Aesthetic judgment of orientation in modern art

Article (PDF Available)ini-Perception 3(1):18-24 · November 2012with40 Reads
DOI: 10.1068/i0447aap · Source: PubMed
Abstract
When creating an artwork, the artist makes a decision regarding the orientation at which the work is to be hung based on their aesthetic judgement and the message conveyed by the piece. Is the impact or aesthetic appeal of a work diminished when it is hung at an incorrect orientation? To investigate this question, Experiment 1 asked whether naïve observers can appreciate the correct orientation (as defined by the artist) of 40 modern artworks, some of which are entirely abstract. Eighteen participants were shown 40 paintings in a series of trials. Each trial presented all four cardinal orientations on a computer screen, and the participant was asked to select the orientation that was most attractive or meaningful. Results showed that the correct orientation was selected in 48% of trials on average, significantly above the 25% chance level, but well below perfect performance. A second experiment investigated the extent to which the 40 paintings contained recognisable content, which may have mediated orientation judgements. Recognition rates varied from 0% for seven of the paintings to 100% for five paintings. Orientation judgements in Experiment 1 correlated significantly with "meaningful" content judgements in Experiment 2: 42% of the variance in orientation judgements in Experiment 1 was shared with recognition of meaningful content in Experiment 2. For the seven paintings in which no meaningful content at all was detected, 41% of the variance in orientation judgements was shared with variance in a physical measure of image content, Fourier amplitude spectrum slope. For some paintings, orientation judgements were quite consistent, despite a lack of meaningful content. The origin of these orientation judgements remains to be identified.
a Pion publication
dx.doi.org/10.1068/i0447aap
i-Perception (2012) volume 3, pages 18 24
ISSN 2041-6695 perceptionweb.com/i-perception
Aesthetic judgement of orientation in modern art
George Mather
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9QH, UK [Present address: School of
Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK]; e-mail: gmather@lincoln.ac.uk;
Received 13 May 2011, in revised form 16 December 2011; published online 18 January 2011
Abstract. When creating an artwork, the artist makes a decision regarding the orientation at
which the work is to be hung based on their aesthetic judgement and the message conveyed by
the piece. Is the impact or aesthetic appeal of a work diminished when it is hung at an incorrect
orientation? To investigate this question, Experiment 1 asked whether naïve observers can appreciate
the correct orientation (as defined by the artist) of 40 modern artworks, some of which are entirely
abstract. Eighteen participants were shown 40 paintings in a series of trials. Each trial presented
all four cardinal orientations on a computer screen, and the participant was asked to select the
orientation that was most attractive or meaningful. Results showed that the correct orientation was
selected in 48% of trials on average, significantly above the 25% chance level, but well below perfect
performance. A second experiment investigated the extent to which the 40 paintings contained
recognisable content, which may have mediated orientation judgements. Recognition rates varied
from 0% for seven of the paintings to 100% for five paintings. Orientation judgements in Experiment
1 correlated significantly with “meaningful” content judgements in Experiment 2: 42% of the variance
in orientation judgements in Experiment 1 was shared with recognition of meaningful content in
Experiment 2. For the seven paintings in which no meaningful content at all was detected, 41%
of the variance in orientation judgements was shared with variance in a physical measure of image
content, Fourier amplitude spectrum slope. For some paintings, orientation judgements were quite
consistent, despite a lack of meaningful content. The origin of these orientation judgements remains
to be identified.
Keywords: aesthetics, visual art, abstract art, orientation.
1 Introduction
Two paintings by the American abstract artist Mark Rothko from his Black on Maroon series
contain broad black stripes across the canvas defining hazy maroon rectangles. The paintings
were donated to the Tate Modern Gallery in London in 1968, which hung them for many
years with the stripes running horizontally. The paintings were then rotated so that they were
hung with the stripes running vertically for a time, before being returned to the horizontal
orientation for an exhibition of Rothkos work. Controversy has surrounded the question
of the correct orientation in which to hang the paintings. Rothkos signature indicates a
horizontal orientation (see Jamieson 2008), but the works are currently hung vertically. The
artist himself may have been unsure; paint dribbles run in both directions on the canvas.
Art historians have debated the relative aesthetic merits of the two orientations. There are
anecdotal reports of other disputes about the correct orientation of other paintings, including
work by Matisse, Rauschenberg, van Gogh, and O’Keeffe. But does orientation really matter
in an abstract or semiabstract painting, at least to the untrained eye?
