ArticlePDF Available

The Impact of Mirrors on Body Image and Classroom Performance in Female College Ballet Dancers

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study investigated the effect of teaching with mirrors on the Body-Areas Satisfaction component of body image and dance performance of female college ballet dancers. With the same instructor in two beginning ballet classes, 15 females were taught using mirrors and 15 females were taught without mirrors. Subjects completed the Cash 69-item Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire (MBSRQ) during the first and last week of classes of a 14-week semester to measure body image. Dance instructors used the Radell Evaluation Scale for Dance Technique to assess students' performance of the same adagio and allegro phrases danced during the fifth and fourteenth week of the semester. For the non-mirror class, there was a significant increase in adagio performance scores and a trend increase in allegro performance scores from pretest to posttest. Significant increases in adagio and allegro scores were not shown for the mirror class. Higher performing students, performing without the presence of the mirror, had increased Body-Areas Satisfaction scores on the MBSRQ. However, the higher performing students in the mirror class had decreased Body-Areas Satisfaction scores. These results suggest that the use of the mirror in a beginning ballet classroom may negatively affect the skill acquisition of the dancer and may contribute to the low body image scores of higher performing dancers.
Content may be subject to copyright.
47
Original Article
Sally A. Radell, M.F.A., M.A., directs the Dance Program and Daniel D. Adame,
Ph.D., M.S.P.H., teaches health education in the Department of Health, Physical
Education and Dance, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia. Steven P. Cole, Ph.D., is
the director of Research Design Associates, Inc., Yorktown Heights, New York.
Correspondence: Sally A. Radell, M.F.A., M.A., Dance Program, Department of
Health, Physical Education and Dance, 115 Rich Building, Emory University,
Atlanta, Georgia 30322.
Abstract
This study investigated the effect of
teaching with mirrors on the Body-Areas
Satisfaction component of body image
and dance performance of female college
ballet dancers. With the same instructor
in two beginning ballet classes, 15 females
were taught using mirrors and 15 females
were taught without mirrors. Subjects
completed the Cash 69-item Multidimen
-
sional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire
(MBSRQ) during the first and last week
of classes of a 14-week semester to mea
-
sure body image. Dance instructors used
the Radell Evaluation Scale for Dance
Technique to assess studentsperformance
of the same adagio and allegro phrases
danced during the fifth and fourteenth
week of the semester. For the non-mirror
class, there was a significant increase in
adagio performance scores and a trend in
-
crease in allegro performance scores from
pretest to posttest. Significant increases in
adagio and allegro scores were not shown
for the mirror class. Higher performing
students, performing without the presence
of the mirror, had increased Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores on the MBSRQ. How
-
ever, the higher performing students in
the mirror class had decreased Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores. ese results suggest
that the use of the mirror in a beginning
ballet classroom may negatively affect the
skill acquisition of the dancer and may
contribute to the low body image scores
of higher performing dancers.
B
ody image is the perception we
have of our body; it is our body
concept.
1,2
A dancers personal
vision of their body image is an impor-
tant component of her psychological
health and well being and can help
or hinder their performance in the
classroom. A review of the literature
on body image and ballet has shown
that the study of ballet may negatively
influence ballet dancersbody image
(i.e., heightening self-attention, self-
focus or self-consciousness). Bettle
and colleagues,
3
for example, com-
pared a group of adolescent female
ballet dancers with a comparison
group of non-dancers and found that
the dancers scored higher than the
non-dancers on questions indicating
feelings of undesirability for person
-
ality and unattractiveness for body.
In a similar study, Brooks-Gunn and
Warren
4
compared the body image
scores of girls attending national ballet
company schools with non-dancing
girls who attended private schools.
e researchers found that the on-
time maturing dancers body image
scores were lower than the body im
-
age scores of the on-time maturing
non-dancing students. Similarly, the
research of Pierce and Daleng
5
and
Druss and Silverman
6
corroborated
the incidence of body image distor-
tion among professionally-focused
ballet dancers.
e use of mirrors as a teaching tool
has long been a tradition in ballet. To
our knowledge only two studies have
addressed how the mirror affects the
learning environment of dancers. Radell
and associates
7
investigated the effect of
mirrors on the body image of female
dancers enrolled in college beginning
ballet classes during the course of a
14-week academic semester. Using the
Cash
8
69-item Multidimensional Body-
Self-Relations Questionnaire (MBSRQ),
they found that the use of mirrors in
teaching may have contributed to low
Body-Areas Satisfaction scores among
the dancers. e Body-Areas Satisfaction
Subscale (BASS) of the Cash MBSRQ
assesses satisfaction or dissatisfaction
with discrete aspect of ones body areas.
In a more recent study, Radell and co
-
workers
9
investigated the use of mirrors
on ballet classroom performance. Using
the Radell Evaluation Scale for Dance
Technique (RESDT), which was de
-
signed to examine technical performance
growth in the classroom, they reported
that the use of mirrors in ballet instruc
-
tion might have harmed the technical
skill acquisition of the dancers in the
classroom setting.
e Impact of Mirrors on Body Image and
Classroom Performance in Female College Ballet
Dancers
Sally A. Radell, M.F.A., M.A., Daniel D. Adame, Ph.D., M.S.P.H., and Steven P. Cole, Ph.D.
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science48 Volume 8, Number 2, 2004
Research with non-dancers links the
use of the mirror to a heightened sense
of self-consciousness, thus creating an
impact on one’s body image. Carver
and Scheier
10
investigated the effect of
mirrors on the self-consciousness of
79 female non-dancers and found that
the mirrors heightened their subjects
self-awareness resulting in increased
self-attention or self-focus. In other
words, the mirrors were instrumental
in manipulating self-awareness by
drawing direct attention to the self
(private self-consciousness) as when,
for example, individuals are aware
they are being noticed by others or are
aware of an audience. Martin Ginis
and colleagues
11
found that exercise
performed in front of mirrors had a
negative effect on the feeling states
of sedentary women non-dancers.
Although the body image concerns
of the women did not moderate the
effects of the mirror intervention,
women with greater body image con
-
cerns experienced increases in physical
appearance anxiety regardless of their
exercise condition. Martin Ginis and
colleagues
11
hypothesized the negative
effects of mirrors within the frame-
work of the Objective Self-Awareness
eory of Duval and Wicklund.
