ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Zoos aspire to bring people closer to nature through visitor education and by creating environments that immerse visitors into naturalistic surroundings. Yet to date, the psychological and behavioral consequences of these attempts have not been well-documented through empirical study. The present study was designed to determine if visiting a zoo had a measurable impact on visitors' relationships with nature and to test whether this effect was moderated by the zoo setting and/or type of zoo experience. Data were collected from three different types of zoos in New York City. A total of 242 zoo visitors participated in this study by completing a set of scales measuring explicit and implicit self-nature associations. Findings suggest that zoo experiences do promote an increased implicit connectedness with nature, but no changes were found for self-reported explicit connectedness with nature. The authors found no evidence that this effect was moderated by a specific design strategy or scale of institution.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Visitor Studies, 2008, 11(2), 139–150
Copyright
C
Visitor Studies Association
ISSN: 1064–5578 print / 1934-7715 online
DOI: 10.1080/10645570802355489
The Value of Zoo Experiences
for Connecting People with Nature
by Coral M. Bruni,
1
John Fraser
2
, and P. Wesley
Schultz
1
(1) California State University, San Marcos, California, USA
(2) Institute for Learning Innovation, Edgewater, Maryland, USA
ABSTRACT
Zoos aspire to bring people closer to nature through visitor education and by cre-
ating environments that immerse visitors into naturalistic surroundings. Yet to date,
the psychological and behavioral consequences of these attempts have not been well-
documented through empirical study. The present study was designed to determine if
visiting a zoo had a measurable impact on visitors’ relationships with nature and to test
whether this effect was moderated by the zoo setting and/or type of zoo experience.
Data were collected from three different types of zoos in New York City. A total of 242
zoo visitors participated in this study by completing a set of scales measuring explicit
and implicit self-nature associations. Findings suggest that zoo experiences do promote
an increased implicit connectedness with nature, but no changes were found for self-
reported explicit connectedness with nature. The authors found no evidence that this
effect was moderated by a specific design strategy or scale of institution.
Why are zoos so popular? Given the wide assortment of recreational and leisure activi-
ties afforded to people today, why do they continue to patronize zoos in record numbers?
This perplexing (albeit welcome) level of public support has been pondered by the zoo
community for nearly 200 years. As a result, many studies of zoo attendance tend to-
ward exploratory surveys of visitor motivations based on satisfaction (e.g., Morgan &
Hodgkinson, 1999; Tomas, Crompton, & Scott, 2003) or more recently, self-described
personal learning objectives (e.g., Falk, Heimlich, & Bronnenkant, 2008; Pekarik, 2004).
Unlike other museum types, zoos tend to garner substantially large audiences and atten-
dance profiles appear to demonstrate that these audiences are more demographically
diverse attendance than at other types of institutions. Yet surprisingly, there has been
little empirical research connecting zoo learning outcomes, the reported popularity of
zoos, and the motivations reported in marketing studies at zoos. As a result, it seems that
there should be concern among professionals about the lack of connection between the
perceived popularity of zoos revealed in marketing studies, actual zoo attendance, and
139
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
the role of zoos as conservation education centers. Although zoos are substantially sup-
ported through public funding (Association of Zoos and Aquariums, 2007), there remains
little empirical data assembled to indicate where social desirability and the personal
value of visiting a zoo overlap in public perception. The present study sought to under-
stand how visitors report their perceived explicit connectedness to nature when visiting
zoos, whether these explicit reports change as a result of visiting a zoo, and whether the
zoo experience itself provides implicit connections to nature that may not be apparent to
the visitor.
The work reported in this article draws on the concept of connectedness with nature.
Connectedness with nature refers to an individual’s belief about the extent to which he
or she is part of the natural environment (Schultz, 2002). An individual who feels that
he or she is part of nature would have schemas of self and nature that highly overlap.
On the other hand, the schemas of an individual who feels that he or she is not part of
nature would have very few points of intersection. The concept of connectedness with na-
ture has garnered considerable attention by social and behavioral scientists and several
self-report measures have been developed to assess a person’s explicit connectedness
with nature (e.g., Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale, Schultz, 2002; Environmental Identity
Scale, Clayton, 2003; Connectedness with Nature, Mayer & Frantz, 2004; Connection to
Nature Scale, Pennisi, 2007). In addition to these traditional self-report measures, con-
nectedness with nature has also been measured implicitly using the Implicit Association
Test (IAT) (Bruni & Schultz, under review; Schultz, Shriver, Tabanico, & Khazian, 2004;
Schultz & Tabanico, 2007).
The traditional IAT, createdbyGreenwald, McGhee, and Schwartz (1998), was designed
to measure unconscious cognitive associations (e.g., attitudes, self-esteem, self-concept)
by using reaction time data. In the IAT procedure, participants are presented with a series
of stimuli (words or pictures) and are asked to assign each to a particular category.
Reaction time data is collected to assess how quickly a person can correctly categorize
the stimulus. Schultz et al. (2004) examined the psychometric properties of the IAT
used to measure implicit connections with nature (IAT-Nature) and found good evidence
for the test-retest reliability of this measure. In addition, the IAT-Nature was found to
be significantly correlated with explicit measures of environmental concern (biospheric
concerns positively and egoistic concerns negatively) and connectedness with nature
(Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale).
For many people in urban settings, the local park, zoo, aquarium, or natural history
museum provides one of the only opportunities to encounter nature. This nature narrative
has accreted around zoos in part because, in the United States and many larger urban city-
centers worldwide, zoos have moved toward creating simulated naturalistic experiences
that immerse the visitor into the same landscapes that envelop the animals, on the
premise that these experiences build a close affiliation and appreciation of the animals
on display (e.g., Coe, 1984; Polakowski, 1987). However, over the past 30 years or so,
a protracted public critique has arisen around the concept of zoos as a form of nature
experience that is incompatible with building links to nature because it is, as Berger
(1980) first suggested, a constructed view of how humans wish nature to be. Although
Berger’s critique may hold true, the question of whether people perceive these exhibitions
as connecting them with the built world, or with the things they believe to be natural,
140 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008
Zoo Experiences and Connections with Nature
remains at the heart of the whether zoos provide the affiliative services that can help to
promote concern and care for nature.
Zoo visits have also been described as restorative experiences (Korpela, Hartig, Kaiser,
& Fuhrer, 1996), but how they are restorative remains undemonstrated through empirical
study. Fraser, Condon, and Gruber (2008) demonstrated that visitors are attracted to
zoos for a kind of metaphoric tourism in nature, with the living animals offering visitors
an opportunity to passively speculate on their relationships with the natural world. Other
researchers have found that a passive recreational aspect is valued in zoos; however,
those studies did not operationalize that value in a manner that could identify if associ-
ation with the animals and nature content was the recreational aspect that aligned with
the mission of zoos (Caldwell, Andereck, & Debbage, 1991; Turley, 2001).
