ArticlePDF Available

The Role of Self-concept Clarity in Relationship Quality

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Previous research has linked self-concept clarity to positive outcomes (Campbell et al., 199617. Campbell , J. D. , Trapnell , P. D. , Heine , S. J. , Katz , I. M. , Lavallee , L. F. and Lehmann , D. R. 1996 . Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates and cultural boundaries . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 70 : 141 – 156 . [CrossRef], [Web of Science ®], [CSA]View all references), but has not systematically explored its potential positive association with relationship quality. The current set of studies hypothesizes that self-concept clarity will positively correlate with relationship satisfaction and commitment, and that inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem will mediate the association. Study 1 examined correlations between self-report measures from 199 undergraduates, while Study 2 randomly assigned 78 undergraduates to either engage in a self-concept clarity or self-concept confusion manipulation. As hypothesized, in both studies, higher self-concept clarity was associated with higher relationship satisfaction and commitment. Inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem mediated the association between self-concept clarity and relationship quality measures (the only exception was for self-esteem and commitment in Study 1). These results demonstrate how a person's self-concept relates to romantic relationship quality.
Content may be subject to copyright.
The Role of Self-concept Clarity in
Relationship Quality
GARY W. LEWANDOWSKI, JR.
NATALIE NARDONE
ALANNA J. RAINES
Monmouth University, West Long Branch, New Jersey, USA
Previous research has linked self-concept clarity to positive outcomes (Campbell
et al., 1996), but has not systematically explored its potential positive association
with relationship quality. The current set of studies hypothesizes that self-concept
clarity will positively correlate with relationship satisfaction and commitment, and
that inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem will mediate the association.
Study 1 examined correlations between self-report measures from 199 under-
graduates, while Study 2 randomly assigned 78 undergraduates to either engage in a
self-concept clarity or self-concept confusion manipulation. As hypothesized, in both
studies, higher self-concept clarity was associated with higher relationship
satisfaction and commitment. Inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem mediated
the association between self-concept clarity and relationship quality measures (the
only exception was for self-esteem and commitment in Study 1). These results
demonstrate how a person’s self-concept relates to romantic relationship quality.
Keywords: Self-concept clarity; Self-esteem; Inclusion of other in self; Individual
differences; Relationship satisfaction; Commitment.
A study of exceedingly happy people revealed that all but one of the happiest 10% were
currently involved in a romantic relationship (Diener & Seligman, 2002). Clearly,
romantic relationships are an important part of our lives. Similarly, one’s self-concept
(i.e., the totality of beliefs one has and can describe about one’s self) permeates nearly
all of life’s experience (Leary & Tangney, 2003). Thus, it seems natural to examine
the confluence of these concepts (Aron, 2003). It is important to explore a variety of
factors that may influence relationship quality because they may consequently help
improve relationships. Self-concept clarity is a construct that has remained largely
unexplored in the context of romantic relationships. Self-concept clarity involves a
person’s maintenance of a distinct, cohesive, and consistent conception of their
characteristics (Campbell, 1990). The purpose of this paper is to determine the
potential influence of self-concept clarity on relationship satisfaction and commitment.
Received 5 June 2008; accepted 9 July 2009; first published online 31 October 2009.
This research was supported, in part, with funding from the Anthony Marchionne Foundation Small
Grants Program.
We would to thank the following individuals for their help on this project: Rebecca Hampson,
Annette Resenhoeft, Dani Sahner, Tara Scarponi, and Mike Zdunek.
Correspondence should be addressed to: Gary W. Lewandowski Jr., Department of Psychology,
Monmouth University, West Long Branch, NJ 07764, USA. E-mail: glewando@monmouth.edu
Self and Identity, 9: 416–433, 2010
http://www.psypress.com/sai
ISSN: 1529-8868 print/1529-8876 online
DOI: 10.1080/15298860903332191
Ó2009 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business
Relationship Satisfaction and Commitment
One of the most common methods of measuring relationship quality is the
investment model, which involves the related constructs of satisfaction, investment,
quality of alternatives, and commitment (Rusbult, 1983). These constructs correlate
with one another, but are distinct (Le & Agnew, 2003). Satisfaction involves an
individual’s evaluation of the relationship as a whole, the perceived positive or
negative affect that one experiences within the relationship, and overall how the
relationship gratifies the individual’s overall needs (Agnew, Van Lange, Rusbult, &
Langston, 1998; Rusbult, Johnson, & Morrow, 1986). In contrast, commitment
involves an affective attachment to one’s partner, a conative motivation to persist,
and a cognitive decision to remain within the relationship (Arriaga & Agnew, 2001).
Commitment also has been characterized as a long-term orientation and a perceived
obligation toward the relationship (Adams & Jones, 1997).
A wide variety of variables, including remaining within the relationship (Rusbult,
1983), trust (Rusbult, Martz, & Agnew, 1998), relationship closeness (Miller, 1997),
partner forgiveness (McCullough et al., 1998), and pro-relationship behaviors, such
as accommodation and willingness to sacrifice for the partner (Wieselquist, Rusbult,
Foster, & Agnew, 1999) have been associated with satisfaction and commitment.
Predictors related to the self have been associated with satisfaction and commit-
ment. Most notably, this has been in the context of the overlap between romantic
relationship partners’ self-concepts. One way this occurs is through cognitive inter-
dependence, whereby mental representations of partners become integrated into
their sense of self (Agnew et al., 1998). As partners’ cognitive interdependence
increases and they regard their partners as part of themselves, commitment increases
(Agnew et al., 1998). Cognitive interdependence has also been associated with the
experience of more positive emotions within the relationship (Van Lange & Rusbult,
1995).
One of the primary means of measuring cognitive interdependence is through
inclusion of other in the self. This is a form of relationship closeness that focuses on
the extent to which one’s self-concept is interconnected or overlaps one’s partner’s
self-concept (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). In cases of high inclusion of other in the
self, the overlap is so great that partners have a difficult time distinguishing between
their own traits and their partner’s traits (Mashek, Aron, & Boncimino, 2003). This
process results in the incorporation of a relationship partner’s characteristics within
one’s sense of self, thereby enhancing the self (Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991).
These changes to the self, known as self-expansion, produce gains in abilities,
resources, and perspectives that increase feelings of competency and self-efficacy
(Aron & Aron, 1996). Due to this process, inclusion of other in the self should have
positive implications for relationships. In fact, research has established that inclusion
of other in the self positively correlates with relationship satisfaction (Agnew et al.,
1998; Aron & Aron, 1996), commitment (Agnew et al., 1998), and relationship
longevity (Aron et al., 1992).
Self-concept Clarity
Previous research has established that the self-concept plays a role in the perception
of relationship quality (Aron & Aron, 1996). Measures of the self-concept remain
largely unexplored in the context of relationship quality. One such difference, self-
concept clarity, involves the level of clarity, consistency, stability, and confidence in
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 417
one’s self-concept (Campbell et al., 1996). Individuals with high self-concept clarity
have more consistent self-beliefs, are less likely to change their self-descriptions over
time or endorse mutually exclusive self-descriptive traits such as careless and careful
(Campbell, 1990).
Not surprisingly, self-concept clarity has benefits for psychological adjustment.
Differentiation of the self (i.e., low self-concept clarity), coincides with maladjust-
ment in the form of low self-esteem, high neuroticism and depression (Donahue,
Robins, Roberts, & John, 1993). Self-concept clarity also negatively correlates with
depression and anxiety (Bigler, Neimeyer, & Brown, 2001). Research that links an
individual’s self-concept clarity with romantic relationships is sparse. However, one
study examined how clearly a person viewed their partner’s self-concept and found
that greater clarity of the partner’s self predicted greater relationship quality
(Gurung, Sarason, & Sarason, 2001). Although not the focus of their study, these
researchers also found that self-concept clarity was positively correlated with dyadic
adjustment, a measure that focuses on satisfaction. Unfortunately, the reasons
for the association, and any potential association with commitment were left
unexamined.
