Content uploaded by Geraldine Leader
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Geraldine Leader on Nov 04, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
Using a Time Timer
TM
to increase appropriate waiting
behavior in a child with developmental disabilities
Ian Grey
a
, Olive Healy
b,
*, Geraldine Leader
b
, Deirdre Hayes
b
a
Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland
b
National University of Ireland Galway, Galway, Ireland
Impulsiveness and self-control can be operationalised as choice-making behavior between a larger,
delayed reinforcer and a smaller, more immediate reinforcer (Jackson & Hackenberg, 1996). Impulsive
behavior occurs when responding produces more immediate, relatively smaller reinforcers at the cost
Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
ARTICLE INFO
Article history:
Received 26 June 2008
Accepted 3 July 2008
Keywords:
Delayed reinforcement
Developmental disabilities
Time Timer
TM
ABSTRACT
This study aimed to examine the use of a predictive stimulus (Time
Timer
TM
) and delayed reinforcement to increase appropriate
waiting behavior in a child with developmental disabilities and
problem behavior maintained by access to tangible items and
activities. The study employed a changing criterion design across
settings to gradually increase reinforcement delay from 1 s to
10 min. Firstly a baseline phase was conducted to measure the
duration of appropriate waiting behavior to access tangible
reinforcers/activities. Phase 2 involved the use of a red cue card
and the verbal instruction ‘‘wait’’. Phase 3 involved the introduction
of the Time Timer
TM
with the cue card attached, and the verbal
instruction ‘‘wait’’. Finally, Phase 4 utilised the Time Timer
TM
without the cue card. This method was an effective strategy for
increasing appropriate waiting behavior with this participant in a
school setting. The role of adding a concurrent activity during the
reinforcement delay, using cues to predict reinforcement, future
generalization, maintenance and the teaching of functionally
equivalent skills are discussed.
ß2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
* Corresponding author at: School of Psychology, National University of Ireland Galway, St. Anthony’s, Newcastle Road,
Galway, Ireland. Tel.: +353 86 3893128; fax: +353 91 521355.
E-mail address: olive.healy@nuiglaway.ie (O. Healy).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Research in Developmental
Disabilities
0891-4222/$ – see front matter ß2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.07.001
of delayed, larger reinforcers. Self-control occurs when responding produces greater delayed
reinforcers at the expense of more immediate, smaller reinforcers (Logue, 1995; Dixon et al., 1998).
Behaviors that yield delayed reinforcement are highly adaptive in day-to-day life (Stromer, McComan,
& Rehfeldt, 2000), and as such are a socially significant behavior and worth investigating in applied
behavior analysis.
Various methods have been used to establish and maintain reinforcement delay. Schweitzer and
Sulzer-Azaroff (1988) described one method whereby the delay is gradually increased for a larger
reinforcer while the smaller reinforcer remains immediately available. Dixon and Cummins (2001)
extended this research further by illustrating that self-control may be increased by establishing a
history in which participants are gradually exposed to progressive delays, and are concurrently given
the choice to engage in an intervening activity during that delay. Participants had a choice between:
(a) a small immediate reinforcer, (b) a larger delayed item without a response requirement during the
delay, and (c) a larger delayed item with a response requirement during the delay. All participants
showed a preference for the latter option, and during this contingency, no problem behaviors
occurred. However, these studies do not examine visual supports or cues that may serve to predict the
availability of reinforcement.
Williams (1999) found that choice for delayed reinforcement would be more likely when stimuli
presented during the delay reliably predicted reinforcement. Further studies have used tokens, points
and star charts as the predictive stimuli (Kazdin, 1982). In these cases the stimuli that predict
reinforcement at the end of the delay may serve as conditioned reinforcers through multiple pairings
with the delayed reinforcers. However, it has been noted that more naturally occurring conditioned
reinforcers as predictive stimuli would be useful for ensuring generalization to other settings
(Williams & Dunn, 1991). These could be verbal in nature, such as praise from a parent, teacher or
caregiver, or verbal reminders of the reinforcement that is to occur (Hayes & Hayes, 1993).
