ArticlePDF Available

Celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility and social complexity

Authors:
  • North American Journal of Psychology

Abstract and Figures

To extend previous consideration of cognitive factors in celebrity worship, the aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility and social complexity. Two samples of non-student (n=260) and student respondents (n=168) completed measures of entertainment–social, intense–personal, borderline–pathological celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility and social complexity. Findings suggest that cognitive flexibility predicts unique variance in intense–personal aspects of celebrity worship. These findings help researchers to further conceptualise and understand the individual who follows a celebrity in an intense and personal way.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility, and social complexity
John Maltby
a,*
, Liz Day
b
, Lynn E. McCutcheon
c
,
Matthew M. Martin
d
, Jacob L. Cayanus
d
a
School of Psychology, University of Leicester, University Road, Leicester, LE1 7RH England, United Kingdom
b
Sheffield Hallam University, United Kingdom
c
DeVry University, USA
d
West Virginia University, USA
Received 2 June 2003; received in revised form 27 December 2003; accepted 4 February 2004
Available online 12 March 2004
Abstract
To extend previous consideration of cognitive factors in celebrity worship, the aim of the present study
was to examine the relationship between celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility and social complexity. Two
samples of non-student (n¼260) and student respondents (n¼168) completed measures of entertainment–
social, intense–personal, borderline–pathological celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility and social com-
plexity. Findings suggest that cognitive flexibility predicts unique variance in intense–personal aspects of
celebrity worship. These findings help researchers to further conceptualise and understand the individual
who follows a celebrity in an intense and personal way.
Ó2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Celebrity worship; Cognitive flexibility; Social complexity; Cognitive; Attitude
1. Introduction
Not only is there growing interest in celebrities in terms of fans and media coverage, but there is
growing evidence to suggest that celebrity worship may be of interest to psychologists. The
phenomenon occurs more in adolescents or young adults than older persons (Ashe &
McCutcheon, 2001; Larson, 1995), celebrity worshippers are more likely to value a ‘‘game-
playing’’ love style (McCutcheon, 2002), celebrity worship shares a negative association with some
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-116-252-2165; fax: +44-116-252-2067.
E-mail address: jm148@le.ac.uk (J. Maltby).
0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.004
Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482
www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
aspects of religiosity (Maltby, Houran, Lange, Ashe, & McCutcheon, 2002), and related to per-
sonality (Maltby, Houran, & McCutcheon, 2003). However, celebrity worship does not appear to
be related to authoritarianism (Maltby & McCutcheon, 2001) and at best is only very weakly
associated with shyness or loneliness (Ashe & McCutcheon, 2001).
McCutcheon, Lange, and Houran (2002) proposed an ‘‘Absorption–Addiction’’ model to ex-
plain such cases of celebrity worship. According to this model, a compromised identity structure
in some individuals facilitates psychological absorption with a celebrity in an attempt to establish
an identity and a sense of fulfilment. The dynamics of the motivational forces driving this
absorption might in turn take on an addictive component, leading to more extreme (and perhaps
delusional) behaviours to sustain the individual’s satisfaction with the parasocial relationship.
Several studies based on the Celebrity Attitude Scale (McCutcheon et al., 2002; Maltby et al.,
2002; Maltby, McCutcheon, Ashe, & Houran, 2001) are consistent with this proposed model and
suggest that there are three increasingly more extreme set of attitudes and behaviours associated
with celebrity worship. Low levels of celebrity worship have Entertainment–social value and
comprise attitudes and behaviours like ‘‘My friends and I like to discuss what my favourite
celebrity has done,’’ and ‘‘Learning the life story of my favourite celebrity is a lot of fun.’’ This
stage reflects social aspects to celebrity worship and are consistent with Stever’s (1991) observa-
tion that fans are attracted to a favourite celebrity because of their perceived ability to entertain
and capture our attention. Intermediate levels of celebrity worship, by contrast, are characterised
by more Intense–personal feelings, defined by items like ‘‘I consider my favourite celebrity to be
my soul mate,’’ and ‘‘I have frequent thoughts about my celebrity, even when I don’t want to.’’
This stage arguably reflects individuals’ intensive and compulsive feelings about the celebrity, akin
to the obsessional tendencies of fans often referred to in the literature (Dietz et al., 1991; Giles,
2000). The most extreme expression of celebrity worship is labelled Borderline–pathological,as
exemplified by items like ‘‘If someone gave me several thousand dollars (pounds) to do with as I
please, I would consider spending it on a personal possession (like a napkin or paper plate) once
used by my favourite celebrity’’ and ‘‘If I were lucky enough to meet my favourite celebrity, and
he/she asked me to do something illegal as a favour I would probably do it.’’ This factor is
thought to reflect an individual’s social–pathological attitudes and behaviours that are held as a
result of worshipping a celebrity.
