ArticlePDF Available

Pre-performance routines in sport: Current understanding and future directions

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The development of a pre-performance routine continues to be an intuitively appealing and widely accepted technique to enhance preparation for performance in sport. However, actual understanding falls somewhat short of that required to recommend with confidence the development of a pre-performance routine. At a fundamental level it is still not clear what function routines fulfil, what they should consist of or the most effective way to teach them. This review seeks to clarify current understanding and highlight areas requiring further attention. Greater understanding is required to understand the psychological components of routines as well as understanding the process through which a routine is developed/taught.
Content may be subject to copyright.
For Peer Review Only










URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
1
Running Head: PRE-PERFORMANCE ROUTINES IN SPORT
Pre-performance routines in sport: Current understanding and future directions
Page 1 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
2
Abstract
The Development of a pre-performance routine continues to be an intuitively appealing and
widely accepted technique to enhance preparation for performance in sport. However, actual
understanding falls somewhat short of that required to recommend with confidence the
development of a pre-performance routine. At a fundamental level it is still not clear what
function routines fulfil, what they should consist of or the most effective way to teach them. This
review seeks to clarify current understanding and highlight areas requiring further attention.
Greater understanding is required to understand the psychological components of routines as
well as understanding the process through which a routine is developed / taught.
Keywords: pre-performance routines, preparation, performance, strategies
Page 2 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
3
Pre-performance routines in sport: Current understanding and future directions
Introduction
Over the past 30 years the development and application of pre-performance routines in sport has been an
area of specific interest to sport psychologists, coaches, and performers alike. Developing,
understanding, and enhancing the preparation for performance has been seen as a very real way of
ultimately enhancing performance level and consistency. The use of this understanding to develop a
consistent approach in preparing for performance has been advocated as a tool through which these
higher levels of performance and consistency can be achieved. Authors such as Lidor and Tenenbaum
(1993), Boutcher (1990), and Lobmeyer and Wasserman (1986) have advocated the use of structured
routines prior to performance, which are believed to be an extremely important behavioural technique to
help performers to attain high levels of achievement in sport. A number of definitions have been offered
in the literature in an attempt to clarify what is meant by the term pre-performance routine. These range
from Crampton (1989, p.9) who referred to an “ordered collection of thoughts and behaviours” to Foster,
Weigand, and Baines (2006) who suggested that pre-performance routines involve “cognitive and
behavioural elements that intentionally help regulate arousal and concentration (p.167). However, the
definition suggested by Moran (1996) appears to have been adopted most readily by other studies
exploring the pre-performance routine phenomenon. Moran (1996) defined pre-performance routines as
“a sequence of task-relevant thoughts and actions which an athlete engages in systematically prior to his
or her performance of a specific sports skill” (p.177). This definition clearly highlights the importance of
both cognitive processes and behaviours in preparation to successfully execute performance
At this point it is important to clarify differences in the associated literature
regarding terminology. Authors have tended to refer to either pre-performance routines (Cotterill,
2008; Holder, 2003; Jackson, 2001, 2003; Lonsdale & Tam, 2007; Marlow, Bull, Heath &
Page 3 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
4
Shambrook, 1998; McCann, Lavallee, & Lavallee 2001; Moran, 1996, 2004; Singer, 2002) or
pre-shot routines (Gayton, Cielinski, Francis-Keniston, & Hearns, 1989; Cohn, Rotella, & Lloyd,
1990; Crews & Boutcher, 1986; Douglas & Fox, 2002; Harle & Vickers, 2001; Mack, 2001;
Moore & Stevenson, 1994; Shaw, 2002). All of the references to pre-shot routines occur in
studies where performance was described in terms of shots (either basketball or golf). As a
result, pre-shot routines could be classified as a more sport / activity specific description of a pre-
performance routine. In the literature there is also reference to a ‘mental preparation routine’.
However the definition of this routine as a “systematic, ritualized patterns of physical actions and
pre-planned sequences of thoughts and arousal related cues” (Gould & Udry, 1994, p.483) again
refers to the same phenomenon described by Moran (1996) whilst defining pre-performance
routines.