However, people with developmental disabilities may not respond well to verbal instructions due
to limited or absent verbal comprehension repertoires. Individuals with a developmental disability
often present with a restricted verbal repertoire, which is a common factor associated with
impulsivity (Mischel & Mischel, 1983). It has been suggested that an adult human’s increased
preference for delayed, larger reinforcers may somehow be linked to their advanced verbal abilities
(Schweitzer & Sulzer-Azaroff, 1988). This may explain why people with a limited verbal capacity
experience difficulties in waiting for delayed reinforcement.
Another important aspect of stimuli that predict reinforcement is the length of time one is required
to wait before the reinforcer is delivered. Vollmer, Borrero, Lalli, and Daniel (1999) demonstrated that
participants with developmental disabilities and severe challenging behaviors were more likely to
exhibit self-control than impulsive choices when the longer delays were signalled rather than
unsignalled. They also recommended that a further investigation into ‘timed’ delays (the availability of
a visual timer throughout the delay) for future research.
An examination of impulsivity and self-control with regards to individuals with intellectual
disabilities and problem behavior is important. Access to consumable or tangible reinforcers
frequently maintains severe behavior problems displayed by such individuals (Vollmer et al., 1999).
Many studies have focused on impulsive behavior maintained by token reinforcement (money) and
not on tangible or attention maintained behaviors that are also sensitive to impulsivity (Vollmer et al.,
1999).
In the case of signalled reinforcement delay to promote coping and tolerance skills, the appropriate
‘waiting’ behavior is essentially under the stimulus control of the signal (verbal instruction or visual
cue). Approaching the treatment of tolerance in delayed reinforcement with persons with
developmental disabilities using signalled reinforcement delay could be very useful for maintenance
and generalization of the appropriate behavior. In addition, the use of stimulus fading that includes
highlighting a physical dimension (e.g., colour, size, etc.) of a stimulus to increase the likelihood of a
correct response followed by a systematic fading of the exaggerated dimension could be applied to
promote appropriate behavior during delayed reinforcement (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007).
The present study aims to increase appropriate ‘waiting’ behavior using a Time Timer
TM
with an
individual presenting with problem behavior maintained by access to tangible reinforcement. It
employs a changing criterion design with the application of a Time Timer
TM
as a predictive stimulus. A
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
360
delay in the delivery of a preferred reinforcer is gradually increased and appropriate waiting behavior
is reinforced during the time delay.
1. Method
1.1. Participant and setting
The participant was an 11-year-old girl who lived with her parents and attended a school for
children with intellectual disabilities and pervasive developmental disorders. She shared a classroom
with four other students—two of whom are her siblings. She is one of a set of triplets. The participant
had been diagnosed by a registered clinical psychologist with a moderate level of intellectual
disability. She also presented with cerebral palsy, which affects the fine motor movement of her left
arm, hand and foot. The participant emited challenging behavior in the form of physical aggression
and tantrum behaviors. She could name objects in her environment, but had difficulties asking for
objects or help with activities. She has an age-equivalent of oral expression of 2 years. She can follow
two-step verbal directions.
1.2. Response measurement and reliability
Inappropriate waiting behavior was defined as tantrum behavior that included crying, dropping to
the floor, hitting other students or adults, screaming and repeating the word ‘no’ or the name of a
preferred object or activity at a volume above normal conversational level. The dependent variable
was identified as appropriate waiting behavior and was recorded when an absence of the behaviors
outlined above was observed during an interval.
During both functional assessment and treatment evaluation, a trained observer recorded the
target behaviors during observation periods via paper and pencil. A second observer independently
collected data during 82% of functional assessment observation periods and 65% of intervention
evaluation sessions. Interobserver agreement was examined on a session-by-session basis by dividing
the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100%. Agreement values were 88%
for functional assessment sessions, and 94% for intervention evaluation.
