ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Participants completed measures of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI), happiness, personality, and cognitive ability. Neuroticism was negatively related to happiness, whereas Extraversion and Openness to Experience were positively related to it. Cognitive ability was not related either to happiness or to trait EI. A three-step hierarchical regression showed that trait EI explained over 50% of the total variance in happiness. The positive relationship between trait EI and happiness persisted in the presence of the Big Five. In contrast, the Big Five did not account for a significant amount of happiness variance when trait EI was partialled out.
Content may be subject to copyright.
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
AND HAPPINESS
ADRIAN FURNHAM
University College London, UK
K. V. PETRIDES
Institute of Education, University of London, UK
Participants completed measures of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI), happiness, person-
ality, and cognitive ability. Neuroticism was negatively related to happiness, whereas
Extraversion and Openness to Experience were positively related to it. Cognitive ability was
not related either to happiness or to trait EI. Athree-step hierarchical regression showed that
trait EI explained over 50% of the total variance in happiness. The positive relationship
between trait EI and happiness persisted in the presence of the Big Five. In contrast, the Big
Five did not account for a significant amount of happiness variance when trait EI was par-
tialled out.
Psychologists have mainly focused on human unhappiness (depression, anxi-
ety, emotional disorders, etc.) and neglected the positive aspects of human poten-
tial (Seligman, 2003). It was only comparatively recently, especially after
Bradburn's (1969) discovery of the independence of positive and negative affect,
that psychological research started to examine the definitions, correlates, and
predictors of happiness (e.g., Argyle, 1992, 2001; Diener, 1984, 2000; Eysenck,
1990; Myers, 1992; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Argyle (2001) pre-
sented a tripartite conceptualization of happiness comprising: 1) the average
level of satisfaction over a specific time period, 2) the frequency and degree of
positive affect, and 3) the relative absence of negative affect.
Personality traits, particularly Extraversion and Neuroticism, have consistent-
SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY, 2003, 31(8), 815-824
© Society for Personality Research (Inc.)
815
Professor Adrian Furnham, University College London, UK; K. V. Petrides, Institute of Education,
University of London, UK.
Appreciation is due to reviewers including: Donald Saklofske, PhD, Department of
Psychology, University of Saskatchewan, Canada, S7N 5A5. Email: <Don.saklofske@usask.ca>
Keywords: happiness, emotional self-efficacy, personality, IQ, trait EI
Please address correspondence and reprint requests to: Professor A. Furnham, Department of
Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H OAP, UK.
Phone: (020) 7679-5395; Fax: (020) 7436-4276; Email: <a.furnham@ucl.ac.uk>
ly been found to be the strongest predictors of general happiness levels, account-
ing for up to half of the total reliable variance in the various measures (Argyle &
Lu, 1990; Brebner, 1998; Eysenck, 1990; Francis, Brown, Lester, & Philipchalk,
1998; Furnham & Brewin, 1990; Furnham & Cheng, 1997, 1999, 2000; Heady
& Wearing, 1991; Lewis, Francis, & Ziebertz, 2002; Myers & Diener, 1995).
Indeed, Francis entitled his 1999 publication in Personality and Individual
Differences "Happiness Is a Thing Called Stable Extraversion". While the links
between happiness and personality have been the subject of much research,
comparatively few studies have examined the association between happiness and
cognitive ability. One objective of this study was to investigate the relationship
between happiness and cognitive ability, as operationalized via four distinct IQ
tests. Eysenck (1990; p. 33) argued: "Despite the fact that it definitely seems
preferable to be clever rather than dull, there is very little evidence that intelli-
gence is related in any way to happiness". In a similar vein, Argyle (2001) noted
that education is related to happiness, but once the confounding influences of
occupational status and income are taken into account, the relationship virtually
disappears.
Another omission in the literature on individual-differences correlates and pre-
dictors of happiness concerns the concept of emotional intelligence (EI). Two
types of EI can be distinguished based on the measurement method used to oper-
ationalise the construct (Austin, Saklofske, Huang, & McKenney, in press;
Petrides & Furnham, 2000a, b; Saklofske, Austin, & Minski, 2003).