Although the use of pre-performance routines is more readily implemented with closed
skills (such as a golf shot, basketball free throw, long jump, hockey penalty flick, etc), they have
also been examined in some open skill sports. Pre-performance routine research in sport to date
has explored basketball (Czech, Ploszay, & Burke, 2004; Gayton, Cielinski, Francis-Keniston, &
Hearns, 1989; Hall & Erffemeyer, 1983; Harle & Vickers, 2001; Lamirandt & Rainey, 1994;
Lidor, Arnon, & Bronstein, 1999; Lidor & Tenenbaum, 1993; Lobmeyer & Wasserman, 1986;
Lonsdale & Tam, 2007; Mack, 2001; Predebon & Docker, 1992; Southard & Miracle, 1993;
Wrisberg & Penn, 1992); Bowling (Kirschenbaum, Ordman, & Tomarken, 1982) dance (Vergeer
& Hanrahan, 1998); diving (Highlen & Bennett, 1983); football (Vealey, 1986); golf
(Beauchamp, Halliwell, Fournier, & Koestner (1996); Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Cohn, Rotella,
& Lloyd, 1990; Cotterill, 2008; Fairweather & Potgeiter, 1993; Kingston & Hardy, 2001;
Kirschenbaum & Bale, 1980; McCann, Lavallee, & Lavallee, 2001; Rotella & Bunter, 1981;
Page 4 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
5
Shaw, 2002; Thomas & Over, 1994; Yancey, 1977); Gymnastics (Mahoney & Avener, 1977;
Schack, 1997); rugby union (Jackson, 2003; Jackson & Baker, 2001); skiing and skating (Orlick,
1986); tennis (Moore, 1986); Bowling (Kirschenbaum, 1987); volleyball (Lidor & Mayan,
2005); track and field athletics (Cotterill & Greenlees, 2003);water polo (Marlow, Bull, Heath, &
Shambrook, 1998); and wrestling (Gould, Weiss, & Weinberg, 1981). This review will seek to
clarify the extent of current knowledge regarding pre-performance routines. Seeking to critically
review the suggested impact that routines have on performance. Clarification will also be sought
regarding current understanding of how the pre-performance routine actually impacts on
performance, and how we can seek to measure and understand this component of performance.
Finally, this article will look to review current understanding regarding the teaching and
development of pre-performance routines in sport.
Impact of pre-performance routines on performance
The area relating to pre-performance routine use in sport that has received the greatest
attention is the link with performance. Numerous authors have explored this link across a range
of sports. A summary of the various experimental studies exploring pre-performance routines is
presented in Table 1. The most popular approach has sought to test a control group’s
performance against that of an experimental group, which has been taught and practiced a pre-
performance routine. Hall and Erffemeyer (1983) in their study of 10 highly skilled female
basketball players reported that visuomotor behaviour rehearsal improved free throw accuracy.
This positive impact of routines in basketball was also reported by Lobmeyer and Wasserman
(1986) with 43 participants, and Gayton, Cielinski, Francis-Keniston, and Hearns (1989) with 25
high school basketball players, and Wrisberg and Pein (1992) exploring success rates with and
without pre-shot routines. Taking this one step further in golf, Crews and Boutcher (1987);
Page 5 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
6
Boutcher and Zinsser (1990) and, Douglas and Fox (2002) all highlighted that better performers
had more consistent routines in their behavioural composition and their temporal duration. The
positive impact of routine usage on performance has also been reported by Czech et al. (2004),
and Hill and Borden (1993). While the link between pre-performance routine usage has been
widely reported, what is less clear is the function that routines actually fulfil in enhancing
performance.
***Insert table 1. near here!***
The function(s) that pre-performance routines fulfil
The popularity of pre-performances routine use in sport stems largely from the belief that
they enable performers to concentrate more effectively. To this end, Boutcher (1992) suggested
five main benefits that pre-performance routines may provide to golfers. These included:
improving concentration by encouraging the golfer to focus their thoughts on the task-relevant
cues, helping the golfer overcome a natural tendency to dwell on negatives, allowing the golfer
to select the appropriate motor schema, preventing ‘warm-up’ decrements and the devotion of
excessive attention to the mechanics of their automatic skill.
