ArticlePDF Available

Using a Time Timer™ to increase appropriate waiting behavior in a child with developmental disabilities

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study aimed to examine the use of a predictive stimulus (Time Timer) and delayed reinforcement to increase appropriate waiting behavior in a child with developmental disabilities and problem behavior maintained by access to tangible items and activities. The study employed a changing criterion design across settings to gradually increase reinforcement delay from 1s to 10 min. Firstly a baseline phase was conducted to measure the duration of appropriate waiting behavior to access tangible reinforcers/activities. Phase 2 involved the use of a red cue card and the verbal instruction "wait". Phase 3 involved the introduction of the Time Timer with the cue card attached, and the verbal instruction "wait". Finally, Phase 4 utilised the Time Timer without the cue card. This method was an effective strategy for increasing appropriate waiting behavior with this participant in a school setting. The role of adding a concurrent activity during the reinforcement delay, using cues to predict reinforcement, future generalization, maintenance and the teaching of functionally equivalent skills are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Using a Time Timer
TM
to increase appropriate waiting
behavior in a child with developmental disabilities
Ian Grey
a
, Olive Healy
b,
*, Geraldine Leader
b
, Deirdre Hayes
b
a
Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland
b
National University of Ireland Galway, Galway, Ireland
Impulsiveness and self-control can be operationalised as choice-making behavior between a larger,
delayed reinforcer and a smaller, more immediate reinforcer (Jackson & Hackenberg, 1996). Impulsive
behavior occurs when responding produces more immediate, relatively smaller reinforcers at the cost
Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
ARTICLE INFO
Article history:
Received 26 June 2008
Accepted 3 July 2008
Keywords:
Delayed reinforcement
Developmental disabilities
Time Timer
TM
ABSTRACT
This study aimed to examine the use of a predictive stimulus (Time
Timer
TM
) and delayed reinforcement to increase appropriate
waiting behavior in a child with developmental disabilities and
problem behavior maintained by access to tangible items and
activities. The study employed a changing criterion design across
settings to gradually increase reinforcement delay from 1 s to
10 min. Firstly a baseline phase was conducted to measure the
duration of appropriate waiting behavior to access tangible
reinforcers/activities. Phase 2 involved the use of a red cue card
and the verbal instruction ‘‘wait’’. Phase 3 involved the introduction
of the Time Timer
TM
with the cue card attached, and the verbal
instruction ‘‘wait’’. Finally, Phase 4 utilised the Time Timer
TM
without the cue card. This method was an effective strategy for
increasing appropriate waiting behavior with this participant in a
school setting. The role of adding a concurrent activity during the
reinforcement delay, using cues to predict reinforcement, future
generalization, maintenance and the teaching of functionally
equivalent skills are discussed.
ß2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
* Corresponding author at: School of Psychology, National University of Ireland Galway, St. Anthony’s, Newcastle Road,
Galway, Ireland. Tel.: +353 86 3893128; fax: +353 91 521355.
E-mail address: olive.healy@nuiglaway.ie (O. Healy).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Research in Developmental
Disabilities
0891-4222/$ – see front matter ß2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.07.001
of delayed, larger reinforcers. Self-control occurs when responding produces greater delayed
reinforcers at the expense of more immediate, smaller reinforcers (Logue, 1995; Dixon et al., 1998).
Behaviors that yield delayed reinforcement are highly adaptive in day-to-day life (Stromer, McComan,
& Rehfeldt, 2000), and as such are a socially significant behavior and worth investigating in applied
behavior analysis.
Various methods have been used to establish and maintain reinforcement delay. Schweitzer and
Sulzer-Azaroff (1988) described one method whereby the delay is gradually increased for a larger
reinforcer while the smaller reinforcer remains immediately available. Dixon and Cummins (2001)
extended this research further by illustrating that self-control may be increased by establishing a
history in which participants are gradually exposed to progressive delays, and are concurrently given
the choice to engage in an intervening activity during that delay. Participants had a choice between:
(a) a small immediate reinforcer, (b) a larger delayed item without a response requirement during the
delay, and (c) a larger delayed item with a response requirement during the delay. All participants
showed a preference for the latter option, and during this contingency, no problem behaviors
occurred. However, these studies do not examine visual supports or cues that may serve to predict the
availability of reinforcement.
Williams (1999) found that choice for delayed reinforcement would be more likely when stimuli
presented during the delay reliably predicted reinforcement. Further studies have used tokens, points
and star charts as the predictive stimuli (Kazdin, 1982). In these cases the stimuli that predict
reinforcement at the end of the delay may serve as conditioned reinforcers through multiple pairings
with the delayed reinforcers. However, it has been noted that more naturally occurring conditioned
reinforcers as predictive stimuli would be useful for ensuring generalization to other settings
(Williams & Dunn, 1991). These could be verbal in nature, such as praise from a parent, teacher or
caregiver, or verbal reminders of the reinforcement that is to occur (Hayes & Hayes, 1993).