Self-concept clarity’s positive influence on relationships is not automatically
evident since some research finds that self-concept clarity can relate to negative
personal outcomes. A longitudinal study found that individuals high in self-concept
clarity were more likely to be perfectionists who had inflexible and specific
requirements for the self (Campbell & Di Paula, 2002). Perfectionism has been
associated with increased stress, depression, unrealistically high self-standards, and
self-punishment when one has not achieved goals (Hewitt & Flett, 1993). This may
be especially problematic due to perfectionism’s negative correlation with dyadic
adjustment (Flett, Hewitt, Shapiro, & Rayman, 2003).
The structure or clarity of the self-concept can be independent of the content of
the self-concept, which may explain why high self-concept clarity is not a guarantee
of positive outcomes (Campbell, Assanand, & Di Paula, 2000). Whereas self-concept
clarity involves the cognitive component of the self, self-esteem focuses on self-views
based upon evaluations of the self (Rosenberg, 1965). For example, individuals could
hold very clear and highly articulated negative self-beliefs, or very unclear and
poorly articulated positive self-beliefs. Instead, self-concept clarity may produce
positive outcomes independent of self-evaluations.
The association between self-concept clarity and self-esteem has been well
established (Baumgardner, 1990; Bigler et al., 2001; Campbell, 1990; Campbell &
Lavallee, 1993; Campbell et al., 1996). Specifically, those with high self-concept
clarity derive a positive attitude toward the self from highly articulated beliefs
about the self (Campbell & Lavallee, 1993; Campbell et al., 1996). Empirical results
show that those with low self-esteem are less certain when describing their own
attributes (Baumgardner, 1990), less confident in their self-descriptions (Campbell,
1990), and have less stability in their ratings over time (Baumgardner, 1990;
Campbell, 1990).
Thus, the psychological benefit of self-concept clarity may lie within its
well-established relation to self-esteem. In fact, self-esteem positively correlates
with psychological well-being (e.g., Diener, 1984; Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg,
Kumashiro, & Rusbult, 2004), and with relationship satisfaction (e.g., Hansson,
Jones, & Carpenter, 1984; Rosenberg, 1965). In the context of relationships, there is
a strong positive correlation between self-esteem and relationship satisfaction
(Hendrick, Hendrick, & Adler, 1988). Further, low self-esteem coincides with
418 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
lower overall marital happiness (Hawkins & Booth, 2005), and a greater likelihood
of sabotaging one’s own relationship (Murray, Holmes, Griffin, Bellavia, & Rose,
2001).
The Present Study
A great deal of past research has focused on the predictors of relationship quality.
Some research has examined the role of the self in relationship satisfaction and
commitment as it relates to self-esteem (e.g., Hendrick et al., 1988), and cognitive
closeness (e.g., Agnew et al., 1998) in determining relationship quality. However, the
influence of self-concept clarity on relationships quality has been largely unexplored.
Specifically, we are not aware of any other study that focuses on one’s own self-
concept clarity in the context of relationship quality, nor could we identify any
study that examined self-concept clarity as it related to inclusion of other in the
self. We address the dearth of research in this area by examining the role of self-
concept clarity in relationship satisfaction and commitment through their direct
association, as well as the potentially mediating roles of self-esteem and inclusion of
other in the self.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Outcomes
The established role of the self-concept in romantic relationships suggests that
the self-concept could contribute to relationship quality (Aron & Aron, 1996).
Specifically, a construct such as self-concept clarity that involves stability of the
self-concept should benefit relationships. In light of this, we predicted that self-
concept clarity would positively correlate with relationship satisfaction and
commitment.
Mediation of Self-concept Clarity’s Association with Satisfaction and Commitment
In addition to predicting the association of self-concept clarity with relationship
satisfaction and commitment, we also suggested several mediators of the path.
Inclusion of other in the self. A person with high self-concept clarity should
experience greater relationship satisfaction and commitment, in part, because of
inclusion of other in the self. Individuals with greater self-concept clarity who hold a
clear and consistent view of the self should be more stable and should present the
self more clearly to others; characteristics that have been associated with lower levels
of neuroticism (Campbell et al., 1996). Further, research also suggests that partners
who are less neurotic (i.e., more stable) are more desirable (Figueredo, Sefcek, &
Nelson, 2006).
Inclusion of the other in the self is a largely dyadic process in which inclusion is
greater with a desirable partner (Aron & Aron, 1996). Higher self-concept clarity
may relate to greater inclusion of other in the self because a person with a clear and
consistent self-view can more judiciously seek out aspects of the partner to add to
their sense of self. By holding a clear view of the self, the high self-concept clarity
individual can avoid the situation where greater inclusion of other in the self
may result in loss of identity or loss of freedom (Mashek & Sherman, 2004). Instead
of threatening the self, self-expansion can occur whereby the included elements
can augment the self by increasing the individual’s sense of efficacy and ultimately
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 419
promoting relationship quality (Aron & Aron, 1996). We hypothesized that
self-concept clarity would positively correlate with inclusion of other in the
self and that inclusion of other in the self would mediate the association
between self-concept clarity and relationship satisfaction, as well as the association
between self-concept clarity and commitment.
Self-esteem. Until now, self-concept clarity has not been linked to relationship
quality. Past research has established a link between self-concept clarity and self-
esteem (e.g., Baumgardner, 1990), and has linked self-esteem with relationship
quality (e.g., Hansson et al., 1984). However, greater self-concept clarity may not
independently promote relationship satisfaction and commitment. For example,
regardless of how confidently they are held, if a person has negative self-esteem
relationship quality may suffer. For example, those with low self-esteem have a
tendency to undermine their relationship by misperceiving their partner’s love
(Murray et al., 2001). Thus, those who hold clear and certain views of the self should
have higher relationship satisfaction and commitment to the extent that the self-
views are positive. We hypothesized that self-concept clarity would positively
correlate with self-esteem and that self-esteem would mediate the association
between self-concept clarity and relationship satisfaction, as well as the association
between self-concept clarity and commitment.
Study 1
Method
Participants
Participants in this study consisted of a convenience sample of 199 undergraduates
(47 males, 147 females, 5 non-response) from the Psychology Department’s online
participant pool at a small private university in the Northeastern United States.
Ages ranged from 18 to 30 (M¼18.96). We did not collect information on year in
school. Of those indicating ethnicity, 88.4% were Caucasian, 1.0% Asian American,
2.5% African American, 5.0% Hispanic American, and 3.0% Other. Participants
were required to be in a current relationship, varying from dating exclusively
(88.9%), dating casually (9.0%), and engaged (2.0%). Mean relationship length was
approximately 19 months (range ¼1–81 months). Participants received course credit
for taking part in the study.
Measures
Groups of 4–8 participants completed a questionnaire packet that asked
about individual differences, their relationship, as well as a short demographic
questionnaire (gender, age, ethnicity, current relationship status, and relationship
length).
Self-concept clarity. A 12-item scale developed by Campbell et al. (1996) was
used measure the clarity and cohesiveness of the characteristics within the self-
concept. Self-concept clarity was measured on a scale of: strongly disagree (1),
disagree (2), neither agree nor disagree (3), agree (4), and strongly agree (5). Example
items included, ‘‘In general, I have a clear sense of who I am and what I am,’’ and
‘‘I seldom experience conflict between the different aspects of my personality.’’
Cronbach’s alpha ¼.86.