Numerous hypotheses have been developed to explain the role that pre-performance
routines satisfy. In particular it has been suggested that they prescribe an attentional focus
(Boutcher, 1992; Czech, Ploszay, & Burke, 2004; Harle & Vickers, 2001); reduce the impact of
distractions (Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Moore & Stevenson, 1984; Weinberg, 1988); act as a
trigger for well learnt movement patterns (Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Moran, 1996); divert
attention from task irrelevant thoughts to task relevant thoughts (Gould & Udry, 1994; Maynard,
1998); improve concentration (Foster, Weigand, & Baines, 2006; Holder, 2003); enhance the
Page 6 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
7
recall of physiological and psychological states (Marlow et al., 1998); help performers achieve
behavioural and temporal consistency in their performance (Wrisberg & Penn, 1992); prevent
performers focusing on the mechanics of their skills and the resulting unravelling of automaticity
(Beilock & Carr, 2001; Beilock, Carr, MacMahon, & Starks, 2002); or allow performers to
evaluate conditions and calibrate their responses (Schack, 2002). Singer (2002) more generally
suggested that the purpose of pre-performance routines is to “put oneself in an optimal
emotional, high self-expectant, confident, and focused state immediately prior to execution, and
to remain that way during the act” (p.6). Shaw (2002) also hypothesized that the value of pre-
performance routines may be in that they pre-sensitize the movement system to the appropriate
perception-action coupling (Williams, Davids & Shaw, 1999) between the environment and the
player. Each of these suggested functions have emerged as a result of other analyses and have not
themselves been the focus of investigation. For example, Boutcher (1992) suggested that pre-
performance routines help by providing an attentional focus, but did not test whether the routines
do actually provide an attentional focus.
Although intuitively appealing there is not sufficient depth of research explicitly
exploring and testing the function that pre-performance routines fulfil. Of the above mentioned
studies most are based upon suggestions made in prior related publications (Beilock & Carr,
2001; Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Czech, 2004; Foster et al., 2006; Gould & Udry, 1994; Harle &
Vickers, 2001; Holder, 2003; Shaw, 2002; Singer, 2002; Wrisberg & Penn, 1992) which in turn
can be traced back to authors including Keele (1973), Nideffer (1976), and Schmidt, (1981).
Neither of these three authors explicitly explored pre-performance routines. Of the other studies
that have actually explored the function of pre-performance routines, Hill and Borden (1995)
reported that attentional cueing scripts aided performance and therefore should be considered a
Page 7 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
8
component of pre-performance preparation. Most pre-performance experimental studies have
sought to explore the link between routine use and performance, but have not taken the next step
to explore the mechanism through which this occurs. As a result, further investigation is required
to explore the function of the pre-performance routine. Both the coaches’ and the athletes’
perception of pre-performance routine function should be explored in greater detail.
Experimental designs should also seek to test for evidence to support the proposed functions
highlighted in earlier publications.
Components of the pre-performance routine
Behavioural Characteristics
Early research exploring pre-performance routines focused heavily on the behaviours and
the timing of pre-performance routines. Authors such as Crews and Boutcher (1986), Boutcher
and Crews (1987), Wrisberg and Pein (1992) Southard and Miracle (1993), Southard and Amos
(1996), and Cotterill and Collins (2003) sought to describe the temporal and behavioural
characteristics of pre-performance routines. Initial research focused on comparing novice and
elite performers or comparing successful and unsuccessful performance to ascertain if
differences existed. These descriptions enhanced our understanding of the behavioural
components of the routines used and either the overall duration of the routines or the duration of
discrete behavioural components but the importance of this information is limited. Differences
were reported, but these differences were not explained. Is the fact that expert performers have
more consistent routines a contributory factor to the higher level of performance and consistency
they produce? Or is the consistency merely a function of longer time spent practicing?
