related to reading comprehension and mathematics via inference, vocabulary, and decoding, with a substantial total effect on reading comprehension and mathematics (0.56 respectively). Short-term memory was not directly nor indirectly related to reading comprehension and mathematics. Overall these results show that language and cognitive skills are shared resources of reading comprehension and mathematics, and highlight the roles of attentional control and inference skill in reading comprehension and mathematics.
... This may be particularly true for lengthier text passages that require not only comprehension at the word and sentence level but also integration across sentences to a more general discourse level. Evidence supportive of this hypothesis would include indications that only when children gain in multiple foundational language skills do they also show robust improvements on listening comprehension, particularly for texts with novel content, consistent with predictive models reported by Kim (2016) and Lepola et al. (2012) , among others. ...
Article
The strong association of early language skills to later reading ability suggests that supporting the development of these skills in children who enter school with below average language abilities may lead to stronger literacy development. Despite this, few evidence-based supplemental language interventions exist for school-based implementation. The current paper reports on 2 large-scale randomized trials of 5 small-group, intensive language-focused interventions implemented in preschool and kindergarten settings. After screening on an expressive language measure, 342 preschool children were randomized to either business-as-usual general education or to 1 of 3 10-or 12-week interventions. Comparably, after screening, 905 kindergarten children were randomized to business-as-usual or to 1 of 4 8-12-week interventions. Children were assessed pre-and post-intervention on a battery of distal standardized language, including listening comprehension, and early literacy measures. Results indicated significant impacts for 3 of the 5 interventions in 1 or 2 grades, on at least 1 standardized measure, although none of the interventions directly impacted listening comprehension measures. Implications for supporting language development and for the early prevention of reading comprehension difficulties are discussed.
Article
The beginning of the preschool period (4-5 years) is a time of active formation and improvement of the neuropsychological mechanisms of the main linguistic levels of spoken language (phonological, lexical-semantic and grammatical) in their two main aspects - receptive (perception and processing of verbal information ) and expressive (language encoding and generation of independent utterances). However, the existing diagnostic procedures, both in Bulgaria and worldwide, mainly focus on the state of children‘s expressive language skills. The processes related to the perception and processing of linguistic information often remain outside the attention of specialists. The probable reason for this fact is that in the development of children‘s language, perception and understanding precede expressive abilities. This unproven claim directs researchers‘ attention to the realization of children‘s language skills at the expense of the state of impressive language. It is known that there are variants in which children remember and use „ pattern expressions“ without a sufficient degree of understanding. Incomplete decoding of grammatical forms is most fully highlighted in the process of speech therapy diagnostics and has a high prognostic value regarding both the oral and written language of children. In the literacy period, similar type of deficits have a negative impact on reading skills related to decoding and understanding of written texts. The difficulties multiply as the volume and complexity of the learning texts increase during the different stages of schooling. The diagnostic tools available in our country for assessing early language development mainly focus on the state of children‘s expressive language ability (as in Diagnostics and prevention of language 3 – 4, D.P.E. 3 – 4, Stoyanova, Yosifova, Poppandova, Netsova, 2010; VERBA-2, Andonova 2022a, 2022b). This explains the need to develop and test a detailed diagnostic model for evaluating receptive language abilities at the main levels of the language system – lexical, morphological and syntactic. The material presents the conceptual framework of the first in our country hierarchically structured Model for the evaluation of receptive language in children with typical development at the age of 4 – 5 years. The purpose of the model is to derive age norms for decoding and understanding the Bulgarian language, which have a high prognostic value for the early diagnosis and prevention of children at risk of future learning difficulties.
Article
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterised by symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity that may begin in early childhood and continue into adolescence and adulthood and are incompatible with the developmental level of the individual. Most children with ADHD are at risk of facing significant academic problems throughout their educational life. In recent studies aiming to reveal the causes of academic problems experienced by children with ADHD, academic problems have been associated with the difficulties in reading and listening comprehension skills of children with ADHD. The relatively common prevalence of ADHD, the extent of academic problems experienced by children with ADHD, and the lack of clarity about reading difficulties require further examination of the relationship between ADHD and reading and listening comprehension skills, which are often based on the same language and cognitive skills. Listening comprehension, which is defined as the ability to understand, interpret, organise and evaluate the ideas and thoughts in a speech heard or a text read aloud, is one of the most important early literacy skills necessary for the ultimate goal of reading, which is reading comprehension. In this review study, listening comprehension skills of children with ADHD were discussed together with the literature.
Article
Purpose Using a longitudinal design, this study aimed to describe inferential comprehension abilities of neglected French-speaking preschool children from 42 to 66 months of age in comparison to non-neglected peers, to examine the association with receptive vocabulary, and to determine whether rates of change in inferential abilities over time was stable between the two group conditions. Method An inferential comprehension task and the French version of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth Edition were administered to a group of neglected children ( n = 37–40) and to a group of same-age non-neglected children ( n = 71–91) at 42, 54, and 66 months old, as part of the Early Longitudinal Language and Neglect study. Results Results show that children exposed to neglect obtain significantly lower scores compared to their same-age peers on inferential comprehension and receptive vocabulary measures at all three time points ( p < .001) with large to very large effect sizes and indicate moderate to strong correlations between the two variables. Children from the neglected group present difficulties in inferencing compared to same-age non-neglected peers, a disadvantage that remains stable over time. Conclusions This study demonstrates the significant gap in inferential comprehension abilities between neglected and non-neglected preschool children. These results reiterate the importance of early detection of language comprehension difficulties in young children coming from vulnerable environments.