Trait EI (or 'emotional self-efficacy') is operationalized through self-report
questionnaires, whereas ability EI (or “cognitive-emotional ability”) is opera-
tionalized through maximum performance tests, that is, tests comprising items
that may be answered correctly or incorrectly. The operationalization of ability
EI is considerably complicated by the fact that emotional experiences are inher-
ently subjective (see, e.g., Spain, Eaton, & Funder, 2000) and, therefore, not
amenable to objective scoring criteria.
Trait EI is a constellation of emotion-related self-perceived abilities and dis-
positions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Petrides &
Furnham, 2001). Individuals with high trait EI scores believe that they are “in
touch” with their emotions and that they can regulate them in a way that pro-
motes well-being. These individuals should enjoy higher levels of happiness.
The present study set out to replicate previous findings on the relationship
between personality and happiness as well as to investigate the association of
happiness with cognitive ability and trait EI. It was hypothesized that
Extraversion and Neuroticism would be, respectively, positively and negatively
correlated with happiness (H1 and H2). It was further hypothesized that trait EI
would be a positive predictor of happiness (H3) and that this relationship would
remain statistically significant even after controlling for the effects of the Big
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
816
Five (H4). Last, it was hypothesized that cognitive ability would not be signifi-
cantly associated with either trait EI (H5) or happiness (H6).
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
In total, 88 individuals participated in the study, of whom 11 were male and 77
female. The mean age for the sample was 19.79 yrs (SD = .83 yrs; Range = 18
to 23 yrs).
MEASURES
1. Personality
NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992). This is a 60-item questionnaire measuring
the "Big Five" personality factors, viz., Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to
Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Items are responded to on a
5-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.
2. Cognitive ability
a) Wonderlic Personnel Test (Wonderlic Personnel Test Inc., 1998). This 50-
item test is administered in 12 minutes and provides a measurement of general
intelligence. Scores range from 0 to 50. Items include word and number com-
parisons, disarranged sentences, serial analysis of geometric figures, and story
problems requiring mathematical and logical solutions. The test has impressive
norms and correlates very highly (r = .92) with the WAIS-R.
b) The Baddeley Reasoning Test (Baddeley, 1968). This 60-item test is
administered in 3 minutes and measures fluid intelligence through logical rea-
soning. Scores range from 0 to 60. Each item is presented in the form of a gram-
matical transformation that is either "true" or "false", e.g., "A precedes B - AB"
(true) or "A does not follow B - BA" (false). The test has been used in many stud-
ies to obtain a quick and reliable measurement of cognitive ability (e.g.,
Furnham, Gunter, & Peterson, 1994).
c) AH5 - Part 1 (Heim, Watts, & Simmonds, 1970). This is a well-established
20-minute measure of verbal and spatial ability. It was designed for use on
selected and highly intelligent samples (notably university students).
d) WAIS Vocabulary Subscale (Wechsler, 1981). This 30-item WAIS subscale
was used to assess verbal intelligence. Verbal IQ is a very good index of crys-
tallized, as opposed to fluid, intelligence (Cattell, 1971; Horn, 1989). The
vocabulary subscale is the subtest with the highest loading on the verbal IQ fac-
tor of the WAIS.
3. Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire - Short Form (TEIQue-SF;
Petrides, Pérez, & Furnham, 2003)
This is a 30-item questionnaire designed to measure global trait emotional
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE 817
intelligence (trait EI). It is based on the full form of the TEIQue (Petrides &
Furnham, 2003), which covers the trait EI sampling domain comprehensively.
The TEIQue-SF provides highly reliable global trait EI scores that correlate
meaningfully with a wide range of diverse criteria, including coping styles, life
satisfaction, personality disorders, perceived job control, and job satisfaction
(Petrides et al., 2003). Items are responded to on a 7-point Likert scale.
4. The Oxford Happiness Inventory (Argyle, Martin, & Crossland, 1989).
This is a 29-item questionnaire that has been used extensively in happiness
research (Argyle, 2001; Cheng & Furnham, 2003; Furnham, Cheng, & Shirasu,
2001). Items are responded to on a 7-point rating scale.
PROCEDURE
All participants were first-year undergraduate students, who completed a bat-
tery of questionnaires and tests shortly after arriving at university. Testing took
approximately 3 hours. All participants were fully debriefed about their scores.