However, people with developmental disabilities may not respond well to verbal instructions due
to limited or absent verbal comprehension repertoires. Individuals with a developmental disability
often present with a restricted verbal repertoire, which is a common factor associated with
impulsivity (Mischel & Mischel, 1983). It has been suggested that an adult human’s increased
preference for delayed, larger reinforcers may somehow be linked to their advanced verbal abilities
(Schweitzer & Sulzer-Azaroff, 1988). This may explain why people with a limited verbal capacity
experience difficulties in waiting for delayed reinforcement.
Another important aspect of stimuli that predict reinforcement is the length of time one is required
to wait before the reinforcer is delivered. Vollmer, Borrero, Lalli, and Daniel (1999) demonstrated that
participants with developmental disabilities and severe challenging behaviors were more likely to
exhibit self-control than impulsive choices when the longer delays were signalled rather than
unsignalled. They also recommended that a further investigation into ‘timed’ delays (the availability of
a visual timer throughout the delay) for future research.
An examination of impulsivity and self-control with regards to individuals with intellectual
disabilities and problem behavior is important. Access to consumable or tangible reinforcers
frequently maintains severe behavior problems displayed by such individuals (Vollmer et al., 1999).
Many studies have focused on impulsive behavior maintained by token reinforcement (money) and
not on tangible or attention maintained behaviors that are also sensitive to impulsivity (Vollmer et al.,
1999).
In the case of signalled reinforcement delay to promote coping and tolerance skills, the appropriate
‘waiting’ behavior is essentially under the stimulus control of the signal (verbal instruction or visual
cue). Approaching the treatment of tolerance in delayed reinforcement with persons with
developmental disabilities using signalled reinforcement delay could be very useful for maintenance
and generalization of the appropriate behavior. In addition, the use of stimulus fading that includes
highlighting a physical dimension (e.g., colour, size, etc.) of a stimulus to increase the likelihood of a
correct response followed by a systematic fading of the exaggerated dimension could be applied to
promote appropriate behavior during delayed reinforcement (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007).
The present study aims to increase appropriate ‘waiting’ behavior using a Time Timer
TM
with an
individual presenting with problem behavior maintained by access to tangible reinforcement. It
employs a changing criterion design with the application of a Time Timer
TM
as a predictive stimulus. A
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
360
delay in the delivery of a preferred reinforcer is gradually increased and appropriate waiting behavior
is reinforced during the time delay.
1. Method
1.1. Participant and setting
The participant was an 11-year-old girl who lived with her parents and attended a school for
children with intellectual disabilities and pervasive developmental disorders. She shared a classroom
with four other students—two of whom are her siblings. She is one of a set of triplets. The participant
had been diagnosed by a registered clinical psychologist with a moderate level of intellectual
disability. She also presented with cerebral palsy, which affects the fine motor movement of her left
arm, hand and foot. The participant emited challenging behavior in the form of physical aggression
and tantrum behaviors. She could name objects in her environment, but had difficulties asking for
objects or help with activities. She has an age-equivalent of oral expression of 2 years. She can follow
two-step verbal directions.
1.2. Response measurement and reliability
Inappropriate waiting behavior was defined as tantrum behavior that included crying, dropping to
the floor, hitting other students or adults, screaming and repeating the word ‘no’ or the name of a
preferred object or activity at a volume above normal conversational level. The dependent variable
was identified as appropriate waiting behavior and was recorded when an absence of the behaviors
outlined above was observed during an interval.
During both functional assessment and treatment evaluation, a trained observer recorded the
target behaviors during observation periods via paper and pencil. A second observer independently
collected data during 82% of functional assessment observation periods and 65% of intervention
evaluation sessions. Interobserver agreement was examined on a session-by-session basis by dividing
the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100%. Agreement values were 88%
for functional assessment sessions, and 94% for intervention evaluation.
1.3. Apparatus
A stopwatch was used to measure appropriate waiting behavior during baseline conditions and
intervention phases. A Time Timer
TM
and red card (4 in. 4 in.) were used as predictive stimuli. The
Time Timer
TM
is 12 in. 12 in. and resembles a clock. The numbers on the Time Timer
TM
reflect the
passage of time in minutes, and range from 55 to 5 min in a countdown style. To indicate an amount of
time, there is a movable piece on the timer that can be ‘set’ for a particular time frame, e.g., 10 min.
When this piece is moved, a red wedge represents the amount of time, and reduces in size gradually as
the time passes. The red wedge disappears when the time frame is over.
1.4. Experimental design
A changing criterion design with a baseline and three intervention phases was employed to
increase appropriate waiting behavior.
1.5. Functional assessment
The participant was observed directly for a 2-week period in naturally occurring situations that had
been identified by care staff as being related to a high probability of the occurrence of target behaviors.
Staff responses to target behaviors were recorded, as were probable antecedents in an anecdotal
fashion. Every occurrence of the target behavior was analysed according to the anecdotal accounts of
possible antecedents and consequences maintained by the experimenter. Results indicated that most
of the participant’s problem behaviors occurred when objects or activities she wanted were not
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
361
temporally available such as wanting to look at preferred pictures of cartoon characters or wanting to
hold preferred toys. Behaviors also occurred when waiting for lunch or snack time or wanting to get
access to the school playground. These behaviors had significantly impacted on acquisition of novel
skills at the time of assessment. The participant would not sit at a desk in her classroom, avoided
engagement with any academic curriculum-based activity and spent most of her school day at a
computer looking at preferred cartoon characters either on a computer or in a book in an adjacent
room. Results of the functional assessment demonstrated that the inappropriate behaviors were
maintained by immediate access to tangible items and activities.