Article
Purpose This study focuses on 7- to 9-year-old children attending primary school in Swedish areas of low socioeconomic status, where most children's school language is their second language. The aim was to better understand what factors influence these children's narrative listening comprehension both in an ideal listening condition (in quiet) and for the primary school classroom, a typical listening condition (with multitalker babble noise). Method A total of 86 typically developing 7- to 9-year-olds performed a narrative listening comprehension test (Lyssna, Förstå och Minnas [LFM]; English translation: Listen, Comprehend, and Remember) in two listening conditions: quiet and multitalker babble noise. They also performed the crosslinguistic nonword repetition test and a digit span backwards (DSB) test. A predictive statistical model including these factors, the children's degree of school language exposure, parental education level, and age was derived. Results Listening condition had the strongest predictive value for LFM performance, followed by school language exposure and nonword repetition accuracy. Parental education level was also a significant predictor. There was a significant three-way interaction effect between listening condition, age, and DSB performance. Conclusions Multitalker babble noise has a negative effect on children's narrative listening comprehension. The effect of multitalker babble noise could be explained by age differences in the ability to allocate working memory capacity during the narrative listening comprehension task, suggesting that younger children may be more vulnerable for missing information when listening in background noise than their older peers. Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.25209248
Article
Full-text available
Çalışmanın amacı kelime tanıma, akıcı okuma ve okuduğunu anlama becerilerini etkileyen önemli bir değişken olan ortografik farkındalığın ilkokul düzeyinde ve Türkçe bağlamında yapılandırılmış bir testini geliştirmektir. Bu amaç doğrultusunda kelime düzeyinde ortografik farkındalık testi geliştirilmeye çalışılmıştır. Testin geliştirilme sürecinde test maddelerinin yazılması, pilot uygulamalar yapılması, madde analizi, geçerlilik ve güvenirlik çalışmaları işlem basamakları olarak takip edilmiş ve sunulmuştur. Araştırmanın çalışma grubunu Manisa ili Şehzadeler ilçesinde bulunan bir devlet okulunun üç 3. sınıf şubesi (N=111) oluşturmuştur. Çalışmada veri toplama aracı olarak Kelime Düzeyinde Ortografik Farkındalık Testi kullanılmıştır. Bu testin uygulama formunda toplam 40 madde yer almıştır. Çalışmada elde edilen veriler önce Microsoft Excel programına girilmiştir. Daha sonra veriler IBM SPSS ve Jamovi programlarına aktarılmıştır. Elde edilen veriler üzerinde madde ve güvenirlilik analizleri gerçekleştirilmiştir. Yapılan madde güçlük ve ayırt edicilik indekslerine ilişkin hesaplamalar ve güvenirlik analizleri sonucunda, geliştirilen kelime düzeyinde ortografik farkındalık testinin ilkokul düzeyinde okuma becerisi ve ilişkili bilişsel yapıları daha nitelikli bir şekilde değerlendirilebilecek güvenilir ve geçerli bir test olduğu ortaya konulmuştur.
Article
During a study in a Kindergarten classroom, wordless and almost wordless picturebooks were presented as aesthetic objects that are read for pleasure, reward slow looking, and require engagement in significant semiotic work. Instruction about and adult mediation of picturebooks throughout the research communicated to the children that elements of visual art, design, and layout in picturebook artwork are fundamental to meaning-making with this format of literature. One component of the research procedures involved the students participating in small group interactive discussion sessions of six almost wordless picturebooks. Content analysis of transcript excerpts from the discussions of one picturebook revealed the Kindergarten children generated inferences about characters’ goals, actions, states, activities, emotions, and utterances, as well as made place, object, causal antecedents, and causal consequences inferences. Furthermore, analyses of the transcripts revealed how inference generation was central to the children’s meaning-making during their transactions with an almost wordless picturebook. The findings contribute to the limited scholarship on Kindergarten students’ inferences and interpretations about elements of artwork, design, and layout in wordless and almost wordless picturebooks, and overall, add to the literature on young children’s inferential thinking.
Article
Full-text available
Prelingual single-sided deafness (SSD) not only affects children’s hearing skills, but can also lead to speech-language delays and academic underachievement. Early cochlear implantation leads to improved spatial hearing, but the impact on language development is less studied. In our longitudinal study, we assessed the language skills of young children with SSD and a cochlear implant (CI). In particular, we investigated their narrative skills in comparison to two control groups: children with SSD without a CI, and children with bilateral normal hearing. We found that children with SSD and a CI performed in line with their normal-hearing peers with regard to narrative and verbal short-term memory skills. Children with SSD without a CI had worse narrative (group difference = − 0.67, p = 0.02) and verbal short-term memory (group difference = − 0.68, p = 0.03) scores than the implanted group. Verbal short-term memory scores and grammar scores each correlated positively with narrative scores across all groups. Early grammar scores (at 2–3 years of age) could partially predict later narrative scores (at 4–6 years of age). These results show that young children with prelingual SSD can benefit from early cochlear implantation to achieve age-appropriate language skills. They support the provision of a CI to children with prelingual SSD.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the dimensionality of oral discourse skills (comprehension and retell of texts) and the relations of language and cognitive skills to the identified dimensions. Data were from 529 English-speaking second graders (Mage = 7.42; 46% female; 52.6% Whites, 33.8% African Americans, 4.9% Hispanics, 4.7% two or more races, .8% Asian Americans, .6% American Indians, .2% Native Hawaiians, 2.5% unknown; data from 2014-2015 to 2016-2017). Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that oral discourse skills are best described as four related but dissociable dimensions of narrative comprehension, narrative retell, expository comprehension, and expository retell (rs = .59-.84). Language and cognitive skills had different patterns of relations to the identified dimensions and explained larger amounts of variance in comprehension than in retell.
