ArticlePDF Available

Enhancing Children's Attitudes About the Humane Treatment of Animals: Generalization to Human-Directed Empathy

Authors:

Abstract

We assessed the impact of a year-long, school-based humane education program on younger (first and second graders) and older (fourth and fifth graders) children's attitudes toward the treatment of animals. Generalization to human-directed empathy was also measured. Using a pretest-posttest design and ANCOVA, we found that the program enhanced the animal-related attitudes of children differentially, depending on grade level. For younger children, there was no significant difference between experimental (E) and control (C) group attitude means; however, qualitative analysis showed that greater enhancement of attitudes occurred for first grade E group children than for C group children at that grade level. No differences were present on the generalization measure of empathy. For older children, there was a significant difference between E and C group attitude means qualified by grade level—there was greater enhancement of humane attitudes for E group than for C group fourth graders but no difference for fifth graders. On the generalization measure of empathy, posttest means for the E group were significantly greater than means for the C group regardless of grade level. The results contribute to the growing literature on the relation between children and animals and serve to encourage and validate the efforts of humane educators to improve children's caring and kindness toward companion and noncompanion animals.
Title:
Humane education research: Evaluating efforts to encourage children's kindness and caring toward animals.
Authors:
Ascione, Frank R.. Utah State U, Dept of Psychology, Logan, UT, US Source:
Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, Vol 123(1), Feb, 1997. pp. 57-77.
HUMANE EDUCATION RESEARCH: EVALUATING EFFORTS TO ENCOURAGE
CHILDREN'S KINDNESS AND CARING TOWARD ANIMALS
ABSTRACT. Humane education includes instructional approaches to teaching children kindness toward animals.
Although efforts to teach children to be caring probably began within the first human social groups, formalized
programs aimed at fostering children's compassion and responsibility toward people and animals are a more recent
phenomenon, emerging in the United States not much earlier than a century ago. This article describes what has
been evaluated in humane education programs, why such programs are being scrutinized, how programs have been
evaluated (with a listing of the shortcomings of some evaluations and suggestions for improvement), and where
programs and their evaluations should be directed in the future. The focus is on preschool and elementary grade
programs. After a discussion of historical perspectives, recent approaches to implementing and evaluating humane
education are reviewed. Special attention is given to the issue of whether teaching children to be caring toward
animals has effects that are generalizable to human-directed empathy. Suggestions are offered for future research on
this relatively neglected topic in child psychology.
Research in the special sphere of humane education as well as in the more general sphere of moral education is a
neglected and thus backward branch of pedagogics. (Teutsch, 1982, p. 238)
Although efforts to teach children kindness and caring probably began within the first human social group, formalized
programs aimed at fostering children's compassion and responsibility toward people and animals are a more recent
phenomenon, emerging in the United States not much earlier than a century ago. If such programs are in their
infancy, their evaluation is still in a neonatal period. Yet all signs point to a healthy future development of the field of
humane education. In this article, I describe what has been evaluated in humane education programs, why such
programs are being scrutinized, how programs have been evaluated, with a listing of the shortcomings of some
evaluations and suggestions for improvement, and where programs and their evaluations should be directed in the
future. My focus is on preschool and elementary grade programs.
Definitions and Areas of Emphasis
Milburn (1989) defined humane education as "an attempt to develop altruism and a sense of compassion in a world
where all other pressures are in opposition to it" (p. 74). This definition includes an action component related to
fostering prosocial behavior and an affective component related to empathy and sympathy. Arkow (undated)
articulated components of humane education as defined by the American Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to
Animals. These components include animal needs and rights, empathy toward humans and animals, respect for all
forms of life, even those that some may find difficult to love or like, and the interdependence of humans and animals.
Listing other definitions of humane education would highlight their diversity, yet common threads in the fabric of these
educational efforts would be evident. Instilling, reinforcing, and enhancing young people's knowledge, attitudes, and
behavior toward the kind, compassionate, and responsible treatment of human and animal life are examples of the
shared components of most humane education programs.
Areas of Emphasis Within Humane Education
Eddy (1899, p. 227) stated that "children who are taught to be kind to animals and to each other make good citizens."
Humane education programs can be categorized according to their relative emphasis on interpersonal relations or
interspecies relations. The Child Development Project (Battistich, Watson, Solomon, Schaps, & Solomon, 1991) uses
a variety of educational interventions, including cooperative learning, opportunities to rehearse helping, and examples
of prosocial values, to foster positive interpersonal relations within classroom and home environments. Programs that
emphasize interspecies relations include Project WILD (Western Regional Environmental Education Council, 1986),
which restricts its coverage to nondomestic animals, some humane society programs that primarily address issues
related to pet care, and programs like People and Animals (Savesky & Malcarne, 1981) that are broader in scope and
focus on farm animal, wild animal, and pet animal issues.
Saunders (1922, p. 176) said that "children who are taught to love and protect dumb creatures will be kind to their
fellow men when they grow up." One of the explicit assumptions of humane education programs that focus on inter-
species relations is what Finch (1989) referred to as transference. This concept, which could also be labeled
generalization, suggests that teaching children to be attentive to animal needs and to treat animals with kindness,
compassion, and care will, in turn, affect the way children will treat each other. It is clear that programs such as
People and Animals and Operation Outreach-USA (Golden, 1992) aim to enhance interpersonal relations through
emphasis on interspecies relations, a goal subscribed to in the earliest attempts at humane education in the United
States (Arkow, 1992). Attempts to assess such generalization will be discussed later in this article.
Variations in Program Objectives
Although the focus of this article is on humane education programs developed and implemented by animal welfare
organizations, mention must also be made of a program developed by the Western Regional Environmental
Education Council. Project WILD has as its goal "to assist learners of any age in developing awareness, knowledge,
skills, and commitment to result in informed decisions, responsible behavior, and constructive actions concerning
wildlife and the environment upon which all life depends" (Western Regional Environmental Education Council, 1986,
p. vii). Project WILD is reminiscent of early programs designed to create objective appreciation for wildlife through
nature study (Guyer, 1905; Hodge, 1900) and to instill positive attitudes and reverence for life (Guillet, 1904; Hodge,
1899). Its objectives relate to children's understanding, for example, of the similarity of human and animal needs,
issues related to habitat, and knowledge of the effects of pollution, littering, and crowding on the quality of
nondomestic animal life. Although more circumscribed in focus than the National Association for Humane and
Environmental Education's (NAHEE) People and Animals and the Massachusetts Society for the Prevention of
Cruelty to Animals' Operation Outreach-USA, Project WILD shares with these programs their supplementary and
curriculum-blended features.
Animal Welfare Organization Programs
Operation Outreach-USA, a program of the American Humane Education Society (Golden, 1992), uses literacy skills
development within a cumulative, curriculum-blended approach to teach children respect for all living things. This
objective is designed to be achieved by introducing children to classic literary works, thereby coupling cognitive
(reading ability) and affective (prosocial attitudes and empathy) goals. NAHEE's People and Animals curriculum
guides have as their objectives the following (as described by Savesky & Malcarne, 1981):
1. assist children in developing compassion, a sense of justice, and a respect for all living creatures; 2) provide
the knowledge and understanding necessary for children to behave according to these principles; and 3) foster a
sense of responsibility on the part of children to affirm and act upon their personal beliefs. (p. iii)
The objectives incorporated in People and Animals make explicit the general cognitive, affective, and behavioral
outcomes that are desirable as a result of implementation of the program. These objectives (and those of other
humane education programs) are praiseworthy and would, no doubt, be endorsed by most human and animal welfare
organizations. There may be less consensus about methods of achieving these objectives, especially where
controversial issues, like trophy hunting, are involved. Yet it has often been humane educators who are in the
forefront of distinguishing education from indoctrination (see Finch & Soltow, 1991, p. 25).