RESULTS
CORRELATIONS
Table 1 summarizes the zero-order correlations between happiness and the
other measures in the study. With the exception of a significant correlation with
the WAIS vocabulary score, happiness was not related to any cognitive-ability
tests. As would be expected (Spearman, 1904), scores on the cognitive-ability
tests were significantly intercorrelated (Pearson product-moment correlations
varied between .37 and .69). Thus, the four tests were combined into a single
reliable composite ('g'; alpha = .73). g was not related to happiness (r= .14, p=
ns). Trait EI was the strongest correlate of happiness, though Neuroticism,
Extraversion, and Openness also showed statistically significant associations
(see Table 1). As expected, trait EI was not significantly correlated to any of the
cognitive-ability tests.
REGRESSIONS
A three-step hierarchical regression was performed, whereby happiness was
regressed on trait EI (step 1), g (step 2), and the Big Five (Step 3). These results
are summarized in Table 2 and show that trait EI is by far the strongest predic-
tor of happiness, accounting for over 50% of the total variance. In fact, in the
presence of trait EI, none of the Big Five factors was reliably associated with
happiness, even though Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness all showed
significant zero-order correlations (see Table 1). The R2 change in the last step
of the hierarchical regression (where the Big Five were collectively entered into
the equation) was not statistically significant (F-change(5, 57) = 1.30, p= ns).
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
818
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
DISCUSSION
All hypotheses (H1-H6) were borne out by the data. Thus, Extraversion and
Neuroticism were statistically significantly associated with happiness, as was
trait EI. In addition, happiness was positively related to Openness. In line with
819
TABLE 1
CORRELATES OF THE OXFORD HAPPINESS INVENTORY
Mean SD r with happiness
IQ Wonderlic 25.91 5.89 -.07
Baddeley 29.85 13.11 .01
Alice Han 14.10 4.89 .13
WAIS vocabulary 54.15 11.19 .26*
Trait EI TEIQue-SF 143.93 21.69 .70**
Big Five N 18.58 6.79 -.37**
E 23.13 5.77 .33**
O 19.35 5.32 .38**
A 18.51 4.25 .15
C 24.05 5.58 .10
*p<.05, **p<.01
TABLE 2
RESULTS OF THREE-STEP HIERARCHICAL REGRESSIONS WITH THE OXFORD HAPPINESS
INVENTORY AS THE DEPENDENT VARIABLE
Regression Beta t
Step 1. F (1, 63) = 72.14; Adj. R2= .53
Trait EI .73 8.5*
Step 2. F(2, 62) = 36.38; Adj. R2= .53
Trait EI .74 8.5*
g .08 .91
Step 3. F(7, 57) = 11.60; Adj. R2= .54
Trait EI .59 5.19*
g .04 .44
N -.08 -.78
E .11 1.10
O .15 1.47
A .09 0.94
C -.01 -0.07
*p< .001
H4, the positive association between trait EI and happiness remained statistical-
ly significant even after controlling for scores on the Big Five. Scores on gener-
al cognitive ability were associated neither with happiness nor with trait EI.
With respect to the last finding, a larger and more representative sample might
have resulted in stronger correlations between cognitive ability and the other
variables in the study, although, as far as trait EI is concerned, the obtained
results are fully in line with theoretical expectations.
The last step in the hierarchical regression is important because it shows that
the strong relationship between happiness and personality (particularly
Extraversion and Neuroticism) clearly diminishes once trait EI is taken into
account. Of course, trait EI is conceptually and empirically related to happiness
and well-being (Palmer, Donaldson, & Stough, 2002; Petrides & Furnham, 2001;
Saklofske et al., 2003). What is especially interesting, however, is that this rela-
tionship not only retains its statistical significance in the presence of the Big
Five, but also nullifies the association between happiness and personality.
The issue of whether trait EI has incremental validity over the basic personal-
ity dimensions has been discussed at length and conclusively answered else-
where (e.g., Petrides & Furnham, 2003). One of the issues raised there is that
hierarchical regression analyses aimed at assessing the incremental validity of
trait EI vis-à-vis the major dimensions of personality are inherently biased
because they pitch the single degree of freedom for trait EI against the three or
five degrees of freedom for the personality structures. In spite of this, it has been
repeatedly shown that trait EI predicts criteria over and above the Giant Three or
the Big Five (e.g., Petrides, Frederickson, & Furnham, in press; Saklofske et al.,
2003). The extraordinary finding in the present study is the collective failure of
the Big Five to account for a significant portion of happiness variance over and
above trait EI.