Article
Full-text available
This research aims were to examine teachers’ perceptions of listening inferencing skills (LIS) and their significance for listening comprehension. In addition, the pedagogical techniques that teachers report to have employed in teaching LIS were also investigated. In order to achieve these goals, a questionnaire consisting of 48 five-point Likert scale items and one open-ended question was utilized as the research instrument. Seventy-two EFL teachers working at different universities, colleges and private language centres in a province in the Mekong Delta, Vietnam gave their responses to the questionnaires. The results revealed that a significant number of teachers appeared to hold a limited understanding of what LIS actually refer to. However, when they were introduced to what LIS involve, they noticeably reached the agreement that these skills are beneficial for listening comprehension in particular and language learning in general. Besides, a large number of the participants also reported that they applied LIS in their own English language learning and teaching. In terms of pedagogical techniques used to teach LIS, the teachers mainly concentrated on pre-listening tactics with the hope of equipping their students well for the listening process regarding linguistic and background knowledge while techniques for the while-, post- and extension stages in listening instruction seemed to be overlooked. Article visualizations: </p
Article
Full-text available
O estudo investigou a compreensão oral de textos narrativos, expositivos e argumentativos em crianças da Educação Infantil. O objetivo principal foi examinar se o nível de compreensão variaria em função do tipo de texto e identificar a natureza das dificuldades experimentadas pelas crianças em relação ao estabelecimento de inferências em cada um desses textos. Trinta crianças (média de idade de 5 anos e 8 meses) foram solicitadas a responder perguntas de natureza inferencial (causal e estado) sobre cada um dos textos que lhe foram lidos. Verificou-se que o nível de compreensão variava em função dos textos, visto que as crianças tinham mais facilidade em compreender o texto narrativo que os demais textos. Por outro lado, apresentaram o mesmo nível de dificuldade no texto expositivo e argumentativo. A principal dificuldade identificada foi em relação à compreensão das relações de causalidade no texto expositivo. Implicações educacionais são discutidas no sentido de promover intervenções precoces para desenvolver a compreensão oral de textos antes mesmo que a criança saiba ler. Essa implicação confere à Educação Infantil papel relevante no desenvolvimento de habilidades linguísticas que são preditoras da compreensão leitora em anos escolares subsequentes.
Chapter
Text comprehension is a critical area of psychological and educational research, and has particular relevance to educational context. The general aim of this international volume Reading Comprehension in Educational Settings is to encourage excellence in research and to bring together teachers, students, researchers and other professionals from different disciplines (e.g. psychology, linguistics, neuroscience, education, information technology, and communication), as well as all those members of the general public who have an interest in the study of reading. The specific objectives of the different chapters in this volume are to analyze existing methods of studying the various aspects of reading comprehension, disseminate results already obtained by research groups working in the field and debate current and future trends in the study of reading.
Article
Research on monolingual children has shown that listening comprehension is predicted by a range of language and cognitive skills; less is known about predictors of listening comprehension in bilingual children and about the role of language input. This study presents longitudinal data on predictors of English listening comprehension in 100 bilingual children between the ages of 5;8 and 6;8 years. The children were tested three times on their literal and inferential comprehension of stories. Vocabulary, morphosyntax, attention, and memory were included as predictors of listening comprehension alongside a measure of English input. The children showed growth over time in both literal questions and global inference questions, with performance on local inferences remaining stable over time. Vocabulary depth and morphological knowledge explained listening comprehension abilities in all types of questions, but not their growth; that is, all children improved in comprehension over time regardless of their initial morphological and vocabulary depth skills. English input had a mediated effect on listening comprehension via morphological knowledge and vocabulary depth, but no direct effect.
Chapter
The COVID-19 pandemic has affected teachers’ practices at all education levels worldwide. Alternative educational practices are, in a smaller or larger extent, implemented online in response to the global pandemic, including early childhood and primary education. Access to recent research outcomes on effective approaches to quality online teaching is fundamental in preschool and in the first critical years of primary school. In this chapter, we provide a review of the key challenges that online classes pose for young children and teachers. In parallel we discuss, based on professional and research-informed insights, best practice principles for online teaching to support preschool and primary school teachers to transform online approaches into effective teaching practices for meeting children’s needs. Both the challenges and the effective online approaches are grouped under two main headings with each heading being related to several outcomes. A major challenge occurring during online classes, falling under the first heading, is the limited face-to-face interaction between learners and teachers. The second challenge is concerned with difficulties in oral and written language. This chapter concludes with a reflection on the implications for the use of best practice principles for online teaching in the early childhood and primary school setting.
Chapter
This chapter focuses on what we know about the development of reading comprehension in the early school years, with a particular emphasis on understanding causal processes. According to the simple view of reading, reading comprehension is the product of decoding and language comprehension. Linguistic comprehension is typically assessed using tests of listening comprehension in which a person answers questions about the meaning of a spoken passage. A different approach to measuring the development of reading comprehension over time is to use individual growth curve modeling. The chapter emphasizes on evidence from longitudinal studies of typically developing children published between 2004 and 2020. Reading comprehension is a highly complex skill that is undoubtedly heavily dependent on language comprehension ability. There is very strong support for the simple view of reading: Variations in reading comprehension are strongly predicted by variations in decoding and linguistic comprehension.
Article
Objective Preschool-age children experience many home accidents, and school nurses play a role in preventing these accidents. Methods Accordingly, this study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of training preschool-age children through an imagined hide-and-seek game using a model home environment, in cases that may pose an accident risk. This pretest–posttest control-group study was conducted between March and June 2019. In total, 72 preschool students were included in the study. The study group’s education through the model home environment and game was assessed using the Determination of Home Environment Risks Form. Results The post-education mean accident risk scores of the children in the study group increased in the kitchen (pre-education: 4.89 ± 3.258; post-education: 9.56 ± 3.549), children’s room (pre-education: 6.17 ± 3.776; post-education: 8.56 ± 3.722), and garden (pre-education: 4.39 ± 2.697; post-education: 6.72 ± 2.953). In the study group, the mean scores of children for recognizing the circumstances that pose a risk of accident in the home environment were determined to increase significantly ( P < 0.001). The corresponding scores of the children in the control group did not change in the kitchen (pre-education: 3.89 ± 2.025; post-education: 3.44 ± 2.006), children’s room (pre-education: 3.72 ± 1.667; post-education: 3.50 ± 2.158), or garden (pre-education: 2.83 ± 1.813; post-education: 2.17 ± 2.049). Conclusions The new techniques, which will help children learn while having fun, should be integrated into preschool children’s education programs to reduce home accidents.
Chapter
This chapter reports the work of a teacher educator/researcher as she supported teacher candidates to assess and tutor struggling readers in a public school in a rural, economically depressed, yet diverse, area. Alerted by the scores for listening comprehension the candidates were finding over several semesters that indicated little reading potential for the students being assessed, she worked with the school's principal to reassess one group of students at the end of the year to determine growth, and therefore potential success, of the school's new intervention program in raising listening levels. No significant results were found, yet school personnel made no change in their program to address it. The teacher educator/researcher subsequently followed the implications of the research to provide instruction in listening skills to students in two other schools. The chapter closes with a discussion of what may truly make a difference in developing listening skills for the children in this community beyond a commercial program.