Understanding the behavioural components of routines and their relationship with mental
processes is the next step (Cohn et al., 1990)
Page 8 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
9
Underlying Psychological Processes
Research exploring pre-performance routines in sport has not, to date, comprehensively
explored in depth the psychological processes that occur during the routine stage prior to the
execution of the required movement, even though the importance of a positive affective state
prior to competition has been identified (Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996). Indeed, it could be
argued that inappropriate mental states can lead to task-irrelevant thoughts, loss of attention, and
inappropriate physical states which can result in poor performance (Boutcher, 1990). The
majority of previous researchers in the area have suggested a number of potential psychological
processes, which include the utilization of imagery (Hall, Rodgers, & Barr, 1990) and
distracting the attentional focus from irrelevant thoughts to task relevant thoughts (Maynard,
1998). It has also been suggested that the pre-performance routine specifically improves both
concentration and performance (Harle & Vickers, 2001), and enhances the recall of physiological
and psychological states (Marlow et al., 1998). But as previously highlighted, these suggestions
are not based upon empirical findings. Cohn, Rotella and Lloyd (1990) in their study exploring
the effect of a cognitive-behavioural intervention on a pre-shot routine concluded that future
research should explore the covert mental routine, but to date this does not appear to have been
explored. Shaw (2002), in his case study of a professional golfer, reported that the professional
golfer had experienced some attentional benefits arising from the use of a pre-performance
routine. Specifically, the golfer reported that “the new routine had made him more focused for
each shot and therefore, less distracted by irrelevancies” (p.117). However, although a number of
psychological processes have been suggested to accompany the pre-performance routine, to date
only Jackson and Baker (2001), and Cotterill, Sanders and Collins (In-press) have explored these
processes in detail despite the potential benefits that a greater understanding of these processes
Page 9 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
10
would have for the performer, coach and sport psychologist. Jackson and Baker (2001) in their
case study of an elite rugby kicker found that the player utilised a range of psychological
strategies including specific mental cues, thought stopping, inverse simulation, visualization and
relaxation techniques, but interestingly these differed from attempt to attempt. Indeed the
authors concluded that the most important determinant of kicking performance in the
competitive environment was the successful application of specific psychological strategies
rather than the temporal consistency of the pre-performance routine. Cotterill et al. (In-press) in
their phenomenological study of elite golfers interviewed six participants to explore the
psychological strategies and techniques used. They concluded that “the development of pre-
performance routines, and in particular the psychological skills employed within the routines, is
dependent on the personality, coping resources, and situational appraisals of each individual
performer”(p.19). To date, there is limited research exploring the psychological processes and
strategies adopted by performers. Do performers employ a consistent set of skills and strategies
with each performance or are they more transient? How do performers develop these
psychological strategies and skills? Are pre-performance routines taught or evolve independently
over time? Answers to these questions would allow the development of more effective routines.
Teaching and developing pre-performance routines
A number of approaches have been suggested in order to attempt to achieve consistent and highly
effective performance. These include Singer’s (1988) 5-step approach and Murphy’s (1994) 4-point
model. The 5 steps proposed by Singer (see figure 1.) were readying, imaging, focusing attention,
executing and evaluating. Lidor and Tenenbaum (1993) implemented this 5-step model with basketball
players and found the most important stage to be the readying stage, the preparation to perform. They
reported that the greater the preparation time the greater the success rate. Cotterill (2008) in a review of
Page 10 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
11
the practical guidelines to developing pre-performance routines in golf suggested that the first four steps
of Singer’s (1988) 5-step approach (readying, imaging, focusing attention, executing) could be used as a
global template for golf routines. It was suggested that this global template would allow the flexibility
for the routine to be modified regarding behaviours and timing, but still fulfil the requirements of the
individual golfer. Liu and Zhang (2003) used Singer’s approach for their ‘specific pre-performance
routine group’ when comparing the effects of a specific and a self-developed routine. Results
demonstrated significant differences with the Specific group performing better.
***Insert figure 1. near here!***
Murphy‘s (1994) (see figure 2.) model was dubbed the ‘performance management model’. The
steps he advocated were: practice, preparation, performance and analysis to create the ‘flow state’.
Murphy (1994) suggested this approach lead to optimal performance as performers have control over the
internal environment in which the game is played, but not the external environment. There is currently
limited publications that have explored the application of this particular approach.
***Insert figure 2. near here!***
A significant portion of research exploring pre-performance routines in sport has focused
on the behavioural and temporal characteristics of the routines. As a result, recommendations
have in the main focused on developing consistency in both. Crews and Boutcher’s (1986) study
of elite golfers highlighted that consistency of the timing and behavioural characteristics were
key to the eventual impact of the routine itself. Indeed Crews and Boutcher (1986) advocated
Page 11 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
12
that the timing of the routines was essential with expert players taking longer to execute their
routine at both putting and full swing shots. Extending this contention, Boutcher (1990)
recommended that performers should assess the consistency of their pre-performance routines by
time analysis. Douglas and Fox (2002) further argued that professional golfers drill themselves to
perform a pre-shot routine regardless of the situation, contending that this factor may actually
distinguish the very best and highly competent professional golfers. This very prescriptive
approach has been questioned by a number of authors including Cotterill (2008), Holder (2003),
and Jackson and Baker (2001). Jackson and Baker suggested that although consistency in the
sequence of behaviours is important, other factors such as task difficulty significantly influenced
the duration of the routine. Holder (2003) emphasized that the most critical feature of the
application of pre-performance routines is their individualization, highlighting that what
performers do, as opposed to how long it takes them, is key. Cotterill (2008) further endorsed
this ‘individualized’ approach advocating that each routine should be modelled around the needs
of the individual performer. Going one step further Cotterill (2008) suggested that a generic
template for the routine, based upon the psychological demands, was crucial. This suggestion
was made specifically with golf in mind where research had indicated that golfers utilised
different behavioural and temporal routines for different shot types. This solution offers the
opportunity to develop one routine which could be applied differently depending on the task
demands.