1.6. General procedure
The general procedure involved a baseline phase and three intervention phases and, in the interest
of clarity, each phase is outlined separately below. The purpose of employing three intervention
phases was to establish stimulus control for waiting using a red card during an activity that the
participant enjoyed. Following this the goal of intervention was to transfer stimulus control from the
red card to the Time Timer
TM
. The red card was selected because the moveable piece of the Time
Timer
TM
is coloured red.
1.7. Phase 1: Baseline
Baseline consisted of Sessions 1–10. An activity that the participant enjoyed engaging in outdoors
was selected in order to determine the maximum amount of time the participant would wait
appropriately for a reinforcer. The participant enjoyed an activity that involved running up a series of
steps in the playground while holding the hands of an adult. Before the activity began the participant
was told to ‘wait’ and the experimenter set a stopwatch to measure appropriate waiting behavior. If
the participant waited appropriately for 30 s the activity was delivered. Once inappropriate waiting
behavior occurred the stopwatch was stopped and the time was recorded. Contingent on the
occurrence of inappropriate behavior the participant was directed to another activity in the
playground. Ten baseline probes of this activity were conducted over 2 consecutive days.
1.8. Phase 1: Red card and verbal instruction
Phase 1 included Sessions 11–38. During this phase the experimenter held the red card in front of
the participant and gave the verbal instruction ‘wait’. The card was removed after one second and the
next interval was initiated. This was repeated until four consecutive 1 s intervals were complete. The
amount of time the red card was present and the participant was required to wait for the game to
resume was systematically extended. Intervals of time for waiting were 1, 3, 6, 10, 16, 7 and 20 s,
respectively (see Table 1). Only the word ‘wait’ preceded the presentation of the red card. The criterion
for increasing the interval of time was four consecutive intervals with no occurrence of inappropriate
waiting behavior.
During interval 31, instead of increasing the criterion to a longer delay in reinforcement, the delay
in the delivery of reinforcement was actually reduced from 16 to 7 s. This reduction in criterion was
implemented randomly as a decrease in demand to ensure the participant would continue to wait
appropriately in the absence of inappropriate waiting behavior. Although a very gradual change in
criterion was implemented across this phase, it was important to set up an opportunity for success as
the change in criterion became more difficult to reach. During interval 35 the criterion was once again
increased from 7 to 20 s.
1.9. Phase 2: Red card on Time Timer
TM
and verbal instruction
Phase 2 included Sessions 39–82. During Phase 2 the intervention was transferred to the classroom
setting. The participant was seated at a desk with other students and contingent on sitting
appropriately she was presented with a picture of a favourite cartoon character. After approximately
10 s the picture was removed and the red card was placed on the Time Timer
TM
for 15 s along with the
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
362
verbal instruction ‘wait’. Contingent on the non-occurrence of target behavior the cartoon picture was
delivered. The intervals for waiting were systematically extended up to 2 m. The criterion for
increasing the time interval was four consecutive intervals without the occurrence of inappropriate
waiting behavior (see Table 1). Two minutes was considered the shortest interval that could be
represented on the Time Timer
1
. No target behaviors occurred during this phase. During interval 63
the delay in the delivery of reinforcement was reduced from 65 to 30 s instead of increasing the
criterion. Similarly to Phase 2, this reduction in criterion was implemented randomly as a decrease in
demand to ensure the participant would continue to wait appropriately when a gradually increasing
demand was placed upon her. During interval 67 the criterion was once again increased from 30 to
75 s. The criterion for this phase for increasing the time interval was four consecutive intervals with no
occurrence of inappropriate waiting behavior.
1.10. Phase 3: Time Timer
TM
Phase 3 included Sessions 83–122. During Phase 3, the red card was removed and replaced by the
red moveable wedge on the Time Timer
TM
. The amount of time required to wait prior to the
presentation of a preferred reinforcer was systematically extended to 10 min by gradually increasing
the amount of time the participant was required to wait (see Table 1). Similarly to Phases 1 and 2,
Table 1
Baseline duration of appropriate waiting behavior and systematic increases in intervals during Phases 2–4
Sessions Maximum duration of appropriate waiting (s) Did inappropriate behavior occur?
Phase 1: Baseline
1–10 1 +
Sessions Intervals of appropriate waiting (s) Did inappropriate behavior occur?
Phase 2: Red card and verbal instruction
11–14 1
15–18 3
19–22 6
23–26 10
27–30 16
31–34 7
35–38 20
Phase 3: Red card on Time Timer
TM
and verbal instruction
39–42 15
43–46 20
47–50 30
51–54 40
55–58 50
59–62 65
63–66 30
67–70 75
71–74 90
75–78 105
79–82 120
Phase 4: Time Timer
TM
83–86 150
87–90 180
91–94 240
95–98 180
99–102 300
103–106 360
107–110 420
111–114 480
115–118 540
119–122 600
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
363
instead of increasing the criterion at interval 95, the delay in the delivery of reinforcement was
reduced from 240 to 180 s. This reduction in criterion was randomly applied. During Session 99 the
criterion was once again increased from 180 to 300 s. Sessions 99–122 showed increases in the
waiting intervals from 300 to 600 s.