There is still a paucity of information about the relationship between specific cognitive variables
and celebrity worship. Martin, Cayanus, McCutcheon, and Maltby (2003) examined the rela-
tionship between celebrity worship and cognitive flexibility. Cognitive flexibility refers to a per-
son’s (a) awareness that in any given situation there are options and alternatives available, (b)
willingness to be flexible and adapt to the situation, and (c) self-efficacy in being flexible (Martin &
Anderson, 1998, 2001; Martin, Anderson, & Thweatt, 1998; Martin & Rubin, 1995). People that
are cognitively flexible are also more interpersonally competent (Parks, 1994). Competent com-
municators are able to adapt their behavior in order to achieve their personal goals. People that
report being cognitively flexible, also view themselves as being assertive, responsive, attentive, and
perceptive (Martin & Anderson, 1996, 1998). Martin and Anderson (2001) found that people who
were higher in cognitive flexibility were more likely to use affinity-seeking strategies. Earlier re-
search has shown that people who have more affinity-seeking strategies in their repertoires and are
able to communicate these strategies are more effective in developing interpersonal relationships
(Martin & Rubin, 1995). People high in celebrity worship are believed to have a weak identity
1476 J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482
structure and seemingly weak interpersonal skills (McCutcheon, Ashe, Houran, & Maltby, 2003),
people that are higher in cognitive flexibility would be expected to be lower in their celebrity
worship. Martin et al. (2003) found cognitive flexibility was negatively related to the dimensions
of intense–personal (r¼0:22, p<0:01) and borderline–pathological (r¼0:17, p<0:05), but
not with the dimension of entertainment/social (r¼0:06, p>0:05). Thus this hypothesis was
partially supported.
There are not only opportunities to examine and confirm this relationship among non-student
samples, but to extend the consideration to other aspects of cognitive processing. One such aspect
is social complexity (Wann & Hamlet, 1994). Social complexity has been defined as one’s desire to
join and maintain memberships in diverse groups (Wann, 2000). Social complexity has been found
to be positively related to positive emotions, personal self-esteem, and collective self-esteem (see
Wann & Hamlet, 1994). Further, negative relationships were found between scores on the
instrument and experiences of negative emotions, stress, and loneliness. Thus similar to the
prediction made by Martin et al. (2003), it is argued that people high in celebrity worship are
believed to have a weak identity structure and seemingly weak interpersonal skills (McCutcheon
et al., 2003) and there would additionally be a significant negative relationship between celebrity
worship and social complexity.
The aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between celebrity worship,
cognitive flexibility and social complexity.
2. Method
2.1. Sample
Data was collected from two samples. The first sample consisted of 260 respondents (127 males,
133 females) aged between 18 and 59 years (Mean ¼32.93, SD ¼8.0) sampled from a number of
workplaces and community groups in the North of England. Participants were selected from those
willing to be respondents from the workplaces and community groups visited. Among this sample
the most often reported demographic was White (n¼174), married (n¼116), employed
(n¼111); with most leaving school reported with the equivalent of an education with at least 1 ‘0’
level/GCSE (n¼86).
The second sample comprised 168 university undergraduate students (78 males, 90 females).
Respondents were aged from 18 to 35 years (Mean ¼21.96, SD ¼2.6) and among reported ethnic
demographic was White (n¼113).
2.2. Questionnaires
Respondents were administered:
(1) The Celebrity Attitude Scale (CAS: McCutcheon et al., 2002). Originally this scale is a 34-
item Likert-type scale in which respondents are asked to indicate their attitude towards a
favourite celebrity (that they themselves have named) using a number of items that use a response
format ‘‘strongly agree’’ equal to 5 and ‘‘strongly disagree’’ equal to 1. However, from analysis
reported in Maltby et al. (2002) and Maltby et al. (2003), among UK samples, three ‘‘subscales’’
J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482 1477
were formed from 22 of the items; entertainment–social (10 items; e.g. ‘My friends and I like to
discuss what my favourite celebrity has done’, item 5); intense–personal (9 items; ‘I share with my
favourite celebrity a special bond that cannot be described in words’, item 2), and borderline–
pathological (3 items; If I were lucky enough to meet my favourite celebrity, and he/she asked me
to do something illegal as a favour, I would probably do it, item 22). In the present study, this 22-
item measure was used.