420 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
Self-esteem. This 10-item scale assesses participants’ global self esteem, or
satisfaction with the self (Rosenberg, 1965). Participants responded on a 4-point
scale: strongly disagree (1), disagree (2), agree (3), and strongly agree (4). Example
items included, ‘‘On the whole I am satisfied with myself,’’ and ‘‘I feel I’m a person of
worth, at least on an equal plane with others.’’ Cronbach’s alpha ¼.88.
Inclusion of other in self (IOS). This scale measures the cognitive overlap
between an individual and their partner (Aron et al., 1992). A series of increasingly
overlapping circles from two separate circles to two circles that overlap almost
completely captures this phenomenon. Participants were instructed to circle the
scenario that best described their relationship. In a sample in romantic relationships,
Aron et al. (1992) reported alternate-form reliability of .95 and test-reliability over
2 weeks of .85. The scale has demonstrated predictive and construct validity as a
general measure of closeness and has been widely used in relationship research.
Relationship satisfaction. This measure assesses participants’ perception of their
relationship (Rusbult et al., 1998). Example items included, ‘‘I feel satisfied with our
relationship’’, ‘‘My relationship is close to ideal’’, and ‘‘Our relationship makes me
very happy.’’ Participants were asked to: ‘‘Indicate how you feel for each item’’ on a
scale of 0 through 8, 0 being ‘‘do not agree at all’’ and 8 being ‘‘agree completely.’’
Cronbach’s alpha ¼.92.
Commitment. This measure assesses participants’ sense that the relationship will
continue indefinitely (Rusbult et al., 1998). Example items included, ‘‘I want our
relationship to last for a very long time’’, ‘‘I want our relationship to last forever’’,
and ‘‘I am committed to maintaining my relationship with my partner.’’ Participants
were asked to: ‘‘Indicate how you feel for each item’’ on a scale of 0 through 8, 0
being ‘‘do not agree at all’’ and 8 being ‘‘agree completely.’’ Cronbach’s alpha ¼.92.
Results and Discussion
Self-concept Clarity, Satisfaction, and Commitment
Means, standard deviations, and correlations among key variables are shown in
Table 1. As shown in the table, our hypotheses regarding self-concept clarity were
supported. There was a positive correlation between self-concept clarity and the
proposed mediators: self-esteem and IOS. As hypothesized, there was a significant
association between self-concept clarity and relationship satisfaction and commit-
ment such that those with high self-concept clarity were more likely to report higher
levels of relationship satisfaction and commitment. In addition, the proposed
mediators, inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem were significantly positively
correlated with satisfaction and commitment.
Mediation of Self-concept Clarity’s Association with Satisfaction and Commitment
In order to test whether we had met the conditions for mediation as delineated by
Baron and Kenny (1986), we conducted a series of three mediation analyses to
determine if the relation of self-concept clarity to relationship satisfaction and
commitment was mediated by inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem. As seen
in Table 1, those with high self-concept clarity are more likely to report greater
relationship satisfaction and commitment. Further, self-concept clarity was
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 421
significantly associated with the hypothesized mediators. These steps were consistent
through each of the following mediation analyses.
Inclusion of other in the self. As seen in Figure 1, the beta between inclusion of
other in the self and relationship satisfaction was .41, t(194) ¼6.36, p5.001,
sr
2
¼.42. The beta for self-concept clarity in this regression was reduced to .21, and
remained significant, t(194) ¼3.26, p¼.001, sr
2
¼.23. This supports the hypothesis
that inclusion of other in the self mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on relationship
satisfaction.
A parallel analysis with commitment revealed that the beta between inclusion of
other in the self and commitment was .51, t(194) ¼8.38, p5.001, sr
2
¼.52. The beta
for self-concept clarity in this regression was reduced to .12, and remained
TABLE 1 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations among Key Variables (Study 1)
12345
1. Self-concept
clarity
3.51 (0.68)
2. Inclusion of
other in self
.17* 4.86 (1.57)
3. Self-esteem .62** .17* 3.31 (0.48)
4. Satisfaction .28** .44** .32** 6.28 (1.49)
5. Commitment .21** .53** .19** .64** 6.67 (1.63)
Notes:N¼198–99. Higher scores indicate a greater magnitude of each variable. Means and
standard deviations appear on the diagonal. *p5.05; **p5.01.
FIGURE 1 Mediation of self-concept clarity’s association with satisfaction and
commitment by inclusion of other in the self (Study 1). Reported values are
standardized betas. The betas in parentheses represent the direct effects of self-
concept clarity on satisfaction (or commitment). The betas not in parentheses
represent the association between self-concept clarity and satisfaction (or commit-
ment) controlling for the mediator. *p5.05; **p5.01; ***p5.001.
422 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
significant, t(194) ¼1.96, p¼.052, sr
2
¼.14. This supports the hypothesis that
inclusion of other in the self mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on commitment.
Self-esteem. As seen in Figure 2, the beta between self-esteem and relationship
satisfaction was .24, t(195) ¼2.76, p¼.01, sr
2
¼.19. The beta for self-concept clarity
in this regression was reduced to .13, and was not significant, t(194) ¼1.50, p¼.14,
sr
2
¼.11. This supports the hypothesis that self-esteem mediates self-concept clarity’s
effect on relationship satisfaction.
A parallel analysis with commitment revealed that, contrary to the hypothesis, the
beta between self-esteem and commitment was .11, t(195) ¼1.20, p¼.23, sr
2
¼.09.
The beta for self-concept clarity in this regression was .14, and was not significant,
t(195) ¼1.57, p¼.12, sr
2
¼.11. This fails to support the hypothesis that self-esteem
mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on commitment.
Study 2
In Study 2, we sought to replicate the basic results from Study 1; that self-concept
clarity relates to relationship satisfaction and commitment. We hoped to extend
these findings by experimentally manipulating participants’ self-concept clarity to
determine its influence on satisfaction and commitment.
Method
Participants
Participants in this study consisted of a convenience sample of 78 undergraduates
(21 males, 57 females) from an upper level psychology course at a large public
FIGURE 2 Mediation of self-concept clarity’s association with satisfaction and
commitment by self-esteem (Study 1). Reported values are standardized betas. The
betas in parentheses represent the direct effects of self-concept clarity on satisfaction
(or commitment). The betas not in parentheses represent the association between
self-concept clarity and satisfaction (or commitment) controlling for the mediator.
*p5.05; **p5.01; ***p5.001.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 423
university in the Northeastern United States. Ages ranged from 18 to 36 (M¼21.69).
Of the sample, 2.5% were college freshman, 11.4% were sophomores, 55.7% were
college juniors, college seniors comprised 22.8%, and 6.3% were in their fifth year of
college. Of those indicating ethnicity, 51.9% were Caucasian, 25.3% were Asian,
5.1% were Hispanic or Latino, 3.8% were African American, and 12.7% were
Other. All participants were in a current romantic relationship. Most of the sample
were dating exclusively (83.6%), 8.9% were dating casually, 2.5% were engaged,
1.3% were married, and 2.5% indicated Other. Mean relationship length was
approximately 25 months (range ¼1–76).
Measures
Participants completed a questionnaire packet that included a self-concept
questionnaire (Setterlund & Niedenthal, 1993), measures of relationship satisfaction
and commitment, self-concept clarity, self-esteem, and inclusion of other in the self.
Participants also completed a short demographic questionnaire parallel to Study 1.
Self-concept questionnaire. Participants rated 30 personality traits based on how
much each trait was descriptive of the self (Setterlund & Niedenthal, 1993).
Participants made ratings on a 7-point Likert scale (1 ¼does not describe,
7¼describes very well). Example traits included, ‘‘open-minded,’’ ‘‘friendly,’’
‘‘logical,’’ and ‘‘insecure.’’