Article
Full-text available
Successful text comprehension results in a coherent mental model of the situation being described. To achieve this, the reader has to infer certain information by connecting parts of the text to their prior knowledge. An important construct involved in this process is vocabulary knowledge, usually divided into breadth and depth. We conducted a meta-analysis on 23 studies, and explored the fit of five different models to establish an effect size of both dimensions of vocabulary on inference making, as well as its developmental trajectory in children aged 3-12. We found a significant and moderate effect of vocabulary knowledge of both modalities. Vocabulary type was not a significant moderator, but age was, meaning that there was a similar effect for both breadth and depth and that the strength of the correlations decreased with age. Heterogeneity was high overall, meaning that more moderators should be assessed in future studies.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to examine the effect on preschool aged children of an intervention reinforcing comprehension of the structural framework of stories, so that they can produce their own stories. The sample consisted of 78 children, ages 4-6. The sample was separated into two groups, one experimental and one control group. The children in the experimental group were taught how to create original fictional stories through a multilevel intervention programme. During sessions, well-structured books were used that had very well-structured contents and an instructional strategy was implemented on five levels (creating prior knowledge, discussion, modelling, monitoring the process and producing stories). The children in the control group were read the same books and a discussion followed on the interesting parts of the stories. The results showed that the intervention programme significantly improved the children’s ability to understand the structural elements of a story and to generate comprehensible and organised fictional stories.
Article
Full-text available
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Full-text available
Fifty-six first-grade children were administered measures of general intelligence, decoding speed, phonological awareness, and listening comprehension. All four types of measures were moderately related to end-of-year reading comprehension. Decoding speed accounted for the largest amount of unique variance. The hypothesis that reading is strongly related to general intelligence once differences in decoding ability have been accounted for was not supported. Other relationships among the variables were explored via multiple regression, factor analysis, and path analysis. Developmental comparisons were made with groups of third- and fifth-grade children. The relationships between decoding, intelligence, and reading comprehension found in the first-grade sample were replicated in the fifth-grade sample but were somewhat different in the third-grade sample. The interrelationships between the various subskills of reading and intelligence increased with age, probably due to mutual facilitation. /// [French] On a administré à cinquante six enfants de cours primaire des mesures d'intelligence générale, de vitesse de décodage, de conscience phonologique et de compréhension d'écoute. Les quatre facteurs étaient modérément reliés à une mesure de compréhension de lecture de fin d'année. La vitesse de décodage rendait compte de la plus large quantité d'écart unique. L'hypothèse qui veut que la lecture soit fortement reliée à l'intelligence générale après avoir tenu compte des différences de compétence de décodage, n'a pas été soutenue. On a exploré d'autres rapports parmi les écarts à travers une régression multiple, une analyse de facteur et une analyse de parcours. On a établi des comparaisons de développement avec des groupes d'enfants de neuvième et septième. Les rapports entre décodage, intelligence, et compréhension de lecture trouvés dans l'échantillon de cours primaire sont répétés dans l'échantillon de la septième mais ont été quelque peu différents dans celui de la neuvième. Les rapports étroits entre les différentes sous-compétences de lecture et intelligence ont augmenté avec l'âge, ceci étant probablement dû à une facilitation réciproque. /// [Spanish] Se administraron medidas de inteligencia general, velocidad de descifre, conocimiento fonológico y escuchar y comprender. Los 4 factores estaban moderadamente relacionados a una medida de comprensión de lectura de fin de año escolar. La velocidad de descifre dio cuenta del mayor número de variabilidad singular. No recibió apoyo la hipótesis de que la destreza de lectura está fuertemente relacionada a inteligencia general, una vez que se han considerado las diferencias de habilidad de descifre. Otras relaciones entre variables fueron exploradas por medio de regresión múltiple, análisis factorial y análisis de trayecto. Se hicieron comparaciones de desarrollo con grupos de alumnos de tercer y quinto grado. La relación entre descifre, inteligencia y comprensión de lectura encontrada en la muestra del primer grado, fue repetida en el quinto grado, pero resultó algo diferente en la muestra del tercer grado. La correlación entre las varias subdestrezas de lectura e inteligencia aumentó con la edad, probablemente debido a facilitación mútua.
Article
Full-text available
The authors assert that, in order to teach vocabulary more effectively and better understand its relation to comprehension, we need first to address how vocabulary knowledge and growth are assessed. They argue that “vocabularly assessment is grossly undernourished, both in its theoretical and practical aspects—that it has been driven by tradition, convenience, psychometric standards, and a quest for economy of effort rather than a clear conceptualization of its nature and relation to other aspects of reading expertise, most notably comprehension.”
Article
Full-text available
An analysis of the underlying structure of simple stories is presented. It is claimed that this type of representation of stories is used to form schemata which guide encoding and retrieval. A type of tree structure containing basic units and their connections was found to be adequate to describe the structure of both single and multi-episode stories. The representation is outlined in the form of a grammar, consisting of rewrite rules defining the units and their relationships. Some transformational rules mapping underlying and surface structures are discussed. The adequacy of the analysis is first tested against Bartlett's protocols of “The War of the Ghosts.” Then a developmental study of recall is presented. It is concluded that both children and adults are sensitive to the structure of stories, although some differences were found. Finally, it is suggested that the schemata used to guide encoding and recall are related but not identical and that retrieval is dependent on the schemata operative at the time of recall.
Article
Full-text available
In this study, 132 first graders' comprehension-monitoring skills were examined and related to their decoding and listening-comprehension skills. Decoding skills were tested with a word-picture matching task and with word-naming and word-nonword decision tasks. Story-listening tasks were used in determining the level of listening comprehension. Children's comprehension monitoring was measured by means of an online method in which reactions to embedded comprehension obstacles (lexical, syntactical, factual falsehoods, or contradictions) were assessed. The number of "lookbacks" and the time spent in reading the target words/sentences were recorded and used as indicators of activities involved in comprehension monitoring. The results indicated that monitoring one's comprehension is already present in beginning reading but that the level of decoding and listening-comprehension skills affects the ways and efficacy of monitoring.