(2) The Cognitive Flexibility Scale (Martin & Rubin, 1995) was used to measure cognitive
flexibility. Example items include; ‘‘I can communicate an idea in many different ways’’, ‘‘I am
willing to work at creative solutions to problems’’ and ‘‘I have the self-confidence necessary to try
different ways of behaving’’. Participants respond to 12 items using a 6-point scale: strongly
disagree (1) to strongly agree (6), with possible scores ranging from 12 to 72 and higher scores
indicating greater cognitive flexibility.
(3) The Joiner’s Scale (Wann & Hamlet, 1994) was used to assess social complexity. Example
items include ‘‘I tend to be a member of many different groups’’ and ‘‘I actively seek out new
group memberships’’. Response options to the Likert-style items range from 1 (Low social com-
plexity)to9(High social complexity). Four items are reverse scored, leading to a scale range of 7–
63, with higher scores indicating a greater level of social complexity,
2.3. Results
Table 1 shows the mean scores and standard deviations of all the scales by sex for each sample.
Among the non-student adult sample, females were found to score higher than males on the
measure of social complexity and the intense–personal subscale of the Celebrity Attitude Scale.
Among the student sample, females were found to score higher than males on the measure of the
borderline–pathological subscale of the Celebrity Attitude Scale. In addition, Table 1 shows the
Table 1
Mean scores and alpha coefficients of all the scales by sex
Scale aMales (n¼127) Females (n¼133) t
Non-student adult sample
CAS-entertainment–social 0.92 25.04 (13.1) 24.71 (12.9) 0.21
CAS-intense–personal 0.91 23.84 (10.9) 26.55 (10.2) )2.07
CAS-pathological 0.79 3.92 (2.3) 4.28 (2.5) )1.20
Cognitive flexibility 0.93 37.42 (12.0) 35.77 (12.2) 1.10
Social complexity 0.90 37.98 (15.0) 41.65 (13.2) )2.21
Males (n¼106) Females (n¼115)
Student sample
CAS-entertainment–social 0.82 25.59 (12.4) 25.30 (12.6) 0.15
CAS-intense–personal 0.77 23.95 (9.8) 26.77 (8.8) )1.95
CAS-pathological 0.70 3.67 (1.5) 4.48 (2.7) )2.36
Cognitive flexibility 0.84 38.13 (12.0) 37.76 (12.7) 0.19
Social complexity 0.86 38.87 (14.9) 42.49 (12.8) )1.69
p<0:05.
1478 J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482
Cronbach’s alpha (Cronbach, 1951) computed for all the scales. The present findings suggest that
all the scales demonstrate adequate internal reliability among the present samples.
Table 2 shows the correlations between each of the subscales of the Celebrity Attitude Scale,
cognitive flexibility and social complexity controlling for sex and age. Among both the samples,
the intense–personal celebrity attitude subscale shared a significant negative correlation with both
cognitive flexibility and social complexity. Within the non-student adult sample, the entertain-
ment–social celebrity attitude subscale and shared a significant negative correlation with both
cognitive flexibility and social complexity.
Due to the significant associations between two of the celebrity worship dimensions and both
cognitive flexibility and social complexity, the associations between celebrity attitude, cognitive
flexibility, and social complexity were examined within three standardised multiple regressions
(entertainment–social, cognitive flexibility, social complexity among non-student adults and in-
tense–personal, cognitive flexibility, social complexity among both non-student adults and stu-
dents. Within these models scores on the cognitive flexibility and social complexity scale were used
as predictor variables and the entertainment–social and intense–personal celebrity attitude sub-
scales were used as independent variables.
Among the non-student adult sample, the regression statistic (R) was not significantly different
from zero for scores on the entertainment–social celebrity worship subscale (Fð2;257Þ¼2:25,
p>0:05) using cognitive flexibility and social complexity. Therefore, no further analysis was
performed.
However, among the non-student and student samples the regression statistic (R) was signifi-
cantly different from zero for scores on the intense–personal celebrity worship subscale (Non-
student adult, Fð2;257Þ¼6:44, p<0:01; Student, Fð2;257Þ¼4:66, p<0:05).
Table 3 shows the full results for the unstandardised multiple regression. Included in this table
is the unstandardised regression coefficient (B), the standardised regression coefficients ðBÞ, the
semipartial correlations (sr2), r,r2and adjusted r2. Among both samples, cognitive flexibility
accounted for unique variance in intense–personal celebrity attitude subscale.