1.3. Apparatus
A stopwatch was used to measure appropriate waiting behavior during baseline conditions and
intervention phases. A Time Timer
TM
and red card (4 in. 4 in.) were used as predictive stimuli. The
Time Timer
TM
is 12 in. 12 in. and resembles a clock. The numbers on the Time Timer
TM
reflect the
passage of time in minutes, and range from 55 to 5 min in a countdown style. To indicate an amount of
time, there is a movable piece on the timer that can be ‘set’ for a particular time frame, e.g., 10 min.
When this piece is moved, a red wedge represents the amount of time, and reduces in size gradually as
the time passes. The red wedge disappears when the time frame is over.
1.4. Experimental design
A changing criterion design with a baseline and three intervention phases was employed to
increase appropriate waiting behavior.
1.5. Functional assessment
The participant was observed directly for a 2-week period in naturally occurring situations that had
been identified by care staff as being related to a high probability of the occurrence of target behaviors.
Staff responses to target behaviors were recorded, as were probable antecedents in an anecdotal
fashion. Every occurrence of the target behavior was analysed according to the anecdotal accounts of
possible antecedents and consequences maintained by the experimenter. Results indicated that most
of the participant’s problem behaviors occurred when objects or activities she wanted were not
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
361
temporally available such as wanting to look at preferred pictures of cartoon characters or wanting to
hold preferred toys. Behaviors also occurred when waiting for lunch or snack time or wanting to get
access to the school playground. These behaviors had significantly impacted on acquisition of novel
skills at the time of assessment. The participant would not sit at a desk in her classroom, avoided
engagement with any academic curriculum-based activity and spent most of her school day at a
computer looking at preferred cartoon characters either on a computer or in a book in an adjacent
room. Results of the functional assessment demonstrated that the inappropriate behaviors were
maintained by immediate access to tangible items and activities.
1.6. General procedure
The general procedure involved a baseline phase and three intervention phases and, in the interest
of clarity, each phase is outlined separately below. The purpose of employing three intervention
phases was to establish stimulus control for waiting using a red card during an activity that the
participant enjoyed. Following this the goal of intervention was to transfer stimulus control from the
red card to the Time Timer
TM
. The red card was selected because the moveable piece of the Time
Timer
TM
is coloured red.
1.7. Phase 1: Baseline
Baseline consisted of Sessions 1–10. An activity that the participant enjoyed engaging in outdoors
was selected in order to determine the maximum amount of time the participant would wait
appropriately for a reinforcer. The participant enjoyed an activity that involved running up a series of
steps in the playground while holding the hands of an adult. Before the activity began the participant
was told to ‘wait’ and the experimenter set a stopwatch to measure appropriate waiting behavior. If
the participant waited appropriately for 30 s the activity was delivered. Once inappropriate waiting
behavior occurred the stopwatch was stopped and the time was recorded. Contingent on the
occurrence of inappropriate behavior the participant was directed to another activity in the
playground. Ten baseline probes of this activity were conducted over 2 consecutive days.
1.8. Phase 1: Red card and verbal instruction
Phase 1 included Sessions 11–38. During this phase the experimenter held the red card in front of
the participant and gave the verbal instruction ‘wait’. The card was removed after one second and the
next interval was initiated. This was repeated until four consecutive 1 s intervals were complete. The
amount of time the red card was present and the participant was required to wait for the game to
resume was systematically extended. Intervals of time for waiting were 1, 3, 6, 10, 16, 7 and 20 s,
respectively (see Table 1). Only the word ‘wait’ preceded the presentation of the red card. The criterion
for increasing the interval of time was four consecutive intervals with no occurrence of inappropriate
waiting behavior.
During interval 31, instead of increasing the criterion to a longer delay in reinforcement, the delay
in the delivery of reinforcement was actually reduced from 16 to 7 s. This reduction in criterion was
implemented randomly as a decrease in demand to ensure the participant would continue to wait
appropriately in the absence of inappropriate waiting behavior. Although a very gradual change in
criterion was implemented across this phase, it was important to set up an opportunity for success as
the change in criterion became more difficult to reach. During interval 35 the criterion was once again
increased from 7 to 20 s.