12
is
theory posits that a state of heightened
self-awareness can be created when
an object in one’s environment, like
a mirror, focuses an individuals at
-
tention on one’s self. is state of
self-awareness causes the individual to
compare one’s self to ideals presented
in the environment. In a ballet class,
this could be other studentsperfor
-
mances or a teacher’s demonstration.
If the student feels she is not matching
the ideal characteristics presented to
her, then negative self-evaluation may
result.
Other research has shown a rela
-
tionship between body image and
performance. Dasch
13
studied 33
female and male undergraduates en-
rolled in modern dance, social dance,
square dance, tap, and jazz. Dancers
with higher body cathexis,
14
that is,
Body-Areas Satisfaction, were judged
by dance instructors to have better
dance posture and technique. In a
study of 112 weight-lifting males,
Tucker
15
found that psychological
traits, especially body cathexis, were
significant predictors of physical per
-
formance in weight training. More
recently, Nowicki and associates
16
found that prediction of college stu-
dents’ physical tness performance
varied depending on the value placed
on their body image, locus of control,
their gender, and whether they were
tested by a same-sex or an opposite-
sex examiner. However, none of
these studies focused on the dance
performance of ballet dancers.
Performance among ballet dancers
is founded on dance experience. In
beginning ballet dance classes, there
are often students who have had
some previous dance experience. As
students develop, they acquire the
skills with which to evaluate and
finely tune their performance. Ac
-
cordingly, Kimmerle and Côté-Lau-
rence
17
noted that dancers develop
their ability to see and evaluate their
movement performance based on
their dance experience. One of the
aims of this study was to assess dance
performance. To achieve this goal,
the RESDT was used to access class
-
room technical growth in movement
performance and is described in the
methods section below.
This study investigated the effect
of teaching with mirrors on body
image and dance performance of
female collegiate beginning ballet
dancers. To achieve this goal, the
following research questions where
addressed:
1. Is there a difference in dance
performance for dancers taught
in a classroom with mirrors versus
dancers taught in a classroom
without mirrors?
2. Is there a difference in body im
-
age for dancers taught in a class-
room with mirrors versus dancers
taught in a classroom without
mirrors?
3. Is the effect of teaching with mir
-
rors on body image different for
dancers with low scores on the
RESDT versus dancers with high
scores on the RESDT?
Question 1 is essentially a replication
of Radell and colleagues
9
and, based
on their results, we hypothesized that
the performance of dancers in the
non-mirror class would improve to a
greater degree than the performance of
dancers in the mirror class. Question
2 is a replication of Radell and associ
-
ates
7
and, based on their results, we
hypothesized that the body image of
dancers in the non-mirror class would
be higher at the end of the semester
than the body image of dancers in the
mirror class. is is the first empirical
study investigating changes in body
image and performance measures for
the same dancers. Because of the lack
of research regarding the relationship
of body image and dance performance
in mirrored versus non-mirrored
teaching environments, it was difficult
to formulate directional hypotheses
about the results of the current inves
-
tigation for Question 3. In general,
better body image has been associated
with better physical performance for
non-dancers.
13,15
Method
Participants
Fifteen females enrolled in a begin
-
ning ballet class were taught using
mirrors and a second group of 15 fe
-
males also enrolled in a beginning bal-
let class were taught without mirrors.
Students did not have to fulfill specific
body-type or weight requirements for
enrollment. e classes were taught
within a liberal arts college curricu
-
lum at a medium-sized university in
a suburb of a large southeastern U.S.
city. Motives for taking these classes
included personal enjoyment, to earn
physical education elective credit, or
to earn credit toward either the dance
major or minor degree.
Evaluators
e three evaluators scoring the danc-
ers were full-time dance faculty at the
university where the study was con
-
ducted. e evaluators held the same
terminal degree in the field of dance
(Master of Fine Arts) and were primar
-
ily modern dancers who had studied
ballet as a secondary technique since
childhood. All three faculty members
had taught beginning ballet at the col
-
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science 49Volume 8, Number 2, 2004
lege level for a minimum of five to a
maximum of fifteen years.
Procedure
Informed Consent
Before any testing instruments were
administered, all participants signed
informed consent documents to indi
-
cate willingness to take part in a study
that examined different methodolo
-
gies for teaching ballet. e Institu-
tional Review Board of the university
approved all aspects of the study.
Body Image
Students completed the Cash 69-item
Multidimensional Body Self-Rela
-
tions Questionnaire (MBSRQ) during
the first and last classes of a 14-week
semester. e MBSRQ is a 69-item
standardized, attitudinal assessment of
body-image and weight-related vari
-
ables with acceptable reliability and
validity in a number of settings.
18-20
Of the 10 body-image scales, the most
salient scale to assess the effects of mir
-
rors on body image is the Body-Areas
Satisfaction Scale (BASS). e BASS
(8 items) measures body image evalu
-
ation as satisfaction to dissatisfaction
with discrete body parts or features.
Subjects were asked to rate how satis
-
fied they were with areas or aspects of
their body (such as face, lower torso,
and muscle tone) using a scale rang
-
ing from 1 to 5 (where 1 indicated
very dissatisfied and 5 very satisfied).
We selected the BASS because of its
demonstrated reliability and valid
-
ity,
21,22
and because the scale focuses
on discrete body areas known to be
of concern to dancers.
23
Cronbach
alpha coefficients for Body-Areas
Satisfaction reported by Adame and
colleagues
24
were .69 and .86 for col-
lege females and males, respectively.
Martin Ginis and associates
11
also
used the BASS as a measure of body
image in their study on the impact of
mirrors on exercising women.