Modern art often contains little or no recognisable representation of real-world objects
or scenes. Abstraction allows the artist to focus on composition and medium rather than on
subject matter. In the process of creating a modern artwork, an artist may make an aesthetic
decision regarding the orientation at which the work should be hung, based on their intended
message. The correct orientation is often specified on the back of the canvas. However, this
intended orientation is not always obvious to others viewing the work, especially if there is
no recognisable content at all, which raises questions about aesthetics. Is there sufficient
information in a modern artwork for a naïve viewer’s judgement to align with the correct
Orientation judgement in modern art 19
orientation? Is the impact or aesthetic value of a work diminished by viewing at an incorrect
orientation? Relatively few studies have addressed such questions, but answers to them
should throw some light on the cognitive processes underlying visual aesthetic judgement.
Lindauer (1969) asked participants to indicate their preferred orientation for 54 abstract
paintings and found that half were in agreement with the artists intended orientation. On
the other hand, Swartz and Hewitt (1970) showed their participants a series of abstract
paintings, either in the original format or rotated or mirror-reversed, and asked participants
to rate each image aesthetically. Aesthetic ratings were not affected by rotation or mirror
reversal. Latto et al (2000) were primarily interested in preferences for vertical and horizontal
orientations over oblique orientations in Mondrian paintings, though they did report slightly
higher aesthetic ratings for paintings presented at the intended orientation. Plumhoff and
Schirillo (2009) more recently found some evidence of higher aesthetic ratings for Mondrians
paintings presented at the correct orientation. In Johnson et al’s (2010) study of the same
Mondrian paintings, pupil size was significantly higher for the correct orientation than for
other orientations.
The present study aims to build on previous studies by (i) systematically investigating the
relevance of recognisable content in preferences for orientation (a potential confounding
factor in previous studies employing a range of abstract artworks) and (ii) assessing whether
a commonly used statistical measure of information content in images has any bearing
on orientation-dependent judgements. Recent work suggests that the Fourier amplitude
spectrum slope plays a role in aesthetic judgements of art (Graham and Field 2008; Redies
2007; Redies et al 2007). It may also play a role in orientation judgements.
In the first experiment, participants were shown four versions of each artwork, oriented
at 0, 90, 180, and 270 degrees clockwise with respect to the intended orientation, and asked
to select the most pleasing or meaningful image. Was the correct orientation chosen more
often than would be expected on the basis of chance?
2 Experiment 1
2.1 Method
2.1.1 Participants
Eighteen naïve participants were recruited from the student body at Sussex University, none
of whom had any previous formal training in art.
2.1.2 Visual materials
Forty modern artworks were selected from three art books (Gerhardus and Gerhardus 1979;
Gibson 1997; Hunter 1997), scanned electronically, and rescaled so that the longer dimension
of each image was 300 pixels (to allow all four versions to be visible simultaneously on a
computer monitor). Fifteen images had a vertical portrait aspect ratio, and twenty-two
had a horizontal “landscape” aspect ratio. The remaining three were square. The criteria for
inclusion of paintings were that (i) there was a clear intended or “correct” orientation, (ii)
the work was produced by an artist of international renown, and therefore of high aesthetic
quality, (iii) the set as a whole represented a sample of modern art from the early to mid
20th century, and (iv) paintings varied in the extent to which they exhibited recognisably
representational content. Figure 1 shows three of the paintings in the set. A full list of artists
and titles for all 40 paintings is available as supplementary material.
20 G Mather
Figure 1.
Three of the paintings used in the experiments. (a) Kasimir Malevich (1878–1935) Suprematist
Composition: Airplane Flying, 1914–15. (b) Piet Mondrian (1872–1944) Composition, 1933. (c) Jackson
Pollock (1912–56) One, Number 31, 1950. All digital images
©
2011, The Museum of Modern Art, New
York/Scala, Florence.
2.1.3 Apparatus and procedure
The experiment was controlled by a Flash application running on a Dell desktop PC. Stimuli
appeared on the screen of a Sony Trinitron Multiscan G400 monitor (resolution 1024
×
768).
A single trial involved presentation of one of the 40 artworks (order randomised across
participants). All four cardinal orientations were shown on-screen (0, 90, 180, 270 deg
clockwise with respect to the correct orientation as defined in the source book; oblique
frame orientations were not used because they occur much less frequently in true artworks,
and only a few orientations could be presented on the screen at an acceptable resolution).
The screen was divided virtually into four quarters, with one image in each quarter. The
on-screen ordering of the four orientations varied randomly from trial to trial and between
participants. The centre-to-centre separation of the images was 17 deg horizontally and 12
deg vertically at the 57-cm viewing distance. The longer dimension of each image subtended
10.3 deg. Images were presented against a uniform dark grey background (2.3 cd/m
2
); the
mean image luminance varied from image to image between approximately 3.7 and 38
cd/m
2
.