The vehicles through which such objectives are approached are quite varied. They include direct physical contact
with farm and pet animals (George, 1992) and exposure to animals in zoological parks (Kaufmann, 1992), lessons
about animal behavior (Zawistowski, 1992), and exposure to classic literature including animal protagonists (Golden,
1992). These vehicles may be present in the home and family life of many children in our culture. However, the
greatest attention in this domain has been given to socialization outside the home (Eisenberg, 1992), especially
school-based humane education programs. These programs range from brief, one-time classroom visits by a humane
educator (Vockell & Hodal, 1980) to programs spanning an entire school year (Ascione, 1992).
Evaluating Humane Education: Process and Outcome
Although research on children's relations with animals, including reference to teacher-implemented humane
education, is mentioned in documents prepared before the beginning of this century (Angell, 1884), systematic
research in this area is a recent phenomenon. What was perhaps the first study of child-animal relations was not
notable for its emphasis on fostering positive relations. I refer here to Watson and Rayner's (1920) study of little
Albert, in which creating fear of an animal was the object. Before 1980, only a few scattered research reports focused
on fostering positive child-animal relations; included among these are Mary Cover Jones's (1924) work in the counter-
conditioning of fear of rabbits, and Bandura, Grusec, and Menlove's (1967) study using modeling to eliminate fear of
dogs in young children.
One study that concentrated directly on prosocial behavior between children and animals was conducted by Sprafkin,
Leibert, and Poulos (1975), in which they used the modeling of rescue behavior (Jeff rescuing one of Lassie's
puppies in the serialized TV program) in assessing first graders' willingness to help puppies in distress. In the classic
study, "Learning Concern for Others," by Yarrow, Scott, and Waxler (1973), opportunities to be kind to animals were
prominent among the pictures, dioramas, and behavioral incidents used to measure and train children in prosocial
behavior. Although they did not directly assess such behavior, even Hartshorne, May, and Mailer (1929) mentioned
kindness to animals as an index of children's dispositions to be of service.
The role of animals, especially pets or companion animals, in the psychological and social development of children
has been the focus of numerous calls for increased research attention (e.g., Levinson, 1983; Westerlund, 1982).
Heeding these calls are researchers whose efforts have been aimed at the study and enhancement of children's
attitudes toward the care of, treatment of, and respect for animals. Developmental changes in the quality of children's
humane attitudes have been assessed by Kellert and Westervelt (1983); Rheingold and Emery (1986) have explored
the roots of such attitudes in the second year of human life; Fogel, Melson, and Mistry (1986) have included values
and attitudes toward animals as underlying one of the many forms of nurturance that children may display; Kanner,
Feldman, Weinberger, and Ford (1987) included pet-related items in their measures of uplifts and hassles in early
adolescence; and Bryant (1985) has explored how relations with companion animals may relate to children's
empathic tendencies toward people. Other evidence of revitalized attention to this area is the number of child-related
articles included in the 1985 special issue of the journal, Marriage and Family Review, which focused on pets and the
family.
Pets or companion animals are an integral part of the environment of many children in the United States. Pet
ownership has been found to be significantly more common in families with school-aged children and adolescents
than in families without children (Albert & Bulcroft, 1988). In samples from California and Connecticut, children
reported pet ownership ranging from 52%, for kindergartners, to 75%, for fifth graders (Ascione, Latham, & Worthen,
1985). Higher rates of pet ownership in a sample of older children have been reported by Bryant (1990). The
numbers confirm the importance of attending to the human--companion animals relation (Kidd & Kidd, 1987; National
Institutes of Health, 1988).
The study of the role of child-animal relations potentially pervades most of the significant domains of developmental
analysis (Poresky, Hendrix, & Woroby, 1988) and, historically, is represented in the early developmental psychology
literature (e.g., Bucke, 1903; Hall & Browne, 1904; Lehman, 1927). In 1870, the Pedagogical Society of Berlin,
Germany (Dennis, 1972) conducted a study of thousands of 6-year-olds entering Berlin schools. A number of the
questions asked of these children, to gauge their understanding of various concepts, related to the children's
knowledge of various animals. The authors of the report noted that of 10,000 children tested, approximately 6,000
understood what a swan was, 4,000 knew what a zoo was, and only approximately 2,500 knew what a rabbit was.
G. Stanley Hall, in his 1883 report on a study of the contents of children's minds (Dennis, 1972), incorporated some
of the questions that had been used in the Berlin study. For example, he found that 10% of kindergarten children
tested knew what a beehive was, 50% could identify a frog, and 72% knew what a cow was. In addition, only 7% of
the children knew that "leathern things come from animals," 31% knew the origin of woolen things, and 80% were
able to identify the source of milk. Hall complained of the methodological difficulties in scoring responses as correct
when interviewing children. For example, in describing some of the children's responses, he said, "A worm may be
said to swim on the ground, butchers to kill only the bad animals .... but when hams are said to grow on trees or on
the ground... or wool as growing on hens... the deficiency is obvious" (p. 121).
There are also more recent attempts to assess children's knowledge and attitudes about animal-related issues.
These studies are primarily descriptive and in some cases examine the correlates of children's knowledge and
attitudes. Westervelt and Llewellyn (1985) focused on fifth- and sixth-grade children's beliefs and behaviors regarding
wildlife; Kidd and Kidd (1985, 1990) have examined, across significant age spans, children's beliefs and attitudes
about pets, focusing on cross-sectionally derived developmental changes in attitudes and their relation to parental
attitudes. Arreola (1989) surveyed first- through fifth-grade children on their knowledge about and attitudes toward a
variety of animal control concepts. An especially exciting development in this field is the emergence of cross-cultural
research, including evaluations of educational programs in Costa Rica (Zuman, 1993) and assessments of attitudes
toward animal welfare and environmental issues in East German and Russian youth (Szagun & Pavlov, 1993).
Research on School-Based Programs
Eisenberg (1988, p. 16) wrote: "Although there is very little research concerning the teaching of humane attitudes
towards animals to children, it appears as if there is a need to do so." In a number of studies, researchers have
attempted to assess educative efforts to enhance children's attitudes toward the humane treatment of animals. Some
of these studies focus on animals in general, and others emphasize the treatment of companion animals.
I begin with evaluations of Project WILD (Western Regional Environmental Education Council, 1986) as an example
of a school-based program that is restricted to the study of nondomestic animals. Charles (1988) has summarized
Project WILD's effects in terms of teacher evaluations, studies of changes in knowledge and attitudes, and anecdotal
evidence of children's behavioral change. Although this is a widely disseminated program and one to which
participating teachers devote considerable instructional time (Gilchrist, 1991), evidence of Project WILD's
effectiveness is not extensive. Fleming (1985) evaluated Project WILD in kindergarten through fifth-grade classrooms
in Lee County, Florida, using pretest and posttest instruments of moderate to good reliability. However, significant
positive changes attributable to the Project WILD curriculum were found only on the assessment of knowledge.
Statistically significant differences were not found on the attitudinal measures.
Race, Decker, and Taylor (1990) completed a similar study in Colorado with sixth and seventh graders. Although
psychometric information about the assessment instruments was not provided, gender differences were found. Boys
performed significantly better on the knowledge measure, girls performed better on the attitudinal measure. However,
there were no statistically significant differences between Project WILD and control classrooms. The authors attribute
this result to the pervasiveness of wildlife and environmental issues in the modern classroom, making it more difficult
to assess the effects of supplemental programs like Project WILD.
Vockell and Hodal (1980) focused on the impact of "typical 'one-shot' humane education programs" on a measure of
children's humaneness. The programs consisted of a single school visit by a humane educator coupled with the
provision of printed materials and posters. Third- through sixth-grade classes received either a visit (of unspecified
duration but presumably the length of one class period) and print material (referred to as the intensive treatment) or
print material alone (light treatment), or neither (control classrooms).