Performance Evaluation
Performance evaluation was assessed
via the Radell Evaluation Scale for
Dance Technique (RESDT), which
was specifically designed for the
study to measure technical growth
in the performance of classroom ma
-
terial throughout the semester. e
RESDT includes both a videotape
experience and resultant observa
-
tion score made by evaluators of the
students’ performance of the same
adagio and allegro phrases during
the fifth and fourteenth week of a
14-week semester. An adagio phrase
is a series of slow and successive dance
movements that frequently highlight
a dancer’s alignment, balance, and
strength. A grand allegro phrase is a
series of quick and elevated large steps
that frequently focuses on a dancer’s
mastery of weight change, movement
flow, and rhythmic accuracy. At the
end of the semester three ballet teach
-
ers viewed the videotapes for both
classes and completed the RESDT
for each dancer. e instructor of
the course served as one of the three
evaluators. The other two evalua
-
tors were experimentally blind and
had no knowledge of which was the
mirror or non-mirror class during
the evaluation process. Following
RESDT guidelines, the evaluators
were instructed to choose a score for
each dancer ranging from 1 (low skill
level) to 5 (high skill level) for both
the adagio and allegro phrases. e
teachers first evaluated two students
at both skill level extremes to assure
the validity of the evaluation criteria.
ey then shared information on how
each rating was chosen. After this step
they independently rated the perfor
-
mance of each dancer for the adagio
and allegro phrases. The teachers
selected a score for each phrase that
corresponded with their view of the
dancers performance based on a com
-
pilation of the four selected criteria.
ese criteria were:
1. Rhythmic accuracy;
2. Ease and flow of movement;
3. Mastery of steps and
4. Alignment.
None of these criteria were considered
in isolation but rather they were col
-
lectively taken into account by each
evaluator when assigning a score for
each dancer’s performance of either
the adagio or allegro phrase. A mean
performance score was calculated for
each dancer on both the adagio and
allegro phrase by using the three scores
each evaluator assigned each dancer
for a given phrase. Both classes were
taught by the same instructor and re
-
ceived the same curricular instruction
for 2.5 hours each week.
Radell and coworkers
9
in a 2003
study reported high inter-rater reli-
ability for the RESDT instrument
using two evaluators and a sample
of 27 collegiate beginning ballet stu
-
dents. e intra-class correlations for
adagio and allegro scores were .95 (p <
.001) and .91 (p < .001) respectively.
e Pearson correlation coefficients
for adagio and allegro scores were .91
(p < .001) and .84 (p < .001) respec
-
tively.
Results
Inter-rater reliabilities between the
three ballet teachers were excellent.
e intra-class correlations for adagio
and allegro scores were .95 (p < .001)
and .93 (p < .001), respectively. e
Pearson correlation coefficients for
adagio scores were .88 (p < .001), .85
(p < .001), and .87 (p < .001). e
correlation coefficients for allegro
scores were .90 (p < .001), .87 (p <
.001), and .89 (p < .001).
Preliminary t-tests for independent
samples revealed no age difference be
-
tween the mirror class (Mean: 19.85;
SD: 1.38) and the non-mirror class
(Mean: 20.58; SD: 1.66), p = .20, and
no difference in the number of years
of prior ballet experience between the
mirror class (Mean: 1.07; SD: 1.53)
and the non-mirror class (Mean: 1.07;
SD: 1.71), p = 1.00.
Performance With and Without
Mirrors
Means and standard deviations at
pretest and posttest for the mirror
and non-mirror class are presented
in Table 1. ere were no differences
in pretest means between classes for
adagio or allegro scores as assessed
by t-tests for independent samples.
A 2 (pretest, posttest) x 2 (mir
-
ror, non-mirror) repeated measures
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
conducted separately for adagio and
allegro measures. For adagio scores,
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science50 Volume 8, Number 2, 2004
there was a statistically signicant
interaction: the increase in mean test
scores from pretest to posttest was
larger for the non-mirror class than
for the mirror class. Adagio scores
increased 36.0% for the non-mirror
class from pretest to posttest and in
-
creased 6.7% for the mirror class, F(1,
28) = 8.62, p = .007, eta squared =
0.24. Simple effects analysis revealed
that the increase for the non-mirror
class was statistically significant (p
< .001) while the increase for the
mirror class was not significant (p =
.310). For allegro scores, there was
a non-significant interaction trend.
Allegro scores increased 22.5% for
the non-mirror class from pretest to
posttest and increased 4.0% for the
mirror class, F(1, 28) = 3.02, p = .093,
eta squared = 0.10. Simple effects
analysis revealed that the increase for
the non-mirror class was statistically
significant (p < .01) while the increase
for the mirror class was not significant
(p = .625). In summary, adagio scores
were significantly higher for dancers
taught without mirrors and there was
a trend for allegro scores to be higher
for dancers taught without mirrors.
Body Image With and Without
Mirrors
ere were no differences in pretest
means between classes for Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores as assessed by t-tests
for independent samples. A 2 (pretest,
posttest) x 2 (mirror, non-mirror)
repeated measures ANOVA revealed
a non-significant trend for the interac
-
tion: there was 4.3% increase in mean
test scores from pretest to posttest
for the non-mirror class while there
was a 3.2% decrease for the mirror
class, F(1, 28) = 3.80, p = .061, eta
squared = 0.12. In summary, there
was a non-significant trend for Body-
Areas Satisfaction scores to increase
for dancers taught without mirrors
and to decrease for dancers taught
with mirrors.
Body-Areas Satisfaction and
Performance Differences
To further assess changes in Body-
Areas Satisfaction scores, dancers were
categorized as high performers or low
performers based on their pretest per
-
formance scores. Dancers who scored
in the top half of their class for pretest
adagio and allegro performance were
classified as high performers. Dancers
who scored in the bottom half of their
class for adagio and allegro perfor
-
mance were classified as low perform-
ers. Means and standard deviations
at pretest and posttest for the mirror
and non-mirror class by performance
group are presented in Table 2. A 2
(pretest, posttest) x 2 (mirror, non-
mirror) repeated measures ANOVA
for Body-Areas Satisfaction scores
was conducted separately for low and
high performers. For high performers,
there was a statistically significant in-
teraction: Body-Areas Satisfaction in-
creased 16.4% from pretest to posttest
for the non-mirror class while there
was a 4.2% decrease in scores for the
mirror class, F(1, 11) = 5.60, p = .04,
eta squared = .34. For the high per
-
formers in the non-mirror class, none
of the dancers exhibited a decrease
in Body-Areas Satisfaction scores.