Participants were given an unrestricted time to view each set of four on-screen images
and used a mouse to click an on-screen button next to each image according to the following
instruction: “Select the orientation which is the most pleasing or meaningful.” Given that
participants may detect some meaningful content in a given painting (even if none was
intended by the artist), this instruction acknowledged the possibility that they may use that
information in their judgement. Successive trials were separated by a 0.5-s interval during
Orientation judgement in modern art 21
which the screen remained blank. Responses were recorded by the Flash application and
transferred to an Excel spreadsheet at the end of the session.
2.1.4 Spectral slope
The amplitude spectrum slope was estimated for each image using a script written in
MATLAB
®
as follows. Each image was converted to grey-scale using a standard YIQ transform
(L = 0.2989
×
R + 0.5870
×
G + 0.1140
×
B). A two-dimensional discrete Fourier transform
was performed on the largest central square section of the image. The slope of the best-
fit line to the rotationally averaged amplitude spectrum (plotted on log–log coordinates)
was calculated. In order to avoid spatial frequency artefacts owing to image sampling and
nonlinearities, line fitting was restricted to the middle range of frequencies between 0.25
×
and 0.5
×
the maximum spatial frequency available in the Fourier transform (cf Redies et al
2007). To assess possible orientational anisotropies in the images, the ratios of horizontal to
vertical (H:V ) power and oblique to cardinal power (O:C, where C = ([H + V ] / 2)) were also
calculated.
2.2 Results and discussion
Each participant provided four scores that correspond to the proportion of responses at each
of the four available orientations (0, 90, 180, 270 deg clockwise) across the 40 images. Figure 2
shows the mean scores across the 18 participants (
±
1 SE). Since there were four orientations,
chance performance falls at 25%. Observers reliably selected the correct orientation over
the other three available orientations in almost 50% of trials, a figure which is close to
that reported by Lindauer (1969). One-factor ANOVA revealed a highly significant effect of
orientation, F(3, 51) = 85.06; p < .0001. Pairwise comparisons showed that responses at the
correct orientation (0 deg) were significantly higher than responses at each other orientation.
Notice that all the other available orientations were selected much less frequently and at
about the same rate of 16–18% or so each. This result indicates that participants were not
generally prone to confuse upright with inverted versions of the images consistently; they
either selected the correct orientation or chose one of the others equally often overall.
Figure 2.
Mean percentage responses at each orientation across the 18 participants (
±
1 SE). The
correct orientation corresponds to 0 deg; other orientations are expressed in degrees clockwise relative
to this orientation. The dotted line corresponds to chance performance.
22 G Mather
The mean spectral slope of the 40 images was –1.248 (SD 0.221), close to the value cited
in a previously published study of abstract art (–1.13; Graham and Field 2008). There was no
significant correlation between image spectral slope and the percentage of correct orientation
judgements (r = 0.0124, ns). Correlations of orientation judgements with measures of
anisotropy were higher but nonsignificant: for O:C (oblique to cardinal power), r = 0.171; for
H:V (horizontal to vertical power), r = –0.181.
On inspection of the data for individual images, it is clear that the correct orientation was
judged quite consistently in some cases but not in others. For certain images, it is possible to
discern a pictorial element in the image that may have facilitated a decision. For example,
a tree shape may be seen on careful viewing of Braques House at Estague (88.9% correct).
In other cases, it is not clear how participants were able to infer the correct orientation.
Miros Birth of the World is quite abstract, as is von Wiegand’s City Lights, but both supported
reliable performance (55.6% and 77.8%, respectively).
Responses in Experiment 1 may have been influenced by detection of meaningful content
in some paintings, rather than by aesthetic pleasure alone, and this factor may have masked
any effect of spectral slope. A second experiment assessed the extent to which recognisable
content in the form of a meaningful object or scene was detected in the experimental stimuli.
3 Experiment 2
3.1 Method
3.1.1 Participants
Eight naïve participants were recruited from the student body at Sussex University, none of
whom had taken part in Experiment 1.
3.1.2 Materials, apparatus, and procedure
The same 40 modern artworks were used as for Experiment 1. The equipment was identical to
that in Experiment 1. The experimental task was also almost identical to that in Experiment
1; all four orientations were displayed for each painting, but in this experiment the on-screen
instruction read: “Does each painting contain recognisable or meaningful content?” Two on-
screen response buttons were available, labelled “Abstract” and “Recognisable.” Observers
were instructed to click on “Recognisable” if they saw any meaningful content in the painting,
otherwise to click on “Abstract. They were also instructed to base their answer on any of the
four orientations presented and were given an unlimited time to make a judgement.