Zoos are more than recreational experiences. Past research has suggested that ani-
mals are known to elicit emotional responses in zoo visitors (Myers, Saunders, & Birjulin,
2004). Clayton, Fraser, and Saunders (in press) found that conversations of zoo visitors
provided evidence that visitors have a feeling of connection to the animals themselves.
However, these studies were unable to determine the consequences, degree of mag-
nitude, or whether this explicit expression of connection was a pre-existing value or
developed as part of a zoo experience. Although these studies suggest that the zoo ex-
perience itself would predispose visitors to have explicit and implicit connections with
nature, they do not provide evidence that zoo experiences have any direct consequential
impact on visitors’ association with nature or the environment beyond the increase in
knowledge of nature and conservation content that has been measured in innumerable
studies of exhibit learning.
Schultz and Tabanico (2007) used the IAT-Nature to explore how implicit connected-
ness with nature is affected by context. Findings suggest that spending time at a highly
naturalistic 640-acre zoo with rides into a simulated African veldt (San Diego Wild Ani-
mal Park) increased a person’s connectedness with nature. Zoo visitors exiting the park
showed significantly higher implicit connectedness with nature than those entering the
park (a finding not replicated in the explicit measures taken at the same time). In a sep-
arate study measuring visitors’ implicit connectedness with nature throughout the day,
implicit connectedness with nature increased with the duration of their stay. Although
the above reported results suggest that zoo experiences have a measurable impact on
a visitor’s connections with nature, their study was unable to determine whether this
connection was true of zoos in general or unique to conditions at the San Diego Wild
Animal Park. For example, it may only pertain to larger institutions with longer stay times
and more naturalistic immersive exhibit strategies.
The present study sought to further understand whether zoo experiences have a
measurable impact on a visitor’s connectedness with nature, both explicitly and implicitly,
and if such an impact is evident in either dimension, to understand whether that impact
is generalizable to a variety of zoo types. One possible outcome of the zoo experience is
an increase in implicit connectedness with nature, as suggested by Schultz and Tabanico
(2007). Thus, in the present study it was hypothesized that spending time in a zoo
setting would result in an increase in implicit connectedness with nature, such that
connectedness with nature at exit would be greater than connectedness with nature at
entry. In addition, taking into consideration prior findings that explicit connections with
Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008 141
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
Table 1. Participant demographic information for all participants and by zoo location
Participants Total Bronx Central Park Prospect Park
N2421514942
Total Males 106 61 20 25
Total Females 136 90 29 17
Annual zoo members 23% 17% 27% 38%
Mean age 35 33 41 37
Visits to zoo per year
Total 1st visit 33% 29% 45% 38%
Total <1 time 15% 19% 10% 5%
Total 1 time 15% 15% 18% 12%
Total 2–4 times 24% 25% 12% 33%
Total 5–10 times 6% 6% 4% 7%
Total 11+ times 7% 6% 10% 5%
nature were unchanged from entry to exit in data collected at the San Diego Wild Animal
Park, it was hypothesized that the explicit connectedness with nature in this study would
also be largely unchanged, irrespective of the scale or type of display strategy. This
article presents results from one aspect of a larger study investigating the value of zoo
and aquarium visits to the community and the role these institutions serve in culture.
METHOD
Participants
Data were collected from three zoos operated by the Wildlife Conservation Society in
New York City (Bronx Zoo, Central Park Zoo, and Prospect Park Zoo) from April to July
2007. Participants were 242 zoo visitors; demographic data matched that of the typical
attendance profile of zoo visitors for these zoos (Table 1).
Materials
Random samples of entry and exit data were collected from the Bronx Zoo, and paired
entry/exit data were collected from the Central Park and Prospect Park zoos. In addi-
tion to the demographic data reported above, we measured both explicit and implicit
connectedness with nature. Explicit connectedness with nature was measured using the
“Please circle the picture below which best describes your relationship with the natural
environment. How interconnected are you with nature?”
Self Nature Self Nature Self Nature Self
Self
Nature
Self Nature Self Nature Nature
Figure 1. Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale.
142 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008
Zoo Experiences and Connections with Nature
Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale (INS; Schultz, 2002). This single-item graphical scale
is used to measure the extent to which an individual includes nature within his or her
cognitive representation of self. The scale contains a series of seven overlapping circles
labeled self and nature (see Figure 1). Scores range from 1 to 7, with the least overlapping
circle receiving a score of 1 and the most overlapping circle receiving a score of 7. The
circle with the least overlap represents an individual who views him or herself as sepa-
rate from nature. The circle with complete overlap represents a person who views him or
herself as the same as nature.
Implicit connectedness with nature was measured using a game version of the Implicit
Association Test (IAT), known as FlexiTwins (Bruni & Schultz, under review). The game
activity asks the player to sort words as they appear on the screen into two categories
according to a predetermined set of criteria. If the word belongs to the category on the
right, the participant presses a button with their right index finger; if the word belongs
to the category on the left, the participant presses a different button with their left index
finger (e.g., see Figure 2, where the participant chooses to place the word “you will need
to ask the authors for the word from the Figure as I can’t read it” in the “Me” or “Other”
category). Reaction time data (in milliseconds) are collected to measure the amount of
time it takes a participant to correctly categorize a word. In FlexiTwins, block refers to the
different levels within FlexiTwins and each stimulus within each level is referred to as a
trial. The mean reaction time of a block is compared to the mean reaction time of other
blocks, allowing the researcher to establish which blocks of associations are preferred
(i.e., more strongly associated in a person’s semantic network). Of important note, using
reaction time data provides a measure of connectedness that eliminates self-report bias
(see Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003).
In our studies, FlexiTwins was used to examine self-concept (cf. Greenwald et al.,
2002), or more specifically, associations between self and the categories nature and
built (IAT-Nature). To measure these associations, the participant’s name was used as
the me category, and a random list of other names was used as the other category.
The stimuli for the built and nature categories were taken from previous work using the
IAT-Nature (Bruni & Schultz, under review). The following words were used as the stimuli
for the built category: car, chair, church, and truck. The following words were used
as the stimuli for the nature category: flower, mountain, tree, and waterfall. FlexiTwins
contains seven blocks. Table 2 shows information regarding blocks and trials used in this
study. Figure 2 highlights screen shots from FlexiTwins—FlexiTwins can also be viewed
by visiting www.conservationpsychology.org/game.
Table 2. Breakdown of FlexiTwins blocks and trials
Block Block type Category # of trials
1 Practice Me/ and Other 8
2 Practice Nature/Built’ 8
3 Compatible test Nature/Me Built/Other 16
4 Compatible test Nature/Me Built/Other 24
5 Practice Built Nature 8
6 Incompatible test Built/Me Nature/Other 16
7 Incompatible test Built/Me Nature/Other 24
Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008 143
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
Figure 2. Screen shots from FlexiTwins.