Although the children in this study were not pretested, they were posttested on two forms of the authors' Fireman
test, designed to assess the degree of children's favorable attitudes toward animal life (children are asked to select,
from a list of inanimate possessions and pets, which they would attempt to rescue from a burning home). Vockell and
Hodal (1980) reported that the two treatment classrooms' mean scores exceeded that of the control group for one
form of the Fireman test but not for the other. Neither treatment condition was judged superior. One problem not
addressed was why the control groups' performances on the two presumably equivalent tests differed so greatly (.99
for the Johnny form and 1.68 for the Billy form). Furthermore, without pretest information it was possible that the
various groups differed significantly in their humane attitudes prior to the implementation of the programs.
Nevertheless, the authors concluded that the intervention failed.
In a similarly designed study, Fitzgerald (1981) compared a variety of school-based interventions in 16 fifth- and sixth-
grade classrooms and included the important design feature of pretesting children. The four conditions were (a)
repeated treatment (RT), in which a master teacher presented four humane education lessons over a 2-month period
(once every 2 weeks); (b) intensive treatment (IT), in which the information contained in RT was covered in a single
class session; (c) light treatment (LT), which involved the provision of reading material without any direct instruction;
and (d) a control condition, in which no humane education efforts occurred.
All classrooms were pre- and posttested using the Fireman test. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) indicated that the
mean score for the IT condition was significantly more humane than mean scores for any of the three other groups
(RT, LT, and control). The three other groups also did not differ significantly from each other. Fitzgerald concluded that
a focused classroom presentation conducted by a master teacher could have a positive impact on children's humane
attitudes.
Cameron (1983) investigated two forms of humane education and their effects on animal-related attitudes using
seven eighth-grade classrooms. Two classrooms were given reading material and media presentations (PRINT); two
were provided both presentations along with lectures delivered by the instructor (LECTURE); and the remaining three
classrooms served as a control group. The instruction in the PRINT and LECTURE groups lasted for 3 school weeks
for a total of approximately 14 hr of in-class instruction. All children were pre- and posttested on an instrument tapping
animal-related attitudes. Using analysis of variance (ANOVA), Cameron found that the mean posttest attitude scores
of the PRINT and LECTURE groups were more positive than the control group's mean score. Although the PRINT
and LECTURE groups did not differ, and the LECTURE and control group did not differ, the PRINT group mean score
was higher than that of the control group.
This study demonstrated that a more intensive intervention can have a positive impact on children's attitudes toward
animals even with children older than those studied by either Vockell and Hodal (1980) or Fitzgerald (1981). One
limitation of this study, however, was that the attitude instrument developed by Cameron included only three items (of
the 25 total) clearly related to the care and treatment of companion animals (pets). Also, in studies of this nature, the
individual who provides the instruction should not conduct the assessments (to avoid potential bias). That was not the
case in this study (nor in a more recent study by Nutter, 1992).
Malcarne (1981) studied a small group (33 children) of third and fourth graders to assess the effects of drama and
role playing on children's empathy and prosocial behavior toward humans and animals. One third of the sample
received dramatization and role-playing experience related to human victims of distress, one third with animal victims,
and the remaining third were read The Girl of the Sacred Dogs, with discussion focused on the story content rather
than on role taking.
Each condition lasted for 1 hr. All the children were then posttested (no pretests were given) on three measures: (a)
story resolution in which either a human or animal victim of distress was the subject, and the child's response was
scored for helping and empathy; (b) the Fireman test, and (c) children's willingness to volunteer time at either a
children's hospital or an animal shelter (the number of hours mentioned was the dependent variable).
Malcarne found that children trained to role play animal distress scored higher on the Fireman test than did children in
the other two groups. Children in both treatment groups had higher scores on the animal version of the story-
resolution test than children in the control group did and expressed a greater willingness to volunteer at a children's
hospital than did members of the control group. Willingness to volunteer at an animal shelter was higher for the group
trained to role play animal distress than for the other two groups. Although this study provided important information
regarding the relation between empathy for humans and for animals, an issue also addressed by Bailey and
Doescher (1991) and Meison (1991), the absence of pretesting makes interpretation problematic.
In an evaluation of school-based humane education efforts sponsored by the Massachusetts Society for the
Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, Hein (1987) reported the effects of a single humane education presentation on
second graders and a series of three presentations on third, fourth, and fifth graders on children's attitudes toward the
treatment of animals. As described in the report, the presentations comprised less than 3 hr of total instruction. Hein
found that, compared with a no-intervention control group, children receiving humane education in the second, third,
and fourth grades demonstrated statistically significant increases in humane attitudes. No effects were obtained for
fifth graders.
Hein cautioned that changes obtained at the second grade level could be attributed to large increases in humane
attitudes for a small number of children, and that changes for third and fourth graders were restricted to attitude scale
items directly related to the specific instruction provided (i.e., changes may have resulted from teaching to the test).
One of Hein's recommendations was that substantially more intensive instruction is needed in humane education to
effect practically significant changes in attitudes.
Using the National Association for Humane and Environmental Education's (NAHEE) curriculum guides, Ascione,
Latham, and Worthen (1985) assessed the effects of a school-based intervention, implemented by teachers, on
children's humane attitudes. Seventy-seven teachers and their 1,800 pupils (kindergarten through sixth grade) were
randomly assigned to either an intervention condition (E group) or a no-intervention condition (C group). Children
were pre- and posttested on a Primary Attitude Scale (PAS; Ascione, 1988b) or Intermediate Attitude Scale (lAS;
Ascione, 1988a) depending on their grade level.[2] These instruments assessed attitudes toward companion and
noncompanion animals. Teachers implemented NAHEE's curriculum over the course of the school year and reported
that, on the average, 10 hr were spent on humane education material. Results showed that mean PAS posttest
scores were higher for E-group kindergarten and first graders than for C-group children.
Although the direction of the E-C group difference was in the expected direction for second graders, this difference
was not statistically significant. No gender differences were found in the PAS analysis. For the IAS, significant grade
and gender differences were found (mean humane attitude scores were higher for girls and higher for fourth, fifth, and
sixth graders than for third graders) and a treatment (E vs. C) effect that approached significance (p <.08).
Subsequent analyses showed that E-group third graders at one site (California) and fourth graders at the other site
(Connecticut) had higher scores for humane attitudes than C-group children at those grades and sites.
Results of this study demonstrated that children's attitudes toward the treatment of animals could be measured
reliably in a developmentally sensitive manner. The attitude scales developed were also sensitive to gender and
grade differences, and to a relatively weak educational intervention. Given that only 10 total hr of instruction were
devoted to this program over the entire school year, a question that needed to be addressed was whether a more
intensive intervention would produce more dramatic and consistent increases in children's animal-related attitudes (a
point stressed in Eisenberg, 1988).
Ascione (1992) assessed the impact of a year-long, school-based humane education program using NAHEE's
People and Animals curriculum with KIND News (a weekly-reader-type newsletter coveting animal-related issues) as
a supplement. Thirty-two classrooms were involved, with separate analyses for younger (first and second grade) and
older (fourth and fifth grade) pupils. Assignment of the volunteer teachers' classrooms to the experimental group (E),
which implemented the curriculum, or to the control group (C) was random. The effect of the program on children's
attitudes toward animals as well as generalization of effects to human-directed empathy (Bryant, 1982, 1987a,
1987b) were assessed. In contrast to teachers in the Ascione, Latham, and Worthen (1985) study, teachers in the
experimental group reported spending an average of nearly 40 hr of instructional time, over the school year, on the
humane education curriculum.
Using ANCOVAs, it was shown that the program enhanced the animal-related attitudes of children differentially,
depending on grade level. For younger children (first and second graders), there was no significant difference
between E and C group attitude means; however, qualitative analysis suggested that greater enhancement of
attitudes occurred for first-grade E-group children than for C-group children at that grade level. In contrast, no
difference between E and C groups was evident for second graders. No differences were present on the
generalization measure of empathy.