1.9. Phase 2: Red card on Time Timer
TM
and verbal instruction
Phase 2 included Sessions 39–82. During Phase 2 the intervention was transferred to the classroom
setting. The participant was seated at a desk with other students and contingent on sitting
appropriately she was presented with a picture of a favourite cartoon character. After approximately
10 s the picture was removed and the red card was placed on the Time Timer
TM
for 15 s along with the
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
362
verbal instruction ‘wait’. Contingent on the non-occurrence of target behavior the cartoon picture was
delivered. The intervals for waiting were systematically extended up to 2 m. The criterion for
increasing the time interval was four consecutive intervals without the occurrence of inappropriate
waiting behavior (see Table 1). Two minutes was considered the shortest interval that could be
represented on the Time Timer
1
. No target behaviors occurred during this phase. During interval 63
the delay in the delivery of reinforcement was reduced from 65 to 30 s instead of increasing the
criterion. Similarly to Phase 2, this reduction in criterion was implemented randomly as a decrease in
demand to ensure the participant would continue to wait appropriately when a gradually increasing
demand was placed upon her. During interval 67 the criterion was once again increased from 30 to
75 s. The criterion for this phase for increasing the time interval was four consecutive intervals with no
occurrence of inappropriate waiting behavior.
1.10. Phase 3: Time Timer
TM
Phase 3 included Sessions 83–122. During Phase 3, the red card was removed and replaced by the
red moveable wedge on the Time Timer
TM
. The amount of time required to wait prior to the
presentation of a preferred reinforcer was systematically extended to 10 min by gradually increasing
the amount of time the participant was required to wait (see Table 1). Similarly to Phases 1 and 2,
Table 1
Baseline duration of appropriate waiting behavior and systematic increases in intervals during Phases 2–4
Sessions Maximum duration of appropriate waiting (s) Did inappropriate behavior occur?
Phase 1: Baseline
1–10 1 +
Sessions Intervals of appropriate waiting (s) Did inappropriate behavior occur?
Phase 2: Red card and verbal instruction
11–14 1
15–18 3
19–22 6
23–26 10
27–30 16
31–34 7
35–38 20
Phase 3: Red card on Time Timer
TM
and verbal instruction
39–42 15
43–46 20
47–50 30
51–54 40
55–58 50
59–62 65
63–66 30
67–70 75
71–74 90
75–78 105
79–82 120
Phase 4: Time Timer
TM
83–86 150
87–90 180
91–94 240
95–98 180
99–102 300
103–106 360
107–110 420
111–114 480
115–118 540
119–122 600
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
363
instead of increasing the criterion at interval 95, the delay in the delivery of reinforcement was
reduced from 240 to 180 s. This reduction in criterion was randomly applied. During Session 99 the
criterion was once again increased from 180 to 300 s. Sessions 99–122 showed increases in the
waiting intervals from 300 to 600 s.
In addition, during this phase (Session 95) desktop activities such as matching tasks, reading tasks
and art activities were introduced while the participant waited for a reinforcer. The purpose of this
was to ensure that the participant learned to wait while engaging in demanding tasks. Access to
choosing additional reinforcers, other than cartoon pictures, was introduced at this point. When stable
responding was observed (no occurrence of the inappropriate waiting behavior during Sessions 95–
106), the Time Timer
TM
was used in a novel setting involving contingent access to snacks at
lunchtimes (Session 107).
2. Results
Results of Phases 1–4 are presented in Fig. 1. During Phase 1, the maximum amount of time spent
waiting appropriately was 1 s during Sessions 1 and 2. During the eight remaining probes there was no
instance of appropriate waiting and the duration recorded was 0 s.