Relationship satisfaction. This was identical to the measure used in Study 1
(Rusbult et al., 1998). Alpha in current study ¼.92.
Relationship commitment. This was identical to the measure used in Study 1
(Rusbult et al., 1998). Alpha in current study ¼.83.
Self-concept clarity. This was identical to the measure used in Study 1 (Campbell
et al., 1996). Alpha in current study ¼.97.
Self-esteem. This was identical to the measure used in Study 1 (Rosenberg,
1965). Alpha in current study ¼.92.
Inclusion of other in self. This was identical to the measure used in Study 1 (Aron
et al., 1992).
Design and Procedure
Participants in a large lecture hall class engaged in a randomly assigned self-concept
manipulation, and the completed follow-up measures. The self-concept manipula-
tion followed the procedure described by Setterlund and Niedenthal (1993). At the
beginning of class, participants completed the self-concept questionnaire. Afterward,
a research assistant placed trait adjectives in the Part 2 questionnaire packet labeled
Condition 1 (self-concept confusion) or Condition 2 (self-concept clarity) and
returned the packets to the participants. Participants in the self-concept confusion
condition received three adjectives in Part 2 that that they indicated were not
descriptive of them in Part 1. Participants in the self-concept clarity condition
received three adjectives in Part 2 that they indicated were very descriptive of them in
Part 1. At the end of class (approximately 2 hours later), all participants described
three times when they displayed behaviors relevant to each of the adjectives in
424 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
their packet. For example, in the self-concept clarity condition, if a participant
indicated that they were highly open-minded, they described three times they acted
open-minded. Upon completion of the self-concept clarity manipulation, partici-
pants completed measures of satisfaction, commitment, self-esteem, and inclusion of
other in the self.
Results and Discussion
Manipulation Check
To determine the self-concept manipulation’s efficacy in creating differences in self-
concept clarity, we conducted a t-test for independent means comparing the self-
concept confusion and self-concept clarity confusion conditions on a follow-up
measure of self-concept clarity. Means and standard deviations for the two
experimental conditions were: self-concept confusion (M¼2.71, SD ¼1.07), self-
concept clarity (M¼4.01, SD ¼0.87). The analysis was significant and had a
moderate effect size; t(76) ¼5.89, p5.001, effect size (Z
p2
)¼.31, such that those in
clarity condition reported higher self-concept clarity compared to those in the
confusion condition.
Self-concept Clarity, Satisfaction, and Commitment
Means, standard deviations, and correlations for key variables are shown in Table 2.
Satisfaction. We conducted a regression analysis in which the dependent vari-
able was relationship satisfaction and the independent variable was experimental
condition (dummy coded with 1 ¼clarity and 0 ¼confusion). The regression yielded
a beta of .32, t(76) ¼9.85, p5.001, effect size (Z
p2
)¼.32, indicating that those
with high self-concept clarity are more likely to report greater relationship
satisfaction. Relationship satisfaction was higher for the self-concept clarity
condition (M¼5.94, SD ¼0.96), than for the self-concept confusion condition
(M¼5.13, SD ¼1.44).
Commitment. We conducted a regression analysis in which the dependent
variable was relationship commitment, and the independent variable experimental
condition. The results yielded a beta of .28, t(76) ¼9.96, p5.001, effect size
TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations among Key Variables (Study 2)
12345
1. Self-concept
clarity
1.51 (0.50)
2. Inclusion of
other in self
.23* 4.85 (1.35)
3. Self-esteem .39*** .22* 3.16 (0.71)
4. Satisfaction .32** .45*** .66*** 5.52 (1.29)
5. Commitment .41*** .41*** .53*** .85*** 5.75 (1.35)
Notes:N¼78. Higher scores indicate a greater magnitude of each variable. Means and
standard deviations appear on the diagonal. Self-concept clarity in this study represents the
experimental condition of self-concept clarity vs. confusion. *p5.05; **p5.01; ***p5.001.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 425
(Z
p2
)¼.28, indicating that those with high self-concept clarity are more likely to
report greater relationship commitment. Relationship commitment was higher for
the self-concept clarity condition (M¼6.15, SD ¼0.89), than for the self-concept
confusion condition (M¼5.38, SD ¼1.61).
Mediation of Self-concept Clarity’s Association with Satisfaction and Commitment
In order to test whether we had met the conditions for mediation as delineated by
Baron and Kenny (1986), we conducted a series of three mediation analyses to
determine if the relation of experimental condition (self-concept clarity vs. self-
concept confusion) to relationship satisfaction and commitment was mediated by
inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem. As seen in Table 2, those with high self-
concept clarity are more likely to report greater relationship satisfaction and
commitment. Further, self-concept clarity was significantly associated with the
hypothesized mediators. These steps were consistent through each of the following
mediation analyses.
Inclusion of other in self. As seen in Figure 3, the beta between inclusion of
other in the self and relationship satisfaction was .40, t(75) ¼3.91, p¼.00, sr
2
¼.15.
The beta for self-concept clarity in this regression was reduced to .23, and
remained significant, t(75) ¼2.22, p¼.03, sr
2
¼.05. This supports the hypothesis
that inclusion of other in the self mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on relation-
ship satisfaction.
A parallel analysis with commitment revealed that the beta between inclusion of
other in the self and relationship commitment was .36, t(75) ¼3.42, p5.001,
sr
2
¼.12. The beta for self-concept clarity in this regression was reduced to .20, and
was no longer significant, t(75) ¼1.91, p¼.06, sr
2
¼.03. This supports the hypothesis
FIGURE 3 Mediation of self-concept clarity’s association with satisfaction and
commitment by inclusion of other in the self (Study 2). Reported values are
standardized betas. The betas in parentheses represent the direct effects of self-
concept clarity on satisfaction (or commitment). The betas not in parentheses
represent the association between self-concept clarity and satisfaction (or commit-
ment) controlling for the mediator. *p5.05; **p5.01; ***p5.001.
426 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
that inclusion of other in the self mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on relationship
commitment.
Self-esteem. As seen in Figure 4, the beta between self-esteem and relationship
satisfaction was .63, t(75) ¼6.74, p5.001, sr
2
¼.34. The beta for self-concept clarity
in this regression was reduced to .07, and was no longer significant, t(75) ¼0.78,
p¼.24, sr
2
¼.004. This supports the hypothesis that self-esteem mediates self-
concept clarity’s effect on relationship satisfaction.
A parallel analysis with commitment revealed that the beta between self-esteem
and relationship commitment was .49, t(75) ¼4.64, p5.001, sr
2
¼.20. The beta for
self-concept clarity in this regression was reduced to .09, and was no longer
significant, t(75) ¼0.88, p¼.38, sr
2
¼.006. This mediation supports the hypothesis
that self-esteem mediates self-concept clarity’s effect on relationship commitment.
General Discussion
The purpose of this set of studies was to examine the role of self-concept clarity in
relationship satisfaction and commitment through their direct associations, as well as
the potential mediating roles of self-esteem and inclusion of other in the self.
As hypothesized, in Study 1 self-concept clarity was positively correlated with
relationship satisfaction and commitment. In Study 2, those who engaged in a self-
concept clarity manipulation subsequently reported higher relationship satisfaction
and commitment compared to those in the self-concept confusion condition.
Inclusion of other in the self and self-esteem (the only exception was for self-esteem
and commitment in Study 1) mediated the association between self-concept clarity
and relationship quality measures. Taken together, these findings suggest that a
person’s self-concept clarity positively influences relationship satisfaction and
commitment.
FIGURE 4 Mediation of self-concept clarity’s association with satisfaction and
commitment by self-esteem (Study 2). Reported values are standardized betas. The
betas in parentheses represent the direct effects of self-concept clarity on satisfaction
(or commitment). The betas not in parentheses represent the association between
self-concept clarity and satisfaction (or commitment) controlling for the mediator.