The present findings confirm that well-being is a salient component of trait EI.
It is important to recognize, however, that it is neither the sole nor the most
important component of the construct. Thus, Petrides et al. (in press) demon-
strated that trait EI predicts important life outcomes, such as exclusions from
school and truancy, incrementally over the Giant Three, even when its general-
mood/well-being component has been removed. Such findings provide empiri-
cal answers to questions about whether the associations between trait EI and cer-
tain external criteria are simply due to the well-being component of the construct
(Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2000). In addition, they raise questions about the
overrepresentation of this component in certain trait EI measures, such as the
Bar-On EQ-i (Bar-On, 1997).
This study showed that a large amount of variance in happiness is determined
by people's emotion-related self-perceptions and dispositions like, for example,
emotion regulation, relationship skills, and social competence. Apoint to note is
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
820
that the strength of this relationship could vary across different cultures
(Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, Dzokoto, & Ahadi, 2002). An important
task for future research is to examine the relationship between trait EI and real-
life criteria that have been variously linked to happiness (e.g., creativity, job pro-
ductivity, health, etc.). This could be an important first step towards the devel-
opment and implementation of research-based assessment systems and interven-
tion programs designed to improve performance, relationship quality, and gen-
eral well-being.
REFERENCES
Argyle, M. (1992). Benefits produced by supportive social relationships. In H. O. F. Veiel & U.
Baumann (Eds.), The meaning and measurement of social support. New York: Hemisphere
Publishing.
Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness. London: Routledge.
Argyle, M., & Lu, L. (1990). The happiness of extroverts. Personality and Individual Differences,
11, 1011-1017.
Argyle, M., Martin, M., & Crossland, J. (1989) Happiness as a function of personality and social
encounters. In J. P. Forgas & J. M. Innes (Eds.), Recent advances in social psychology: An inter-
national perspective. North Holland: Elsevier.
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., Huang, S. H. S., & McKenney, D. M. (in press). Measurement of trait
emotional intelligence: Testing and cross-validating a modified version of Schutte et al.'s (1998)
measure. Personality and Individual Differences.
Baddeley, A. (1968). A3 minute reasoning test based on grammatical transformation. Psychonomic
Science, 10, 341-342.
Bar-On, R. (1997). BarOn Emotional Quotient Inventory: Technical manual. Toronto: Multi-Health
Systems Inc.
Bradburn, N. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldire.
Brebner, J. (1998). Happiness and personality. Personality and Individual Differences,25, 279-296.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality, self-esteem, and demographic predictions of happi-
ness and depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 921-942.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and
NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL.: Psychological
Assessment Resources.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542-575.
Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national
index. American Psychologist, 55, 34-43.
Eysenck, M. (1990). Happiness: Facts and myths. Hove: LEA.
Francis, L. (1999). Happiness is a thing called stable extraversion. Personality and Individual
Differences, 26, 5-11.
Francis, L., Brown, L., Lester, D., & Philipchalk, R. (1998). Happiness is stable extraversion.
Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 167-171.
Furnham, A., & Brewin, C. (1990). Personality and happiness. Personality and Individual
Differences, 11, 1093-1096
Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (1997). Personality and happiness. Psychological Reports, 83, 761-762.
Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (1999). Personality as predictor of mental health and happiness in the East
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE 821
and West. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 395-403.
Furnham, A., & Cheng, H. (2000). Perceived parental behavior, self-esteem and happiness. Social
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 35, 463-470.
Furnham, A, Cheng, H., & Shirasu, Y. (2001). Lay theories of happiness in the east and west.
Psychologia, 44, 173-187.
Furnham, A., Gunter, B., & Peterson, E. (1994). Television distraction and the performance of intro-
verts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 705-711.
Heady, B., & Wearing, A. (1991). Subjective well-being: Astocks and flows framework. In F. Strack,
M. Argyle, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Subjective well-being. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Heim, A., Watts, K., & Simmonds, V. (1970). AH4, AH5, and AH6 tests.Windsor: NFER.