With the introduction of the discriminative stimulus and verbal instruction during Phase 2,
appropriate waiting was increased using a changing criterion from 1 to 20 s. During Phase 3 the delivery
of reinforcement was delayed from 15 to 120 s using both the discriminative stimulus and verbal
direction paired with the Time Timer
TM
. During Phase 4 the delay in reinforcement and appropriate
waiting behavior was increased from 150 to 600 s in the presence of the Time Timer
TM
alone.
Over the three intervention phases the changing criterion was increased by greater increments. For
example, during Phase 2 the mean increment was 4 s. During Phases 3 and 4 the mean increments
were 11.6 and 56 s, respectively.
No inappropriate behavior was demonstrated during the changing criterion phases (see Table 1).
Appropriate waiting behavior was gradually increased using a changing criterion from 1 s to 10 min
across 122 Sessions.
3. Discussion
This study aimed to increase the appropriate waiting behavior of a child with an intellectual
disability and problem behavior maintained by access to tangible reinforcement. The results illustrate
that the participant’s appropriate waiting behavior was successfully increased from 1 s to 10 min. This
outcome also resulted in an increase in classroom instruction time whereby during the waiting
interval, the experimenter introduced task-demand at the desktop with access to a choice of multiple
reinforcers. Gradually the reinforcers for waiting were increased from one choice to a variety of
choices in different settings. These results will be discussed in terms of the addition of the activity to
Fig. 1. Duration of appropriate waiting behavior across Phases 1–4.
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
364
engage in during the waiting period, the role of cue’s in predicting reinforcement, the future fading of
the discriminative stimulus to further generalisation of the skill, and with regards to functionally
equivalent skills teaching.
3.1. Addition of concurrent activity
These results support Dixon and Cummins (2001) findings that self-control can be increased by
gradually extending reinforcement delay, while concurrently given an activity to engage in during the
delay. When the participant reached 180 s of appropriate waiting behavior, the experimenter
introduced task-demand at the desk in the classroom. This was a significant outcome for the
participant, as before this intervention was introduced, she would not engage in any learning activity
at the desk. This was having a great impact on her learning, so the present results indicate an
improvement in her overall quality of life. Her quality of life has improved in terms of her ability to
engage in discrete-trial instruction in the classroom, and her new adaptive skill of waiting
appropriately in the presence of the timer.
3.2. Visual stimulis to predict reinforcement
The present study used a visual cue as a discriminative stimulus to predict reinforcement and to
indicate when reinforcement would be delivered. The successful outcome of this study would appear
to support Vollmer et al.’s (1999) finding that participants with developmental disabilities and
challenging behaviors are more likely to exhibit self-control when delays in reinforcement are
signalled. The present results also corroborate Bower, McLean, and Meacham (1966) findings that
participants respond positively to being able to predict when a reinforcer would be delivered. The
participant in the current study did not engage in any challenging behaviors during the signalled and
informed delays. A possible limitation of this study is that it employed both a visual and verbal cue
(red card and ‘‘wait’’), and then a timed visual cue and verbal cue (timer and ‘‘wait’’). Perhaps future
research could compare the use of a visual (cue card) with a timed signal (Time Timer
TM
), with and
without a verbal cue to accompany it to investigate which of these methods are more effective—in
order to establish the most effective method of teaching this vital skill.
3.3. Generalisation of the skill
Hayes and Hayes (1993) suggested the use of a more naturally occurring predictive stimulus, in the
form of verbal statements from a parent or teacher, to ensure generalisation to other settings. In this
case, the participant’s language skills were limited (2-year-old level), thus verbal reminders of the
reinforcement to occur may have had limited comprehensive value. The addition of the Time Timer
TM
provided a visual predictive stimulus as well as a verbal stimulus (experimenter saying ‘‘wait’’).
However, in order for the participant to establish and maintain her new skill/behavior across settings,
the timer will eventually need to be faded out over time.