*p5.05; **p5.01; ***p5.001.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 427
Self-concept Clarity, Satisfaction, and Commitment
Previous research has established that individuals’ self-concepts change due to
the relationships that they form with close others (Aron, Paris, & Aron, 1995;
Leary & Baumeister, 2000). The present findings suggest that individual differences
(Study 1) and manipulated states (Study 2) in self-concept clarity influence
relationship satisfaction and commitment. Though not the focus of the study,
research has identified a positive correlation between self-concept clarity and dyadic
adjustment (Gurung et al., 2001). The present findings replicate those results for
relationship satisfaction in a new measure, and extend the results by establishing a
positive correlation between self-concept clarity and commitment.
Study 1, however, cannot rule out the possibility that greater relationship
satisfaction and commitment promote greater self-concept clarity. Study 2’s
experimental manipulation of self-concept clarity and its subsequent influence on
relationship satisfaction and commitment helped establish the directionality of the
association. Self-concept clarity benefits may be due to an added sense of stability in
the relationship. Previous research suggested that greater clarity about a partner’s
self-concept benefits relationships (Gurung et al., 2001). In the present context,
greater self-concept clarity should make it easier for one’s partner to have greater
clarity about the self, resulting in greater stability and relationship quality. For
example, individuals with low self-concept clarity, who have uncertainty about
their own traits, may be unable to reliably share information about the self with
their partner. Inconsistency, particularly in the form of self-disclosure, may inhibit
relationship development and lead to lower satisfaction and commitment (Altman &
Taylor, 1973).
The present findings also establish that self-concept clarity has a positive influence
on relationships. This contrasts with previous research that linked self-concept
clarity to perfectionism (Hewitt & Flett, 1993), and perfectionism to less satisfaction
(Flett et al., 2001). However, the present findings regarding the benefits of self-
concept clarity for romantic relationships extend previous research on the general
benefits of self-concept clarity (e.g., Donahue et al., 1993).
Mediation of Self-concept Clarity’s Association with Satisfaction and Commitment
In addition to establishing the basic association between self-concept clarity and
relationship satisfaction and commitment, we also found support for inclusion of
other in the self as a mediator. Both studies supported our hypothesis that higher
self-concept clarity was associated with higher inclusion of other in the self.
This suggests that a person’s ability to clearly describe the self in a consistent and
stable fashion may facilitate inclusion of others in the self. The present findings also
argue against the possibility that those who have lower self-concept clarity would
have higher inclusion of other in the self as a means of counteracting their own
uncertainty regarding the self.
Another possibility is that greater self-concept clarity may promote greater
inclusion of other in the self because a clear self provides a more stable frame of
reference for interacting with and assimilating the external environment. This would
allow individuals with greater self-concept clarity to incorporate aspects of the other
in the self without subsequent self-confusion or loss of identity. While it was not
possible to test this possibility in the present study, future research should explore
this possibility in greater detail.
428 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
Inclusion of other in the self’s role as mediator is not surprising given that it
represents how relationship partners’ self-concepts relate to one another; an
experience that positively correlates with relationship satisfaction and commitment
(Agnew et al., 1998). This is also consistent with previous research showing that
cognitive closeness based on greater clarity of the partner’s self-concept positively
correlated with satisfaction and commitment (Steiner-Pappalardo & Gurung, 2002).
We also expected self-esteem to mediate the association between self-concept
clarity and relationship satisfaction and commitment. In Study 1 and Study 2, self-
esteem fully mediated self-concept clarity’s association with relationship satisfaction.
The nature of the mediation suggests that self-esteem is a more important influence
on relationship satisfaction than self-concept clarity. The evaluative nature of
self-esteem may help explain its importance for relationship satisfaction. Self-esteem
involves a positive or negative assessment of the self. Similarly, relationship satis-
faction involves a positive or negative assessment of the relationship. Some have
suggested that an overall positive view of oneself can lead to a positive view of the
relationship that should increase satisfaction (Gagne & Lydon, 2004). Those
with high self-concept clarity may feel positively about the relationship to the
extent they feel positively about the self. These findings extend past research
suggesting that self-concept clarity coincides with high self-esteem (Baumgardner,
1990) and that high self-esteem coincides with relationship satisfaction (Hendrick
et al., 1988).
Self-esteem’s hypothesized mediation of the link between self-concept clarity and
commitment was supported in Study 2, but not in Study 1. The different results may
be due to differences in the nature of self-esteem (trait vs. state) between the studies.
Study 1 measured trait self-esteem using an individual-difference measure that
asked participants to generalize from many different experiences. In Study 2, the
manipulation was focused on the participant’s present state, rather than generalized
feelings. This may have increased the salience of the participant’s self-esteem,
resulting in a greater influence on commitment. In fact, the correlation between self-
esteem and commitment in Study 1 was (r¼.19, p5.01) while in Study 2 it was
(r¼.53, p5.001).
On the other hand, the positive feelings associated with high self-esteem could
lead to more commitment as a means of promoting additional positive feelings
through the relationship (Stafford, 2003). This seems especially likely following the
Study 2 manipulation where participants listed and reflected upon mostly positive
traits (all but two of the traits were positive) that were either descriptive or not
descriptive of the self (Setterlund & Niedenthal, 1993). As a result, we cannot rule
out the possibility that the procedure may have manipulated self-esteem along with
concept clarity. However, the established connection between self-concept clarity
and self-esteem suggests the inherent difficulty in clearly distinguishing the two
constructs (e.g., Baumgardner, 1990; Campbell, 1990). Future research should focus
on self-concept clarity manipulations that are more independent from self-esteem.
Strength and Limitations
The main strength of this set of studies was the novel approach to understanding
relationship satisfaction and commitment by incorporating individual differences
and manipulated states related to the self-concept. To our knowledge, this is the
first set of studies to systematically examine how a person’s own self-concept clarity
relates to their romantic relationship. In addition, this research helps elucidate
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 429
the role of self-concept clarity by examining potential mediators. Although the list
of potential mediators is numerous, and we by no means consider our examina-
tion exhaustive, our study tests two mediators that come from strong theoretical
backgrounds.
A few limitations of the study should also be noted. First, the use of a college
sample may not allow the results to generalize to married relationships. In fact,
the additional investments (e.g., combined finances, children, etc.) associated with
marriage should increase commitment (Rusbult, 1983) and could reduce the influence
of self-concept clarity, relative to the findings from the present sample. In addition,
early in relationships the self-concept undergoes many changes due to the relation-
ship’s novelty (Aron et al., 1991). However, more established married couples may
experience fewer reasons for self change, making the stability of the self-concept less
important. Second, the present samples were predominantly female. Although there
were more females, we did have a sizable amount of males (n¼70 across two studies).
Further, this distribution is comparable to previously published work in this area
(e.g., Agnew et al., 1998; Campbell et al., 1996). Finally, although fairly common in
research on relationship satisfaction and commitment (e.g., Agnew et al., 1998), and
in previous research dealing with self-concept clarity (e.g., Campbell et al., 1996), the
studies rely on self-report to measure relationship quality, self-esteem, and self-
concept clarity (in Study 1). It is possible that participant’s provided a biased view of
their self-concept and relationship. However, other research suggests that self-report
is common in personality work (Jones & Nisbett, 1987) partly because the participant
is in a privileged position to know about their own self-concept (Hofstee, 1994).
Future Directions and Implications
These studies represent an important first step toward understanding the role of self-
concept clarity in romantic relationships making it incumbent upon future research
to untangle the mechanisms through which self-concept clarity benefits relationships.