Table 2
Correlations between all the variables
12345
Non-student adult sample (n ¼260)
1. Entertainment–social 0.23 0.14)0.15)0.12
2. Intense–personal 0.19 )0.21 )0.22
3. Borderline–pathological 0.10 0.09
4. Cognitive flexibility 0.37
5. Social complexity
Student sample (n ¼168)
1. Entertainment–social 0.34 0.30)0.08 )0.04
2. Intense–personal 0.28 )0.21 )0.17
3. Borderline–pathological 0.11 0.13
4. Cognitive flexibility 0.44
5. Social complexity
p<0:05; p<0:01;  p<0:001.
J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482 1479
3. Discussion
The findings extend on previous findings (Martin et al., 2003) that celebrity worship is related to
cognitive variables, by finding a relationship between celebrity worship for intense–personal
reasons and cognitive flexibility and social complexity in the United Kingdom. However the
findings also fail to replicate previous findings by Martin et al. (2003) that celebrity worship for
borderline–pathological reasons was related to cognitive flexibility and by finding a, albeit very
small, correlation between entertainment–social celebrity worship and cognitive flexibility and
social complexity.
Reasons for the lack of the relationship between the celebrity worship for borderline–patho-
logical reasons and cognitive variable among the present sample may be due to differences be-
tween the samples (USA students and United Kingdom adults and students) and needs to be
further explored. One possible explanation is that the mean scores for the borderline–pathological
celebrity attitude scale (Adults; Male, M¼3:92 Females ¼4.28; Students; Male, M¼3:67
Females ¼4.48) are a lot lower than that reported by Martin et al. (2003), (Mean ¼7.28). Though
these differences in findings may be expected between the different samples, the restricted vari-
ability of scores among the present sample might account for the lack of the relationship.
The significant relationship between entertainment–social celebrity worship and cognitive
flexibility and social complexity probably can be dismissed as due to the size of the sample. The
size of the correlations suggest that the variables share, at best 2%, of the variance, and a model
using these two these cognitive variables as predictors of entertainment–social celebrity worship
do not emerge as statistically significant.
However, the use of the multiple regression suggests that celebrity worship for intense–personal
reasons is associated with lower levels of cognitive flexibility rather than social complexity. This
finding suggests that people who worship individuals for intense–personal reasons tend not to be
aware that in any given situation there are options and alternatives available, don’t tend to be
Table 3
Multiple regression analysis with intense–personal celebrity attitude subscale score used as a dependent variable and
cognitive flexibility and social complexity scores used as predictor variables
Scale BBsr
2
Intense–personal (non-student adult sample)
Cognitive flexibility )0.14 )0.16 0.03
Social complexity )0.07 )0.09
r2¼0:05
Adj r2¼0:04
r¼0:22
Intense–personal (student sample)
Cognitive flexibility )0.12 )0.17 0.03
Social complexity )0.08 )0.11
r2¼0:05
Adj r2¼0:04
r¼0:23
p<0:05; p<0:01;  p<0:001.
1480 J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482
willing to be flexible, show low self-efficacy in being flexible. These findings aid the description of
the celebrity worshipper who worships celebrities for intense–personal reasons, as it locates this
cognitive variable to be associated with one particular aspect of celebrity worship. The finding
that this type of celebrity worship is related to unwillingness to become a member of social groups
(social complexity) is better explained by a failure to be cognitive flexible.
These findings may provide an individual cognitive processes context to understand celebrity
worship and its consequences. The present findings suggest that individuals who engage in
celebrity worship for intense–personal reasons are unable to consider options, implement alter-
natives to problems and are unwilling to be flexible. Therefore, when it comes to stressful situ-
ations or daily life, those who are intense–personal in their celebrity worship are ‘locked’ into a
way of viewing the world, and therefore are unable to deal with novel or unusual situations. Such
conclusions are consistent with descriptions of the obsessive or intense–personal celebrity wor-
shipper as someone who are unable to deal with the real world or only see singular value in their
celebrity and very little else (Ashe & McCutcheon, 2001; Larson, 1995; Maltby et al., 2001).
Future research could use such a characterisation in practical ways to test the relationship be-
tween celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility, and mental health indictors to see whether this
cognitive inflexibility has consequences for the celebrity worshipper doing so for intense–personal
reasons.
In summary the present findings suggest that the variable of cognitive flexibility is a useful
factor in describing the celebrity worshipper who does so for intense–personal reasons and this
finding adds to conceptualisation of celebrity worship.
References
Ashe, D. D., & McCutcheon, L. E. (2001). Shyness, loneliness, and attitude toward celebrities. Current Research in
Social Psychology, 6, 124–133.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334.
Dietz, P. E., Matthews, D. B., Van Duyne, C., Martell, D. A., Parry, C. D., Stewart, T., Warren, J., & Crowder, J. D.
(1991). Threatening and otherwise inappropriate letters to Hollywood celebrities. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 36,
185–209.