The timer is a useful tool and presents with the possibility of gradual stimulus fading. It closely
resembles a clock, apart from the red wedge to indicate the passage of time. The colour of the wedge
could be gradually faded and eventually the hands of the clock could be introduced to indicate the
passage of time. In addition, fading the coloured wedge could involve the use of coloured stickers to be
placed on the face of the timer to indicate when the time was up, and these stickers could then be
transferred to a regular clock. A portable Time Timer
TM
wristwatch is also available, where the red
wedge of the clock is displayed digitally on a wristwatch. The portable watch could be gradually
introduced in place of the desktop timer and could be used across settings and instructors to ensure
maintenance and generalisation of the skill.
3.4. Functional equivalence
Vollmer et al. (1999) highlighted the need for future research to investigate impulsive behavior in
light of a tangible or attention-based function—as these behaviors are also sensitive to impulsivity. The
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
365
results of the functional assessment in the present study indicated that the participant’s tantrums
(inappropriate impulsive behavior) were maintained by access to tangible items/activities. Thus the
present research provided a little more insight into this area. Functionally equivalent-based
interventions have been an effective meta-strategy in the treatment of challenging behaviors. Cooper
et al. (2007) state that if a problem behavior serves a specific function, then the intervention should
provide the reinforcer (e.g., tangible item) contingent on a more appropriate response. This new
response serves the same function for the individual and, if the intervention is implemented
successfully, provides a more efficient, effective and socially valid means of reaching the specific goal.
Campbell and Lutzker (1993) illustrate this point in their study, where they successfully employed
functional communication training to eliminate tantrums and property destruction in an 8-year-old
boy. Appropriate waiting is an essential adaptive skill in day-to-day life for all people, and further
applied research in this area is greatly needed.
4. Conclusion
The present study examined the use of a Time Timer
TM
to gradually increase reinforcement delay in
a child with developmental disabilities and problem behavior. This study supports previous findings
that participants respond well to knowing that reinforcement will be available (Vollmer et al., 1999)
and when reinforcement will be available (Bower et al., 1966). It also lends substantiation to the
proposal that functional based interventions are an effective strategy in the treatment of problem
behaviors (Campbell & Lutzker, 1993). Further research could compare the efficacy of using either a
predictive stimulus (cue card), and timed stimulus (timer) or both—with and without verbal cues, in
order to gain a greater understanding of the behavioral mechanisms in effect in an intervention of this
nature.
References
Bower, G., McLean, J., & Meacham, J. (1966). Value of knowing when reinforcement is due. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 62, 184–192.
Campbell, R. V., & Lutzker, J. R. (1993). Using functional equivalence training to reduce severe challenging behavior: A case study.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 203–316.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Dixon, M. R., & Cummins, A. (2001). Self control in children with autism: Response allocation during delays to reinforcement. Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 491–495.
Dixon, M. R., Hayes, L. J., Binder, L. M., Manthey, S., Sigman, C., & Zdanowski, D. M. (1998). Using a self-control training procedure to
increase appropriate behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 203–210.
Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Applied implications of current JEAB research on derived relations and delayed reinforcement.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 507–511.
Jackson, K., & Hackenberg, T. D. (1996). Token reinforcement, choice, and self control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 66, 29–49.
Kazdin, A. E. (1982). The token economy: A decade later. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 431–445.
Logue, A. W. (1995). Self control: Waiting until tomorrow for what you want today. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self control strategies. Child Development, 54, 603–
619.
Schweitzer, J. B., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1988). Self control: Teaching tolerance for delay in impulsive children. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 173–186.
Stromer, R., McComan, J. J., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Designing interventions that include delayed reinforcement: Implications of
recent laboratory research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 359–371.
Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Lalli, J. S., & Daniel, D. (1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsivity in children with severe behavior
disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 451–466.
Williams, B. A. (1999). Value transmission in discrimination learning involving stimulus chains. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 72, 177–185.
Williams, B. A., & Dunn, R. (1991). Preference for conditioned reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 37–46.
I. Grey et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 30 (2009) 359–366
366