For older children (fourth and fifth graders), there was a significant difference between E- and C-group attitude means
qualified by grade level--there was greater enhancement of humane attitudes for E-group than for C-group fourth
graders (see Figure 1 for posttest data) but no significant difference for fifth graders. On the generalization measure
of empathy, posttest means for the E group were significantly greater than means for the C group regardless of grade
level.
Ascione (1992) identified three issues that should be addressed in follow-up research. First, one important question
that must be asked of humane education research is the duration of the changes that occur as a result of educational
interventions. This issue has both research and practical significance, because children may encounter humane
education sporadically rather than consistently across their school years. If program-enhanced attitudes are
maintained across at least a school year, more cost effective programs could be developed (e.g., implementing
concerted humane education at every other grade level), especially in areas where resources are limited.
A second issue is the relation between having a pet and children's attitudes toward animals. Although pet ownership,
per se, was not found to be a significant factor by Ascione (1992), future research should examine the quality of the
relation between child and pet. See examples of this approach with children by Poresky (1990), Poresky and Hendrix
(1990), and Poresky, Hendrix, Mosier, and Samuelson (1987).
Third, future researchers should directly address the relation between children's humane attitudes and their actual
treatment of companion and noncompanion animals. One area of focus should be on children who may be at risk for
mistreating animals. Much of the information available on this issue is anecdotal or based primarily on retrospective
reports by adults (Ascione, 1993). I am currently exploring the use of the attitude scales incorporated in this project
with children who have engaged in animal maltreatment, an issue of both historical (Jersild, 1954) and current
interest (National Advisory Mental Health Council, 1990, section on Conduct Disorder). This research would focus on
the potential screening use of these scales and their value as pre-post measures of intervention effectiveness.
Two of these issues were examined in a follow-up study by Ascione and Weber (1993). Because the effects of the
humane education curriculum were clearest for fourth graders in the Ascione (1992) study, this description of the
follow-up focuses on the results for that grade level.
Ascione and Weber were able to locate and re-test over 80% of the original sample 1 year after the earlier study was
completed. Fourth graders from both the experimental (E) and control (C) groups were assessed on the measures of
humane attitudes (lAS), human-directed empathy (B-I), and, for children with companion animals, the quality of their
relations with their pets (as reported by parents). Analyses of the follow-up data showed that E-group fourth graders
still had a more humane attitude mean score than C-group children (see Figure 1 for follow-up data). When the
quality of children's relations with pets was considered in the analyses (as a covariate), intervention effects were
shown to have generalized to human-directed empathy. This is the first demonstration of the relatively long-term
maintenance of effects of a humane education program and, although the effects were clearest for only one grade
level and the study warrants replication, the results are encouraging.
Humane Education Evaluation: An Agenda for Future Research
The experience of French and English schools during many years has shown that children taught kindness to animals
only, become not only more kind to animals, but also more kind to one another. (From a paper by George T. Angell,
President of the Massachusetts Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals and Director of the American Social
Science Association, presented before the National Association of Superintendents of Public Schools at their annual
meeting, Washington, DC, February 14, 1884.)
It has been difficult to locate the research on which George Angell based his remarks, but it is encouraging that we
are beginning to assemble data that may eventually validate Angell's observation. We need to acknowledge,
however, that this validation may be more difficult to accomplish than we initially presumed. This is a condition not
unique to humane education--one has only to examine current dialogue about the questionable effectiveness of other
school-based programs like D.A.R.E. for drug education, Channel ONE for education on current events, and
programs dealing with sexuality. Evaluation in real world settings is a complex and challenging endeavor.
In closing, I would like to focus on some suggestions for future research on the evaluation of humane education
programs implemented in school settings. These remarks address the way we assess humane education outcomes,
characteristics of the children who are involved in this research, how humane education programs are implemented,
the need to move beyond assessing knowledge and attitudes alone, and the relation of humane education outcomes
to broader societal issues that affect our children.
How do we measure humane education effects? It is clear that there is a need for continued research on developing
reliable and valid tests of humane education program outcomes, especially for use with preschool and early
elementary grade children. In fact, what is sorely needed is a form of assessment common to a variety of humane
education programs (e.g., People and Animals and Operation Outreach-USA) that could be used across a wide age
span. The Intermediate Attitude Scale used in Ascione (1992) and Ascione and Weber (1993) holds some promise in
this regard because it has been shown to be reliable with older children (second to third graders and older) and with
adults (Weber & Ascione, 1992). However, continued attention to this issue by other researchers is needed.
When we do assess humane education programs, we must be sensitive to the potential for examiner bias to affect
our results. It is surprising that there continue to be studies that use testers who are not naive as to the tested
children's classroom experience with humane education. Deliberate attempts to influence outcomes are not being
suggested; rather, we know that subtle, unconscious effects may occur and can distort our findings. These effects can
easily be controlled for and would substantially improve the quality of outcome data.
Whom do we assess? One question we should be addressing is whether the children we have studied in humane
education programs represent the economic, ethnic, and cultural diversity that exists in our country. Have we focused
our attention on children from middle and upper income families and neglected to examine humane education effects
with children from impoverished environments? How do the messages conveyed in humane education programs
affect children who grow up in frightening and violent environments? Broadening our research samples will help us
address these issues and, in turn, feed back to the development of humane education program content as well as
methods of implementation.
What should be considered in implementing humane education programs? Humane education programs that are to
be formally evaluated should obviously be comparable to typical programs implemented in schools. However, when
evaluations are to be conducted, we should take care to document the quantity and quality of the intervention. For
example, in one study (Ascione, 1992), 1 asked teachers to report on the amount of time spent on humane
education, but I did not include assessment of the quality of instruction or its consistency with curriculum guidelines. A
related issue is assessing the effects of variations in teacher preparation for humane education, including
comparisons of preservice and in-service workshops.
Better documentation also needs to occur in the so-called control classrooms or comparison groups we use in such
research. As humane and environmental issues become more "mainstreamed," determining the effects of
supplemental programs may become more challenging.
One additional consideration related to program implementation is the duration of programs. It is noteworthy that in
many other curriculum areas, instruction is cumulative across grades. Yet most humane education evaluations have
examined programs lasting no longer than one school year. If health, sexuality, drug, and citizenship education
require years of instruction to be effective, is this not also the case with humane education? We cannot use an
inoculation model and assume that if humane education is "covered" in the third grade, the effects will last till
adulthood. We need longitudinal studies that follow children exposed to multiple years of humane education. These
studies will allow for a more realistic assessment of intervention effectiveness.
ABCs--Two Bottom Lines
Despite one hundred years of humane education efforts, many people still do not take proper care of their pets, and
shelters see the results. (Jasper, J. M., & Nelkin, D., 1992, p. 67)
We know that there are complex interrelations between attitudes, behaviors, and cognitions (Olson & Zanna, 1993),
yet most humane education evaluations have focused on affective and/or knowledge change, with little attention to
action. This is understandable, as attitudes and knowledge are easier and less costly to measure. We must be willing
to expend the energy and resources to determine whether children who experience humane education in fact behave
more kindly toward animals, act more responsibly in caring for pets, engage in cruelty less frequently, take action on
behalf of threatened species, support habitat preservation for wildlife, and so forth.
Saunders (1922, p. 35) stated, "If he is bad enough to ill-treat his dog, he will ill-treat his wife and children." To a
certain degree, we have more information about the relation between violence toward animals and family violence
(Ascione, 1993) than we do about the beneficial effects of humane education. As noted by Finch (1989), humane
education should also address general issues of moral education that are related to social problems like child
maltreatment and delinquency. The pioneers of humane education efforts acknowledged this responsibility (Angell,
1884); it is a responsibility that requires attention more urgently than ever.