Giles, D. (2000). Illusions of immortality: A psychology of fame and celebrity. London: MacMillan.
Larson, R. W. (1995). Secrets in the bedroom: Adolescents’ private use of media. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24,
535–550.
McCutcheon, L. E. (2002). Are parasocial relationship styles reflected in love styles? Current Research in Social
Psychology, 7, 82–94.
McCutcheon, L. E., Ashe, D. D., Houran, J., & Maltby, J. (2003). A cognitive profile of individuals who tend to
worship celebrities. The Journal of Psychology, 137, 309–322.
McCutcheon, L. E., Lange, R., & Houran, J. (2002). Conceptualization and measurement of celebrity worship. British
Journal of Psychology, 93, 67–87.
Maltby, J., Houran, J., Lange, R., Ashe, D., & McCutcheon, L. E. (2002). Thou shalt worship no other gods––unless
they are celebrities: The relationship between celebrity worship and religious orientation. Personality and Individual
Differences, 32, 1157–1172.
Maltby, J., Houran, J., & McCutcheon, L. E. (2003). Locating celebrity worship within Eysenck’s personality
dimensions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 25–29.
Maltby, J., & McCutcheon, L. E. (2001). Correlations between scores on attitudes toward celebrities and
authoritarianism. Psychological Reports, 88, 979–980.
J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482 1481
Maltby, J., McCutcheon, L. E., Ashe, D. D., & Houran, J. (2001). The self-reported psychological well-being of
celebrity worshippers. North American Journal of Psychology, 3, 441–452.
Martin, M. M., & Anderson, C. M. (1996). Communication traits: A cross-generalization investigation. Communication
Research Reports, 13, 58–67.
Martin, M. M., & Anderson, C. M. (1998). The Cognitive Flexibility Scale: Three validity studies. Communication
Reports, 11, 1–9.
Martin, M. M., & Anderson, C. M. (2001). The relationship between cognitive flexibility and affinity-seeking strategies.
Advances in Psychological Research, 4, 69–76.
Martin, M. M., Anderson, C. M., & Thweatt, K. S. (1998). Individuals’ perceptions of their communication behaviors:
A validity study of the relationship between the Cognitive Flexibility Scale and the Communication Flexibility Scale
with aggressive communication traits. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 13, 531–540.
Martin, M. M., Cayanus, J. L., McCutcheon, L. E., & Maltby, J. (2003). Celebrity worship and cognitive flexibility.
North American Journal of Psychology, 5, 75–80.
Martin, M. M., & Rubin, R. B. (1995). A new measure of cognitive flexibility. Psychological Reports, 76, 623–626.
Parks, M. R. (1994). Communication competence and interpersonal control. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.),
Handbook of interpersonal communication (pp. 589–620). CA: Sage: Beverly Hills.
Stever, G. S. (1991). The Celebrity Appeal Questionnaire. Psychological Reports, 68, 859–866.
Wann, D. L. (2000). The Joiner’s Scale. In J. Maltby, C. A. Lewis, & A. P. Hill (Eds.), Commissioned reviews of 250
psychological tests (2, pp. 503–505). Lampeter, Wales: Edwin Mellen Press.
Wann, D. L., & Hamlet, M. A. (1994). The Joiner’s Scale: Validation of a measure of social-complexity. Psychological
Reports, 74, 1027–1034.
1482 J. Maltby et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 1475–1482
... Developing the measurement and understanding of celebrity worship within a clinical personality context. Personality and Individual Differences Journal. Volume 40, Issue 2, January 2006. Hal 274. 22 Maltby, John, d.l.l. (2004). Celebrity worship, cognitive flexibility, and social complexity. ...