Pre-performance routines for learners and novices
Lidor and Singer (2000) suggested that in order to effectively use pre-performance
routines in sport one should acquire the technique(s) as early as possible in the process of
learning the relevant skills. However, they also highlighted that at the time the current literature
Page 12 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
13
did not provide any recommendations regarding (1) how to teach them to learners, (2)
incorporating the routine with successful performance, or (3) integrating routines into
competitive situations. Lidor and Singer (2000) sought to rectify this by suggesting their
guidelines for pre-performance phases. These phases, outlined in table 2. Include: readying,
focusing attention, and evaluating. Lidor and Mayan (2005) suggested that another benefit of
developing pre-performance routines early in the learning process assisted in developing a plan
of action and activating appropriate physical and cognitive processes. This suggests that learners
“would then know more about how to manage their own cognitive processes and how to analyze
themselves and situational demands” (p.360). Further detailed exploration of individual cases of
a longitudinal nature here would provide a greater insight into the teaching and development of
routines from novice through to expert. Regarding the learning and development of pre-
performance routines, the views, perceptions and understanding of the coach / instructor have
been widely overlooked. Coaches influence and mould the development of pre-performance
routines but where does their knowledge and understanding originate? How do coaches teach
and develop routines? Future research should look to answer these questions and explore the
impact that the coach has on the routines’ development and the performers understanding of
routine importance and function.
***Insert table 2. near here***
Approaches to measuring, quantifying, and understanding routines
There are a number of research approaches that have been utilised to explore pre-performance
behaviours, techniques and strategies in sport. Specifically, research has focused on recording the
Page 13 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
14
duration, order and instances of certain categories of behaviours, measured attention-related events using
psychophysiology, and explored the psychological techniques and strategies utilised by players through
direct interviews.
Behavioural / temporal description
Wrisberg and Pein (1992) suggested that in order to study pre-performance routines
meaningfully the researcher must decide how to quantify them. Previous measures exploring pre-
performance behaviour in sport have focused on measuring the frequency, duration and
consistency of the participants’ behaviours prior to, and during the execution of performance. For
example, Boutcher and Zinsser (1990) in their study exploring cardiac deceleration in elite and
beginning golfers during putting, specifically looked at the number of practice swings and the
number of glances at the hole of participants. A similar approach was adopted also by Cotterill
and Collins (2003), Crews and Boutcher (1986), and, Douglas and Fox (2002), who used a
modified version of events recording (Siedentop, 1983), including a number of discrete
behavioural actions (practice swings, waggles and glances at the target or hole) occurring pre and
post shot; the number of actions; their order; and certain time aspects. Jackson (2001), and
Jackson and Baker (2003) followed similar guidelines in recording the glances at the posts and
number of steps in rugby penalty kicking. This approach involves the recording of the type and
sequence of behaviours and actions that a performer demonstrates whilst executing their pre-
performance routine. Categorizing all observed behaviours is important to reduce the overall
number of variables. The sequence and duration of these behaviours are important to understand
whether performers are doing the same things in the same order prior to performance. This can
then be explored in relation to good Vs poor performance or elite Vs novice performers.
Recording behaviours also allows the clear identification of the beginning and end of a routine.
Page 14 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
15
As a result overall durations of the routines can be compared. Taking this a step further, the
relative durations of the discrete behavioural routines can also be compared, in particular
checking for the impact of other variables such as task difficulty on the overall and relative
durations.