If pedagogy is ever to become adequate to the needs of the soul, the time will come when animals will play a far
larger educational role than has yet been conceived, that they will be curriculized, will acquire a new and higher
humanistic or culture value in the future comparable with their utility in the past. (Hall, 1904, p. 228)
To the young child, there is no gap between his soul and that of animals. (Hall, 1904, p. 221)
Sections of this article appeared in Ascione (1992) and are reproduced with the permission of the editor of
Anthrozoos. Teresa Thompson and Claudia Weber assisted with bibliographic work and Karen Ranson prepared the
manuscript. Their professional efforts are sincerely appreciated.
Address correspondence to Frank R. Ascione, Department of Psychology, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322-
2810. E-mail: FRANKA@FS1.ED.USU.EDU.
1 Evaluation of Operation Outreach-USA has not been included, because their program has not been in existence
long enough to be the subject of evaluation studies. Evaluation of the program's effects on reading ability and
attitudes was planned for 1993-1994.
2 Psychometric information on these scales can be found in Ascione (1992).
GRAPHS: FIGURE 1. Pretest, posttest, and follow up mean attitude scores for the eight classrooms (four
experimental and four control) participating in an evaluation of humane education. Higher scores on the attitude
measure (IAS) reflect more humane attitudes. Pretest and posttest data are derived from Ascione (1992) and follow-
up data from Ascione and Weber (1993). IAS/PRE and Poresky/Past covariates are described in Ascione and Weber
(1993).
REFERENCES
Albert, A., & Bulcroft, K. (1988). Pets, families, and the life course. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 50, 543-552.
Angell, G. T (1884). The new order of mercy; or, crime and its prevention. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Department of Superintendence of the National Educational Association, Washington, DC.
Arkow, P. (undated). Humane education. In Humane education: Operational guide for animal care and control
agencies. Denver: American Humane Association.
Arkow, P. (1992). Humane education: An historical perspective. In M. E. Kaufmann (Ed.), Progress in humane
education (pp. 2-3). Englewood, CO: The American Humane Association.
Arreola, P.M. (1989). A survey of children's existing knowledge and beliefs regarding animal control concepts.
Unpublished master's thesis, Texas A & M University, College Station.
Ascione, F. R. (1988a). Intermediate Attitude Scale. Assessment of third through sixth graders' attitudes toward the
treatment of animals. Logan, UT: Wasatch Institute for Research Evaluation.
Ascione, F. R. (1988b). Primary Attitude Scale. Assessment of kindergarten through second graders' attitudes toward
the treatment of animal. Logan, UT: Wasatch Institute for Research and Evaluation.
Ascione, F. R. (1992). Enhancing children's attitudes about the humane treatment of animals: Generalization to
human-directed empathy. Anthrozoos, 5, 176-191.
Ascione, F. R. (1993). Children who are cruel to animals: A review of research and implications for developmental
psychopathology. Anthrozoos, 6, 226-247.
Ascione, F. R., Latham, G. I., & Warthen, B. R. (1985). Final report, Year 2: An experimental study. Evaluation of the
Humane Education Curriculum Guides. Unpublished report to the National Association for the Advancement of
Humane Education, East Haddam, CT.
Ascione, F. R., & Weber, C. V. (1993, March). Children's attitudes about the humane treatment of animals and
empathy: One-year follow up of a school-based intervention. Presentation at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for
Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Bailey, C., & Doeschef, S. M. (1991, April). The effects of pet ownership and home intervention on preschool
children's affective role taking, cognition, and popularity. Paper presented as part of the symposium, "Pets and
children's socioemotional development: Relations to cognitive role-taking, attachment, adjustment, and empathy" at
the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.
Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. (1967). Vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 5, 16-23.
Battistich, V., Watson, M., Solomon, D., Schaps, E., & Solomon, J. (1991). The Child Development Project: A
comprehensive program for the development of prosocial character. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.),
Handbook of moral behavior and development (Vol. 3: Application). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bryant, B. K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents. Child Development, 53, 413-425.
Bryant, B. K. (1985). The neighborhood walk: Sources of support in middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 50, Serial No. 210.
Bryant, B. K. (1987a). Critique of comparable questionnaire methods in use to assess empathy in children and adults.
In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 361-373). New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Bryant, B. K. (1987b). Mental health, temperament, family, and friends: Perspectives on children's empathy and social
perspective taking. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 245-270). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Bryant, B. K. (1990). The richness of the child--pet relationship: A consideration of both benefits and costs of pets to
children. Anthrozoos, 3, 253-261.
Bucke, W. F. (1903). Cyno-psychoses. Children's thoughts, reactions, and feelings toward pet dogs. The Journal of
Genetic Psychology, 10, 459-513.
Cameron, R. K. K. (1983). The effects of two instructional treatments on eighth-grade students' attitudes toward
animal life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Purdue University.
Charles, C. (1988). Summary of research findings on Project WILD. Transactions of the Fifty-third North American
Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference (pp. 531-541). Washington DC: Wildlife Management Institute.
Dennis, W. (Ed.). (1972). Historical readings in developmental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Eddy, S. J. (1899). Friends and helpers. Boston: Ginn.
Eisenberg, N. (1988). Empathy and sympathy: A brief review of the concepts and empirical literature. Anthrozoos, 2,
15-17.
Eisenberg, N. (1992). The caring child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Finch, P. (1989). Learning from the past. In D. Paterson & M. Palmer (Eds.), The status of animals: Ethics, education
and welfare (pp. 64-72). Oxon, England: C.A.B International.
Finch, P. A., & Soltow, W. A. (1991). KIND Workshop Leader's Guide. East Haddam, CT: National Association for
Humane and Environmental Education.
Fitzgerald, T. A., Jr. (1981). Evaluating humane education: The Jefferson County study. Humane Education, 5, 21-22.
Fleming, M. L. (1985, August). Evaluation report of the 1985 implementation of Project WILD in the elementary
schools of Lee County, Florida. Boulder, CO: Western Regional Environmental Education Council.
Fogel, A., Meison, G. F., & Mistry, J. (1986). Conceptualizing the determinants of nurturance: A reassessment of sex
differences. In A. Fogel & G. F. Meison (Eds.), Origins of nurturance: Developmental, biological and cultural
perspectives on caregiving (pp. 53-67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
George, H. (1992). A new look at an old relationship. In M. E. Kaufmann (Ed.), Progress in humane education (p. 2).
Englewood, CO: The American Humane Association.
Gilchrist, S.C. (1991). Effects of Project Wild on .fourth grade students in Wisconsin. Presentation at the Conference
of the North American Association for Environmental Education, St. Paul, MN.
Golden, J. (1992). Humane education in elementary school. In M. E. Kaufmann (Ed.), Progress in humane education
(pp. 10-11). Englewood, CO: The American Humane Association.
Gulliet, C. (1904). A glimpse at a nature school. Pedagogical Seminar, 11, 91-98.
Guyer, M, F. (1905). The question of method in nature study. Pedagogical Seminary, 12, 86-92.
Hall, G. S. (1883). The contents of children's minds. Princeton Review, 249-272.
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence (Vol. 2). New York: D. Appleton & Co.
Hall, G. S., & Browne, C. E. (1904). The cat and the child. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 11, 3-29.
Hartshorne, H., May, M. A., & Mailer, J. B. (1929). Studies in the nature of character: Studies in service and self-
control (Vol. 2). New York: Macmillan.
Hein, G. E. (1987). Massachusetts Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Outreach Program Evaluation.
Final report. Boston, MA: SPCA.
Hodge, C. E (1899). Foundations of nature study. Pedagogical Seminary, 6, 536-553.
Hodge, C. E (1900). Foundations of nature study. (III). Pedagogical Seminary, 7, 208-228.
Jasper, J. M., & Nelkin, D. (1992). The animal rights crusade. New York: The Free Press.
Jersild, A. T. (1954). Emotional development. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (2nd ed., pp. 833-
917). New York: Wiley.
Jones, M. C. (1924). A laboratory study of fear: The case of Peter. Pedagogical Seminary, 31,308-315.