Article
Artikel ini, adalah hasil penelitian tentang kajian kriminologi yang membahas potensi kejahatan dan penyimpangan dari penggemar, akibat pemujaan pada idola, khususnya idola dalam serial TV. Serial TV memiliki pengaruh besar dalam membentuk perspektif audience terutama perspektif penggemar. Salah satunya adalah pembentukan ide - ide akan idola yang menyebabkan keterikatan antara idola dan penggemar. Keterikatan ini pada beberapa kasus dapat menyebabkan terjadinya pemujaan atau obsesi penggemar kepada idola mereka. Hal ini dapat menjadi potensi terjadinya kejahatan atau penyimpangan oleh penggemar. Penelitian ini akan melihat bagaimana potensi terjadinya kejahatan dan penyimpangan oleh penggemar atas dasar pemujaan mereka kepada idola, dengan menggunakan konsep obsesi dan celebrity worship. Pendekatan yang digunakan adalah pendekatan deskriptif yang menggunakan kuesioner, mengenai persepsi penggemar atas idola dan observasi penggemar pada habitat asli mereka, yakni dalam komunitas penggemar. Subyek penelitian di khususkan pada penggemar idola serial TV Thailand. Penelitian ini berbasis pada dunia siber dan komunikasi yang terjadi secara online. Berdasarkan penelitian ini yang terjadi pada kelompok penggemar aktor serial TV Thailand, maka mereka memiliki kemungkinan terikat tidak hanya pada aktor namun juga pasangan dalam drama. Hal ini menciptakan dua ilusi yaitu kepemilikan diri mereka atas idola dan kepemilikan kuasa untuk menentukan status hubungan romantis idola. Kondisi ini menyebabkan mereka melakukan cyberstalking berlebihan terhadap idola, termasuk pada ranah pribadi dan melakukan cyberbullying kepada kelompok kelompok yang memberikan respon negatif atau bahkan pada individu dekat idola yang mereka tidak setujui. Walaupun secara statistik kelompok melakukan penyimpangan dan kejahatan ini kecil, namun mereka turut serta membentuk opini dan perilaku dari keseluruhan komunitas penggemar aktor serial TV Thailand.
... In the study, it was seen that the caregivers' disease burden and feeling of loss decreased as their cognitive flexibility increased. The individuals with cognitive flexibility are able to evaluate the alternative situations in human relationships and they succeed in adapting to conditions (Maltby et al., 2004). It is known that the individuals with higher levels of cognitive flexibility have more effective ways of problem solving (Buğa et al., 2018). ...
Article
This research was conducted with the aim of evaluating empathic tendency, cognitive flexibility level and disease burden of caregivers to individuals with mental problems as a descriptive and cross-sectional study. The research sample is consisted of 45 caregivers who are in-patients, participated voluntarily and met the criteria for involvement in the study between January 2021 and January 2022 in psychiatric service of a university hospital in Ankara. The data of the study was collected face to face and Participant Information Form, Cognitive Flexibility Scale, Empathic Tendency Scale and Disease Burden Scale were used. Descriptive statistics were identified with mean, standard deviation, number and percent. Whether the data followed normal distribution were tested and Mann-Whitney U test and Kruskal-Wallis were used in between-group comparisons as it didn’t follow normal distribution. Spearman’s Correlation test was used for correlating the data. Statistical significance was taken as p ≤ 0.05. As a result; it was seen that cognitive flexibility and empathy were associated with disease burden in caregivers to individuals with mental problems.
... The amount of points obtained indicates that the level of cognitive flexibility is also high. In another study, Maltby et al. (2004) calculated the internal coefficient of BEO to be 0.92. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bu amaç için lisede teslim alınabilecek, duygusal zeka ve başa çıkabilse içinden birinden bebektir. Araştırmaya 460 lise öğrencisi katılmıştır. Lise nakliyesinin içinde; seçilen, fitness, yaş, sınıf düzeyi, okul bölgelerine göre manzara yokken, türü ve spor bölümlerine göre görünürde olduğu gibi. Hayattaki Zeka bölümündeki yaşamda, sınıf seviyesinde ve sporı boylarındaykenzken seçimler, uygunluk durumu, okul türlerinde ve görünümlerinde görünümlerinde. Başa geleceğinde; spor yapma ortamına göre görezlerde bulunmazken,yaş sınıfı, sınıf düzeyi, okul türü ve bölüm türü, spor branşında görüşüyle belirlenir. sonuç olarak; Neyden yüksek seviyedeki ve herhangi bir bölgeden ve herhangi bir noktasal noktasları kaba çevrelerinin herhangi bir noktasından ve tepeden yoksun, liseden ve duygusal bölgeden herhangi bir noktası yoktur.
... According to Jacobson and Spiro (1995), CF is the ability to switch between thoughts appropriately and evaluate more than one thought at the same time. CF usually manifests itself in new or unexpected situations and is expressed in awareness of alternatives (Maltby et al., 2004;Martin & Anderson, 1998Martin & Rubin, 1995). Statements about CF emphasize evaluating from broad perspectives rather than focusing on a single point. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to investigate the predictor power of cognitive flexibility, anxiety, and neuroticism, and openness personality traits on defensive pessimism in the context of a high-stakes test. For this purpose, the study conducts the adaptation of the Defensive Pessimism Questionnaire (DPQ) and the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI), and measures the validity and reliability of these tools on Turkish teenagers. The participants in the study consisted of three independent groups of participants with a total number of 1361 students enrolling at high schools. The DPQ, CFI, Big-Five Inventory, and Trait-State Anxiety Scale were administered to the participants. The findings demonstrated that the factor structures of both scales were similar to the original forms, and both of the scales had a high level of validity and reliability. The study also concluded that cognitive flexibility, anxiety, and personality explained 44% of defensive pessimism after a regression analysis.