Article
Full-text available
The first goal of this study was to examine young children's developing narrative comprehension abilities using theory-based, authentic measures of comprehension processes. The second goal was to examine the relations among young children's comprehension abilities and other early reading skills. Children ages 4 and 6 listened to or watched two authentic narratives. We measured their comprehension of these narratives as well as vocabulary and skills associated with word decoding. The results revealed that even the younger children were sensitive to the underlying structure of the narratives and that this sensitivity increased with age. Measures of narrative comprehension were not consistently correlated with skills associated with word decoding, such as phonological awareness. The results are discussed in terms of theoretical models of comprehension and of reading development. Practical implications of the findings are also explored.
Article
Full-text available
What is the role of oral language in reading competence during the transition to school? Is oral language in preschool best conceptualized as vocabulary knowledge or as more comprehensive language including grammar, vocabulary, and semantics? These questions were examined longitudinally using 1,137 children from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Children were followed from age 3 through 3rd grade, and the results suggest that oral language conceptualized broadly plays both a direct and an indirect role in word recognition during the transition to school and serves as a better foundation for early reading skill than does vocabulary alone. Implications of these findings are discussed in terms of both theoretical models of early reading and practical implications for policy and assessment.
Article
Full-text available
We report a longitudinal study investigating the predictors of reading comprehension and word reading accuracy between the ages of 7 to 8 (UK Year 3) and 10 to 11 years (Year 6). We found that different skills predicted the development of each. Reading comprehension skill measured in Year 3 was a strong predictor of comprehension in Year 6; vocabulary and verbal IQ also made significant unique contributions to the prediction of comprehension ability across time. Three comprehension components (inference, comprehension monitoring, and knowledge and use of story structure) emerged as distinct predictors of reading comprehension in Year 6, even after the autoregressive effect of comprehension was controlled. For word reading accuracy, early measures of word reading accuracy and phonemic awareness predicted later performance.
Article
Full-text available
According to multicomponent models (Oakhill & Cain, 2007a45. Oakhill , J. V. and Cain , K. 2007a . “ Introduction to comprehension development. ” . In Children's comprehension problems in oral and written language: A cognitive perspective Edited by: Cain , K. and Oakhill , J. V. 41 – 73 . New York : Guilford. . View all references), text comprehension is a complex process that requires the processing of explicit (i.e., information presented in the text) and implicit information (i.e., information inferable from the text or from previous knowledge), and involves various components. This study investigated (a) preschoolers' understanding of explicit and implicit information in oral texts and (b) the role of verbal and inferential skills in the processing of explicit and implicit information. Two hundred twenty-one 4- to 6-year-olds were evaluated as to their listening text comprehension and the following components: receptive vocabulary, verbal intelligence, and inferential skills. Working memory was a control variable. Results showed that (a) explicit information was easier to process than implicit information; and (b) all the components considered, except receptive vocabulary, accounted for comprehension of both types of information, and their role was stable in the age range considered.
Article
Full-text available
The authors examined the development of oral language and decoding skills from preschool to early elementary school and their relation to beginning reading comprehension using a cross-sequential design. Four- and 6-year-old children were tested on oral language and decoding skills and were retested 2 years later. In all age groups, oral language and decoding skills formed distinct clusters. The 2 clusters were related to each other in preschool, but this relation became weaker in kindergarten and 2nd grade. Structural equation modeling showed that both sets of skills in 2nd grade independently predicted a child’s reading comprehension. These findings confirm and extend the view that the 2 clusters of skills develop early in a child’s life and contribute to reading comprehension activities in early elementary school, with each cluster making a considerable, unique contribution. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Two experiments were conducted to assess how children who differ in vocabulary knowledge learn new vocabulary incidentally from listening to stories read aloud. In both experiments, 4-yr-old children were classified as having either high or low word knowledge on the basis of a median split of their Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised (PPVT—R) standard scores. In Exp 1, children either listened passively or labeled pictures using novel words during the book readings. We found that children with larger vocabularies produced more novel words than did children with smaller vocabularies, and children who answered questions during the book readings comprehended and produced more words than did children who passively listened to the story. In Exp 2, children either listened to readings of a book, pointed to pictures during the readings, or labeled pictures during the readings. Children with larger vocabularies comprehended more novel words than did children with smaller vocabularies. Children who actively participated by labeling or pointing learned more words than did children who listened passively to book readings. Findings clarify the role of active responding by demonstrating that verbal and nonverbal responding are effective means of enhancing vocabulary acquisition. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Hypothesized that children characterized by deficits in narrative skills, relative to other language skills (e.g., syntactic and semantic), would be most at risk for general academic problems, especially in reading comprehension. Using data from a previous longitudinal study by the 1st author and J. D. McKinney (see record 1985-02522-001), different subtypes of language disability were identified in 63 6- and 7-yr-old learning disabled (LD) children. Comparison data had been obtained from 66 non-LD children matched to the LD sample on age, race, and sex. Results, obtained with hierarchical cluster analysis, indicate that 6 language subtypes were derived and that these were both internally consistent and externally valid, being differentially linked to reading and math achievement over a 3-yr period. Narrative ability was shown to be relatively important in predicting academic outcomes. The 3 subtypes showing the poorest academic outcomes had the highest relative scores in syntax and semantics. (21 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments investigated how 206 adult readers represent causal relations among events in a narrative. Models of text comprehension were tested. In each experiment Ss read brief narratives and received a speeded-recognition test of their memories for story events. Each story could be represented by a linear chain or by a network. On each trial in the recognition procedure Ss read a priming sentence that reminded them of either a story (general prime) or a specific event in a story (specific prime). Across the experiments, positive responses were faster when the target followed a specific prime that was causally related than when it followed a specific but unrelated prime or a general prime. Importantly, this was the case when the specific prime and target were adjacent and when they were nonadjacent in the surface structure of the story. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The course of language acquisition from infancy to public primary school was followed in a sample of 56 Finnish children to examine precursors to reading at first grade. Structural equation modeling of continuity suggested effects from growth in early vocabulary to mastery of inflectional forms at preschool age. The early language directly influenced early phonological awareness (PA) and only indirectly influenced later development in PA and word reading. The course of development in PA progressed from detecting larger multiphonemic units toward recognizing and producing phonemes in words, which, in turn, were positively associated with differences in producing new words by deleting and blending phonemes at kindergarten age. Including word reading before school entry levelled out the influence of the concurrent phonemic awareness factor on reading at first grade. Hence, in a highly inflected language with a transparent orthography, the pathway to reading consisted of skills learned in succession, the last phase being characterized by simultaneous development involving phonemic awareness and emerging reading skill. The finding led to the conclusion that, in addition to universal routes, language- and culture-specific routes to literacy must be acknowledged when searching for the precursors to reading at school age. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
A structural equation model of second language (L2; English) reading comprehension was tested on a sample of 135 Spanish-speaking 4th-grade English-language learners (ELLs). The model included 2 levels: decoding and oral language. English decoding measures included alphabetic knowledge and fluency. English oral language measures included vocabulary knowledge and listening comprehension. The model had reasonable goodness of fit. Decoding skills played a less predictive role than oral language proficiency. L2 listening comprehension made an independent, proximal contribution to L2 reading comprehension, whereas L2 vocabulary knowledge assumed both proximal and distal relationships with L2 reading comprehension. Results suggest that, given adequate L2 decoding ability, L2 vocabulary knowledge is crucial for improved English reading comprehension outcomes for Spanish-speaking ELLs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
This study was designed to establish whether phonological working memory skills could be assessed in children below 4 yrs of age. A group of 2- and 3-yr-old children were tested on 3 phonological memory measures (digit span, nonword repetition, and word repetition) and were also given tasks that tapped other cognitive skills. Scores on the 3 phonological memory tasks were closely related. In addition, repetition performance was linked with both vocabulary knowledge and articulation rate. Results indicate that phonological memory skills can be reliably assessed in very young children by using conventional serial span and repetition procedures. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The nature of the developmental association between phonological memory and vocabulary knowledge was explored in a longitudinal study. At each of 4 waves (at ages 4, 5, 6, and 8 yrs), measures of vocabulary, phonological memory, nonverbal intelligence, and reading were taken from 80 children. Comparisons of cross-lagged partial correlations revealed a significant shift in the causal underpinnings of the relationship between phonological memory and vocabulary development before and after 5 yrs of age. Between 4 and 5 yrs, phonological memory skills appeared to exert a direct causal influence on vocabulary acquisition. Subsequently, though, vocabulary knowledge became the major pacemaker in the developmental relationship, with the earlier influence of phonological memory on vocabulary development subsiding to a nonsignificant level. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In Exp I, a listening span test, a word span test, and a listening comprehension test were administered to 24 preschoolers (ages 3 yrs 2 mo to 5 yrs 2 mo) to determine whether listening span correlated well with Ss' listening integration skills. In Exp II, with 20 preschoolers (aged 3 yrs 4 mo to 5 yrs) a longer listening comprehension test was administered in which comprehension depended on the integration of at least 2 important ideas in the narrative. Results show that the measure of listening span was a successful measure of the listening comprehension skills of prereaders because it tapped skills at an intermediate level of complexity. The word span measure involved lower-level processes like word encoding and lexical access, but listening span captured many of the processes of normal sentence comprehension. In addition, listening span was easy to administer and had high predictive validity. (24 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
It has been shown that there is a strong relation between children's phonological skills and the progress that they make in reading. But there is some uncertainty whether this is a specific connection or whether it is just a byproduct of variations in general language ability. We report evidence from a longitudinal study showing that the relation between children's sensitivity to rhyme and alliteration and their success in reading is highly specific and cannot be accounted for in terms of general language ability. In this study measures were taken of a group of children's linguistic and metalinguistic skills when they were 3 and 4 years old. The linguistic measures were of the children's vocabulary, their receptive and expressive use of grammar, and their ability to imitate sentences. The metalinguistic measures were of their ability to detect rhyme and alliteration and of their awareness of syntax. Two to three years later, when the children were 6;7, we measured their progress in reading and spelling. The children's rhyme and alliteration scores were related to their reading two years later even after controls for differences in linguistic skills and also for differences in intelligence and in social background. The other metalinguistic task – syntax awareness – did not predict reading after these controls. Awareness of rhyme, we argue, makes a distinctive contribution to reading by helping children to form spelling categories.