For example, Study 1’s findings suggest the possibility that those with greater self-
concept clarity do a better job of selecting relationship partners, and that this skilled
selection may lead to greater relationship quality. In both studies, it is also possible
that the accuracy of self-concept information may moderate self-concept clarity’s
benefits such that high self-concept clarity is most helpful when it is accurate. For
example, holding a very clear, but unfounded, view of the self would seem to inhibit
the quality of one’s relationships.
This present set of studies represents the first known link between self-concept
clarity and inclusion of other in the self and, based on the present results, bears
further examination. For example, inclusion of other in the self is generally
considered a positive experience leading to gains in the self (Aron et al., 1992). For
those with high self-concept clarity this seems likely. However, for those with low
self-concept clarity, the process of inclusion of other in the self may result in taking
on too many of the partner’s qualities, and ultimately may result in a loss of one’s
own self.
The positive influence of self-concept clarity on romantic relationships has
potential implications for therapeutic contexts. For those experiencing low relation-
ship satisfaction and commitment, therapists could use techniques to help clients
improve self-concept clarity perhaps through introspection (Hixon & Swann,
1993), or through an exploration of other sources of self-knowledge (Sedikides &
Skowronski, 1995).
430 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
Conclusion
The purpose of these studies was to explore how self-concept clarity influences
relationship satisfaction and commitment. Results indicate that greater self-concept
clarity coincides with greater relationship satisfaction and commitment. Also,
consistent with predictions, these associations were mediated by inclusion of other in
the self, and, in the case of relationship satisfaction, self-esteem. These results
demonstrate how a person’s self-concept clarity plays a role in the quality of their
romantic relationship.
References
Adams, J. M., & Jones, W. H. (1997). The conceptualization of marital commitment: An
integrative analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,72, 1177–1196.
Agnew, C. R., Van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., & Langston, C. A. (1998). Cognitive
interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation of close relationships.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74, 939–954.
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal
relationships. New York: Holt.
Aron, A. (2003). Self and close relationships. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.),
Handbook of self and identity (pp. 442–461). New York: Guilford Press.
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1996). Love and the expansion of the self: The state of the model.
Personal Relationships,3, 45–58.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the
structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,63,
596–612.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Close relationships as including other
in the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,60, 241–253.
Aron, A., Paris, M., & Aron, E. N. (1995). Falling in love: Prospective studies of self-concept
change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69, 1102–1112.
Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Being committed: Affective, cognitive, and conative
components of relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,27,
1190–1203.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,51, 1173–1182.
Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself: Self-certainty and self-affect.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58, 1062–1072.
Bigler, M., Neimeyer, G., & Brown, E. (2001). The divided self revisited: Effects of self-
concept clarity and self-concept differentiation on psychological adjustment. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology,20, 396–415.
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,59, 538–549.
Campbell, J. D., Assanand, S., & Di Paula, A. (2000). Structural features of the self-concept
and adjustment. In A. Tesser, R. Felson, & J. Suls (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on
self and identity (pp. 67–87). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Campbell, J. D., & Di Paula, A. (2002). Perfectionistic self-beliefs: Their relation to
personality and goal pursuit. In G. L. Gordon & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism:
Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 181–198). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Campbell, J. D., & Lavallee, L. F. (1993). Who am I? The role of self-concept confusion in
understanding the behavior of people with low self-esteem. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.),
Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 3–20). New York: Plenum Press.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 431
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Lehmann, D. R.
(1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates and cultural boundaries.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,70, 141–156.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin,95, 542–575.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science,13, 81–84.
Donahue, E. M., Robins, R. W., Roberts, B. W., & John, O. P. (1993). The divided
self: Concurrent and longitudinal effects of psychological adjustment and social
roles on self-concept differentiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,64,
834–846.
Figueredo, A. J., Sefcek, J. A., & Nelson, D. (2006). The ideal romantic partner personality.
Personality and Individual Differences,41, 431–441.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Shapiro, B., & Rayman, J. (2003). Perfectionism, beliefs, and
adjustment in dating relationships. In N. J. Pallone (Ed.), Love, romance, sexual
attraction: Research perspectives from current psychology (pp. 31–60). New Brunswick,
NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Gagne, F., & Lydon, J. (2004). Bias and accuracy in close relationships: An integrative review.
Personality and Social Psychology Review,8, 322–338.
Gurung, R. A. R., Sarason, B. R., & Sarason, I. G. (2001). Predicting relationship quality and
emotional reactions to stress from significant-other-concept clarity. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,27, 1267–1276.
Hansson, R. O., Jones, W. H., & Carpenter, B. N. (1984). Relational competence and social
support. Review of Personality and Social Psychology,5, 265–284.
Hawkins, D. N., & Booth, A. (2005). Unhappily ever after: Effects of long-term, low-quality
marriages on well-being. Social Forces,84, 451–471.
Hendrick, S. S., Hendrick, C., & Adler, N. L. (1988). Romantic relationships: Love,
satisfaction, and staying together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,54, 980–
988.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1993). Dimensions of perfectionism, daily stress, and depression:
A test of the specific vulnerability hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,102,
58–65.
Hixon, J. G., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (1993). When does introspection bear fruit? Self-reflection,
self-insight, and interpersonal choices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,64,
35–43.
Hofstee, W. K. B. (1994). Who should own the definition of personality? European Journal of
Personality,8, 149–162.
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1987). The actor and observer: Divergent perceptions of the
causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Vallins, &
B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79–94). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Le, B., & Agnew, C. R. (2003). Commitment and its theorized determinants: A meta-analysis
of the investment model. Personal Relationships,10, 37–57.
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer
theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–62). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Leary, M. R., & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of self and identity. New York:
Guilford Press.
Mashek, D. J., Aron, A., & Boncimino, M. (2003). Confusions of self with close others.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,9, 382–392.
Mashek, D. J., & Sherman, M. (2004). Desiring less closeness with intimate others. In D.
Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 343–356). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
McCullough, M., Rachal, K., Sandage, S., Worthington, E., Brown, S., & Hight, T. (1998).
Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships II: Theoretical elaboration and measure-
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75, 1586–1603.
432 G. W. Lewandowski et al.
Miller, R. S. (1997). Inattentive and contented: Relationship commitment and attention to
alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,73, 758–766.
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Griffin, D. W., Bellavia, G. M., & Rose, P. (2001). The
mismeasure of love: How self-doubt contaminates relationship beliefs. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,27, 423–436.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development (and
deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,45, 101–117.
Rusbult, C. E., Johnson, D. J., & Morrow, G. D. (1986). Predicting satisfaction and
commitment in adult romantic involvements: An assessment of the generalizability of the
investment model. Social Psychology Quarterly,49, 81–89.
Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The investment model scale: Measuring
commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size. Personal
Relationships,5, 357–391.
Sedikides, C., Rudich, E. A., Gregg, A. P., Kumashiro, M., & Rusbult, C. (2004). Are normal
narcissists psychologically healthy? Self-esteem matters. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,87, 400–416.
Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1995). On the sources of self-knowledge: The perceived
primacy of self-reflection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,14, 244–270.
Setterlund, M. B., & Niedenthal, P. M. (1993). ‘‘Who am I? Why am I here?’’ Self-esteem,
self-clarity, and prototype matching. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65,
769–780.
Stafford, L. (2003). Maintaining romantic relationships: A summary and analysis of one
research program. In D. J. Canary & M. Dainton (Eds.), Maintaining relationships
through communication: Relational, contextual, and cultural variations (pp. 58–59).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Steiner-Pappalardo, N. L., & Gurung, R. A. R. (2002). The femininity effect: Relationship
quality, sex, gender, attachment, and significant-other concepts. Personal Relationships,9,
313–325.
Van Lange, P. A. M., & Rusbult, C. E. (1995). My relationship is better than—and not as bad
as—yours is: The perception of superiority in close relationships. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin,21, 32–44.