However, for the high performers in
the mirror class, 63% of the dancers
exhibited a decrease in Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores. There were no
differences in Body-Areas Satisfaction
scores between the non-mirror and
mirror classes for low performers. For
the non-mirror class low performers,
scores increased 0.1% from pretest
to posttest. For the mirror class low
performers, scores decreased 2.1%
from pretest to posttest.
Pearson correlation coefficients be-
tween Body-Areas Satisfaction scores
and dance performance scores were
not statistically significant. For pretest
mirror class dancers the correlations
for adagio and allegro were -.09 and
-.11, respectively. For the pretest non-
mirror class, the correlations were .03,
and -.03. For the posttest mirror class,
the correlations were -.02 and .02.
For the posttest non-mirror class, the
correlations were .03 and -.03.
In summary, for dancers with high
scores on the RESDT, Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores increased for the
dancers in the non-mirror class and
decreased for dancers in the mirror
class. ere were no Body-Areas Sat
-
isfaction differences between classes
for dancers with low scores on the
RESDT.
Discussion
Performance With and Without
Mirrors
For the non-mirror class, there was
a significant increase in adagio per
-
Table 1 Performance Measures by Class (Mean and Standard Deviation)
Mirror Class Non-Mirror Class
(N = 15) (N = 15)
Performance Scores Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Adagio 2.69 (1.20) 2.87 (1.22) 2.36 (0.99) 3.20 (1.06)
Allegro 2.76 (1.25) 2.87 (1.29) 2.58 (1.07) 3.16 (1.17)
Table 2 Mean and Standard Deviation for Body Image by Class and
Pretest Performance
Body-Areas Satisfaction
N Pretest Posttest
Mirror Class
Low Performance 7 3.34 (0.34) 3.27 (0.30)
High Performance 8 3.31 (0.60) 3.17 (0.87)
Non-Mirror Class
Low Performance 10 3.53 (0.65) 3.56 (0.53)
High Performance 8 3.35 (0.49) 3.90 (0.54)
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science 51Volume 8, Number 2, 2004
formance scores and a trend increase
in allegro performance scores from
pretest to posttest. e increase for
the mirror class for adagio scores was
significantly less than for the non-
mirror class. The increase for the
mirror class for allegro scores was not
significantly different from the non-
mirror class increase. e mirror class
increases for adagio and allegro scores
were small and not significant. is is
consistent with the findings of Radell
and colleagues
9
who in 2003, also
using the RESDT and a comparable
group of beginning ballet students, re
-
ported a significant increase in adagio
scores and a trend increase in allegro
scores for the non-mirror class. As
Radell and colleagues
9
hypothesized,
the use of the mirror was distracting
and may have inhibited the dancers
ability to focus more internally on
the performance of the phrase and
thus resulted in less performance
improvement. It should also be noted
that since allegro phrases are quick
and lively, there tends to be less time
to fully use the mirror as a feedback
tool. An adagio phrase, in contrast,
is a slow and successive phrase with
generally more opportunity for mir-
ror interaction. Perhaps there was a
greater opportunity for the mirror to
inhibit growth in the adagio phrase.
is possibility was supported by the
large and significant improvement in
performance for the non-mirror class
in the adagio phrase.
Body Image With and Without
Mirrors
A non-significant trend for the effects
of the mirror intervention and Body-
Areas Satisfaction over the course of
the semester provided some support
for our second hypothesis. There
was an increase in mean test scores
for the non-mirror class while there
was a decrease for the mirror class.
Even though these results were not
statistically signicant, there was a
clear trend that was consistent with
Radell and associates
7
who found that
the use of mirrors in teaching begin-
ning ballet may have contributed to
low Body-Areas Satisfaction scores
among dancers. In addition, the aver
-
age number of years of previous dance
experience in our study was less than
those in the Radell and associates
7
study. It is possible that, because of
the decreased dance experience, the
sample of dancers in our study was
less critical of their bodies.
Body-Areas Satisfaction and
Performance Differences
Although a few studies
13,15
that ex-
plored the relationship between body
cathexis (Body-Areas Satisfaction) and
performance found that better body
cathexis was associated with better
performance, we found no correlation
of increased body image with better
performance. However, we did find
that higher performing students,
performing without the presence of
the mirror, had increased Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores, whereas, the high
performing students in the mirror
class had decreased Body-Areas Satis
-
faction scores.
e high performing students in
the non-mirror class may have had
a strong and positive sense of their
bodies and the absence of a mirror
therefore allowed them to fully focus
on their kinesthetic abilities, which
resulted in greater body-areas satisfac
-
tion. No high performing students
in the non-mirror class exhibited a
decrease in their Body-Areas Satisfac
-
tion score. In turn, perhaps the high
performers in the mirror class were
distracted from trusting their physi
-
cal selves and this, in turn, resulted
in 63% of the mirror class dancers
feeling less satisfied with their body
areas.
According to Kimmerle and Cote-
Laurence,
17
experienced dancers, or
higher performing students, develop
their abilities to observe and evaluate,
and thus fine-tune their performance
over time. Hence, as they gaze into the
mirror they may become more criti
-
cal of their performance as reflected
in Duval and Wicklunds Theory
of Objective Self-Awareness which
states that mirrors can be influential
in heightening self-awareness and
self-evaluation. Within mirrored
classrooms then, dancers may be pre
-
occupied with comparing themselves
with other dancers, their instructors,
or some ideal presented to them
within the learning context. e danc
-
ers’ rehearsing in the non-mirrored
classroom, on the other hand, can
make no such comparisons, so they
are presumable free from using the
mirror as a form of negative impact.
Conclusions
These results support the work of
Carver and Scheier
10
who found
that mirrors heightened the self-con-
sciousness of women college students
and the work of Martin Ginis and
coworkers
11
who found that exercise
in front of a mirror caused sedentary
women to feel worse about themselves
than their counterparts who exercised
without a mirror. Our results also are
consistent with previous literature that
associates body image disturbances
with the study of ballet.
3-6
e cur-
rent study’s results also replicate the
findings of Radell and colleagues
7
who found that the use of mirrors
in teaching beginning ballet classes
may contribute to lower Body-Areas
Satisfaction scores. e results of our
study also indicate that the use of
mirrors in the teaching of beginning
ballet classes may negatively affect
classroom performance. Our find
-
ings are consistent with Duval and
Wicklund’s Objective Self-Awareness
eory
12
and the potential negative
effects of mirrors. Further exploration
of variables that allow for the most ef
-
fective learning environments for the
study of dance will only enhance the
effectiveness of training in the profes
-
sion with the ultimate goal to improve
dancers’ body image, well-being, and
performance.