Article
Full-text available
ABSTRACTS A quantitative meta‐analysis evaluating the effects of phonemic awareness (PA) instruction on learning to read and spell was conducted by the National Reading Panel. There were 52 studies published in peer‐reviewed journals, and these contributed 96 cases comparing the outcomes of treatment and control groups. Analysis of effect sizes revealed that the impact of PA instruction on helping children acquire PA was large and statistically significant ( d = 0.86). PA instruction exerted a moderate, statistically significant impact on reading ( d = 0.53) and spelling ( d = 0.59). Not only word reading but also reading comprehension benefited. PA instruction impacted reading under all the conditions examined although effect sizes were larger under some conditions. PA instruction helped various types of children: normally developing readers as well as at‐risk and disabled readers; preschoolers, kindergartners, and first graders; low socioeconomic status children as well as mid‐high SES. PA instruction improved reading, but it did not improve spelling in disabled readers. PA instruction was more effective when it was taught with letters than without letters, when one or two PA skills were taught than multiple PA skills, when children were taught in small groups than individually or in classrooms, and when instruction lasted between 5 and 18 hours rather than longer. Classroom teachers were effective in teaching PA to their students. Effect sizes were larger for studies using more rigorous experimental designs, with rigor assessments drawn from Troia (1999). In sum, PA instruction was found to make a statistically significant contribution to reading acquisition. [See also a letter to the editors regarding this article, and the first author's response: http:dx.doi.org10.1598RRQ.37.2.1 ] Un meta‐análisis cuantitativo que evaluó los efectos de la instrucción en conciencia fonémica (CF) sobre el aprendizaje de la lectura y la escritura fue llevado a cabo por el Panel Nacional de Lectura (National Reading Panel). Se tomaron 52 estudios, publicados en revistas con referato, que aportaron 96 casos en los que se comparó el resultado de los grupos de tratamiento y de control. El análisis de las magnitudes del efecto reveló que el impacto de la instrucción en CF sobre el desarrollo de la CF en los niños fue grande y estadísticamente significativo ( d = 0.86). La instrucción en CF ejerció un impacto moderado, estadísticamente significativo en la lectura ( d = 0.53) y escritura ( d = 0.59). No sólo se benefició la lectura de palabras, sino también la comprensión lectora. La instrucción en CF impactó en la lectura, en todas las condiciones examinadas, sin embargo las magnitudes del efecto fueron mayores en algunas condiciones. La instrucción en CF ayudó a distintos tipos de niños: lectores de desarrollo normal, lectores de riesgo y lectores con dificultades; niños de jardín, preescolar y primer grado; niños de NSE bajo y niños de NSE medio‐alto. La instrucción en CF mejoró la lectura pero no la escritura en los lectores con dificultades. La instrucción en CF fue más efectiva cuando se realizó con apoyo de las letras que cuando no se enseñaron las letras, cuando se enseñaron una o dos habilidades de CF que cuando se enseñaron múltiples habilidades, cuando se insruyó a los niños en pequeños grupos que cuando se realizó individualmente o en el aula y cuando la instrucción duró entre 5 y 18 horas que cuando abarcó más tiempo. Los docentes fueron eficaces en la enseñanza de la CF a los estudiantes. Las magnitudes del efecto fueron mayores en los estudios que utilizaron diseños experimentales más rigurosos, con evaluaciones de rigor tomadas de Troia (1999). En suma, se halló que la instrucción en CF constituye una contribución significativa a la adquisición de la lectura. Eine quantitative Meta‐Analyse, welche Auswirkungen phonischen Aufnahmebewußseins (PA) beim Erlernen des Lesens und Buchstabierens bewertet, wurde vom Nationalen Lesegremium durchgeführt. Es wurden 52 Studien in von Lehrern rezensierten Fachzeitschriften publiziert und diese nannten 96 Fälle, bei denen die Ergebnisse von Anwendungs‐ und Kontrollgruppen verglichen wurden. Die Analyse über Wirksamkeitsausmaße ergab, daß der Einfluß von PA‐Anweisungen in der Unterstützung der Kinder beim Erwerb von PA groß und statistisch bedeutend ( d = 0.86) war. PA‐Anweisungen übten einen ausgleichenden, statitisch bedeutsamen Einfluß aufs Lesen ( d = 0.53) und Buchstabieren ( d = 0.59) aus. Nicht nur das Lesen von Wörtern, sondern auch das Leseverständnis wurde begünstigt. PA‐Anweisungen beeinflußten das Lesen unter all den untersuchten Bedingungen, obwohl die Ausmaße der Auswirkungen bei einigen Bedingungen größer waren. PA‐Anweisungen halfen unterschiedlich gearteten Kindern: normal sich entwickelnde Leser, sowie auch risikobedingte und behinderte Leser; Vorschüler, Kindergartenteilnehmer und Erstklässler; Kinder der unteren Gundschulstufen (SES) als auch mittlerer und höherer Grundschulstufen (SES). PA‐Anweisungen verbesserten das Lesen, nicht jedoch das Buchstabieren bei behinderten Lesern. PA‐Anweisungen waren wirksamer, wenn sie zusammen mit den Buchstaben statt ohne Buchstaben unterrichtet wurden, wenn jeweils eine oder zwei PA‐Fertigkeiten statt ein Mehrfaches an PA‐Fertigkeiten unterrichtet wurde, wenn Kinder in kleinen Gruppen statt einzeln unterrichtet oder in Klassenräumen, und wenn der Unterricht insgesamt zwischen 5 und 18 Stunden andauerte, statt langfristiger. Klassenlehrer waren beim Unterricht von PA erfolgreich mit ihren Schülern. Das Wirkungsausmaß war unter Nutzung strengerer Experimentierpläne für den Unterricht größer, mit den von Troia (1999) abgeleiteten straffen Bewertungen. Zusammengefaßt fand man, daß die PA‐Anweisung einen statistisch nachweisbar bedeutenden Beitrag zum Erwerb von Lesefertigkeiten leisten. Une méta‐analyse quantitative pour évaluer les effets de l'enseignement de la conscience phonémique (CP) sur l'apprentissage de la lecture et de l'écriture a été conduite par la Commission Nationale de Lecture. Elle a porté sur 52 études publiées dans des journaux à comité de rédaction ayant conduit à 96 études comparant les résultats de groupes expérimentaux et contrôles. L'analyse de l'importance des effets révèle que l'enseignement de la CP sur le développement de la CP a un impact large et statistiquement significatif ( d = 0.86). L'enseignement de la CP a un effet modéré, statistiquement significatif ( d = 0.53) sur l'écriture ( d = 0.59). Non seulement la lecture de mots en bénéficie mais également la compréhension. L'enseignement de la CP a un impact sur la lecture dans toutes les conditions examinées, bien que la taille de l'effet soit plus large dans certaines conditions. L'enseignement de la CP est une aide pour différents types d'enfants: aussi bien pour ceux qui apprennent à lire normalement que pour les enfants à risque et pour ceux qui ont des difficultés; pour les enfants d'école maternelle ou de première année de primaire: pour les enfants de milieu social défavorisé aussi bien que pour ceux de classe moyenne. L'enseignement de la CP facilite le développement de la lecture mais pas celui de l'écriture chez les enfants en difficulté. L'enseignement de la CP a plus d'effet quand il a lieu avec des lettres que sans lettres, quand on enseigne une ou deux compétences de CP que de multiples compétences de CP, quand il a lieu en petit groupe que individuellement ou en salle de classe, et quand l'enseignement dureentre 5 et 18 heures plutôt que plus longtemps. L'enseignement de la CP en classe par des enseignants est efficace. La taille des effets est plus grande quand les études utilisent des plans expérimentaux plus rigoureux, et des évaluations rigoureuses provenant de Troia (1999). En bref, il apparaît que l'enseignement de la CP apporte une contribution statistiquement significative à l'acquisition de la lecture.