FlexiTwins produces a standardized score for each participant (D-score). The D-score
is calculated by finding the difference in the average response time between different
blocks of trials, to indicate which types of associations are easier to make. In calculating
the D-score, two subscores are produced by the game (D1 and D2). Specifically, D1 is
calculated by subtracting the average reaction time on block 3 (compatible: me-nature,
not-me-built) from the average reaction time on block 6 (incompatible: me-built, not-
me-nature) and then this difference score is divided by the standard deviation of both
blocks. D2 is calculated in the same manner, using blocks 4 (compatible) and 7 (incom-
patible). The D-score is simply the average of D1 and D2 (cf. Brookfield Zoo, 2006). Using
this scoring algorithm, the scores are standardized such that a score of 0 indicates an
144 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008
Zoo Experiences and Connections with Nature
equal strength of association between self and built as there is between self and nature.
Positive scores indicate stronger self-nature associations and negative scores indicate
stronger self-built associations. Like the Cohen’s d effect size on which the computa-
tions are based, effects of .2 (small), .5 (medium), and .8 (large) represent the typical
range.
In FlexiTwins, the D-score is used to measure implicit connections with nature and its
subscores (D1 and D2) are used to test the internal reliability of the measure. To assure
accurate and reliable data, D-scores are screened using the participants’ percent correct
as directed in the FlexiTwins manual (Brookfield Zoo, 2006). In our prior studies we have
counterbalanced the order of compatible and incompatible trials, and while the order of
administration does affect the magnitude of the D-score, it does not affect the relative
pattern of correlations with other measures. In addition, as suggested by past research
(Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2005), practice blocks (blocks 1 and 2) are included to
lessen internal order effects caused by the presentation order of categories (compatible
blocks first followed by incompatible blocks).
FlexiTwins was administered on an IPAQ h andheld device (PocketPC). Past research
using FlexiTwins was administered on desktop computers. Prior to running the current
study, 77 undergraduate students from California State University completed both the
desktop version and the PocketPC version of FlexiTwins in order to validate the PocketPC
version. In addition, participants were asked to play the PocketPC FlexiTwins twice.
Internal reliability was assessed by correlating two subscales produced by the game (D1
and D2). This data showed that the PocketPC FlexiTwins D1 score (M = .63, SD = .43) was
significantly correlated with the D2 score (M = .54, SD = .49) at Time 1, r = .25, p < .05.
This was also found at Time 2; FlexiTwins D1 score (M = .49, SD = .50) was significantly
correlated with the D2 score (M = .40, SD = .43), r = .52, p <.01. Test-retest reliability
was assessed by correlating the FlexiTwins PocketPC D-score at Time 1 and Time 2.
Results showed that the PocketPC FlexiTwins D-scores at Time 1 (M = .59, SD = .37) and
Time 2 (M = .45, SD = .40) were significantly correlated, r = .43, p < .01. In addition,
the PocketPC FlexiTwins D-scores at Time 1 and Time 2 were found to be significantly
correlated with the desktop version of FlexiTwins (M = .48, SD = .42), Time 1: r = .50,
p < .01; Time 2: r = .42, p < .01; r =
.49 in the aggregated dataset). Finally, the D-scores
of the PocketPC FlexiTwins at Time 1 was found to be significantly correlated with the INS
(M = 3.68, SD = 1.31), r = .27, p < .05. Taken together, these results suggest that the
PocketPC FlexiTwins provides a usable platform for administering FlexiTwins in that it is
both reliable and valid.
In the present study, reliability analyses of the implicit measure revealed good ev-
idence for internal consistency. In the current zoo sample, FlexiTwins reliability was
evident in that D1 scores (M = .58, SD = 1.02) were significantly correlated with D2
scores (M = .58, SD = .75), r = .48, p < .01.
Procedure
Two separate procedures were used in this study due to the logistics of data collection
at the individual zoos. As participants from Prospect Park Zoo and Central Park Zoo
entered the zoo, they were asked if they would like to participate in the study. Once the
participant agreed, they completed a short survey and played FlexiTwins on the PocketPC
Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008 145
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
at the beginning of their visit; they then returned to complete the measures a second
time just before exiting the park at the end of their visit to the zoo. As compensation for
participation in this study, participants received a keychain.
At the Bronx Zoo, independent random samples of guests were sampled entering and
exiting the park. This change in procedure was required because of staffing limitations
(e.g., multiple entrances/exits and extended visits). After agreeing to participate, visitors
were asked to complete the survey and play FlexiTwins on the PocketPC. Upon completion
of the study, these participants also received a keychain.
RESULTS
Upon entry to the zoos, participants showed moderate connectedness with nature
both explicitly (INS: M = 4.06 out of 7, SD = 1.45; N = 242) and implicitly (FlexiTwins:
M = .51, SD = .91; N = 242). When leaving the zoos, explicit connectedness with nature
was largely unchanged (M = 4.10, SD = 1.73), t(239) = .16, p = .88. However, at the
implicit level, FlexiTwins scores were higher at exit (M = .67, SD = .49) when compared
to results of the entry test (M = .51, SD = .91). To test the statistical significance of this
effect, we focused on the more powerful paired samples t test with participants from
Prospect Park Zoo and Central Park Zoo. After removing 22 participants from the analysis
due to incomplete test-retest data, the results showed that implicit connectedness was
significantly higher at exit (M = .72, SD = .39) when compared with results of the entry
test (M = .48, SD = .89), t(68) = 2.11, p < .05; d = .51.
Following the combined analyses, the data were examined separately by location. The
differences between institutions were not statistically significant. At all three locations
the size and direction of the effect were nearly identical to the findings from the combined
analysis (Figure 3).
Bronx Zoo
Upon entry to the Bronx Zoo, participants showed moderate connectedness with nature
(INS: M = 4.07, SD = 1.66; FlexiTwins: M =
.53, SD = .95). However, when leaving the
Bronx Zoo, participants showed higher implicit connectedness with nature (FlexiTwins:
M = .67, SD = .59). Although the means were in the expected direction, this difference
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7
0.8
Implicit Connectedness
with Nature
Bronx Zoo Central Park
Location
Prospect Park
Entry
Exit
Figure 3. Comparison of implicit connections with nature (D-scores) by Park.
146 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008
Zoo Experiences and Connections with Nature
was not statistically significant, t(140) = .99, p = .33. No significant change was seen at
the explicit level; INS at exit, M = 4.11, SD = 1.70, t(140) = .12, p = .90.