Wieselquist, J., Rusbult, C. E., Foster, C. A., & Agnew, C. R. (1999). Commitment, pro-
relationship behavior, and trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,77, 942–966.
Self-concept Clarity and Relationship Quality 433
Copyright of Self & Identity is the property of Psychology Press (UK) and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
... Understanding how internal stress can affect partner's couple identity, in addition to their life and couple satisfaction, is fundamental and in that, creating a sense of shared identity is a key step in the development of a close, intimate relationship (Reid et al., 2006). Indeed, partners' level of couple identity was found to be associated with positive relationship outcomes (e.g., Acitelli et al., 1999;Aron et al., 1992;Lewandowski Jr et al., 2010). Considering the negative associations between experience of internal stress (which is inevitable) and well-being for both partners (e.g., Andrews et al., 1991;El-Sheikh et al., 2013;Uhlich et al., 2022), it is important to identify those factors that may buffer against the detrimental effects of internal stress in daily life, specifically how partners cope with external stress (dyadic coping) and resolve the problems generating internal stress experiences. ...
... Partners' inability to empathize and relate with one another may as well reduce their sense of unity and harmony. Given the relevance of building a strong sense of couple identity for relationship quality (e.g., Lewandowski Jr et al., 2010;Pagani et al., 2020;Parise et al., 2015;Parise et al., 2019), relationship stability (Aron et al., 1992), and commitment (e.g., Lewandowski Jr et al., 2010), evidence on how internal stress may interfere with couple identity has important implications. Before discussing implications of these findings, however, we need to note that it is also possible, given the correlational nature of our data, that partners' feeling of dissatisfaction for their life and relationship, as well as a diminished sense of couple identity, could be manifested in the emergence of more intense tensions and disagreements within the relationship. ...
Article
Couples' experiences of daily stress can be detrimental for partners' individual and relational well‐being, specifically their identity as a couple, their relational satisfaction, and their life satisfaction. Grounded in the Systemic Transactional Model, this study aimed at analyzing factors that may safeguard partners and their relationship from detrimental effects of internal stress (i.e., stress that originates inside the relationship). We examined the buffering effect of partners' positive dyadic coping and internal problem resolution. Daily diary data were collected across 7 days from 82 heterosexual couples. Multilevel dyadic analyses showed that internal stress was negatively associated with partners' individual and relational well‐being. Positive dyadic coping moderated the association between partners' internal stress and couple satisfaction for both partners, but not life satisfaction and couple identity. Moreover, for partners who reported a resolution to the internal problem, the negative associations of internal stress with life satisfaction, couple satisfaction, and couple identity were significantly lower than for those who did not resolve the internal problem. This study confirms the negative role of internal stress on well‐being, shows the associations between internal stress and couple identity, and highlights the protective role of dyadic coping and internal problem resolution in couples' daily lives.
... In some cultures, spirituality has been defined as separating nature from religion (Moshtagh, 2019). Spirituality positively affects the quality of life and feeling of satisfaction by offering hope and reducing anxiety and depression (Sharif Nia et al., 2018), as this helps individuals manage their needs, and provides positive attitudes toward challenging experiences or distressing situations (Lewandowski et al., 2010). Furthermore, beliefs, values, and spiritual behaviors also affect health through physiological and biochemical pathways (Soleimani et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Since spiritual well-being has a significant role in individual and social health, using a valid scale to identify these properties is essential. Comparing factor structure and differences in the number of dimensions and items of subscales could be an indicator of differences in individuals' attitudes toward spirituality among diverse cultures. The present review was performed for psychometric evaluation of the spiritual well-being measures. A systematic review of international databases and Iranian databases was conducted to evaluate studies published between January 1, 1970, and October 1, 2022. QUADAS-2, STARD, and COSMIN scales were used for risk of bias assessment. Following two rounds of screening, 14 articles entered quality assessment. According to the results, studies examining the factor structure of the spiritual well-being scale (SWBS) instrument have been conducted for the years 1998 to 2022. The mean age of the participants in these studies ranged from 20.8 to 79.08 years. During the process of exploratory factor analysis, the researchers reported the presence of two to five latent factors, and the range of explained variance was between 35.6 and 71.4%. However, most of the reports indicated the existence of two or three latent factors. The findings of the present study provide an image of the psychometric status of the SWBS for researchers and clinicians in this field and can help them make optimum decisions in selecting a scale or conducting additional psychometric studies or adopting this scale for studies in new populations.
... También se obtuvieron evidencias de validez convergente de la ECSA al encontrar una relación positiva con la EAR. Aunque se ha discutido y observado que la autoestima y la CA pueden tener una relación curvilínea (Lewandowski et al., 2010), esto parece ser más una excepción dado que la mayoría de los estudios transversales (Kawamoto, 2020;Morawiak et al., 2018;Wong et al., 2019) y longitudinales (Schiller et al., 2016;Yang y Brown, 2016) han encontrado un vínculo positivo y moderado (0.50-0.70) entre estas dos variables. Si una persona tiene una visión positiva de sí, es más probable que esté segura de quién es, se perciba como congruente y estable, y medite acerca de sí. ...
Article
Full-text available
Los propósitos del presente estudio fueron construir una Escala de Claridad Subjetiva del Autoconcepto para adultos, y aportar evidencias de su configuración y red nomológica. Participaron 697 voluntarios de entre 18 y 60 años, residentes de la Ciudad de México y área metropolitana. El instrumento se construyó basándose en dos estudios exploratorios y en un modelo que propone que la claridad del autoconcepto (CA) se compone por la certeza, congruencia y estabilidad de las autodescripciones. Con base en la realización de análisis factoriales exploratorios y de consistencia interna, se obtuvo un instrumento conformado por 18 reactivos, distribuidos en cuatro factores que explican el 49% de la varianza, con una Alfa de Cronbach de .84. y con evidencias de validez convergente al estar relacionado con la autoestima. En conclusión, se aporta un instrumento con evidencias de validez y confiabilidad para medir la CA en México, el cual permitirá conocer el papel de este constructo para el bienestar y salud mental de los adultos.
... The mediating role of self-concept clarity in the relationship between parent and child or peer alienation and life satisfaction Self-concept clarity refers to the cognitive evaluation of oneself 's abilities and weaknesses (Lewandowski et al., 2009). When the real self is inconsistent with the ideal self, the individual's wellbeing maybe reduced (Nelson, 1984). ...
Article
Full-text available
The research on life satisfaction originated in the 1960s and has not been completely studied yet. Life satisfaction is an index related to the state and quality of individual life. With the development of society, the relevant variables affecting life satisfaction have also changed with the times. The purpose of this study is to research the relationship between parent and child or peer alienation, mental resilience, self-concept clarity and life satisfaction, finding the mechanism of action among parent-child or peer alienation, mental resilience, self-concept clarity, and life satisfaction. This cross-sectional study recruites randomly 1,347 adolescents from six middle schools in Chongqing, China, participating in a questionnaire on sociodemographic characteristics, the Inventor of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA), the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC-10), the Self-concept Clarity Scale (SCCS), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS). The sample consists of 62.4% female participants ( n = 841) and 37.6% male participants ( n = 506) aged from 11 to 17 years old (Mean = 14.54, SD = 1.21). We use SPSS 26 to perform the statistical analysis. The study finds that mental resilience—self-concept clarity have mediating effect on the parent or peer alienation to life satisfaction, to the effect that, parent-child or peer alienation explain life satisfaction through the chain mediating effect of mental resilience—self-concept clarity. This study explores the negative multi-use of parent-child or peer alienation on life satisfaction and provides a new perspective for the improvement of life satisfaction of adolescents.