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, we investigated the degree to which children's inference generation ability generalises across different media and predicts narrative comprehension over and above basic language skills and vocabulary. To address both aims, we followed two cohorts of children aged 4 and 6 as they turned 6 and 8 years old, respectively. At each time point we assessed their inference and narrative comprehension skills using aural, televised and written stories. We also assessed their basic language skills and vocabulary. The findings demonstrated that children's inference generation skills were highly inter-related across different media for both cohorts and at both time points. Also, children's inference generation had a significant contribution to children's narrative comprehension over and above basic language skills, vocabulary and media factors. The current set of findings has important theoretical and practical implications for early diagnosis and intervention in young children's high-order comprehension skills.
Chapter
Full-text available
Processes Underlying Text ComprehensionTextbase FormationThe Situation ModelSummary
Chapter
This book is a collection of papers that explore the ways in which bilingual children cope with two language systems. The papers address issues in linguistics, psychology, and education bearing on the abilities that bilingual children use to understand language, to perform highly specialised operations with language, and to function in school settings. All of the papers provide detailed analysis about how specific problems are solved, how bilingualism influences those solutions, and how the social context affects the process. Finally, the implications of these findings for policy-setting and the development of bilingual education programmes are explored. This will be an important and useful volume at the forefront of topical research in an area which is exciting increasing interest among linguists and cognitive scientists.
Chapter
Introduction: Simple Ideas about Reading Comprehension. A Framework for Comprehension. Higher-Level Factors in Comprehension. The Linguistic-Conceptual Machinery for Comprehension. Word Identification, Decoding, and Phonological Awareness .Comprehension Instruction. Conclusion: A More General View of Comprehension Development
Article
An experimental design was used to investigate the effect of active versus passive storybook reading conditions on the vocabulary acquisition of kindergarteners who differed in level of prior vocabulary and phonological working memory. Sixty-six children, pretested on the Senechal Vocabulary Test-Adapted (SVT-A) for target word knowledge, categorized as high or low vocabulary level based on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R) performance, and categorized as high or low working memory level on the Children's Test of Nonword Repetition (CNRep), were randomly assigned to active or passive storybook reading conditions. While listening to a single episode of storybook narrative, children: (a) actively participated by answering a what or where question immediately after each sentence containing a target word, or (b) passively participated by listening to a recast containing a familiar synonym for each target word. Factorial analyses of SVT-A posttest target-word acquisition revealed that children with higher vocabulary knowledge acquired significantly more words than lower vocabulary peers; active participants acquired significantly more words than passive participants; and children with high versus low working memory did not differ in word acquisition. Of additional interest, the strong correlation between PPVT-R and CNRep scores was found to be comparable to that of the British Picture Vocabulary Scale and the CNRep.
Article
In this longitudinal study, the writing skill development of 154 Finnish-speaking children was followed from preschool to the third grade. The focus was on predictive associations between preschool writing readiness skills and later mechanics of writing, as well as between word recognition skill, mechanics of writing, and composition coherence. In addition, comparisons were made between boys and girls to see to what extent writing skill development is gender-specific. Multi-group structural equation modeling was used for statistical analysis. The results indicated that both mechanics of writing and composition coherence could be predicted from performance on the same skill at an earlier point in time. Preschool measures of phonological and visual-motor skills predicted later mechanics of writing. Word recognition worked as a predictor of later mechanics of writing and composition coherence, but only starting from second grade, when the development of the word recognition skill had become stabilized at a high enough level. Furthermore, first grade mechanics of writing predicted second grade composition coherence, but only at this early stage of productive writing when there were still difficulties in the mechanics of writing. Girls were better at tasks measuring mechanics of writing and wrote more coherent stories than boys. The gender difference in the mechanics of writing at the first grade level was explained by the presented model. Educational implications were discussed.
Article
This study provided 5 weeks of direct strategy instruction about narrative elements and relations in 4 first-grade classrooms (n = 83), all with materials that made minimal decoding demands on children's reading. Two comparison classrooms (n = 40) received comparable instruction on language development and poetry. A battery of assessments given at pretest and posttest showed that the intervention benefited children's comprehension of narratives in the picture-viewing modality as well as narrative meaning-making in listening comprehension and oral production modalities. Understanding and recall of main narrative elements improved, as did inference-making skills and understanding the psychological aspects of stories. Implications for enhancing beginning readers’ emerging narrative knowledge in primary grade classrooms are discussed.
Article
To clarify the relationship between oral language and early reading development, the authors administered to 39 children a broad range of oral language measures in 3 areas (metalinguistics, structural language, and narrative discourse); measures of background variables (IQ, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, gender, family literacy); and measures of reading ability (word recognition, pseudoword reading, passage comprehension) in kindergarten and in 1st and 2nd grades. The authors used regression analyses to identify parsimonious models that explained variance in early reading. The main finding of the study was that semantic abilities (i.e., oral definitions and word retrieval), not phonological awareness, predicted 2nd-grade reading comprehension. As expected, phonological awareness skill in kindergarten predicted single-word reading at 1st and 2nd grades. The finding that semantic skills predicted passage comprehension suggests that the importance of different oral language skills to early reading varies as a function of language domain, reading skill, and measurement point.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Evidence strongly suggests that shared book reading at home and in preschool is important for young children's development of the foundational skills required for the eventual mastery of decoding and comprehension. Yet the nuances of how learning from book reading might vary across these contexts and with children's skills are not well understood. One hundred and thirty children participating in a longitudinal investigation of literacy development were videotaped reading a storybook with a parent. Children were also videotaped in their 33 preschool classrooms during the instructional book-reading portion of the day. Readings were coded for adult and child contextualized and decontextualized language relating to both decoding and meaning-making skills, and relations between this talk and emergent literacy outcomes were analyzed. Results demonstrate that parents and teachers overwhelmingly focus their book-related talk on meaning-related rather than code-related information, and that the relations between outcomes and talk depend in part on children's initial levels of vocabulary skills. Implications for practice and research are discussed.