Central Park Zoo
Upon entry to the Central Park Zoo, participants showed moderate connectedness with
nature both explicitly (INS: M = 4.29, SD = 1.29) and implicitly (FlexiTwins: M = .43,
SD = .65). The connectedness scores at exit were higher for the implicit measure (Flex-
iTwins: M = .70, SD = .38), (t(35) =−2.15, p < .05). Scores on the INS scale were largely
unchanged.
Prospect Park Zoo
Upon entry to the Prospect Park Zoo, participants showed moderate connectedness
with nature (INS: M = 4.29, SD = 1.29; FlexiTwins: M =.53, SD = 1.11). At exit, Prospect
Park Zoo visitors had higher implicit connectedness with nature scores (FlexiTwins:
M = .73, SD = .41), but the effect was not significant, t(32) =−1.05, p = .30.
Finally, the associations between demographic variables and explicit and implicit
connectedness with n ature were examined. No significant differences were found in
FlexiTwins D-score by gender (males: M = .51, SD = .75; females: M = .56, SD = .90),
t(236) =−.41, p =
.68, or based on membership status (yes: M = .69, SD = .76; no:
M = .49, SD = .86), t(236) = 1.52, p = .13. In addition, no correlations were found
between age (M = 35, SD = 11.99) and FlexiTwins D-score, r = .06, p = .37, or how often
participants visited the zoo (M = 2.73, SD = 1.59) and FlexiTwins D-score, r =−.04,
p = .52.
DISCUSSION
This study examined how the zoo experience affects a visitor’s connectedness with
nature. The results suggest that spending time in a zoo setting leads to an increase in
implicit connectedness with nature. Interestingly, this effect was found for an implicit
measure based on reaction time, but no effects were observed for self-ratings of explicit
connectedness with nature.
These results suggest that zoo experiences can promote an increase in connectedness
with nature, but that the effect occurs primarily at the implicit level and may not be
apparent to visitors at the time. This study replicates the findings presented by Schultz
and Tabanico (2007) regarding zoo visitors’ connections with nature at the San Diego
Wild Animal Park and confirms that spending time in a zoo setting increases a visitor’s
implicit connectedness with nature, irrespective of the scale of zoo or display concept.
As zoos have changed over the years to target explicit learning outcomes using in-
novative interpretive strategies, visitors continue to report that their primary purpose
for visiting zoos is to be with animals rather than any explicit desire to engage in learn-
ing (Wildlife Conservation Society, unpublished internal report). Environmental immer-
sion concepts, which have become increasingly popular in zoo settings, were intended
to create a more engaged sense of being with animals (e.g., Coe, 1984; Polakowski,
1987). Our results suggest that such an approach is indeed warranted as it promotes
a connection between individuals, animals, and the environment. However, our results
also suggest that non-immersion but naturalistic exhibits at the Prospect Park Zoo and
Central Park Zoo achieved the same degree of implicit association with nature as the
Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008 147
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
more immersive exhibits at the Bronx Zoo and San Diego Wild Animal Park. These
results suggest that while the explicit connection may be of interest to visitors, the
greater implicit connection measured in this study may account for some of the desir-
ability or pleasure associated with visiting zoos and their overall popularity with the
public.
While we are sympathetic toward the explanation that spending time in a zoo setting
increases a visitor’s implicit connectedness with nature, the lack of a control group
limits our ability to draw causal conclusions. As a result, there are a series of alternative
explanations for our findings associated with physical or psychological states evoked by
the zoo experience that have nothing to do with connectedness with nature. For example,
participants in this study may have been experiencing some level of mental or physical
fatigue at the end of their zoo visit, and this fatigue is the source of the difference in
our IAT scores. While this explanation is plausible, findings presented in prior work by
Schultz and Tabanico (2007) suggest otherwise. Using a methodology identical to that
reported in this article, they showed no change in IAT scores pre- and post-visit from
six locations (beach, golf course, gym, indoor rock climbing facility, library, hiking).
However, in two studies at the San Diego Wild Animal Park, there was evidence of change
among zoo visitors from entry to exit, with an increase in connectedness with nature
over time. These findings, coupled with those reported in the present article, support
the conclusion that there is something unique about a zoo setting that promotes an
individual’s connectedness with nature.
Interestingly no gender differences in connectedness with nature were found in this
sample. Past research has found that females tend to have higher connectedness with
nature than males (Schultz et al., 2004; Schultz & Tabanico, 2007). The means of the
present sample were in the expected direction, although no statistically significant dif-
ferences were found. This study did not investigate gender identity, which may be a
more accurate way of understanding how sexuality relates to museum experiences like
implicit connectedness to nature measured here (Mertens, Fraser, & Heimlich, 2008).
This difference may be worthy of future research where gender identity is incorporated
as part of a more comprehensive study of implicit and explicit associations with nature
or perceptions of nature affiliation.
Overall, participants in our sample of zoo visitors showed a moderate level of both
implicit and explicit connectedness with nature. However, the mean scores at entry to
the zoos were not substantially different from those found in prior samples of university
students or in samples of the general public (Bruni & Schultz, under review). This suggests
several important aspects of the z oo experience. First, there is a basic overall tendency to
associate more strongly with natural than with built environments—even among a sample
of urban residents in New York City. Second, zoos are appealing to a broad audience,
and not just people who already have a strong sense of natural connection. Relatedly,
the effect of the zoo experience on implicit connections with nature is broad-based and
does not require an initial “green” mentality. Indeed, more than 1/3 of our samples were
first-time visitors to the zoo.
In conclusion, these results suggest that zoo experiences do promote an increase in
connectedness with nature, but the connection occurs at an implicit level and may not
be readily apparent to the visitor. Although this may account for some of the reported
148 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008
Zoo Experiences and Connections with Nature
popularity for visiting zoos, it may also reveal why it is difficult for visitors to describe
explicitly what they find desirable about being with living animals. Further research could
help advance this new knowledge by investigating whether these implicit affiliations have
residual value or are sustained after the visit.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported in part through funding from the Institute for Museum and
Library Services grant # LG-25-05-0102-0. Our appreciation goes to Erin Johnson, Alexander
J. Wolf, Karelle A. Jones, Randie C. Chance, and Maria A. Aguilar for their help with the
reported studies. Portions of this article were presented at the 20th Annual Meeting of the
International Association for People-Environment Studies, in Rome, Italy, 2008.
REFERENCES
Association of Zoos and Aquariums. (2007). AZA annual survey of members 2007. Washington DC:
Author.
Berger, J. (1980). Why look at animals? In About looking (pp. 3–30). New York: Pantheon.
Brookfield Zoo. (2006). FlexiFrog and FlexiTwins: Measures of implicit social cognition. User’s guide.
Available for download at www.csusm.edu/schultz.
Bruni, C. M., & Schultz, P. W. (under review). Implicit beliefs about self and nature: Evidence from
an IAT game. Unpublished manuscript, California State University, San Marcos.