In addition, during this phase (Session 95) desktop activities such as matching tasks, reading tasks
and art activities were introduced while the participant waited for a reinforcer. The purpose of this
was to ensure that the participant learned to wait while engaging in demanding tasks. Access to
choosing additional reinforcers, other than cartoon pictures, was introduced at this point. When stable
responding was observed (no occurrence of the inappropriate waiting behavior during Sessions 95–
106), the Time Timer
TM
was used in a novel setting involving contingent access to snacks at
lunchtimes (Session 107).
2. Results
Results of Phases 1–4 are presented in Fig. 1. During Phase 1, the maximum amount of time spent
waiting appropriately was 1 s during Sessions 1 and 2. During the eight remaining probes there was no
instance of appropriate waiting and the duration recorded was 0 s.
With the introduction of the discriminative stimulus and verbal instruction during Phase 2,
appropriate waiting was increased using a changing criterion from 1 to 20 s. During Phase 3 the delivery
of reinforcement was delayed from 15 to 120 s using both the discriminative stimulus and verbal
direction paired with the Time Timer
TM
. During Phase 4 the delay in reinforcement and appropriate
waiting behavior was increased from 150 to 600 s in the presence of the Time Timer
TM
alone.
Over the three intervention phases the changing criterion was increased by greater increments. For
example, during Phase 2 the mean increment was 4 s. During Phases 3 and 4 the mean increments
were 11.6 and 56 s, respectively.
No inappropriate behavior was demonstrated during the changing criterion phases (see Table 1).
Appropriate waiting behavior was gradually increased using a changing criterion from 1 s to 10 min
across 122 Sessions.
3. Discussion
This study aimed to increase the appropriate waiting behavior of a child with an intellectual
disability and problem behavior maintained by access to tangible reinforcement. The results illustrate
that the participant’s appropriate waiting behavior was successfully increased from 1 s to 10 min. This
outcome also resulted in an increase in classroom instruction time whereby during the waiting
interval, the experimenter introduced task-demand at the desktop with access to a choice of multiple
reinforcers. Gradually the reinforcers for waiting were increased from one choice to a variety of
choices in different settings. These results will be discussed in terms of the addition of the activity to
Fig. 1. Duration of appropriate waiting behavior across Phases 1–4.
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
364
engage in during the waiting period, the role of cue’s in predicting reinforcement, the future fading of
the discriminative stimulus to further generalisation of the skill, and with regards to functionally
equivalent skills teaching.
3.1. Addition of concurrent activity
These results support Dixon and Cummins (2001) findings that self-control can be increased by
gradually extending reinforcement delay, while concurrently given an activity to engage in during the
delay. When the participant reached 180 s of appropriate waiting behavior, the experimenter
introduced task-demand at the desk in the classroom. This was a significant outcome for the
participant, as before this intervention was introduced, she would not engage in any learning activity
at the desk. This was having a great impact on her learning, so the present results indicate an
improvement in her overall quality of life. Her quality of life has improved in terms of her ability to
engage in discrete-trial instruction in the classroom, and her new adaptive skill of waiting
appropriately in the presence of the timer.
3.2. Visual stimulis to predict reinforcement
The present study used a visual cue as a discriminative stimulus to predict reinforcement and to
indicate when reinforcement would be delivered. The successful outcome of this study would appear
to support Vollmer et al.’s (1999) finding that participants with developmental disabilities and
challenging behaviors are more likely to exhibit self-control when delays in reinforcement are
signalled. The present results also corroborate Bower, McLean, and Meacham (1966) findings that
participants respond positively to being able to predict when a reinforcer would be delivered. The
participant in the current study did not engage in any challenging behaviors during the signalled and
informed delays. A possible limitation of this study is that it employed both a visual and verbal cue
(red card and ‘‘wait’’), and then a timed visual cue and verbal cue (timer and ‘‘wait’’). Perhaps future
research could compare the use of a visual (cue card) with a timed signal (Time Timer
TM
), with and
without a verbal cue to accompany it to investigate which of these methods are more effective—in
order to establish the most effective method of teaching this vital skill.
3.3. Generalisation of the skill
Hayes and Hayes (1993) suggested the use of a more naturally occurring predictive stimulus, in the
form of verbal statements from a parent or teacher, to ensure generalisation to other settings. In this
case, the participant’s language skills were limited (2-year-old level), thus verbal reminders of the
reinforcement to occur may have had limited comprehensive value. The addition of the Time Timer
TM
provided a visual predictive stimulus as well as a verbal stimulus (experimenter saying ‘‘wait’’).
However, in order for the participant to establish and maintain her new skill/behavior across settings,
the timer will eventually need to be faded out over time.
The timer is a useful tool and presents with the possibility of gradual stimulus fading. It closely
resembles a clock, apart from the red wedge to indicate the passage of time. The colour of the wedge
could be gradually faded and eventually the hands of the clock could be introduced to indicate the
passage of time. In addition, fading the coloured wedge could involve the use of coloured stickers to be
placed on the face of the timer to indicate when the time was up, and these stickers could then be
transferred to a regular clock. A portable Time Timer
TM
wristwatch is also available, where the red
wedge of the clock is displayed digitally on a wristwatch. The portable watch could be gradually
introduced in place of the desktop timer and could be used across settings and instructors to ensure
maintenance and generalisation of the skill.