Kanner, A.D., Feldman, S.S., Weinberger, D. A., & Ford, M. E. (1987). Uplifts, hassles, and adaptational outcomes in
early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 371-394.
Kaufmann, M. E. (1992). How did I grow to care? In M. E. Kaufmann (Ed.), Progress in humane education.
Englewood, CO: The American Humane Association.
Kellert, S. R., & Westervelt, M. O. (1983). Children's attitudes. knowledge and behaviors toward animals (DHHS
Publication No. 024-010-00641-2). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Kidd, A. H., & Kidd, R. M. (1985). Children's attitudes toward their pets. Psychological Reports, 57, 15-31.
Kidd, A. H., & Kidd, R. M. (1987). Seeking a theory of the human/companion animal bond. Anthrozoos, I, 140-145.
Kidd, A. H., & Kidd, R. M. (1990). Factors in children's attitudes toward pets. Psychological Reports, 66, 775-786.
Lehman, H. C. (1927). The child's attitude toward the dog versus the cat. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 34, 62-
72.
Levinson, B. M. (1983). The future of research into relationships between people and their companion animals. In A.
H. Katcher & A.M. Beck (Eds.), New perspectives on our lives with companion animals (pp. 536-550). Philadelphia:
University of Pennsylvania Press.
Malcarne, V. (1981). The effects of roleplay and maximization of perceived similarity on children's empathy with other
children and animals. Unpublished honors thesis, Stanford University.
Melson, G. F. (1991, April). The relationship between children's attachment to pets and socioemotional development.
Paper presented as part of the symposium, "Pets and children's socioemotional development: Relations to cognitive
role-taking, attachment, adjustment, and empathy" at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Seattle, WA.
Milburn, C. (1989). Introducing animal welfare into the education system. The status of animals: Ethics, education
and welfare (pp. 73-78). Oxon, England: C.A.B. International.
National Advisory Mental Health Council. (1990). National plan for research on child and adolescent mental disorders.
A report requested by the U.S. Congress, DHHS Publication No. (ADM) 90-1683. Rockville, MD: National Institute of
Mental Health.
National Institutes of Health. (1988). Health benefits of pets. Summary of Working Group, NIH Technology
Assessment Workshop (Publication 1988-216-107). Bethesda, MD: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Nutter, C. (1992). Humane education: The effects of an outreach program for fifth graders. Unpublished master's
thesis, DePaul University.
Olson, J. M., & Zanna, M.P. (1993). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 117-154.
The Pedagogical Society of Berlin. (1870). The contents of children's minds on entering school at the age of six
years. Yearbook of the City of Berlin. (Published in first annual report of the U.S. Commissioner of Education, 1900-
1901, Volume 1, pp. 709-729.) (cited in Dennis, 1972).
Poresky, R. H. (1990). The young children's empathy measure: Reliability, validity and effects of companion animal
bonding. Psychological Reports, 66, 931-936.
Poresky, R. H., & Hendrix, C. (1990). Differential effects of pet presence and pet bonding on young children.
Psychological Reports, 67, 51-54.
Poresky, R. H., Hendrix, C., Mosier, J. E., & Samuelson, M. L. (1987). The Companion Animal Bonding Scale: Internal
reliability and construct validity. Psychological Reports, 67, 743-746.
Poresky, R. H., Hendrix, C., & Woroby, M. (1988). Developmental benefits of pets for young children. Final report to
the Delta Society. Renton, WA: The Delta Society.
Race, T. M., Decker, E., & Taylor, J. (1990). A statewide evaluation of Project WILD's effect on student knowledge and
attitude toward wildlife. Transactions of the Fifty-fifth North American Wildlife and Natural Resources Conference (pp.
101-107). Washington, DC: Wildlife Management Institute.
Rheingold, H. L., & Emery, G. N. (1986). The nurturant acts of very young children. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M.
Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior (pp. 75-96). San Diego: Academic Press.
Saunders, M. (1922). Beautiful Joe. New York: A. L. Burt.
Savesky, K., & Malcarne, V. (1981). (Eds.). People and animals: A humane education curriculum guide. East
Haddam, CT: National Association for the Advancement of Humane Education.
Sprafkin, J. M., Liebert, R. M., & Poulos, R. W. (1975). Effects of a prosocial televised example on children's helping.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 119-126.
Szagun, G., & Pavlov, V. I. (1993, March). German and Russian adolescents' environmental awareness. Presentation
at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
Teutsch, G. M. (1982). Humane education in perspective with general education and the full development of the
humane personality. In M. S. R. Westerlund (Ed.), Humane education and the realms of humaneness (pp. 231-239).
Washington, DC: University Press of America.
Vockell, E., & Hodal, F. (1980). Developing humane attitudes: What does research tell us? Humane Education, 4, 19-
21.
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology; 8, 1-14.
Weber, C. V., & Ascione, E R. (1992, July 23). Humane attitudes and human empathy: Relations in adulthood.
Keynote address at the Sixth International Conference on Human Animal Interactions, Montreal, Canada.
Westerlund, S. R. (Ed.). (1982). Humane education and realms of humaneness. Washington, DC: University Press of
America.
Western Regional Environmental Education Council. (1986). Project WILD: Elementary activity guide (rev. ed.).
Boulder, CO: Author.
Westervelt, M. O., & Llewellyn, L. G. (1985). Youth and wildlife: The beliefs and behaviors of fifth and sixth grade
students regarding nondomestic animals (Report #498-443-42425). Washington, DC: U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service.
Yarrow, M. R., Scott, P. N., & Waxler, C. Z. (1973). Learning concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 8, 240-
260.
Zawistowski, S. (1992). Animal behavior: A building block of humane education. In M. E. Kaufmann (Ed.), Progress in
humane education (pp. 4-5). Englewood, CO: The American Humane Association.
Zuman, J.P. (1993). Promoting respect for all forms of life: A model primary education program in Costa Rica. Annual
Report of the Worm Society for the Protection of Animals Humane Education Project. Arlington, MA: W.S.P.A.
Received April 11, 1996
~~~~~~~~
By FRANK R. ASCIONE, Department of Psychology, Utah State University
Copyright of Genetic, Social & General Psychology Monographs is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express
written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
Back
... A growing body of research suggests that interacting with animals is associated with the development of empathy in children (Poresky, 1990;Ascione, 1992) and empathy has long been shown to predict prosocial behavior (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1990;Eisenberg, Eggum, & Di Giunta, 2010;Eisenberg & Miller, 1987; for review see Castano, 2012;Castano & Kidd, 2018). It is plausible that a relationship also exists between human-animal interaction and prosocial behavior. ...
... While these studies are correlational in nature, they are consistent with quasi-experimental work that has tackled similar processes, such as research using interventions designed to promote the humane treatment of animals. Ascione (1992) found that elementary school children who participated in a humane education program showed higher levels of empathy relative to children who did not. School classrooms were randomly assigned to a control condition or to an experimental condition in which the class participated in a year-long humane education program based on NAHEE (National Association for Humane and Environmental Education) curriculum guidelines. ...
Article
Full-text available
We investigated the relationship between empathy, prosocial behavior, and frequency of humane interactions with animals among 3 rd grade children (n = 158). We measured the frequency of humane interactions with animals via the Children’s Treatment of Animals Questionnaire ( Thompson & Gullone, 2003 ), empathy via the Bryant Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents ( Bryant, 1982 ), and prosocial behavior via teachers’ evaluations of children’s helpfulness towards others in the classroom. Results showed that children who had more frequent interactions with animals that involved a strong element of companionship reported greater empathy, and that this, in turn, related positively to prosocial behavior (measured by teacher’s report). A mediational model in which empathy accounts for the effect of positive interactions with animals on prosocial behavior provides consistent, if not conclusive, support for the relationship between interacting with animals and socio-emotional development.