Horn, J. (1989). Models of intelligence. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Intelligence: Measurement, theory, and
public policy. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Lewis, C., Francis, L., & Ziebertz, H. E. (2002). The internal consistency, reliability and construct
validity of the German translation of the Oxford Happiness Inventory. North American Journal
of Psychology, 4, 211-220.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Competing models of emotional intelligence. In
R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Myers, D. (1992). The pursuit of happiness. New York: Avon Books.
Myers, D., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is happy? Psychological Science, 6, 10-19.
Palmer, B., Donaldson, C., & Stough, C. (2002). Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction.
Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1091-1100.
Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Furnham, A. (in press). The role of trait emotional intelligence
in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personality and Individual Differences.
Petrides, K. V,. & Furnham, A. (2000a). On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence.
Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 45-53.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2000b). Gender differences in measured and self-estimated trait
emotional intelligence. Sex Roles, 42, 449-461.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with
reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 425-448.
Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioral validations in two
studies of emotion, recognition and reactivity to mood induction. European Journal of
Personality, 17, 39-57.
Petrides, K. V., Pérez, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2003, July): The Trait Emotional Intelligence
Questionnaire (TEIQue). Ameasure of emotional self-efficacy. In E. J. Austin & D. H. Saklofske
(Chairs), Emotional intelligence. Symposium conducted at the 11th Biennial Meeting of the
International Society for the Study of the Individual Differences. Graz, Austria.
Saklofske, D., Austin, E., & Minski, P. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a trait emotional intel-
ligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 707-721.
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality,
and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 82, 582-593.
Seligman, M. E. P (2003). Positive psychology: Fundamental assumptions. The Psychologist, 16,
126-127.
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology. American Psychologist, 55,
5-14.
Spain, J. S., Eaton, L. G., & Funder, D. C. (2000). Perspectives on personality: The relative accura-
cy of self versus others for the prediction of emotion and behavior. Journal of Personality, 68,
837-867.
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
822
Spearman, C. (1904). "General intelligence," objectively determined and measured. American
Journal of Psychology, 15, 201-293.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. New York:
Psychological Corporation.
Wonderlic Personnel Test, Inc. (1998). Wonderlic Personnel Test: User's manual. Libertyville, IL:
Wonderlic Personnel Test, Inc.
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE 823
TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
824
... The broad definition of EI includes social behavior, competency, and intelligence in monitoring, perceiving, understanding, and processing our own emotions and others' feelings and emotions (Salovey and Mayer, 1990;Zeidner, Matthews, and Roberts 2009). Two conceptually different approaches are generally used to understand EI, trait (Furnham and Konstantinos, 2003) and ability (Salovey and Mayer, 1990) approaches. The trait approach views EI as emotion-related dispositions focusing on how people typically behave in emotional situations (Peña-Sarrionandia, Mikolajczak, and Gross 2015;Furnham and Konstantinos, 2003). ...
... Two conceptually different approaches are generally used to understand EI, trait (Furnham and Konstantinos, 2003) and ability (Salovey and Mayer, 1990) approaches. The trait approach views EI as emotion-related dispositions focusing on how people typically behave in emotional situations (Peña-Sarrionandia, Mikolajczak, and Gross 2015;Furnham and Konstantinos, 2003). The ability approach is focused on learnable competencies that can be applied in emotional situations. ...
Article
Emotional intelligence, or the ability to perceive, interpret, and regulate emotions, has been linked to numerous adaptive outcomes. However, recent investigations have demonstrated that emotional intelligence can exert a nonlinear impact on motivational, interpersonal, and affective factors. Although past work has provided evidence of optimal levels of emotional intelligence, many studies have relied on statistical procedures that are prone to inferential errors when used to investigate curvilinear relationships. Thus, the current study was designed to test for the presence of a nonlinear association between emotional intelligence and generalized anxiety using an inferential method designed to detect curvilinear relationships between variables. University students (N = 345; 68.79% Caucasian, 86.99% female, X A̅ ge = 26.11) completed the Brief Emotional Intelligence Scale and Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale. Using segmented regression analysis, we determined that the relationship between emotional intelligence and anxiety follows a nonlinear trend. Specifically, our results indicate that emotional intelligence is positively related to anxiety among those with ‘low’ and ‘high’ levels of emotional intelligence. Additionally, our findings revealed emotional intelligence and anxiety were negatively related among those with ‘moderate’ levels of emotional intelligence. We believe the current work has important implications for efforts designed to support university students’ emotional growth and academic success.