... In other words, this might imply that the good emotional regulation and stress coping skills developed by participants with a high level of self-concept clarity allowed them to experience a balanced portion of positive and negative emotions during the MCO, which then result in them feeling more satisfied with the conditions of their life during the COVID-19 pandemic [12]. This is in line with aforementioned past studies, as high levels of self-concept clarity were found to enable individuals to develop good emotional regulation and stress coping skills through their high levels of self-awareness, hence reducing their frequency of experiencing negative emotions [17], [20], [21]. Subsequently, the positive emotions experienced by these individuals will then play a role in facilitating the stress coping mechanisms within them, thus allowing them to deal with stressful events during the MCO effectively and have a higher level of life satisfaction [28], [29]. ...
Article
Full-text available
As one significant indicator of mental wellbeing, satisfaction with life plays important roles in the society and among individuals. Nevertheless, studies have stated that the formation of life satisfaction was different between before and during the pandemic. Among most people, the differences were caused by the implication of social distancing protocols, where people were not fully connected to one another without the help of electronic media; therefore, some of their social roles were altered or became less meaningful. Such changes affected the clarity of how individuals see themselves, as well as the balance of their negative and positive emotional experiences (affect balance). We hypothesized that self-concept clarity predicts life satisfaction through affect balance. We collected our data from 139 Malaysian adults aged between 18 to 60 years through various social media platforms to complete the self-concept clarity scale (SCCS), satisfaction with life scale (SWLS), and scale of positive and negative experience (SPANE). Results of the bootstrap analysis with a 95% confidence interval indicated that the affect balance partially mediated the relationship between self-concept clarity and life satisfaction.
Article
The family can be viewed as an integrated system consisting of interdependent subsystems, among which marital and parent–child subsystems are critical for family functioning. Given the dominance of parents in the family, it is important to investigate the associations between parents’ characteristics and these two subsystems. In addition, researchers have found a unidirectional effect of parents’ self-concept clarity on their children’s self-concept clarity. Exploring whether and how parents’ self-concept clarity is related to family relationships may provide insight into this intergenerational transmission since the family plays an important role in the development of children’s self-concept. Taken together, the main aim of this study was to examine the links across parents’ self-concept clarity and marital and parent–child relationships. A total of 363 Chinese parental dyads of primary and secondary students completed the self-concept clarity, relationship assessment, and child–parent relationship scales, and the data were analyzed by adopting the actor–partner interdependence model (APIM) and the actor–partner interdependence mediation model (APIMeM). The APIM results indicated significant positive associations between parents’ self-concept clarity and parent–child relationship quality perceived by the parents (actor effects) and their partners (partner effects). The APIMeM results showed that fathers’ marital satisfaction partially mediated the actor effects from parents’ self-concept clarity to parent–child relationship quality. Moreover, after controlling for covariates (e.g., family income), parents’ self-concept clarity had significant indirect partner effects on parent–child relationship quality mediated completely via fathers’ marital satisfaction. These findings contribute to a deeper understanding of family system processes and the intergenerational transmission of self-concept clarity and have implications for family interventions.
Article
Humility is thought to be associated with greater accuracy in self‐assessment. However, clear evidence is lacking. Two studies tested this central proposition. In Study 1 (N = 258), participants completed a task on logical reasoning before estimating both their raw and relative performance. Study 2 (N = 214) was aimed at replicating Study 1 with a task on English fluency. Results from both studies were consistent. There was evidence of overestimation bias across each sample in which participants’ estimated performance was higher than their actual performance. More importantly, humility was associated with less overestimation bias, such that the difference in estimated and actual performance was smaller or non‐existent among those higher in humility. We also replicated the Dunning–Kruger effect in which participants of lower ability in these skills (i.e., lower actual scores) were most likely to overestimate their performance. Further analyses found that the negative relationship between humility and overestimation bias was not moderated by actual performances. However, the same analyses revealed that the Dunning–Kruger effect was also not moderated by humility. Hence, there is strong replicable evidence that humility is associated with less overestimation bias, supporting the view that greater accuracy in self‐assessment is an attribute of humility, and that this relationship is independent of actual ability, but humility does not affect the robust Dunning–Kruger effect.
Article
The current research examines how and why self‐concept clarity (i.e., having self‐aspects that are integrated into a well‐defined whole) shapes consumers’ appearance management behaviors. Five (including four pre‐registered) studies and one supplemental study provide correlational and causal evidence for the link between low self‐concept clarity and appearance management (e.g., choice of appearance‐enhancing products, interest in cosmetic procedures and beauty filters). Further, we demonstrate that public self‐consciousness mediates this effect (Studies 3‐4). We also find convergent process‐by‐moderation evidence that low self‐concept clarity increases appearance management only when the appearance management behavior is perceived to be socially acceptable (Study 5). In addition, we rule out global and appearance self‐esteem, private self‐consciousness, self‐improvement, and mood management as potential mechanisms. This research extends the literature on self‐concept, impression management, and appearance management and yields implications for beauty marketing, health communication, and consumer well‐being.
Article
Full-text available
Social media fatigue is a subjective sense of physical and mental exhaustion, lassitude, and irritation, caused by social media use. The current research explored the association between individual differences in attachment styles and the experience of fatigue resulting from extensive social media use. Two studies examined the association between adult attachment style and Facebook fatigue, and the mediating role of stressors related to social media use, self-esteem, and self-concept clarity. The results of the first study (N = 264) revealed an association between attachment anxiety and Facebook fatigue that was mediated by Facebook social comparison and Facebook anxiety. In the second study (N = 294), attachment anxiety was also associated with Facebook fatigue and was mediated by fear of missing out and Facebook anxiety, and these mediation effects were moderated by self-concept clarity. The findings indicate that the experience of social-media fatigue varies in accordance with specific user characteristics. Additionally, they Illustrate the impact of social media use on mental health, and emphasize the need to create a user experience that takes into account the stressors associated with social media use.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Commitment to a relationship is affected by the quality of one's alternatives to that partnership, but one must be aware of those alternatives in order for them to be influential. In a study of the links between attention to one's alternatives and relational outcomes, participants described their relationships, inspected slides of attractive opposite-sex targets, and, 2 months later, reported whether their relationships had ended. Satisfaction with, investment in, commitment to, and adjustment in a dating relationship were negatively correlated with reports of vigilance toward desirable alternatives to that relationship. In the lab, those who had earlier claimed to be attentive to alternatives really did spend more time inspecting pictures of attractive opposite-sex targets. Moreover, there was no better predictor of relationship failure than high attentiveness to alternatives. Inattentiveness may be a maintenance mechanism that helps to preserve and protect desirable relationships. Even if the grass is greener on the other side of the fence, happy gardeners will be less likely to notice.
Article
Used a longitudinal study of heterosexual dating relationships to test investment model predictions regarding the process by which satisfaction and commitment develop (or deteriorate) over time. Initially, 17 male and 17 female undergraduates, each of whom was involved in a heterosexual relationship of 0-8 wks duration, participated. Four Ss dropped out, and 10 Ss' relationships ended. Questionnaires were completed by Ss every 17 days. Increases over time in rewards led to corresponding increases in satisfaction, whereas variations in costs did not significantly affect satisfaction. Commitment increased because of increases in satisfaction, declines in the quality of available alternatives, and increases in investment size. Greater rewards also promoted increases in commitment to maintain relationships, whereas changes in costs generally had no impact on commitment. For stayers, rewards increased, costs rose slightly, satisfaction grew, alternative quality declined, investment size increased, and commitment grew; for leavers the reverse occurred. Ss whose partners ended their relationships evidenced entrapment: They showed relatively low increases in satisfaction, but their alternatives declined in quality and they continued to invest heavily in their relationships. (39 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).