... Similar problems exist for self-report tests of "cognitive flexibility." The original CFI (Dennis & Vander Wal, 2010) has been translated into various languages such as Mandarin (Wang et al., 2016), Turkish (Sapmaz & Doğan, 2013), Russian (Kurginyan & Osavolyuk, 2018), Italian (Portoghese et al., 2020), and Persian (Shareh et al., 2014), while the original CFS (Martin & Rubin, 1995) has been translated into Japanese (Oshiro et al., 2016) and Turkish (Çelikkaleli, 2014), with both tests demonstrating favorable psychometric properties including, but not limited to, internal consistency (Kurginyan & Osavolyuk, 2018;Maltby et al., 2004;Martin & Anderson, 1998;Oshiro et al., 2016;Portoghese et al., 2020;Shareh et al., 2014), test-retest reliability (Dennis & Vander Wal, 2010;Kurginyan & Osavolyuk, 2018;Martin & Rubin, 1995;Oshiro et al., 2016;Shareh et al., 2014), and convergent construct validity (Dennis & Vander Wal, 2010;Johnco et al., 2014a;Portoghese et al., 2020). It is noteworthy that all of the above-mentioned studies with the exception of Johnco et al. (2014a) used student samples, limiting the generalisability of the findings. ...
Article
Objective: Cognitive flexibility has been previously described as the ability to adjust cognitive and behavioral strategies in response to changing contextual demands. Cognitive flexibility is typically assessed via self-report questionnaires and performance on neuropsychological tests in research and clinical practice. A common assumption among researchers and clinicians is that self-report and neuropsychological tests of cognitive flexibility assess the same or similar constructs, but the extent of the relationship between these two assessment approaches in clinical cohorts remains unknown. We undertook a systematic review and meta-analysis to determine the relationship between self-report and neuropsychological tests of cognitive flexibility in clinical samples. Method: We searched 10 databases and relevant gray literature (e.g., other databases and pearling) from inception to October 2020 and used the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses reporting guidelines. Eleven articles including 405 participants satisfied our eligibility criteria. Results: A multilevel random-effects meta-analysis revealed no relationship between self-report and neuropsychological tests of cognitive flexibility (0.01, 95% CI [-0.16 to 0.18]). Individual random-effects meta-analyses between 12 different tests pairs also found no relationship. Conclusion: Based on our results, it is clear that the two assessment approaches of cognitive flexibility provide independent information-they do not assess the same construct. These findings have important ramifications for future research and clinical practice-there is a need to reconsider what constructs self-report and neuropsychological tests of "cognitive flexibility" actually assess, and avoid the interchangeable use of these assessments in clinical samples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Celebrity worship adalah sebuah fenomena yang berkisar dari kekaguman yang rendah hingga tahap berat terhadap selebriti. Salah satu faktor yang terkait celebrity worship adalah harga diri. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengetahui hubungan harga diri dengan celebrity worship pada remaja di Fans Club Musik Sulawesi Selatan. Subjek penelitian terdiri dari 64 remaja. Metode yang digunakan dalam penelitian ini yakni metode penelitian kuantitatif dengan teknik sampling aksidental. Hasil penelitian didapatkan bahwa ada hubungan negatif harga diri dengan celebrity worship pada remaja di Fans Club Musik Sulawesi Selatan (p=0,008, r=-0,328, R-Square=0,107). Penelitian ini menggambarkan bahwa semakin tinggi harga diri individu maka semakin rendah celebrity worship dan sebaliknya. Hasil penelitian bermanfaat sebagai upaya mencegah dampak negatif celebrity worship melalui peningkatan harga diri pada remaja agar dapat terhindar dari berbagai kesulitan psikologis.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to examine whether emotional regulation processes play a mediating role in the relationship between cognitive flexibility and intolerance to uncertainty among university students. The sample consisted of 378 participants aged from 18 to 45 years. Data were obtained using the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI), Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS), and Emotion Regulation Processes Scale (ERPS). Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was used to analyze the data. According to the results obtained from the research, emotion regulation processes have a partial mediating role in the relationship between cognitive flexibility and intolerance to uncertainty. The bootstrapping process also provided additional evidence that this partial mediation was significant. As a result, low cognitive flexibility negatively affects emotion regulation processes, resulting in low tolerance for uncertain situations.