... • Yngre børns socialisering med jaevnaldrende (Hart et al., 1987;Nielsen & Delude, 1989). • Udvikling af omsorgsfuld adfaerd og human indstilling hos børn, som måske vokser op til at blive mere omsorgsfulde voksne (Melson, 1990;Ascione, 1992). • Plejebørns oplevelse af stabilitet (Hutton, 1985). ...
Article
Leo K. Bustad (1920-1998) spillede en central rolle for etableringaf menneske-dyr-interaktion som område for systematiskuddannelse, forskning og praksis. Som professor i veterinærmedicinog dekan ved Washington State University grundlagdehan det første universitetsbaserede servicehundeprogram, oghan var medstifter og leder af organisationen Delta Society (nuPet Partners), der blandt andet står for terapihunde-certificeringi USA, og hvis definitioner af dyreassisteret terapi og dyreassisteretaktivitet i dag er konsensus. I sin afslutningstale til eninternational konference i 1989 opridsede Bustad de væsentligstestrømninger i feltet de foregående år og gav sit bud på visionerog næste skridt. Som redaktion af temanummeret findervi, at talen giver en god introduktion til feltet, og den gengivesher med tilladelse. Mai Andreassen har oversat til dansk.
... Experiences that provoke emotion can be used to target learners' affect (Kamradt & Kamradt, 1999;Simonson, 1979). Likewise, when learning is situated in authentic situations, such as those recommended for CBL, learners can be motivated to develop empathy regarding the target attitude (Ascione, 1992;McGill & Beaty, 2001;Turner, 1992). Furthermore, instructional strategies targeting learners' behaviors seek to have learners perform behaviors in line with the targeted attitude (Kamradt & Kamradt, 1999) or practice using the attitude such as through roleplay (Bednar & Levie, 1993;Smith & Ragan, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Using a mixed methods design, this study examines graduate students’ perceptions of attitudinal learning regarding the use of case-based learning (CBL) methods in an online instructional design course. One hundred and thirty-three students completed a survey taken from the Attitudinal Learning Inventory (ALI) in weeks three and seven of the eight-week course. Using Latent Profile Analysis (LPA), three classes of student types were identified: Skeptics who perceived low and declining attitudinal learning throughout the course, Adopters who perceived high and rising, and Advocates, who perceived very high and rising attitudes. Student reflection assignments on developing instructional design expertise using CBL, and a post-course questionnaire sent out five months after the course concluded were analyzed to gain further insights into student experiences and perspectives of both the course and CBL. Results indicated the majority of students perceived positive attitudinal learning throughout the course and had positive views of CBL as an instructional method. Students who struggled had less prior ID experience and seemed to exhibit a more negative perspective towards the course and CBL despite reporting less external challenges that impacted their learning in the course than the positive groups. Implications highlight the importance of facilitating SRL development and supplementing more novice CBL and ID students with additional support.
... Empathy towards animals seems to have originated in a similar way as that shown towards other humans [150,151]. It has been proposed that animal-directed empathy may generalize to human-directed empathy [152] and that the amount of empathy towards animals may indicate a more general capacity for empathy and related prosocial behavior [153]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The human–animal relationship is ancient, complex and multifaceted. It may have either positive effects on humans and animals or poor or even negative and detrimental effects on animals or both humans and animals. A large body of literature has investigated the beneficial effects of this relationship in which both human and animals appear to gain physical and psychological benefits from living together in a reciprocated interaction. However, analyzing the literature with a different perspective it clearly emerges that not rarely are human–animal relationships characterized by different forms and levels of discomfort and suffering for animals and, in some cases, also for people. The negative physical and psychological consequences on animals’ well-being may be very nuanced and concealed, but there are situations in which the negative consequences are clear and striking, as in the case of animal violence, abuse or neglect. Empathy, attachment and anthropomorphism are human psychological mechanisms that are considered relevant for positive and healthy relationships with animals, but when dysfunctional or pathological determine physical or psychological suffering, or both, in animals as occurs in animal hoarding. The current work reviews some of the literature on the multifaceted nature of the human–animal relationship; describes the key role of empathy, attachment and anthropomorphism in human–animal relationships; seeks to depict how these psychological processes are distorted and dysfunctional in animal hoarding, with highly detrimental effects on both animal and human well-being.
... Any action reserving some part of the site to this effect could be a positive step towards in changing the public's view on tarāpuka, and distinguishing them from the general "gulls are a nuisance" perspective. Recognising tarāpuka as individuals with features distinct from the generic urban background gull, and allowing close but not disturbing encounters might foster a connection leading to greater empathy and understanding for the difficulties they face in maintaining and growing the population (Ascione, 1992;Deering, 2017;Jerolmack, 2008). A purposefully sanctioned habitat could bring us the human well-being benefits of urban renaturing, while bringing the ecological benefit of safer breeding grounds and increased knowledge around tarāpuka. ...
Article
In 2019, a colony of rare, protected gulls established a nesting ground in the ruins of a building demolished after the 2011 earthquake in Christchurch, New Zealand (NZ). Authorities intended to move the colony from their ad hoc centre-city location to a more suitable habitat to be constructed in the wetlands in the city’s eastern suburbs. This new habitat was to resemble the gulls’ natural habitat in NZ’s vast braided river plains. But after numerous delays, it seemed like the gulls were preparing for a third breeding season at the site. The conflict between conserving a protective habitat for the rare gulls to breed and progressing city renewal has called into question who belongs in a city and how science should be considered in urban and wildlife policy. Readers will be able to discuss and debate issues with balancing ecological and urban development interests, conflict that leads to contested spaces, and how these influence urban and biodiversity policy decisions.
... Various studies support this hypothesis. Weatherill (62) and Ascione (63) documented that personal interactions with animals provide the best opportunity for bonding and empathic response. Morris et al. (64) showed that people who have never kept any animals recognize less capacity of animals to experience emotions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research and development of Precision Livestock Farming (PLF) is booming, partly due to hopes and claims regarding the benefits of PLF for animal welfare. These claims remain largely unproven, however, as only few PLF technologies focusing on animal welfare have been commercialized and adopted in practice. The prevailing enthusiasm and optimism about PLF innovations may be clouding the perception of possible threats that PLF may pose to farm animal welfare. Without claiming to be exhaustive, this paper lists 12 potential threats grouped into four categories: direct harm, indirect harm via the end-user, via changes to housing and management, and via ethical stagnation or degradation. PLF can directly harm the animals because of (1) technical failures, (2) harmful effects of exposure, adaptation or wearing of hardware components, (3) inaccurate predictions and decisions due to poor external validation, and (4) lack of uptake of the most meaningful indicators for animal welfare. PLF may create indirect effects on animal welfare if the farmer or stockperson (5) becomes under- or over-reliant on PLF technology, (6) spends less (quality) time with the animals, and (7) loses animal-oriented husbandry skills. PLF may also compromise the interests of the animals by creating transformations in animal farming so that the housing and management are (8) adapted to optimize PLF performance or (9) become more industrialized. Finally, PLF may affect the moral status of farm animals in society by leading to (10) increased speciesism, (11) further animal instrumentalization, and (12) increased animal consumption and harm. For the direct threats, possibilities for prevention and remedies are suggested. As the direction and magnitude of the more indirect threats are harder to predict or prevent, they are more difficult to address. In order to maximize the potential of PLF for improving animal welfare, the potential threats as well as the opportunities should be acknowledged, monitored and addressed.
Article
Various studies have suggested a range of different factors associated with children’s attitudes toward animals; however, in Chile, it is an area that has not been studied. The current study administered the Intermediate Attitude Scale ( IAS , which measures attitudes toward the humane treatment of animals) to 286 children in third and fourth grade and analyzed its association with the following variables: type of school, grade, gender, current pet ownership, the recognition accuracy of dog facial expressions, and human-directed empathy. The association between the IAS and potential factors was measured through linear mixed models. Children with higher human-directed empathy scores, fourth-grade children, and children who were more accurate in recognizing the happiness emotion on the dog’s face were associated with more positive attitudes toward the humane treatment of animals. These findings contribute to the growing literature regarding the importance of positive attitudes toward animals and their connection to human-directed empathy.