3.4. Functional equivalence
Vollmer et al. (1999) highlighted the need for future research to investigate impulsive behavior in
light of a tangible or attention-based function—as these behaviors are also sensitive to impulsivity. The
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
365
results of the functional assessment in the present study indicated that the participant’s tantrums
(inappropriate impulsive behavior) were maintained by access to tangible items/activities. Thus the
present research provided a little more insight into this area. Functionally equivalent-based
interventions have been an effective meta-strategy in the treatment of challenging behaviors. Cooper
et al. (2007) state that if a problem behavior serves a specific function, then the intervention should
provide the reinforcer (e.g., tangible item) contingent on a more appropriate response. This new
response serves the same function for the individual and, if the intervention is implemented
successfully, provides a more efficient, effective and socially valid means of reaching the specific goal.
Campbell and Lutzker (1993) illustrate this point in their study, where they successfully employed
functional communication training to eliminate tantrums and property destruction in an 8-year-old
boy. Appropriate waiting is an essential adaptive skill in day-to-day life for all people, and further
applied research in this area is greatly needed.
4. Conclusion
The present study examined the use of a Time Timer
TM
to gradually increase reinforcement delay in
a child with developmental disabilities and problem behavior. This study supports previous findings
that participants respond well to knowing that reinforcement will be available (Vollmer et al., 1999)
and when reinforcement will be available (Bower et al., 1966). It also lends substantiation to the
proposal that functional based interventions are an effective strategy in the treatment of problem
behaviors (Campbell & Lutzker, 1993). Further research could compare the efficacy of using either a
predictive stimulus (cue card), and timed stimulus (timer) or both—with and without verbal cues, in
order to gain a greater understanding of the behavioral mechanisms in effect in an intervention of this
nature.
References
Bower, G., McLean, J., & Meacham, J. (1966). Value of knowing when reinforcement is due. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 62, 184–192.
Campbell, R. V., & Lutzker, J. R. (1993). Using functional equivalence training to reduce severe challenging behavior: A case study.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 203–316.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Dixon, M. R., & Cummins, A. (2001). Self control in children with autism: Response allocation during delays to reinforcement. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 491–495.
Dixon, M. R., Hayes, L. J., Binder, L. M., Manthey, S., Sigman, C., & Zdanowski, D. M. (1998). Using a self-control training procedure to
increase appropriate behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 203–210.
Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Applied implications of current JEAB research on derived relations and delayed reinforcement.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 507–511.
Jackson, K., & Hackenberg, T. D. (1996). Token reinforcement, choice, and self control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 66, 29–49.
Kazdin, A. E. (1982). The token economy: A decade later. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 431–445.
Logue, A. W. (1995). Self control: Waiting until tomorrow for what you want today. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self control strategies. Child Development, 54, 603–
619.
Schweitzer, J. B., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1988). Self control: Teaching tolerance for delay in impulsive children. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 173–186.
Stromer, R., McComan, J. J., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Designing interventions that include delayed reinforcement: Implications of
recent laboratory research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 359–371.
Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Lalli, J. S., & Daniel, D. (1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsivity in children with severe behavior
disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 451–466.
Williams, B. A. (1999). Value transmission in discrimination learning involving stimulus chains. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 72, 177–185.
Williams, B. A., & Dunn, R. (1991). Preference for conditioned reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 37–46.
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
366
... The protocol employed a DRO procedure during the delay using token reinforcement for each trial without exhibiting problematic behavior and a terminal reinforcement criterion of three tokens. However, as others have found, individuals are more likely to tolerate delay when some other stimuli are available: competing stimuli or alternative sources of reinforcement (Grey et al., 2009;Sumter et al., 2020). Continuing with simplicity in mind, we did not make this a response requirement as others have (Drifke et al., 2020;Hanley et al., 2014), but rather approached it more naturally, as in, "Here are some things you can do while you wait". ...
... While you are waiting, play with your other things to keep you busy." Along the same lines, we used a phone timer as an aid to implementation instead of a specialized "visual timer" as others have used (Grey et al., 2009) thinking this would be what parents or teachers might have naturally available. Of course, there is a great deal of research and behavioral technology embedded in the protocol. ...
Article
Full-text available
Delay tolerance training is used to teach children to accept delayed access to a requested item or event without exhibiting challenging behaviors. It is used during schedule thinning following acquisition of a functional communication response (FCR) or trained on its own in a skills‐based treatment package. Typically, delay tolerance training occurs in a clinic or hospital, and rarely in a school classroom. Most often delay tolerance training has been used with children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This study integrated findings from previous research on delay tolerance training to evaluate training in a classroom, with a child with Smith‐Magenis syndrome, using a simplified protocol. The training resulted in acquisition of delay tolerance for 5 min from a baseline of 32 s (average). The participant's teachers were then trained to use similar strategies throughout the school day and quickly acquired and applied the procedure with reported benefits.