Book
Full-text available
Researchers across disciplines have been studying the psychology of fans for decades. Seeking to better understand fan behavior and the various factors motivating fans, researchers have studied dozens of variables in hundreds of studies of different fan groups. To date, however, there have been relatively few attempts to integrate this sizable body of work, pulling together findings across from the field to with a broader, more holistic perspective. This book does exactly that, identifying and concisely summarizing research on 28 separate lines of inquiry on the psychology of fans and integrating it all into an empirically-validated model known as the CAPE model. Useful as a textbook for a fandom studies course and as a handbook for fan researchers, this book is essential reading for anyone looking to better understand the state of fan psychology and wanting to conduct their own research exploring the ins and outs of fans of all sorts!
Article
The current study tested the effects of a celebrity's virtual reality (VR) video on YouTube on audience transportation, parasocial interaction, identification, worship, and behavioral intention of social campaign participation. In addition to VR effects, fans and nonfans were compared with each other for engagement. The media content of BTS, a Korean pop boy band, was experimented in six groups: VR video with a headset, 360-degree video, 2D video, audio, news article, and control. A posttest only experiment was conducted among 142 participants in total. Results found that the VR with a headset group showed a significant difference from the news article group in transportation into BTS. The VR with a headset group had higher parasocial interaction with BTS than the control group. BTS fans were more likely than nonfans to show significant transportation, parasocial interaction, identification, worship, and behavioral intention.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to establish further validity for the Cognitive Flexibility Scale (Martin & Rubin, 1995) and the Communication Flexibility Scale (Martin & Rubin, 1994) by considering their relationships with argumentativeness, verbal aggressiveness, Machiavellianism, and tolerance for disagreement. Participants (N = 276) completed a questionnaire that included the Cognitive Flexibility Scale, the Communication Flexibility Scale, and measures for the other variables. Cognitive flexibility and communication flexibility were positively related to argumentativeness and tolerance for disagreement and negatively related to verbal aggressiveness. Communication flexibility was also negatively correlated to Machiavellianism.
Article
Full-text available
Measures of shyness, loneliness and attitudes toward a favorite celebrity were administered to 150 participants. We hypothesized that shyness and loneliness would be linked to the strength of one's "parasocial" relationship with celebrities, and to "highly visible" celebrities in particular. Of the 16 correlation coefficients we obtained, 15 were in the predicted direction, but none exceeded .24. Either shy and lonely people are not predisposed to seek strong parasocial bonds with celebrities or the hypothesized relationships are so weak that they account for very little variance.
Article
Full-text available
The complexities of everyday life reinforce the need to be cognitively flexible. Cognitive flexibility refers to a person's awareness of communication alternatives, willingness to adapt to the situation, and self‐efficacy in being flexible. Three studies were conducted to establish further the validity of the Cognitive Flexibility Scale (Martin & Rubin, 1995). In Study One, cognitive flexibility was positively related to two other identified constructs of communication competence, assertiveness and responsiveness. In Study Two, respondents’ assessments of their own cognitive flexibility were positively related to ratings from their friends. In Study Three, a positive relationship was found between being cognitively flexible and confidence in performing communication behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
he Celebrity Appeal Questionnaire was constructed to operationalize constructs related to parasocial attraction or attraction to celebrities by their fans. The article contains a factor analysis of responses on this questionnaire by 81 college students and a group of 367 Michael Jackson concert attendees. Components of parasocial attraction were perceived sex appeal, perceived competence (here as an entertainer), and perception of the artist as a prosocial person. Mystique was not a predictor of fans' attraction. Further work to establish generality of applicability of the scales and findings is discussed.
Article
Full-text available
There is evidence that love styles can be regarded as personality traits. If so, we might expect that people who value a particular love style with a lover might value that same style in non-partnering, parasocial relationships. A sample of 107 college students filled out the Love Attitudes Scale (LAS) and the Celebrity Attitude Scale (CAS). There were few significant correlations. Highly significant correlations were obtained between the CAS and the game-playing and dependent love styles for a sub-sample of 39 students whose favorite celebrities were likely to be fantasy love attractions. Game-playing and dependency may be fairly stable personality traits that influence one's choice of a favorite type of celebrity. Gender differences and possible negative implications of celebrity adulation for game-players were discussed.
Article
This report concerns the validation of a questionnaire designed to assess social-complexity or the extent to which individuals attempt to join and maintain memberships in a number of diverse groups. Three studies (ns of 118, 63, and 125) attest to the single-factor instrument's internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and validity. In addition, higher scores on the joiners' social-complexity measure were significantly correlated with positive scores on several measures of psychological adjustment. Suggestions for use of the instrument are provided