References
1. Adame DD, Frank RE, Serdula MK,
Cole SP, Abbas MA: e relationship
of self-perceived weight to actual
weight, body image, and health be
-
haviors of college freshmen. Wellness
Perspectives: eory, Research and
Practice 7:31-40, 1990.
2. Schilder PF: e Image and Appear
-
ance of the Human Body. New York:
International Universities Press,
1950.
3. Bettle N, Bettle O, Neumarker U,
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science52 Volume 8, Number 2, 2004
Neumarker KJ: Body image and self-
esteem in adolescent ballet dancers.
Perceptual and Motor Skills 93:297-
309, 2001.
4. Brooks-Gunn J, Warren MP: e
effects of delayed menarche in dif
-
ferent contexts: Dance and nondance
students. Journal of Youth and Ado
-
lescence 14:285-300, 1985.
5. Pierce EF, Daleng ML: Distortion
of body image among elite female
dancers. Perceptual and Motor Skills
87:769-770, 1998.
6. Druss RG, Silverman JA: Body im
-
age and perfectionism of ballerinas.
General Hospital Psychiatry 1:115-
121, 1979.
7. Radell SA, Adame DD, Cole SP: Ef
-
fect of teaching with mirrors on body
image and locus of control in women
college ballet dancers. Perceptual and
Motor Skills 95:1239-1247, 2002.
8. ompson JK, Penner LA, Altabe
MN: Procedures, problems, and
progress in the assessment of body
images. In: Cash TF, Pruzinsky T
(eds): Body Images: Development,
Deviance, and Change. New York:
Guilford, 1990, pp. 21-48.
9. Radell SA, Adame DD, Cole SP:
e effect of teaching with mirrors
on ballet dance performance. Percep
-
tual and Motor Skills 97:960-964,
2003.
10. Carver CS, Scheier MF: Self-focusing
effects of dispositional self-conscious
-
ness, mirror presence, and audience
presence. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 36:324-332,
1978.
11. Martin Ginis KA, Jung ME, Gauvin
L: To see or not to see: Effects of
exercising in mirrored environments
on sedentary womens feeling states
and self-efficacy. Health Psychology
22(4):354-361, 2003.
12. Duval S, Wicklund RA: A eory of
Objective Self-Awareness. New York:
Academic Press, 1972.
13. Dasch CS: Relation of dance skills
to body cathexis and locus of control
orientation. Perceptual and Motor
Skills 46:465-466, 1978.
14. Secord PF, Jourard SM: e appraisal
of body-cathexis: Body-cathexis and
the self. Journal of Consulting Psy
-
chology 17:343-347, 1953.
15. Tucker LA: Trait psychology and
performance: A credulous viewpoint.
Journal of Human Movement Stud
-
ies 10:53-62, 1984.
16. Nowicki S Jr, Adame DD, Johnson
TC, Cole SP: Physical fitness as a
function of psychological and situ
-
ational factors. e Journal of Social
Psychology 13(5):549-558, 1997.
17. Kimmerle M, Côté-Laurence P
:
Teaching Dance Skills: A Motor Learn
-
ing and Development Approach. An
-
dover, New Jersey: J. Michael Ryan
Publishing, Inc., 2003, p. 71.
18. Cash TF, Brown TA: Gender and
body images: Stereotypes and reali
-
ties. Sex Roles 21:361-373, 1989.
19. Cash TF, Henry PE: Womens body
images: e results of a national sur
-
vey in the U.S.A. Sex Roles 33:19-28,
1995.
20. Cash TF: The multidimensional
body-self relations questionnaire
user’s manual. Available from the
author, Old Dominion University,
Norfolk, Virginia.
21. Adame DD, Radell SA, Johnson TC,
Cole SP: Physical fitness, body im
-
age, and locus of control in college
women dancers and non-dancers.
Perceptual and Motor Skills 72:91-
95, 1991.
22. Radell SA, Adame DD, Johnson
TC, Cole SP: Dance experiences
associated with body-image and
personality among college students:
A comparison of dancers and non-
dancers. Perceptual and Motor Skills
77:507-513, 1993.
23. Kimmerle M, Côté-Laurence P,
2003, pp. 14-15.
24. Adame DD, Johnson TC, Cole SP:
Body image and physical fitness as
a function of locus of control and
gender in college women and men,
2000. Unpublished data.
... Studies show these negative effects may even appear regardless of skill level (Ehrenberg 2010;Radell et al. 2017) or dance genre (Reel et al. 2005;Ehrenberg 2010;Radell et al. 2020). Body image-the perception, thoughts, and feelings we have about our bodies (Grogan 2007)-is a critical component of a dancer's psychological health with the ability to negatively affect the skill acquisition in classroom performance Cole 2003, 2004) and significantly shape a dancer's overall well-being (Radell, Adame, and Cole 2004;Barr and Oliver 2016). Kinesiologists Draugelis, Martin, and Garn (2014), who study the predictors of well-being in collegiate dancers, describe well-being as a state of engagement involving vigor, dedication, confidence, and enthusiasm, and claim that a positive sense of well-being for dancers can help eliminate depression and burnout. ...
Article
This study investigates the impact of partial versus full mirror use on body image in a beginner-level ballet class. At the end of a seven-week period, researchers distributed an online survey inclusive of qualitative questions to two groups of female collegiate students enrolled in separate beginner-level ballet courses taught by the same professor. One group had full access to the mirror during class, while the other only had access to the mirror during center work. Results show that differences occurred between the two groups regarding how comfortable students felt in their ballet attire, how they sensed their bodies changing over the course of the semester, how they utilized the mirror during class, and how they understood the mirror’s purpose in relationship to their embodied practices. Ultimately, students with less mirror time experienced more holistic, full-body transformations by the end of the seven-week period.