3.2 Results and discussion
The mean percentage of "meaningful” responses was calculated for each of the 40 paintings
and ranged from 0% for seven paintings such as Kasimir Malevichs Suprematist Composition
to 100% for five paintings such as Fernand Leger’s Two Women with Still Life. Across the
40 paintings, the correlation between correct orientation judgements in Experiment 1 and
meaningful” responses in Experiment 2 was 0.65 (p < .0001); thus, recognisable content
clearly did have a significant relation to orientation judgements, though only 42% of the
variance in orientation judgements in Experiment 1 can be attributed to recognition of
meaningful content.
A partial correlation was performed across the 40 paintings to assess whether spectral
slope played a role in orientation judgements independently of “meaningful” judgements.
The correlation was 0.157 (ns), so across the full image set, orientation judgements were
dominated by recognition of meaningful content. The seven paintings in which no partici-
pant reported any meaningful content at all had a mean spectral slope of –1.26, and a mean
Orientation judgement in modern art 23
percentage of correct orientation judgements of 23.02% (range 0–55.6%). In these purely
abstract paintings the correlation between percentage correct and spectral slope was 0.64
(r
2
= 0.41), so there is a hint that purely abstract images with the shallowest spectral slope
are easiest to judge, though the sample size is very small. One painting in this abstract set,
Jackson Pollocks One, was judged correctly in 55.6% of responses and has a spectral slope of
–0.768.
There are other intriguing variations between individual paintings. The three paintings
in Figure 1 are particularly interesting. In the case of Kasimir Malevichs Suprematist
Composition, 72% of responses selected an incorrect orientation rotated 90 deg anticlockwise
from the correct orientation, and none of the 18 observers selected the correct orientation.
No observer discerned any meaningful content in this painting, which has a very steep
spectral slope of –1.56. All observers also agreed that Piet Mondrians Composition contained
no meaningful content, and only 5.6% of them selected the correct orientation (the other
three orientations were chosen equally often by observers). The painting’s spectral slope is
–1.58. Only one participant saw anything meaningful in Joan Miros The Birth of the World, yet
over half of the participants in Experiment 1 selected the correct orientation. The painting’s
spectral slope is –1.38.
A limitation of the present study was that there was no systematic attempt to assess the
degree to which participants were familiar with any of the artworks, though participants
did not report any familiarity during informal debriefing. For example, Pollocks One (see
Figure 1) is famous and should therefore arguably be quite familiar to some participants.
However, given the abstract nature of the work, it seems unlikely that untrained participants
would have retained such detail in memory that they were able to judge its orientation
correctly.
4 Conclusions
In agreement with anecdotal reports from galleries, the judgements of nonexpert viewers
accord with the intended orientation for abstract or semiabstract art at levels well above
chance. However, performance is far from perfect, consistent with results in previous studies.
The most important new result is that orientation judgements are mediated at least in part by
some appreciation of meaningful content in the image. However, in a minority of paintings, a
consistent orientation effect was obtained, even when no meaningful content was apparent.
The origin of these orientation judgements remains to be identified conclusively, but image
spectral slope may play a role.
Acknowledgements. I am grateful to Heather Dowling and Kirsten Challinor for assistance with
stimulus preparation and data collection. I am also grateful to the editor for suggesting the second
experiment.