Caldwell, L. L., Andereck, K. L., & Debbage, K. (1991). Predicting zoo visitor satisfaction. Proceedings
of the Southeastern Recreation Research Conference, 13, 22–29.
Clayton, S. (2003). Environmental identity: A conceptual and an operational definition. In S. Clayton
&S.Opotow(Eds.),Identity and the natural environment: The psychological significance of nature
(pp. 343). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clayton, S., Fraser, J., & Saunders, C. (in press). Zoo experiences: Conversations, connections, and
concern for animals. Zoo Biology.
Coe, J. (1984). Design and perception: Making the zoo experience real. Zoo Biology, 4, 197–208.
Falk, J. H., Heimlich, J. E., & Bronnenkant, K. (2008). Using identity-related visit motivations as a
tool for understanding adult zoo and aquarium visitors meaning making. Curator: The Museum
Journal, 51(1), 55–80.
Fraser, J., Condon, K., & Gruber, S. (2007). Exposing the tourist value proposition of zoos and
aquaria. Tourism Review International, 11, 279–293.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences
in implicit social cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit
Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
85, 197–216.
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellot, D. S. (2002).
A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological
Review, 109, 3–25.
Korpela, K., Hartig, T., Kaiser, F. G., & Fuhrer, U. (1996). Restorative experience and self-regulation
in favorite places. Environment and Behavior, 33, 572–589.
Mayer, F. S., & Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness with nature scale: A measure of individuals’
feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 503–515.
Mertens, D., Fraser, J., & Heimlich, J. E. (2008). Gender, identity, and the transformative research
paradigm. Museums and Social Issues, 3(1), 81–92.
Morgan, J. M., & Hodgkinson, M. (1999). The motivation and social orientation of visitors attending
a contemporary zoological park. Environment and Behavior, 31, 227–239.
Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008 149
Coral M. Bruni, John Fraser, P. Wesley Schultz
Myers, O. E., Saunders, C. D., & Birjulin, A. A. (2004). Emotional dimensions of watching zoo ani-
mals: An experience sampling study building on insights from psychology. Curator, the Museum
Journal, 47, 299–321.
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the implicit
association test: II. Method variable and construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 31, 166–180.
Pekarik, A. (2004). Eye-to-eye with animals and ourselves. Curator: The Museum Journal, 47, 257–
260.
Pennisi, L. (2007). Connection to nature: Development of a measurement scale. Unpublished dis-
sertation, University of Florida.
Polakowski, K. J. (1987). Zoo design: The reality of wild illusions. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan
School of Natural Resources.
Schultz, P. W. (2002). Inclusion with nature: Understanding the psychology of human-nature in-
teractions. In P. Schmuck & P. W. Schultz (Eds.), The psychology of sustainable development.
Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Schultz, P. W., Shriver, C., Tabanico, J. J., & Khazian, A. M. (2004). Implicit connections with nature.
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 31–42.
Schultz, P. W., & Tabanico, J. J. (2007). Self, identity, and the natural environment: Exploring implicit
connections with nature. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1219–1247.
Turley, S. K. (2001). Children and the demand for recreational experiences: The case of zoos. Leisure
Studies, 20,118.
Tomas, S. R., Crompton, J. L., & Scott, D. (2003). Assessing service quality and benefits sought
among zoological park visitors. Journal of Park and Recreation Administration, 21(52), 105–124.
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Coral M. Bruni is a researcher in the Applied Social Psychology lab at California State
University, San Marcos. Her research interests are in conservation psychology and implicit
social cognition. She is currently working on a number of studies investigating environ-
mental attitudes and environmental education programs. E-mail: cbruni@csusm.edu.
John Fraser is an architect and conservation psychologist currently serving as Director,
ILI-NY for the Institute for Learning Innovation, an adjunct lecturer at Hunter College of
CUNY, and a CERC Scientist at Columbia University. He was Director of Public Research and
Evaluation at the Wildlife Conservation Society when this study was conducted.
Wesley Schultz is Professor of Psychology at California State University, San Marcos.
His research interests are in applied social psychology, social influence, and conservation
psychology. He has published extensively in these areas, with recent books on the Psy-
chology of Sustainable Development (Kluwer, 2002) and Attitudes and Opinions (Erlbaum,
2005).
150 Visitor Studies, 11(2), 2008

Supplementary resource (1)

... The connectedness-promoting-effect of spending time in nature seems to hold for mundane or ordinary experiences with nature, such as walking in nature (Mayer et al., 2009;Nisbet et al., 2019), as well as for exceptional experiences with nature, such as nature-based tourism (Burbach et al., 2012;Wheaton et al., 2016) and wilderness expeditions (Barton et al., 2016;Richardson et al., 2016). Further, the interactions one has with nature need not be confined to only organic, wild, or "pristine" nature to promote connectedness but can also involve human-made nature, such as zoos (e.g., Bruni et al., 2008;Schultz & Tabanico, 2007) and urban gardens (Uhlmann et al., 2018). Connectedness can even be promoted by incredibly subtle exposure to nature, such as the presence of plants in a lab space (Weinstein et al., 2009, S4) or, intriguingly, simply removing one's shoes while outside. ...
... Age may influence individuals' sense of connectedness. Studies with adults indicate that age is either positively associated with connectedness (Beery, 2013;Burbach et al., 2012;Diessner et al., 2018;Harvey et al., 2016;Lumber et al., 2017;Nour et al., 2017;Sanguinetti, 2014;Zhang et al., 2014, S1-2) or not at all (Brown, 2017;Bruni et al., 2008;Dutcher et al., 2007;Mayer & Frantz, 2004;Swami et al., 2016;Unsworth et al., 2016, S1-2;Walters et al., 2014;Weinstein et al., 2009, S1-3;Whitburn et al., 2019). In other words, studies with adults do not tend to find an actively negative relationship between age and connectedness. ...