... One examines EI as an ability (Mayer and Salovey, 1993), measured as maximum performance. The other describes EI as a mixed array of affect-related traits (Furnham and Petrides, 2003) or competencies (Boyatzis and Sala, 2004), measured as typical performance. In recent years, there has been a wider recognition from theorists of both approaches that the term "emotional intelligence" (EI) may be reserved for the collection of cognitive and emotional processes aligned with the ability model, while emotion-related behaviors and traits are labeled something other than "intelligence" (Mayer et al., 2008) such as "emotional and social competencies" (Boyatzis, 2007), "affect-related personality" (Hughes and Evans, 2018. ...
... Another related concern that may be addressed by the UPR attitudes of self-regard and regard for others is non-linearity or the "dark side" of EI/EE constructs. EI is generally considered to fall under the umbrella of positive psychology (Salovey et al., 2002) and there is indeed substantial evidence for its positive impact (Furnham and Petrides, 2003 Day et al., 2005). However, Kilduff et al. (2010 p. 147) express concern at the "overly-positive celebration of EI" and the imbalance of research focusing almost exclusively on the prosocial aspects of EI (Antonakis and Dietz, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional intelligence (EI) was originally conceived as an ability, followed soon after by mixed, competency and trait theoretical models, broadly described as emotional efficacies (EE). Several models have attempted to integrate both approaches, with different views on whether EI and EE operate in sequence or parallel. One reason for this may be that EE constructs are given the same ontological status whether they represent underlying attitudes, such as self-regard, or behavioral competencies, such as assertiveness. In this paper, it is proposed that attitudes may predominantly act as underlying antecedents of ability-EI and behavioral-EE. Five benefits of this approach are drawn out that help to address some key concerns with current models and measures of EI and EE. First, the inclusion of implicit and explicit attitudes within integrated models of EI/EE would support the dual-processing of conscious and automated processes. From this, an attitude-based dual-processing framework for EI/EE is recommended. Second, the concept of Unconditional Positive Regard (UPR) for self and others, is identified as a potential attitude that may underpin the two core pillars of intrapersonal and interpersonal EI/EE. Third, UPR attitudes would provide an ethical basis for EI/EE that may support ethical and prosocial behavior. Fourth, UPR attitudes may differentiate between the optimal and sub-optimal elements of EI/EE. Fifth, an attitude-based approach to EI/EE may be more aligned with EI/EE being developmental than are the more static ability or trait-based models of EI/EE.
... Emotional intelligence is commonly defined as a set of emotion-related insights (including self-perceived abilities and behavioral predispositions) that become particularly useful in situations with emotional and social implications (Alegre et al., 2019). Individuals with high emotional intelligence are characterized by the belief that they are "in touch" with their feelings and those of others and that they can regulate their emotions to foster well-being (Furnham and Petrides, 2003). Emotional regulation may entail understanding emotional states as well as the ability to use the information gathered from emotional states to function effectively in everyday life. ...
... One would expect EI to contribute heavily to performance. In fact, emotional regulation, a critical aspect of EI, entails not only understanding emotional states but also the ability to use information about emotional states to function effectively in college (Furnham and Petrides, 2003). Instead, only freshmen's performance benefited from EI, and its contribution was minor. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined the dispositions of female undergraduate law students one semester after their return to face-to-face classes. It aimed to determine the contribution of particular dispositions to academic performance. Dispositions selected were those that are known to promote adaptation, such as self-efficacy and emotional intelligence, as well as resistance-to-change attitudes, which are known to do the opposite. Freshmen ( n = 261) and seniors ( n = 236) were included to assess whether dispositions varied with the length of students’ academic experience. Participants were students from a society in which females’ academic success is seen as key to advancing a sustainable, competitive, and gender-equitable economy. Thus, at the time of the study, these students were expected to quickly readjust to the post-pandemic world of face-to-face instruction. Participants completed questionnaires on the selected dispositions. Academic performance was operationalized as the grade point average obtained in the semester following a return to face-to-face classes. In the study, the length of academic experience mattered. Seniors exhibited greater self-efficacy and emotional intelligence. For both groups, performance increased with emotional reactions to forced change, thereby suggesting that noticing environmental changes that challenge one’s sense of agency is beneficial. However, in seniors, performance decreased with their short-term focus, whereas in freshmen, it increased with emotional intelligence, indicating that college experience shapes the utility of particular traits.