References
Gerhardus M, Gerhardus D, 1979 Cubism to Futurism: The Evolution of the Self-Sufficient Picture
(Oxford: Phaidon)
J
Gibson A E, 1997 Abstract Expressionism: Other Politics (London: Yale University Press)
J
Graham D, Field D, 2008 “Variations in intensity statistics for representational and abstract
art, and for art from the Eastern and Western hemispheres Perception 37 13411352
doi:10.1068/p5971
J
Hunter S, 1997 The Museum of Modern Art, New York: The History and the Collection (New York, NY:
Abrams)
J
Jamieson A, 2008 "Rothko hung ‘wrong way round’ in exhibition" Daily Telegraph 9 November
J
Johnson M G, Muday J A, Schirillo J A, 2010 “When viewing variations in paintings by Mondrian
aesthetic preferences correlate with pupil sizePsychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 4
161167 doi:10.1037/a0018155
J
24 G Mather
Latto R, Brain D, Kelly B, 2000 An oblique effect in aesthetics: Homage to Mondrian (1872–1944)”
Perception 29 981987 doi:10.1068/p2352
J
Lindauer MS, 1969 "The orientation of form in abstract art" Proceedings of the American Psychologi-
cal Association 4 475476
Plumhoff J, Schirillo J A, 2009 “Mondrian, eye movements, and the oblique effect” Perception 38
719731 doi:10.1068/p6160
J
Redies C, 2007 A universal model of esthetic perception based on the sensory coding of natural
stimuli” Spatial Vision 21 97117 doi:10.1163/156856807782753886
J
Redies C, Hasenstein J, Denzler J, 2007 “Fractal-like image statistics in visual art: similarity to natural
scenesSpatial Vision 21 137148 doi:10.1163/156856807782753921
J
Swartz P, Hewitt D, 1970 “Lateral organization in pictures and aesthetic preference Perceptual and
Motor Skills 30 9911007 doi:10.2466/pms.1970.30.3.991
J
Copyright © 2012 G Mather
Published under a Creative Commons Licence
a Pion publication
    • "selecting an up image from four copies - " up " , " down " , " turn left " , and " turn right " ) are 42 % with the standard deviation 3.6 %. In [24], experiments in participants show that the correct orientation was selected in 48 % of trials on average (selecting an up image from four copies). Our results (42 %) has a comparable performance to human, with a little minor accuracy. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: Artists decide the orientation at which an abstract painting should be hung based on their ideas, but the correct orientation is not obvious to other viewers. Some studies have found that abstract paintings at the correct orientations generally get higher aesthetic ratings from viewers. This encourages us to deal with the problem of orientation judgment for abstract paintings through machine learning. First, we design a group of methods to extract features from paintings based on the theories in abstract art. Then a machine leaning framework is proposed using Naive Bayes classifier and BP neural network classifier for training and orientation testing. Experiments show that it can classify abstract paintings into up and non-up ones with performance comparable to human. This is the first work of orientation judgment for abstract paintings through computer simulation, and the results demonstrate the validity of abstract art theories used for feature definition. This work provides a new scheme for exploring the relationship between aesthetic quality of abstract paintings and their computational visual features.
    Full-text · Article · Nov 2015
    • "A completely different methodological focus is taken by Tim Holmes and Johannes Zanker (2012): How can we measure preferences through eye-movements, and how can this technique be utilised? Among the topics with the widest range of articles related to them are the perception of space in paintings (Koenderink et al 2011; van Doorn et al 2011) and the perception of balance and orientation (Bertamini et al 2011; Gershoni and Hochstein 2011; Leyssen et al 2012; Mather 2012; McManus et al 2011), loosely related also to the phenomena of transparency (Sayim and Cavanagh 2011) and occlusion (Gillam 2011). Similarly, our authors approach visual illusions from different angles: illusions as characteristics of particular artworks (Daneyko et al 2011), eye-movement behaviour related to illusions (Hermens and Zanker 2012), and the aesthetics of visual illusions (Stevanov et al 2012). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: The i-Perception special issue Art & Perception is based on the Art & Perception Conference 2010 in Brussels. Our vision with this conference was to bring together artists and vision scientists from different backgrounds to exchange views and state-of-the-art knowledge on art perception and aesthetics. The complexity of the experience of art and of aesthetic phenomena, in general, calls for specific research approaches, for which interdisciplinarity seems to be key. Following this logic, the special issue Art & Perception contains contributions by artists and vision scientists with different methodological approaches. The contributions span a wide range of topics, but are all centred around two questions: How can one understand art perception and aesthetics from a psychological point of view, and how is this reflected in art itself?
    Full-text · Article · Jul 2012
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract] ABSTRACT: When artists express their feelings through the artworks they create, it is believed that the resulting works transform into objects with “emotions” capable of conveying the artists' mood to the audience. There is little to no dispute about this belief: Regardless of the artwork, genre, time, and origin of creation, people from different backgrounds are able to read the emotional messages. This holds true even for the most abstract paintings. Could this idea be applied to machines as well? Can machines learn what makes a work of art “emotional”? In this work, we employ a state-of-the-art recognition system to learn which statistical patterns are associated with positive and negative emotions on two different datasets that comprise professional and amateur abstract artworks. Moreover, we analyze and compare two different annotation methods in order to establish the ground truth of positive and negative emotions in abstract art. Additionally, we use computer vision techniques to quantify which parts of a painting evoke positive and negative emotions. We also demonstrate how the quantification of evidence for positive and negative emotions can be used to predict which parts of a painting people prefer to focus on. This method opens new opportunities of research on why a specific painting is perceived as emotional at global and local scales.
    Article · Jul 2015

    Recommended publications

    Discover more