... Factors associated with the individual, the task, and the environment all interact with each other to affect FOA (Chua et al. 2018). Examples for such factors include the performer's level of expertise, learning strategy and FOA preferences (Weiss et al. 2008;Maurer and Munzert 2013), perceptions and previous experiences, self-awareness, psychological state (Bernier et al. 2011), and body representations (e.g., body schema, body structural description, and body image) (Schwoebel and Coslett 2005;Radell et al. 2004Radell et al. , 2011Holmes and Spence 2004). As for the performer's skill level, a predominant theme in the FOA literature suggests that performers at all skill levels could benefit from EFOA to a greater extent, compared to IFOA (Wulf 2013;Wulf and Su 2007;Bell and Hardy 2009), with IFOA even hampering performance in experts (Wulf 2013;Wulf and Su 2007) and enhancing task complexity in novices (Zentgraf Karen 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Focus of attention (FOA) has been shown to affect human motor performance. Research into FOA has mainly posited it as either external or internal to-the-body (EFOA and IFOA, respectively). However, this binary paradigm overlooks the dynamic interactions among the individual, the task, and the environment, which are core to many disciplines, including dance. This paper reviews the comparative effects of EFOA and IFOA on human motor performance. Next, it identifies challenges within this EFOA–IFOA binary paradigm at the conceptual, definitional, and functional levels, which could lead to misinterpretation of research findings thus impeding current understanding of FOA. Building on these challenges and in effort to expand the current paradigm into a non-binary one, it offers an additional FOA category—dynamic interactive FOA—which highlights the dynamic interactions existing between EFOA and IFOA. Mental imagery is then proposed as a suitable approach for separately studying the different FOA subtypes. Lastly, clinical and research applications of a dynamic interactive FOA perspective for a wide range of domains, from motor rehabilitation to sports and dance performance enhancement, are discussed.
... Surrounded by mirrors and potentially critical eyes, dance classrooms are exposing, be it in relation to body image or virtuosic achievement. Without wanting to demonise the studio as a space full of traps, studies in the field have investigated the impact of this environment on the training of young dancers (see Lakes 2005;Radell, Adame, and Cole 2004). Furthermore, there is a traditionally established expectation of passivity, emerging from the hierarchical structure of the master-pupil relationship, particularly within the context of classical ballet and modern dance techniques. ...
Article
Dance education in the twenty-first century has started to shift with the evolution of contemporary dance techniques and the introduction of more holistic somatic practices into dance training. Nowadays, the idea of developing ‘professional self-sufficiency’ is at the heart of the learning experience for young dancers. We believe that in response to these shifts and in order to future-proof dance education there is a need for a renewed focus on theoretical approaches and academic engagement; a focus reminiscent of and aligned with the conceptual frameworks commonly associated with social sciences, in which the process of arriving at ‘professional self-sufficiency’ is inextricably linked to reflective practice. In this article, we describe a project at the Rambert School of Dance that introduced the use of creative methods for teaching reflection and reflective practice. After a brief introduction to the specific context of Rambert School, we will provide an overview of dance education and reflective practice. Subsequently, we describe the reflective-practice project with its session contents as it was delivered over the course of several weeks before providing a critical evaluation. In the final section we offer concluding thoughts regarding the relationship between reflective practice and dance education.
Article
Introduction: Measurement of performance quality in dance is important but challenging and few dance performance quality measures exist. This study aims to (1) identify and (2) assess the quality of dance performance outcome measures for ballet, modern and contemporary dance. Methods: A 2-step systematic review with two separate literature searches was conducted. Step 1 involved a systematic review to identify all ballet, modern and contemporary dance performance quality outcome measures. Step 2 involved a systematic review to identify studies that reported measurement properties (eg, validity, reliability) of the tools identified in Step 1. A comprehensive electronic search of MEDLINE, SPORTSDiscus, CINHAL, Embase and IADMS Bibliography was conducted from inception to November 2020 (Part 1) and February 2021 (Part 2). To evaluate the quality of each dance performance outcome measure, three steps were conducted (1) assessment of methodological quality using the COSMIN checklists, (2) evaluation of results against criteria of good measurement properties and (3) summary of the evidence and an overall rating of evidence using a modified Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) approach. Results: Fifteen dance performance quality outcome measures were identified (Step 1). Seventeen studies reported measurement property data for 14 of 15 identified outcome measures (Step 2). The majority of the 34 measurement property outcomes were rated as doubtful (COSMIN checklists), and demonstrated sufficient measurement properties. Only one outcome measure, the Radell Evaluation Scale for Dance Technique (RESDT), was assessed as having low and moderate quality evidence for validity and reliability respectively. The remaining 13 tools were rated as having very low-quality evidence (GRADE criteria). Conclusions: Due to low quality, the 15 dance performance quality tools cannot be confidently recommended for use at this time. Dance teachers, clinician and researchers should consider feasibility issues and use the tools with caution until adequate high-quality evidence is available.
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, we examined the immediate effect of allowing healthy participants to view their mirror-reflected body image on static and dynamic balance. We placed a mirror to allow participants to frontally view their own body image while maintaining a quiet stance or while engaged in a dynamic postural standing task. On measures of body sway during quiet stance, there were no effects of this visual feedback, supporting the view that human beings have no central mechanism for viewing the mirror-reflected body image to control body sway during quiet stance. However, the body deviated forward during quiet stance while viewing the mirror-reflected body image, indicating that viewing the mirror-reflected body image contributed to the anterior-posterior positioning of the body, as mediated by an ankle control strategy. For the dynamic standing task, viewing the body image induced unstable peaks of rhythmic lateral shifting of the body weight over the feet. This indicates that viewing the body image caused unstable motor commands for rhythmic lateral weight shifting. When participants made a transition from a bipedal to a unipedal stance in response to a cue, viewing the body image shortened the onset latency of the body sway. Accordingly, viewing the body image seemed to accelerate the motor execution involved in lateral weight shifting, possibly due to predictive activation of the motor system before movement onset. Considered collectively, we found static and dynamic stance balance to be influenced by viewing one’s mirror-reflected body image. Viewing the mirror-reflected body image may be a means of changing static and dynamic balance in patients with impaired postural control.