... Effectiveness includes the accurate perception and understanding of complex affective states of oneself and others, the successful regulation of affective expressions, and the capacity to use emotions to inform reasoning [33]. High trait-emotional intelligence characterizes individuals who understand their emotions and those of others and who can regulate emotions to foster well-being [32,34]. In academic settings, emotional intelligence, including greater emotional regulation and adaptability, is seen as helpful in coping with stress and thus in achieving academic success [35]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The “Dunning–Kruger effect” refers to the tendency of poor performers to overestimate test outcomes. Although a widespread phenomenon, questions exist regarding its source and sensitivity to countermeasures. The present field study aimed to (a) examine whether practice with tests used in previous classes can enhance students’ ability to estimate test outcomes, (b) determine the main source of the effect (i.e., is it unawareness of one’s readiness or wishful thinking?), and (c) assess the extent to which particular individual differences can be used as predictors of test performance. In this study, participants practiced with old tests and then completed the final exam. Before and after the exam, they predicted their grades and indicated their subjective confidence in the predictions made. Furthermore, participants’ emotional intelligence and self-efficacy about their academic abilities were surveyed. Results suggested that poor performers were not unaware of their test preparation, but rather engaged in wishful thinking. In fact, although they overestimated their test grades, their estimates not only improved after completing the final test but also were regarded with little confidence. Overall, estimation bias was a good predictor of students’ final test performance, whereas subjective confidence and emotional intelligence only weakly predicted such performance. Thus, if proactive interventions are to be developed for at-risk students, performance-estimation tasks may offer valuable information regarding such students’ future performance in a course much more than emotional intelligence and self-efficacy measures.
... Specifically, emotional intelligence and social relationships at work contribute positively to subjective wellbeing (Brackett et al., 2004). Looking at personality using the big 5 personality traits, Furnham and Petrides (2003) found that extraversion and openness to experience were positively associated with happiness at work, while neuroticism was negatively related to it. ...
Article
Full-text available
Japan has been experiencing a long decline in its workforce. Companies in Japan are eager to retain their existing employees and diversify their recruitment. Employees with long-term and open-ended employment are also switching companies at a greater rate. Consequently, Japanese firms have started paying attention to employee subjective wellbeing, now recognized as a source of higher job performance. This study empirically explores the predictors of subjective wellbeing at work for Japanese regular employees beyond those already identified in Europe and U.S.-centric research. We applied a two-stage design, consisting of interviews and a questionnaire survey to identify those factors that promote subjective wellbeing in Japanese corporations where long-time employment and group cohesiveness and achievement are valued over individual achievement. We identified eight factors affecting subjective wellbeing at work for Japanese regular employees: meaningful work, relationships, culture, workspace, evaluation, time off, financial benefits, and diversity at work. Consequent regression analyses highlighted the discriminant importance of work relationships, evaluation, diversity, workspace, and meaningful work. Eudaimonic and hedonic happiness were found to be caused by different factors. As expected, meaningful work led to eudaimonic satisfaction of life at work in Japan. In contrast hedonic happiness was affected by factors external to work itself, such as work relationships, work evaluation and diversity. Interestingly, diversity at work was found to have an ambivalent effect as it was related to both positive and negative affects at work. These findings will help Japanese companies create a work environment that can maximize regular employees’ wellbeing, job performance, and retention.
... Low scores in conscientiousness were associated with carelessness at work [59], dishonest behavior [62], and lower academic achievement [25], and this is in line with this study as was shown, for example, in higher error rate (objective Healthcare organizations need to also pay attention to all three psychometric tests used in this study and their outcomes to give resources where needed most wisely [55]. For example, it was found that people with high scores in neuroticism were statistically and significantly unhappy [22]. In this study, it was shown that high scores of neuroticism were associated with less rational thinking (ob- [24], and this could be explained by low self-control scores with all BFI components (objective 2.1.1). ...
... Therefore, subsequent research to address the limitations of the ''Four Branch Model'' , suggested the ''Mixed Model'' . This model contains elements such as non-cognitive abilities [17], emotionally and socially intelligent behaviors [18], constructs from the personality domain [19] and adaptability [20]. An endorser of this model was Bar-On [18] who conceptualized EI in his theory as a ''Model of Emotions and Social Intelligence'' , which lies on emotional self-awareness, assertiveness, self-regard, empathy, interpersonal relationship, reality-testing, stress tolerance, impulse control, flexibility and problem-solving. ...