... study, the delay was represented through various stories, and the rewards were stickers. Other studies have incorporated design features such as signaled delays (e.g., Grey et al., 2009) or visual aids to ensure that children understand the experimental tasks (Staubitz et al., 2018). In addition, gamification has been employed to enhance engagement (e.g., Burns et al., 2021;Jones et al., 2014) in conjunction with children-appropriate choice frames (McCormack et al., 2024). ...
Article
Delay discounting has been shown to occur across the lifespan. However, how time affects the value of nonmonetary rewards in children has received relatively little attention. This study compared choice of Lego bricks by children using two delay discounting estimation procedures, one experiential and one hypothetical task. The tasks involved a choice between two rewards, a large reward (five Lego bricks) after a delay, and an immediate reward of lesser magnitude. The results suggest that the subjective value of Lego bricks changed across delays, and the loss of value was accurately described by a hyperbolic function, but only on the experiential task, whereas functions resulting from the hypothetical task were nonsystematic in most cases. We discuss two possible explanations, one pertaining the abilities needed to grasp hypothetical situations, and another pertaining operational differences between tasks that may influence choice patterns.
... Timers can be a stand-alone device or accessed on a device such as a phone or a tablet. Visual timers can be utilized to increase reinforcement delay (Grey et al., 2009), which can be very helpful while waiting for an event to start or waiting in a line. Visual timers can also enhance time-processing ability (Janeslätt et al., 2014). ...
... People with high-functioning autism have a generally close-to-normal management of millisecond time but the management of interval time and circadian time is often erratic. Some research advises certain remediation techniques for people with autism, allowing them to limit the negative effects of altered time perception (Jurek et al., 2019) through the use of specially designed timers (Grey et al., 2009), visual planning tools (Koyama & Wang, 2011) or software (Campillo et al., 2014). While this research is still in its infancy, it is of obvious organizational interest as its implementation would require almost no financial investment and could be done with only a moderate human cost (Landsiedel & Williams, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper investigates the relationship that people with high functioning autism have with organizational temporality by considering this operationalization within the framework of humanistic management. To do so, it proposes an analysis based on seven propositions. Autism is a disorder that is still poorly understood and often linked to social depictions that are as unfounded as they are repulsive. It remains an unexplored area of study in the field of management sciences. Existing scholarship has established that people with autism have great difficulty finding and retaining employment. While it is well known that they have weak social skills, their difficulties in relation to time have only been studied in medical research, even though organizational temporality substantially shapes the functioning of teams. The operationalization of autistic temporality as a particular temporality within humanistic management allows for the development of a new conceptual framework based on a consideration of neuro-atypia. This paper begins with a presentation of the theoretical background. It then develops the theoretical model. Implications, limitations and directions for further studies are discussed before concluding.
... To compensate for the difficulties in time management, adults with ID can benefit from time assistive devices (TADs) [14]. Commonly used TADs are visual activity schedules, timers, and electronic planning devices, and they are often reported to increase independence and self-confidence [15][16][17][18][19][20][21]. However, insufficient support from the physical environment and insufficient introduction or not enough time training in the use of assistive devices can lead to non-use of provided TADs [22][23][24]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Limited time management skills cause problems in daily life for people with mild intellectual disability (ID) and “Let’s Get Organized” (LGO) is a promising manual-based occupational therapy group intervention aiming to support management skills. Aims/Objectives To evaluate the applicability of the Swedish version of LGO-S by i) exploring enhancements in time management skills, satisfaction with daily occupations, and aspects of executive functioning in people with time-management difficulties and mild ID, and ii) describing clinical experiences of using the LGO-S for people with mild ID. Material and methods Twenty-one adults with mild ID were included. Data were collected pre-/post-intervention and at 3- and 12-month follow-ups with: Swedish version of Assessment of Time Management Skills (ATMS-S), Satisfaction with Daily Occupation (SDO-13), and Weekly Calendar Planning Activity (WCPA-SE). There were few follow-up participants (n = 6-9). Results Significant change in time management skills that maintained at 12-months follow-ups. Significant increase in regulation of emotions at 12-month follow-up. Results at 12-months follow-up indicated sustainability in outcomes as measured by ATMS-S. A non-significant positive trend was observed in other outcomes between pre- and post-intervention. Conclusions and significance LGO-S seems applicable for improving skills in time management, organisation and planning also for people with mild ID.