Article
Bu çalışmanın amacı, halk dansları figürleri öğretilirken, ortamın aynalı ya da aynasız olması durumunun, öğrenciler üzerine etkilerini incelemektir. 2016- 2017 eğitim öğretim yılı güz ve bahar dönemlerini kapsayan bu çalışma, Ankara Büyükşehir Belediyesi Batıpark Çocuk Kulübüne kayıtlı 46’sı 4.sınıf ve 49’u 2.sınıf toplamda 94 öğrenci ile yürütülmüştür. Çalışmada video analizi, memnuniyet anketi ve görüşme yöntemi kullanılmış, verilerin analizi SPSS 18 paket programı ile yapılmıştır. Araştırmanın sonucunda elde edilen nicel bulgulara göre aynalı salonda eğitim gören öğrenciler ile aynasız salonda eğitim gören öğrenciler arasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı farklılıklar tespit edilmiştir. Video analizlerinde hareket uyumu, denge bozukluğu, şaşırmalar ve ritim duygusu bakımından aynalı ve aynasız salon gruplarının karşılaştırılmasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı farklılıklar kaydedilmiştir (p<0.05). Nitel bulgulara göre, anında geri bildirim dönütünün olmasının, hızlı öğrenmeyi ve hatayı anında düzeltmeyi desteklediği vurgulanmıştır. Yine nitel bulgulara göre, özellikle görsel geri bildirimin söz konusu olduğu öğrenme ortamında öğrenci, dansçı ve eğitmenin sınıfta bir uyaran (ayna) ile eğitim vermesi ya da almasının olumlu sonuçlar doğurabileceği kanısına varılmıştır. Çalışmanın nicel ve nitel bulgularına göre, çocuklara halk dansları figürleri öğretilirken, aynalı bir salonda eğitim vermenin olumlu etkilerinin olduğu sonucuna varılmıştır.
Article
Full-text available
z: Bu çalışmanın amacı, halk dansları figürleri öğretilirken, ortamın aynalı ya da aynasız olması durumunun, öğrenciler üzerine etkilerini incelemektir. 2016-2017 eğitim öğretim yılı güz ve bahar dönemlerini kapsayan bu çalışma, Ankara Büyükşehir Belediyesi Batıpark Çocuk Kulübüne kayıtlı 46'sı 4.sınıf ve 49'u 2.sınıf toplamda 94 öğrenci ile yürütülmüştür. Çalışmada video analizi, memnuniyet anketi ve görüşme yöntemi kullanılmış, verilerin analizi SPSS 18 paket programı ile yapılmıştır. Araştırmanın sonucunda elde edilen nicel bulgulara göre aynalı salonda eğitim gören öğrenciler ile aynasız salonda eğitim gören öğrenciler arasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı farklılıklar tespit edilmiştir. Video analizlerinde hareket uyumu, denge bozukluğu, şaşırmalar ve ritim duygusu bakımından aynalı ve aynasız salon gruplarının karşılaştırılmasında istatistiksel olarak anlamlı farklılıklar kaydedilmiştir (p<0.05). Nitel bulgulara göre, anında geri bildirim dönütünün olmasının, hızlı öğrenmeyi ve hatayı anında düzeltmeyi desteklediği vurgulanmıştır. Yine nitel bulgulara göre, özellikle görsel geri bildirimin söz konusu olduğu öğrenme ortamında öğrenci, dansçı ve eğitmenin sınıfta bir uyaran (ayna) ile eğitim vermesi ya da almasının olumlu sonuçlar doğurabileceği kanısına varılmıştır. Çalışmanın nicel ve nitel bulgularına göre, çocuklara halk dansları figürleri öğretilirken, aynalı bir salonda eğitim vermenin olumlu etkilerinin olduğu sonucuna varılmıştır. Abstract: The purpose of this study is to examine the effects of the folk dance figures being taught, the environment being mirrored or without mirrors, on the students. The study, covering the fall and spring semesters of the 2016-2017 academic year, was conducted with a total of 94 students in the 4th grade and 49 second grade classes registered in the Ankara Metropolitan Municipality Westpark Kindergarten. Video analysis, satisfaction questionnaire and interview method were used in the study. Analysis of the data was done with SPSS 18 package program. According to the quantitative findings obtained as a result of the research, statistically significant differences were found between students with mirrored saline and those with mirrorless saline. Statistically significant differences were recorded in the video analysis between comparisons of mirror and non-mirror room groups in terms of motion adaptation, balance disorientation, astonishment and rhythm sensation (p<0.05). Again according to qualitative findings, it has been concluded that, in the learning environment where visual feedback is the subject, it is possible for the student, the dancer and the instructor to give or receive training with a stimulus (mirror) in the classroom may have positive results. According to the quantitative and qualitative findings of this work, it is concluded that children are taught the figures of folk dances and that the positive effects of giving a mirrored saloon training result.
Article
Full-text available
Mirrors in sports halls have changed from an accessory view to an educational material view for the individual who teaches and learns physical activities (dance, boxing, pilates, zumba, gymnastics, etc.) in which visual learning is predominant. In this context, the aim of our research is to examine the studies carried out on 'mirrors in sports' in the field of sports, and to find out what kind of effect the educational environment is mirrored on teaching, learning and psycho-social processes of individuals. In the study, in which the thematic review design was used, a detailed literature review was made, and the research data were obtained by examining the resources from journals, books, theses and papers within the scope of Google Scholar, Pubmed and Yöktez. Document analysis method was used in the analysis of the data obtained. As a result, the achievable national and international studies on mirrors; mirrors provide an advantage in skill learning for the individual who provides education and training during learning, but in the process of learning, especially in professional athletes, if the use of mirrors continues, proprioception (deep sensation) and kinesthesia (joint position awareness) may cause regression and in individual sports branches where visual learning method is used and It has also been observed that the use of mirrors in physical activities involving group exercises may have negative psycho-social effects for beginner athletes or sedentary individuals. Özet Spor salonlarında ki aynalar genelde görsel öğrenmenin ağırlıklı olduğu fiziksel aktiviteler de (dans, boks, pilates, zumba, jimnastik vb.) öğreten ve öğrenen birey açısından aksesuar görünümünden eğitim materyali görünümüne geçmiştir. Bu bağlamda spor alanın da ‘spor da aynalarla’ ilgili yapılmış olan çalışmaları inceleyerek, eğitim ortamının aynalı olması durumunun öğretim, öğrenme ve bireylerin psiko-sosyal süreçlerine ne gibi bir etkisi olduğuna ulaşmak araştırmamızın amacını oluşturmaktadır. Tematik derleme deseninin kullanıldığı çalışmada, alanyazına dayalı detaylı kaynak taraması yapılmış, araştırma verileri Google Akademik, Pubmed ve Yöktez kapsamında, dergi, kitap, tez ve bildirilerden konuya ilişkin kaynaklar incelenerek elde edilmiştir. Ulaşılan verilerin analizinde döküman analizi yöntemi kullanılmıştır. Sonuç olarak aynalarla ilgili ulaşılabilinen ulusal ve uluslararası çalışmaların; aynaların öğrenme esnasında eğitim veren ve eğitim alan birey açısından beceri öğreniminde bir avantaj sağladığı ancak öğrenme sonrasındaki süreçte özellikle profesyonel sporcularda ayna kullanımının sürekliliği halinde propriyosepsiyon (derin duyum) ile kinesteziyi (eklem poziyon farkındalığı) gerilemeye neden olabileceği ve görerek öğrenme yönteminin kullanıldığı bireysel spor dallarında ve grup egzersizlerinin söz konusu olduğu fiziksel aktivitelerde ayna kullanımının başlangıç düzeyindeki sporcular ya da sedanter bireyler için psiko-sosyal açıdan olumsuz etkilere neden olabileceği de gözlemlenmiştir.