Thesis
p>This study investigates the psychology of human-animal relationships and the extent to which our attitude to animals has changed over the latter part of the twentieth century. Empirical investigation and ethical enquiry are used to focus on both practical and moral issues. The question of animal status has been debated since the time of the ancient Greek philosophers, and Part I looks at changing attitudes in Western society. It focuses on three key debates which have traditionally defined the status of animals compared with that of mankind, whether they have instinct rather than rationality, whether an anthropomorphic understanding of them is unscientific, and whether some can be said to have a sufficiently developed form of communication to merit the term 'language'. It is the arguments underlying these debates that have traditionally defined animal status. Of all animals, pets have the highest status, often being treated a family members. However claims made by pet owners about their relationships and shared systems of communication have commonly been dismissed as unscientific, anecdotal and anthropomorphic. By the use of video recording, Part II investigates whether these criticisms are justified and examines the extent to which attributes of cognitive and affective states to animals are influenced by personal experience. Part III addresses the question of how far the experience of living with companion animals affects people's attitudes to non-pet species, and whether there are generational differences in the perception of animals. By means of a three-generational study, it examines changing attitudes to animals over a period of seventy years, and the practical issues of how different species would be treated by each of the generations in particular situations. Part IV investigates the extent to which two institutions, both largely concerned with moral thinking, are including the ethical treatment of animals in their courses: Christian theological colleges and university departments of philosophy.</p
Article
This is the second part of a two-part article presenting the theoretical and empirical case for nonhuman animal (hereafter, ‘animal’) spirituality. Part 1 discussed the relevance of evolutionary theory and species differences for understanding animals’ capacity to have spiritual experience, conceptual issues related to defining animal spirituality, and methodological considerations regarding analogical reasoning and animal-centered anthropomorphism as heuristic strategies in the study of animal spirituality. Issues related to the question of animal consciousness and the use of evolutionary panentheism as a philosophical/theological frame for theorizing about animal spirituality were discussed. Part 2 examines six biopsychosocial capabilities of animals that are building blocks of human spirituality—cognition, imagination, emotion, moral sense, personality, and value-life (Maslow’s phrase)—and proposes an ontic pluralism of animal spiritualities. Part 2 concludes with a discussion of the wide-ranging implications for human society of consciously accepting the possibility of animal spirituality and capacity to have spiritual experience.
Article
Full-text available
The literature on the relationship of companion animals and children shows only a weak effect of human-animal bonding on child development. The use of “pet ownership” or cohabitation rather than the relationship or interaction between the child and the animal as a measure of bonding appears to be a serious and limiting deficiency, which impaired the empirical evidence concerning the development and effects of human-animal bonding. The Companion Animal Bonding Scale is an 8-item behavioral scale describing the extent of child-animal activities. The scale was administered by questionnaire with a childhood focus and a contemporary focus to 121 high school and college students. The Cronbach alpha estimates of internal reliability were 0.82 and 0.77, respectively. Construct validity was indicated by significant correlations between scores on the Pet Attitude Scale and the childhood and contemporary bonding scale of .39 and .40, respectively.
Article
Full-text available
Reviews the literature on sex differences in empathy (defined as vicarious affective responding to the emotional state of another) and related capacities (affective role taking and decoding of nonverbal cues). The literature is discussed according to method used to assess empathy and affective role taking. Where appropriate, meta-analyses were also computed. In general, sex differences in empathy were found to be a function of the methods used to assess empathy. There was a large sex difference favoring women when the measure of empathy was self-report scales; moderate differences (favoring females) were found for reflexive crying and self-report measures in laboratory situations; and no sex differences were evident when the measure of empathy was either physiological or unobtrusive observations of nonverbal reactions to another's emotional state. Moreover, few sex differences were found for children's affective role taking and decoding abilities. (156 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The relationship of daily uplifts and hassles to adaptational outcomes has gained increasing attention in recent years. However, investigators have focused considerably more on hassles than uplifts. In an attempt to rectify this trend, the present study examines the relationship of uplifts and hassles to anxiety, depression, distress, self-restraint, perceived sup-port from friends, perceived social competence, and general self-worth in a sample of 23 sixth graders (141 girls). Results indicate that both hassles and uplifts are substantially related to these outcomes in the expected direction, with the exception of a modest positive association between uplift and anxiety for girls. Moreover, in most cases uplifts add significantly to the relationship between hassles and outcomes, thus high-lighting the importance of uplifts. Other findings idicate that uplifts/hassles patterns vary as a function of gender and the particular outcome being considered. Similarities to and differences from adult findings are discussed in regard to gender-role and general development.
Article
The present study was aimed at identifying meaningful and sensitive measures of the potential social-emotional benefits and costs that children report as associated with their involvement with companion animals. The sample consisted of 213 children in grades 3 through 7 who had pets and another 44 who did not. The high percentage of pet ownership found is not unusual. Two questionnaires measuring pet benefits and pet costs were administered. The benefits identified were mutuality, enduring affection, self-enhancing affection, and exclusivity of relationship. The costs identified were distress stemming from pet death or pet rejection, unfair grief, dissatisfaction with pet's needs, worry about pet safety, “getting into trouble,” and distress at not being allowed to care for pet needs.
Article
Developmental psychologists argue that children's moral-reasoning abilities develop through a sequence of stages, stimulated by interactions with the social environment. This study attempts to extend Lawrence Kohlberg's (1984) theory of human moral-reasoning development to interpret moral thinking about animal treatment. The study addresses the following questions: (1) Are boys capable of using consistent patterns of reasoning about animal-treatment dilemmas? (2) Does each boy reason at different levels for humans and animals and for different types of animals? (3) Are boys' animal moral-reasoning abilities related to their ages, indicating developmental advancement? (4) Do environmental interactions influence boys' levels of animal moral thinking? Interviews were conducted with 81 eighth- and twelfth-grade boys from suburban and rural Connecticut. Each boy was asked to resolve hypothetical human moral dilemmas and animal dilemmas involving three animal types: chimpanzees, dogs, and turkeys. The results support the conclusion that moral-development theory can be extended to interpret boys' moral reasoning about animal treatment. First, the findings show that boys were capable of using consistent reasoning patterns about animal dilemmas, indicating that animal reasoning, like human reasoning, is organized by stage structures. Second, it was found that human reasoning levels tended to be more advanced than animal reasoning levels, although reasoning levels were not found to differ consistently according to animal type. Third, older boys tended to use more advanced animal reasoning levels than younger boys, indicating that animal reasoning abilities, like human reasoning abilities, improve with age. However, fourth, boys' levels of animal reasoning were not found to be related to their residence background. Although the results did not provide evidence that environment influences animal moral reasoning, the study does support the conclusion that moral-development theory can be extended to interpret moral reasoning about animal treatment.
Article
While studies of human/animal interactions have generated much creditable research, have produced a considerable body of related experimental data, and have pointed to many fruitful future lines of inquiry, their authors have been accused of having no theoretical foundations. But studies of the human/companion animal bond (H/CAB) already undertaken have been based on animal/animal, human/human, and human/object relationships as analogous theories most likely to provide the comprehensive inductive, deductive, and functional theoretical bases needed. In order to arrive at a more encompassing theory that can be used to organize data and results, to explain obtained results, and to generate reliable predictions for data not yet obtained, the present weaknesses in each of the models need to be rigorously analyzed for likenesses and differences, and those data that do not seem to fit any of the model analogues must be pinpointed for more exacting research.
Final report to the Delta Society
  • V L Malcarne
  • Poresky R.H.
East Haddam, CT: National Association for the Advancement of Humane Education
  • K Savesky
  • V Malcarne