... For the most part, the evidence often suggests that women feel more connected to nature than men (Beery, 2013;Bruni & Schultz, 2010, S3;Crawford et al., 2017;Hughes et al., 2019;Mayer et al., 2009, S2;Nour et al., 2017;Pensini et al., 2016;Sanguinetti, 2014;Schultz & Tabanico, 2007, S3-4;Spendrup et al., 2016;Swami et al., 2016;Zhang et al., 2014, S1) than it suggests men feel more connectedness than women (Larson et al., 2018;Wyles et al., 2019). However, many studies also report no differences between men and women (Barton et al., 2016;Bruni & Schultz, 2010, S1-2;Bruni et al., 2008;Davis & Stroink, 2016a, b;Di Fabio & Kenny, 2018;Diessner et al., 2018;Duffy & Verges, 2010;Frantz et al., 2005;Harvey et al., 2016;Liu et al., 2019;Lumber et al., 2017;Mayer & Frantz, 2004, S1-2;Mayer et al., 2009, S1 & S3;Unsworth et al., 2016, S1-2;Vess et al., 2012;Weinstein et al., 2009, S1-3;Whitburn et al., 2019;Zhang et al., 2014, S2). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Outdoor education and environmental education developed as separate educational movements, each with distinctive aspects but also closely related, sharing some common content and underlying educational pedagogies. This chapter seeks to anchor this association leading to outdoor environmental education (OEE) as a contemporary form of environmental education in which the outdoors provides a setting conducive for meaningful teaching and learning in environmental education. The chapter opens with a brief presentation of the historical development of each field, continues in identifying several social–environmental factors and educational theories that lay the grounds for linking OE and EE. Through these, this chapter claims that the features of OEE contribute to its promise as progressive transformative education for cultivating environmental citizenship. Many educational systems still do not acknowledge the outdoors as legitimate learning settings that can promote meaningful learning in the contemporary world. This chapter closes in addressing some of the ongoing practical challenges confronting OEE that arise from this situation.KeywordsSituating OEE in contemporary educational theoriesChallenges of OEELinking EE and OEOEE and environmental citizenship
... The connectedness-promoting-effect of spending time in nature seems to hold for mundane or ordinary experiences with nature, such as walking in nature (Mayer et al., 2009;Nisbet et al., 2019), as well as for exceptional experiences with nature, such as nature-based tourism (Burbach et al., 2012;Wheaton et al., 2016) and wilderness expeditions (Barton et al., 2016;Richardson et al., 2016). Further, the interactions one has with nature need not be confined to only organic, wild, or "pristine" nature to promote connectedness but can also involve human-made nature, such as zoos (e.g., Bruni et al., 2008;Schultz & Tabanico, 2007) and urban gardens (Uhlmann et al., 2018). Connectedness can even be promoted by incredibly subtle exposure to nature, such as the presence of plants in a lab space (Weinstein et al., 2009, S4) or, intriguingly, simply removing one's shoes while outside. ...
... Age may influence individuals' sense of connectedness. Studies with adults indicate that age is either positively associated with connectedness (Beery, 2013;Burbach et al., 2012;Diessner et al., 2018;Harvey et al., 2016;Lumber et al., 2017;Nour et al., 2017;Sanguinetti, 2014;Zhang et al., 2014, S1-2) or not at all (Brown, 2017;Bruni et al., 2008;Dutcher et al., 2007;Mayer & Frantz, 2004;Swami et al., 2016;Unsworth et al., 2016, S1-2;Walters et al., 2014;Weinstein et al., 2009, S1-3;Whitburn et al., 2019). In other words, studies with adults do not tend to find an actively negative relationship between age and connectedness. ...
... For the most part, the evidence often suggests that women feel more connected to nature than men (Beery, 2013;Bruni & Schultz, 2010, S3;Crawford et al., 2017;Hughes et al., 2019;Mayer et al., 2009, S2;Nour et al., 2017;Pensini et al., 2016;Sanguinetti, 2014;Schultz & Tabanico, 2007, S3-4;Spendrup et al., 2016;Swami et al., 2016;Zhang et al., 2014, S1) than it suggests men feel more connectedness than women (Larson et al., 2018;Wyles et al., 2019). However, many studies also report no differences between men and women (Barton et al., 2016;Bruni & Schultz, 2010, S1-2;Bruni et al., 2008;Davis & Stroink, 2016a, b;Di Fabio & Kenny, 2018;Diessner et al., 2018;Duffy & Verges, 2010;Frantz et al., 2005;Harvey et al., 2016;Liu et al., 2019;Lumber et al., 2017;Mayer & Frantz, 2004, S1-2;Mayer et al., 2009, S1 & S3;Unsworth et al., 2016, S1-2;Vess et al., 2012;Weinstein et al., 2009, S1-3;Whitburn et al., 2019;Zhang et al., 2014, S2). ...
Book
This edited volume explores the role of outdoor environmental education in the contemporary society. It identifies some of the opportunities and challenges of this educational area, particularly in the growing digitalization of the contemporary society and the distancing between people and nature. Furthermore, it seeks to answer why outdoor environmental education is essential for developing students’ environmental citizenship competencies or developing their relationship with nature. The book also introduces the various approaches existing in the field, discusses their relevance, and highlights their unique features. The book finishes with an overview of the practice of outdoor environmental education in selected countries from North America, Europe, and Asia.
... As peoples' connection to nature and wildlife declines with growing urbanisation [1], zoos are an increasingly important venue for visitors to engage with nature and see wild animals up-close [2,3]. In fact, many visitors report experiencing positive emotional connections with captive animals during their visit to a zoo [4,5]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Negative visitor behaviour in zoos such as banging, shouting and feeding animals are unwanted, but under-studied, visitor actions. It is not known how prevalent negative behaviour is, which species or enclosure type receives the most negative behaviour or how these behaviours affect zoo-housed animals. In this study, a comprehensive assessment of negative visitor behaviour, using an innovative methodology, was conducted at 25 different enclosures at Fota Wildlife Park, Ireland. Additionally, animal activity level and out of sight behaviour was observed. Descriptive statistics and general linear models were used to investigate which variables affected behaviour. Banging was the most common negative behaviour, while Humboldt penguins, lion-tailed macaques and Sumatran tigers were the most harassed species. Negative actions increased as visitor number increased and at traditional-style viewing areas. Active animal behaviour and out of sight animals were effected as negative visitor behaviours increased, but there appeared to be a tolerance threshold before a behavioural response was observed. By understanding negative behaviours, zoos can strive to reduce them and promote positive animal welfare.
... At first glance, this may seem problematic; namely, that knowledge alone is a poor predictor of behaviour [75,76]. However, more progressive conservation education interventions seek to influence a much wider suite of outcomes than just knowledge, such as conservation-related attitudes [77,78], increased time spent in nature [79], an improved sense of connection to nature/wildlife [80][81][82][83], increases in participant health and wellbeing [84], as well as, of course, pro-conservation behaviour change [84][85][86][87]. Indeed, for zoos, the World Zoo and Aquarium Conservation Education Strategy [88] already recommends this diversity of outcomes, suggesting a transformational learning approach that "can support new ways for audiences to consciously make meaning of their lives in relation to sustainable futures for species, ecosystems, and humanity" [88] (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is now well established that human-induced species extinctions and habitat degradation are currently occurring at unprecedented rates. To halt and reverse this decline, the international community adopted the Kunming–Montreal Global Biodiversity Framework (GBF), as part of the Kunming–Montreal Biodiversity Package, in December 2022. We clarify what this new framework means for conservation zoos and aquariums in their mission to prevent species extinction by highlighting areas of focus. We explain why it is necessary that conservation zoos and aquariums establish the appropriate mechanisms for contributing towards such a framework to help validate their role in the 21st Century. Conservation zoos and aquariums should be reassured that much of their work already fits within the GBF. However, the current mechanisms for individual zoos and aquariums to directly contribute to the implementation of the GBF mostly rely on close collaboration with individual national governments and/or are only possible at a national level. It is therefore critical that national, regional, and global zoo membership organisations take a leading role in championing the work of their members. Equally, adequately linking the efforts of zoos and aquariums to the national implementation of international instruments, such as the GBF, is imperative to ensure that these organizations’ contributions feed into the understanding we have of global progress towards the implementation of international instruments.