Article
Full-text available
The academic community has been significantly interested in components that direct decision-making in humans. A considerable amount of research has revealed that emotional intelligence and stress are incontrovertible factors that could predict decision-making outcomes. Specifically, there are noticed studies that yield a positive correlation between emotional intelligence and decision-making as well as a negative relationship between stress and decision making and stress and emotional intelligence. However, there is not detected a study that has assessed all these variables simultaneously. Thus, the present study aimed to address this inadequacy of the literature by examining their correlation together in order to be provided a clarified facet concerning their link. The data were obtained through a convenience sample (N=152), aged 18-50 and the procedure was conducted online via Google Forms due to Covid-19 related issues. The questionnaires that were used for the study were: The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire, Short Form [23], Perceived Stress Reactivity Scale [61], Decision Making Questionnaire [64], because of their satisfying internal reliability. The analysis that was used was the Forced Entry Method of the Multiple Regression since emotional intelligence and perceived stress could seemingly predict the individuals’ ability to make decisions. Markedly, the outcomes yield that the rates of emotional intelligence and perceived stress could significantly predict the decision-making process. In particular, there was found that an individual with high emotional intelligence corresponded to low levels of perceived stress and adaptivity in decision-making. The findings provided an indispensable practical and theoretical implication for the field of psychology. However, there are addressed limitations and suggestions for forthcoming studies.
Article
Key digested message A recent meta-analysis found a negative relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EI) and the dark triad. In the present paper it is proposed that measuring EI as a linear construct (more is always better) does not take into account the dynamic variability in some facets of EI that are better measured as non-linear constructs.
Preprint
Full-text available
Emotional intelligence and educational level are commonly associated with success in life and well-being. While multiple studies have explored these concepts independently, few studies have examined the impact of educational level on emotional intelligence and associated variables, such as well-being. A total of N = 202 participants were recruited online. Participants completed measures of emotional intelligence, well-being (i.e., happiness, self-esteem, personal growth). Overall, participants who previously obtained a bachelor’s degree displayed higher levels of emotional intelligence, happiness, self-esteem, and personal growth compared to those without an undergraduate degree. However, no differences were observed on any variable between individuals currently enrolled in a university and non-students. The results suggest that, while completing a university degree increases emotional intelligence and well-being, the stress of being a student may negatively impact the benefits obtained from attending university. Limitations and possible directions for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional intelligence and, in particular, the component emotion regulation may increase well-being and improve mood and coping with negative emotions. In the present eye-tracking study, we examined whether attention allocation to positive stimuli mediates the relationship between emotion regulation abilities and trait affect. Gaze behavior of 104 healthy adults was analyzed in a free-viewing task, in which happy, sad, angry, and neutral faces were shown simultaneously for ten seconds. Dwell time on facial expressions was used as indicator of attention allocation. Trait emotional intelligence was assessed using the Self-Rated Emotional Intelligence Scale. Self-report measures of state and trait positive and negative affect, trait anxiety, and depression were administered. In general, participants viewed longer at happy than at negative or neutral faces. The results of mediation analyses indicated that intrapersonal and interpersonal emotion regulation abilities were indirectly related to trait positive affect through attention to happy faces. Moreover, dwell time on happy faces had a mediating effect on the relationship between interpersonal emotion regulation ability and trait anxiety. Preference for positive social signals might form one attentional pathway through which emotion regulation abilities promote positive mood and buffer the development of anxiety reactions in everyday life.
Article
The literature on subjective well-being (SWB), including happiness, life satisfaction, and positive affect, is reviewed in three areas: measurement, causal factors, and theory. Psychometric data on single-item and multi-item subjective well-being scales are presented, and the measures are compared. Measuring various components of subjective well-being is discussed. In terms of causal influences, research findings on the demographic correlates of SWB are evaluated, as well as the findings on other influences such as health, social contact, activity, and personality. A number of theoretical approaches to happiness are presented and discussed: telic theories, associationistic models, activity theories, judgment approaches, and top-down versus bottom-up conceptions.