... The children's experiences in the present study reveal that the system of dots helps them under- These findings correspond with the results from other studies that used time tools and schedules (Janeslätt et al., 2014). Grey et al. (2009) show that combining a time tool with a picture indicating waiting increased appropriate waiting behaviour. Furthermore, Knight et al. (2014) show that visual activity schedules had positive effects on on-task behaviour, decreased need for prompting, improvement of correct task and schedule completion steps and also improved transition behaviour in children with autism spectrum disorder. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Children in need of special support often display delays in time processing ability, affecting everyday functioning. MyTime is an intervention programme for systematic training of time processing ability. To support preschool children's development of time processing ability and everyday functioning, it is necessary to include their perspectives of the MyTime intervention programme. A previous study shows that MyTime is feasible with children in the preschool setting and shows positive effects on time processing ability for older children in special schools. Yet, there is a lack of knowledge regarding how preschool children experience the intervention programme and how they understand its meaning. The aim of this study was to explore the experiences and the meaning of using MyTime from the perspective of children with informal needs of special support (INS) 5–6 years of age in the preschool context. Methods To explore the children's perspectives, video‐recorded interviews with 21 children were analysed hermeneutically. To facilitate the interview situation with the children in need of special support, the Talking Mats© was used. Both body and spoken languages were analysed. Results The results reveal children as active participants, willing to share their experiences of using the MyTime intervention in the preschool context. The conceptualization of the children's experiences and expressions uncovers their meaning of using the MyTime intervention as to know and to understand time by doing. Conclusions When children are given the opportunity to use concrete tools to understand and measure time, they experience themselves as active participants involved and engaged in the intervention. They reveal meaningful experiences to be able to manage time that facilitate their everyday functioning and participation in the preschool context.
... ). Dans leur étude,Grey, Healy, Leader et Hayes (2009) rendent compte de l'intérêt d'utiliser ce type d'outil auprès d'un enfant avec des troubles développementaux afin d'augmenter son comportement d'attente. Ils concluent que cette méthode est une stratégie efficace pour augmenter un comportement d'attente appropriée dans un cadre scolaire. ...
Article
Food stealing is often a serious behavioral problem among children with diagnoses of autism and other developmental disorders. Very few empirical studies concerning this behavioral challenge have been reported. We applied a correspondence training procedure to teach self‐control as replacement behavior to four children with autism and developmental disorders who displayed food stealing in the community. A changing criterion design embedded within a nonconcurrent multiple‐probe across participants design was used. The treatment succeeded for all four participants by increasing latency to eating highly preferred food to a predetermined criterion and reducing occurrences of food stealing to zero. Three participants generalized the replacement behavior to natural settings and maintained the behavior for 2 weeks, 1 month, 2 months, 3 months, and 4 months. One participant without expressive language was taught successfully during treatment trials but failed to maintain and generalize the behavior. A functional relation between delaying food eating and Say‐Do correspondence training was demonstrated.
Article
Time is a fundamentally important dimension of human experience and responding adaptively in terms of this dimension is critical to human personal and societal functioning. However, there is an important distinction to be made between responding to time as a physical or nonarbitrary dimension of existence and time as an abstract concept. It is the latter that is critical to the type of self-knowledge and societal organization that is unique to human life. Relational frame theory (RFT) sees relational framing, and temporal relational framing in particular, as key to this uniquely human level of responding. The purpose of this article is to (1) explain temporal relational framing and the relevant research into this skill; (2) discuss existing psychological research into time-related skill sets in humans; and (3) explore how the RFT approach to human temporal responding might amplify and extend the existing research base, in particular with respect to the acquisition and training of key aspects of temporal responding.
Article
The extent to which multiple schedules are an effective schedule thinning method following functional communication training (FCT) relies on the control the schedule‐correlated stimuli exert over behavior. Thus, the stimuli used to signal the schedule in place (e.g., reinforcement and extinction) in a multiple schedule arrangement require special attention. To date, the majority of the research on multiple schedules has evaluated the use of different arbitrary signals as schedule‐correlated stimuli (e.g., poster boards). These signals are considered static as they lack movement. More recently, some studies have successfully used dynamic signals, which include movement or animation, within multiple schedule arrangements. However, the extent to which one type of signal may result in faster stimulus control over behavior has not been evaluated. Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare the use of static and dynamic signals as schedule‐correlated stimuli in multiple schedules used within the context of FCT. Four children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder participated in the study. The results suggest that no differences in discriminated manding were observed for three out of four participants. Only dynamic signals resulted in discriminated manding for one participant.
Article
Full-text available
Pigeons were presented with a concurrent-chains schedule in which both choice alternatives led to the same terminal-link stimulus, which was followed by food. Superimposed on the food-reinforced presentations of the terminal-link stimulus was a second schedule of presentations of the same stimulus that were followed by no food. The absolute number of these no-food stimulus presentations was held constant while their relative frequency assigned to one or the other choice alternative was systematically varied. Preference for a given choice alternative tracked the relative frequency of these stimulus presentations, thus demonstrating that they served as reinforcers. These results resolve conflicts in the literature regarding the effect of conditioned reinforcement on choice.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated a procedure to increase the selection of larger, more delayed reinforcers (i.e., more advantageous in the long run) over smaller immediate reinforcers, in an effort to increase a key aspect of self-control in children. Six preschoolers, including one comparison subject, identified by their teachers as impulsive, were preassessed and found consistently to select smaller immediate reinforcers over larger, more delayed ones. The teaching procedure consisted of gradually increasing the durations of the delay interval over many sessions. The follow-up assessments showed that 5 of these children increased the proportion of their choices of the delayed reinforcers. Before training, indifference points ranged from 1.7 to 51.7 s; following treatment, points rose to a range of 37.5 to at least 90 s, with 3 children preferring the larger reinforcer at all delay intervals tested. The results demonstrated the feasibility of teaching young children to make choices more advantageous to them in the long run.