... Clayton et al. 2011;Miralles et al. 2019). The existence of this emotional connection was confirmed by Bruni et al. (2008) through studying implicit and explicit connections with nature in participants visiting three different zoos. The results showed that, although visitors leaving zoos did not report changes in their explicit connection (conscious perception), significantly greater implicit connection (unconscious perception) was found in exiting visitors when compared to entering visitors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Conservation awareness and behaviour change are some of the main objectives of progressive zoos and aquariums. Among the wide range of visitor experiences these places offer, many also promote animal-visitor interactions as emotional experiences that allegedly promote increased knowledge and attitudes towards conservation. The present study aimed to measure immediate and longitudinal effects on conservation caring and behaviour change of a human-dolphin intervention using the Conservation Caring scale (Skibins and Powell 2013). This scale was originally designed to measure the zoo-goer's connection to a specific species and to relate this to pro-conservation behavioural intentions following the zoo visit. A total of 291 adult zoo visitors participated in this study. Participants were randomly selected and 124 interventions were assessed. Participants were asked to answer the survey before (pre-intervention) or after (post-intervention) the programme. Twelve weeks after the intervention, a follow-up survey was emailed to all participants who stated willingness to participate in the follow-up assessment. A total of 148 pre-intervention, 143 post-intervention and 32 follow-up surveys were collected. Results show a post-intervention increase in some of the variables under study, notably 'existing connection to wildlife' and both 'species-' and 'biodiversity-oriented behaviours'. A longitudinal analysis showed a significant return to baseline values for the 'existing connection to wildlife' and 'conservation caring' variables. Both 'species-' and 'biodiversity-oriented behaviours' remained stable three months after the intervention although with a decreasing trend. Future considerations on improving the acceptance of conservation-focused behaviour changes are suggested.
... Παρόλο που οι ζωολογικοί κήποι έχουν αλλάξει μορφή και με την πάροδο των ετών στοχεύουν σε ρητά μαθησιακά αποτελέσματα, χρησιμοποιώντας καινοτόμες ερμηνευτικές στρατηγικές, οι επισκέπτες συνεχίζουν να αναφέρουν ότι ο κύριος σκοπός τους για την επίσκεψη ζωολογικού κήπου είναι να δουν τα ζώα και όχι οποιαδήποτε ρητή επιθυμία να συμμετάσχουν στη μάθηση (Bruni et al. 2008). ...
... Los videos con mayor contenido educativo y con mayor diversidad de especies lamentablemente son minoría (Llewellyn & Rose, 2021). Algunos zoológicos miembros de la WAZA se han tomado en serio su papel educativo y han implementado estrategias para mejorar su efectividad educativa, el problema sigue siendo que la mayoría de los zoológicos no se apegan a estos estándares pues ni siquiera pertenecen a la asociación (Bruni et al., 2008;Falk et al., 2008;Jensen et al., 2017;Malamud et al., 2010;Moss et al., 2015Moss et al., , 2017. ...
Chapter
Zoos and aquariums are culturally and historically important places where families enjoy their leisure time and scientists study exotic animals. Many contain buildings of great architectural merit. Some people consider zoos little more than animal prisons, while others believe they play an important role in conservation and education. Zoos have been the subject of a vast number of academic studies, whose results are scattered throughout the literature. This interdisciplinary volume brings together research on animal behaviour, visitor studies, zoo history, human-animal relationships, veterinary medicine, welfare, education, enclosure design, reproduction, legislation, and zoo management conducted at around 200 institutions located throughout the world. The book is neither 'pro-' nor 'anti-' zoo and attempts to strike a balance between praising zoos for the good work they have done in the conservation of some species, while recognising that they face many challenges in making themselves relevant in the modern world.
Chapter
Psychology has many insights to offer in our collective attempts to reconnect individuals with nature. Based upon the literature on the antecedents of connectedness to nature, it is important to consider how any given outdoor environmental education experience influences participants’ affective experiences (e.g., awe, compassion), frames of mind (e.g., mindfulness), and ways of thinking about themselves and nature (e.g., introspection, anthropomorphism). This chapter reviews relevant literature on the psychological understanding of the antecedents of connectedness and points to potential applications of this knowledge in outdoor and environmental education. In doing so, we will emphasize the process through which antecedents increase connectedness to identify specific points of intervention and how they can be leveraged to provide impactful outdoor educational experiences. Further, using this extensive body of literature and incorporating critical perspectives, we will address the question of adapting current research and praxis to increasingly urbanized worlds.KeywordsMindfulnessAnthtropomorphismCompassionNature connectionWonder
Article
Full-text available
In reporting Implicit Association Test (IAT) results, researchers have most often used scoring conventions described in the first publication of the IAT (A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998). Demonstration IATs available on the Internet have produced large data sets that were used in the current article to evaluate alternative scoring procedures. Candidate new algorithms were examined in terms of their (a) correlations with parallel self-report measures, (b) resistance to an artifact associated with speed of responding, (c) internal consistency, (d) sensitivity to known influences on IAT measures, and (e) resistance to known procedural influences. The best-performing measure incorporates data from the IAT's practice trials, uses a metric that is calibrated by each respondent's latency variability, and includes a latency penalty for errors. This new algorithm strongly outperforms the earlier (conventional) procedure.
Article
Full-text available
Analyzing museum research data on the basis of a male or female distinction has the potential to create misleading findings about the subject being researched, can perpetuate bias against individuals who do not conform to heterosexual orthodoxy and, most importantly, reveals an institutional homophobia that discriminates against LGBTQ participants in the research. This paper considers the power and privilege of the researcher and choices of specific paradigms that can be engaged when creating museum audience research to limit the ongoing marginalization of the LGBTQ community. It does so through the application of the philosophical assumptions associated with a transformative paradigm that recognizes that realities are socially constructed through the experience of political, cultural, economic, and identity values. The transformative paradigm provides a mechanism for addressing the complexities of LGBTQ identity in cultural institutions.