Article
Full-text available
This study assessed associations among measures of body-image and locus of control for 32 college students in dance classes and 26 students enrolled in a personal health class over a 16-wk. semester. Students took the Winstead and Cash 54-item short-form Body Self-relations Questionnaire and the Adult Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale. For locus of control there was a statistically significant main effect for time. Over-all, subjects scored as more internally oriented from pretest to posttest. However, there was a significant interaction; dancers scored more internally on locus of control at pretest than nondancers, but at posttest there were no significant differences between groups. For the Fitness Evaluation subscale of body-image there was a statistically significant interaction. Dancers had lower Fitness Evaluation scores at pretest than the nondancers but at posttest there were no significant differences between the groups. For Fitness Orientation there was a significant main effect for time. Subjects at pretest had higher Fitness Orientation scores than at posttest. For the Health Evaluation subscale of body-image there was a significant main effect for time. Over-all, subjects at pretest had higher Health Evaluation scores than at post-test.
Article
Full-text available
Notes that self-awareness theory has generated a considerable amount of research activity; however, the most widely used manipulation of self-focus—the mirror—has not been satisfactorily validated. Exp I was an attempt to do so. At the same time an attempt was made to validate the Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS), an instrument designed to measure chronic dispositions to be self-attentive. 79 female undergraduates responded to a sentence completion blank either in an empty room or while facing a mirror. Results indicate that the mirror does manipulate, and the private subscale of the SCS does measure, self-attention. Exp II, with 81 female undergraduates, was a replication of the 1st study, but using an audience instead of a mirror. Results indicate that audience presence also heightens self-attention. Implications for attentional analyses of social behavior are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Chapter
Die Initiierung und Umsetzung eines auf dem Modell des New Public Management (NPM) basierenden umfassenden Prozesses zur Reform der öffentlichen Verwaltungen wird in der verwaltungswissenschaftlichen Literatur meist auf ein Bündel von auslösenden Faktoren zurückgeführt. Allem voran wird auf die globale ökonomische Krise der 80er Jahre verwiesen, in deren Folge es im öffentlichen Sektor nahezu aller Industrienationen zu mehr oder weniger dramatischen Finanzproblemen gekommen ist, auf die mit einschneidenden Budgetrestriktionen reagiert wurde und wird (vgl. Reichard/Röber 2001, 373f.). Als weitere Einflussfaktoren werden die wachsende Politik- und Staatsverdrossenheit vieler Bürger/innen, die eine kritische Einstellung gegenüber der Leistungsfähigkeit des Wohlfahrtsstaates befördert hat, sowie die im Zuge solcher Entwicklungen immer kritischer gesehenen Ineffizienzen und Leistungsmängel des traditionellen ‚bürokratischen‘ Verwaltungssystems genannt.
Article
The premise that effects of maturational timing are mediated by social context is explored by comparing adolescent girls in dance and nondance schools. Because the dance student must maintain a relatively low body weight, being a late maturer (who is often leaner than an on-time maturer) is expected to be more advantageous to the dancer than to the student not required to meet a weight standard. Girls aged 14 to 18 were seen; 276 attended private schools and 69 attended national ballet company schools. AllSs were weighed and measured and asked questions about their secondary sexual development, weight-related concerns, eating concerns, adult sex-role expectancies, body image, emotional functioning, and family relationships. Menarcheal age was used to classify girls as early (before 11.5 years of age), on time (between 11.5 and 14 years), and late maturers (after 14 years). More dance than non-dance school students were late maturers (55% versus 29%). The dance students weighed less and were leaner, had higher eating scores, and had lower family relationship and impulse control scores than the comparison sample. Across groups, late maturing students weighed less, were leaner, and had lower diet and higher oral control scores than on-time maturers, with the differences more pronounced in the dance than nondance students. In addition, the on-time dancers had higher psychopathology, perfection, and bulimia scores and lower body image scores than the late maturing dancers. The findings are discussed in terms of a goodness of fit between the requirements of a social context and a person's physical and behavioral characteristics.
Article
This investigation was a representative survey, conducted in 1993, of the body images of 803 adult women in the United States. Included in the survey instrument were selected subscales from the standardized Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire, which had been used in a U.S. survey in 1985. The results indicated substantial levels of body dissatisfaction, possibly surpassing levels observed in the 1985 survey. Nearly one-half of the women reported globally negative evaluations of their appearance and a preoccupation with being or becoming overweight. Whereas age effects were minimal, significant race/ethnicity effects were found, with clearly more positive body images among African American than Anglo or Hispanic women. The social and clinical implications of the findings were discussed.
Article
Considers the conditions which cause the consciousness to focus on the self as an object. The theory that self-awareness has motivational properties deriving from social feedback is discussed and considered with relation to conformity, attitude-behavior discrepancies, and communication sets. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)