Article
Full-text available
In 4 experiments concerning preference for knowing when reinforcement is to be delivered––although that information has no apparent instrumental value––pigeons chose between informative and noninformative stimulus sequences. Following an informative choice, the stimulus was correlated with the prevailing interval before reinforcement; following a noninformative choice, it was not. Strong preference for the informative sequence, regardless of the proportion of short vs. long reinforcement intervals, was unaffected by addition of a reliable cue at the moment of reinforcement. Cue strength of the various stimuli was assessed. A control experiment excluded explanations in terms of preference for unpredictable sequences, variety, or specific stimuli. The results are discussed in terms of secondary reinforcement.
Article
Full-text available
Pigeons were exposed to self-control procedures that involved illumination of light-emitting diodes (LEDs) as a form of token reinforcement. In a discrete-trials arrangement, subjects chose between one and three LEDs; each LED was exchangeable for 2-s access to food during distinct posttrial exchange periods. In Experiment 1, subjects generally preferred the immediate presentation of a single LED over the delayed presentation of three LEDs, but differences in the delay to the exchange period between the two options prevented a clear assessment of the relative influence of LED delay and exchange-period delay as determinants of choice. In Experiment 2, in which delays to the exchange period from either alternative were equal in most conditions, all subjects preferred the delayed three LEDs more often than in Experiment-1. In Experiment 3, subjects preferred the option that resulted in a greater amount of food more often if the choices also produced LEDs than if they did not. In Experiment 4, preference for the delayed three LEDs was obtained when delays to the exchange period were equal, but reversed in favor of an immediate single LED when the latter choice also resulted in quicker access to exchange periods. The overall pattern of results suggests that (a) delay to the exchange period is a more critical determinant of choice than is delay to token presentation; (b) tokens may function as conditioned reinforcers, although their discriminative properties may be responsible for the self-control that occurs under token reinforcer arrangements; and (c) previously reported differences in the self-control choices of humans and pigeons may have resulted at least in part from the procedural conventions of using token reinforcers with human subjects and food reinforcers with pigeon subjects.
Article
Full-text available
The present study evaluated a technique for teaching self-control and increasing desirable behaviors among adults with developmental disabilities. Results showed that when participants were initially given the choice between an immediate smaller reinforcer and a larger delayed reinforcer, all participants repeatedly chose the smaller reinforcer. Concurrent fixed-duration/progressive-duration reinforcement schedules then were introduced in which initially both the smaller and larger reinforcers were available immediately. Thereafter, progressively increasing delays were introduced for the schedule associated with the larger reinforcer only. When initial short-duration requirements for access to the larger reinforcer were gradually increased, participants repeatedly selected the larger reinforcer, thereby demonstrating increased self-control.
Article
The study of “metacognition” has become vigorous in recent years, with extensive research exploring the development of children’s knowledge of effective strategies in attention, comprehension, and memory (e.g., Brown, 1980; Flavell & Wellman, 1977). In contrast, the child’s developing understanding of essential strategies for self-regulation — a core aspect of human functioning, basic to virtually all conceptions of personality — has been neglected. Perhaps this neglect reflects the fact that until recently there were few objective criteria against which one could assess the relative efficacy of various strategies for self-control. Studies of the conditions that enhance or impede successful delay of gratification in children (e.g., Miller & Karniol, 1976a, 1976b; Mischel, 1974, 1981b; Toner, 1981; Toner & Smith, 1977; Yates, Lippett, & Yates, 1981) now provide a basis for assessing the child’s developing understanding against objective criteria of efficacy.
Article
"Self Control: Waiting Until Tomorrow for What You Want Today" provides a comprehensive answer to the question of why it is so difficult for some people (and animals) to show self-control under certain conditions. Alexandra W. Logue explains how evolution has affected our ability to choose actions that, over the long run, will result in valuable consequences. She argues that evolutionary factors have caused us to discount delayed events, making it difficult to wait or work for things that may be important to us, but which may not occur for some time. Integrating both basic and applied research on self-control, Logue describes the research base that links self-control and evolution, in addition to detailing methods that can be used to lessen the constraints of our evolutionary heritage. The author also describes applications of basic research to understanding and treating a wide variety of self-control problems. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A case study is described in which functional equivalence was used to reduce severe tantrums and property destruction by an 8-year-old boy with autism. Although data were collected on the frequency of these behaviors, no treatment was applied directly to them. Rather, the child and his mother were taught functional communication training and the mother was taught activity planning. As the boy gained communication skills, his tantruming and property damage were virtually eliminated. The value of case studies is also discussed.
Article
In the last decade, the token economy has been extended widely across populations and behaviors in treatment, rehabilitation, educational, and community settings. Outcome research has expanded as well to include large-scale program evaluations and comparative and combined treatment studies of the token economy. In a previous review (Kazdin & Bootzin, 1972), several obstacles were identified for the effective application of the token economy. These included identifying procedures to enhance program efficacy, to train staff, to overcome client resistance, and to promote long-term maintenance and transfer of training. The present paper discusses recent advances in research and reviews progress on the major issues identified previously. New issues have become salient in the last decade that pertain to the extension of the token economy to institutional settings. The demands for maintaining the integrity of treatment, the ability to integrate token economies within existing institutional constraints, and the disseminability of the procedures on a large scale are major issues that may dictate the future of the token economy.