Article
Full-text available
As competition for discretionary expenditures increases and visitor expectations rise, service quality is likely to be a key to zoos remaining viable. For most zoo visitors, the key determinant of quality service is likely to be the tangible elements of the zoo, Thus, managers' success in enhancing visitors' quality of experience is likely to be dependent upon their ability to manipulate and enhance the quality of a zoo's tangible elements. With this in mind, the study's four objectives were to identify: (1) the level of visitor expectations associated with service quality domains; (2) the relative importance of service quality domains; (3) perceptions of performance of service quality domains; and (4) the benefits sought by visitors. Managerial implications were derived from the discrepancy scores between performance and expectations. Service quality is defined by expectancy-disconfirmation theory, which states that a visitor's expectancy level provides a baseline from which confirmatory or disconfirmatory judgments are made about level of performance. The expectations data were collected from adult respondents before they entered the zoo. Data to measure the other constructs were collected from a questionnaire which respondents mailed back after their visit. The sample was comprised of 606 visitors, of whom 373 (62%) returned the mail-back questionnaire. Seven service quality domains were measured by 28 items. They were: wildlife, wildlife information, overall cleanliness and accessibility, general information, staffing, comfort ameni- ties, and education. Six benefits sought domains were measured by 27 items. They were: family togetherness, wildlife enjoyment, wildlife appre- ciation and learning, companionship, escape, and introspection. The most important service quality domains and those for which visitors had the highest expectations were wildlife and wildlife information. The results indicated that the zoo performed well on these key domains. The education domain was rated lowest in performance and in importance, and next to lowest in expectations. This creates a challenge for the zoo, since education is a central element in its mission statement. The most important benefits sought were family togetherness and wildlife enjoyment. Substantially less important were wildlife appreciation and companionship, while escape and introspection were relatively unim- portant. The emphasis on family was reflected in the profile of visitor groups, 80% of which contained children. These types of studies enable organizations to benchmark their performance against visitors' expectations and what is important to them. The benchmarking could be extended by replicating the study periodically to monitor progress over time, and by implementing it at other zoos to identify Fort Worth Zoo's strengths and weaknesses compared to its peer organizations.
Article
Full-text available
Zoological parks are considered to be the most important source of contact between people and animals in modern society. Charged with the dual responsibility of caring for animals and people, zoos need information regarding both aspects of their mission statement. This article describes zoo visitors in relation to their primary motivation (educational vs. recreational) and social orientation (intrinsic vs. altruistic). These components were isolated and compared using t tests for dependent measures. Educational, recreational, intrinsic, and altruistic reasons were found to be statistically significant, thus yielding multiple satisfactions associated with a zoological park. Contemporary zoos may not recognize the value of conducting social research or the importance of meeting visitors’ needs. However, in order to broaden their base of political and financial support, zoos should attempt to increase public benefits.
Article
The family group is the predominant social unit within which people choose to spend their leisure time. In recreational contexts, there is a call for better understanding of the influence children have on family group decision making and behaviour. Based upon an empirical, national study of zoo visiting, and visitors’ and latent visitors’ images of traditional UK zoos, this paper explores the role of children as a determinant of demand. The available literature addressing the influence of children on leisure-related purchase decisions and behaviour is reviewed, together with an examination of research pertaining to the demand for zoos. The presence of children is found to have a notable influence on the demand for this recreational experience. Family life stage is a significant determinant of attraction choice and thus visiting status. Furthermore, the zoo is perceived as a place to take children, a factor that both encourages and deters zoo visiting. It seems the presence of children in a visiting group has a positive influence on the decision to visit a recreational setting when the setting is perceived to be child/family orientated. The implications of these findings are explored, with emphasis placed on the management challenges facing zoos as a result of demographic change and the expanding, traditionally latent visiting, seniors market.
Article
The authors report further evidence bearing on the relations among restorative experiences, self-regulation, and place attachment. University students (n = 101) described their favorite places and experiences in them, and 98 other students described unpleasant places. Natural settings were overrepresented among favorite places and underrepresented among the unpleasant places. In open-ended accounts, frequent mention of being relaxed, being away from everyday life, forgetting worries, and reflecting on personal matters indicated a link between favorite places and restorative experience. Restoration was particularly typical of natural favorite places. Structured evaluations of being away, fascination, coherence, and compatibility indicated they were experienced to a high degree in the favorite places, although fascination to a lesser degree than compatibility. The favorite and unpleasant places differed substantially in all four restorative qualities but especially in being away and compatibility. Self-referencing appears to be more characteristic of favorite place experiences than engaging or interesting environmental properties.
Article
The world's urban population has few opportunities for contact with real wild nature and little chance to develop a connection with nature in everyday life. To redress this problem in Western culture, major urban zoos are attempting to bridge the deficit in nature experience by constructing more simulated nature experiences as part of the animal viewing opportunity. The tourist value proposition in urban zoos, however, may not be in the simulated experience of artificial nature, but in the very real and authentic encounter with live "wild" animals and the contemplation of how our human society relates to the biological world. This article explores how zoo visitors describe their engagement with wildlife, how zoos provoke consideration of personal ethical relationships to nature, and how zoos connect an urban public to the natural world. This article builds on the biophilia hypothesis by considering the sociological attributes of zoo visiting and how the novel experience of encountering captive wild animals helps to develop environmental awareness. The article explores how a poetry installation at one urban zoo served to evidence this awareness in visitor comments, about conservation, personal connections to nature, and one aspect of the restorative role zoos offer. It is suggested that the uniqueness of live animals in the zoo is a forum for individuals to question the continuity between self and the natural world.
Article
Abstract  Considerable time and effort have been invested in understanding the motivations of museum visitors. Many investigators have sought to describe why people visit museums, resulting in a range of descriptive categorizations. Recently, investigators have begun to document the connections between visitors' entering motivations and their exiting learning. Doering and Pekarik have proposed starting with the idea that visitors are likely to enter a museum with an “entry narrative” (1996; see also Pekarik, Doering and Karns 1999). Doering and Pekarik argue that these entry narratives are likely to be self-reinforcing, directing both learning and behavior, since visitors' perceptions of satisfaction will be directly related to experiences that resonate with their entering narrative. Falk took these ideas one step further and proposed that—although people have diverse reasons for choosing to visit museums—these diverse reasons tended to cluster around a relatively small number of motivational categories (2006). These categories appeared to be related to visitors' desires to use the museum for fulfilling identity-related needs. Each of us assumes many identities over the course of our life. Some of our identities are enduring and long-lasting; others are more ephemeral and situation-specific; all help us navigate through the complexities of life.
Article
Using the Implicit Association Test (IAT), we examined the tendency for people to associate self with natural or built environments, the malleability of these scores across context, and the relationship between these implicit associations and explicit attitudes about environmental issues. Five studies are reported using a handheld IAT administration in a variety of field contexts. The psychometric properties of the handheld administration were comparable to those obtained with laboratory administration. The cumulative results across the 5 studies suggest that implicit self–nature associations are malleable, but that change requires long-term or repeated experiences. Findings are interpreted within a model of environmental identity.