Understanding through the participant
A small number of researchers have adopted an interview-based approach to exploring performer
thoughts and perceptions. Vergeer and Hanrahan (1998, p.52) interviewed 16 dancers, asking “what
methods, strategies, and techniques other than physical preparation do you use to train yourself mentally
and emotionally to improve your dancing?” Cohn et al. (1990) used interviews with their 3 male golfers
to discover whether the participants used routines, and how frequent this use was. Lidor and Mayan
(2005) used brief interviews when exploring whether beginners can benefit from pre-performance
routines. However, the focus of this prior research has failed to explore the performer’s views on the
function that their pre-performance fulfilled or the psychological strategies they employed, and more
importantly why they employed them. Jackson and Baker (2001) asked similar questions in their case
study of an elite rugby goal kicker while Cotterill et al. (in-press) conducted a phenomenological
analysis exploring both the function of the routines utilised and the psychological skills employed by
their six elite golfers. Both of these studies have focused on small sample sizes, and as a result, their
generalisability can be questioned. Cotterill et al. (In-press) adopted a ‘think aloud protocol’ similar to
that used by Ericsson and Simon (1993) and further developed by Ram and McCullagh (2003), where
participants were asked to verbalize the underlying cognitive processes relating to specific observed
behaviours. This was achieved using video footage of the pre-performance routines as a prompt.
Direct methods for measuring underlying psychological processes and strategies
Page 15 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
16
An alternative to the use of self-report measures for studying psychological processes
during preparation for performance can be provided by psychophysiological methods (Hassmen
& Kolvula, 2001). Psychophysiology can be defined as “the scientific study of cognitive,
emotional and behavioural phenomena as related to and revealed through physiological
principles and events” (Cassioppo & Tassinary, 1990, p.ix). This research approach was
supported by Lawton, Hung, Saarela, and Hatfield (1998) who advocated the use of
psychophysiological measures as an effective way of observing, and measuring mental processes
during real-time performance. Lawton et al. (1998) further suggested that this measure could
then be related to the underlying nervous system processes being utilised by the performer during
the pre-performance period.
This particular approach to measuring psychological processes is particularly useful as it
can “provide an objective and relatively non-invasive method of examining the complex
processes involved in sports performance as they take place” (Collins, 2002, p.17). This
approach can provide an unobtrusive, objective and real-time measurement of mental processes
associated with sports performance (Hung, 2002). The psychophysiological methods most suited
to the study of mental processes during ‘real-time’ performance and preparation are the use of
heart rate changes and electroencephalography. Singer (2002) suggested that on the basis of the
psychophysiological evidence to date, which has been obtained primarily in aiming sports,
outstanding athletes and performances could be distinguished from other athletes and
performances in the pre-performance state.
Cardiac deceleration
Cardiac deceleration is a measure of the duration of time between a specific, reoccurring point of
the cardiac cycle and the next corresponding point. Lacey and Lacey (1966) proposed that
Page 16 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
17
decreases in cardiac activity prior to performance facilitate attentional processes. This was due to
decreased feedback to the brain associated with a decrease in cardiac activity. Janelle, Singer, and
Williams (1999) suggested that when the act requires an internal attentional focus, heart rate
acceleration results in greater cortical activity. Landers, Petruzzello, Slazar, Crews, Kubitz,
Gannon, and Han. (1991) reported that there tends to be a decrease in heart rate during the
preparation and concentration phase related to performing a motor skill.
Boutcher and Zinsser’s’ (1990) study of university golfers measured the inter-beat
intervals (IBI’s) between heartbeats immediately prior to, during and post completing a putting
task. Their results showed that this IBI increased significantly in length prior to performance
indicating a decrease in heart rate. Boutcher and Zinsser’s overall conclusions indicated that there
was a greater cardiac deceleration associated with superior putting performance. Radlo,
Steinberg, Singer, Barba, and Melnikov (2002) also reported that when each performer’s four
best and worst throws were analyzed in conjunction with heart rate, the results showed that a
significant HRD was associated with the best shots. Additionally, the worst shots were associated
with a significant increase in heart rate.
Electrodermal Activity
A small number of studies have used electrodermal indicators to explore cognitive
function. Tremayne & Barry (2001) took electrodermal measurements to cater for the degree of
activation as a potential confounding variable whilst also recording heart-rate data. Guillot,
Collet, Molinaro, and Dittmar (2004) also recorded electrodermal activity, specifically recording
skin resistance and potential blood flow, skin temperature, and heart rate.. Guillot et al. (2004)
reported that a major negative skin potential response was found in concentration and shooting
Page 17 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
18
phases for best shots. Suggesting that there could be a link between the autonomic nervous
system and good performance.
Electroencephalography
Electroencephalography (EEG) uses electrodes attached to the scalp to record the changes
in electrical potential, which accompany peripheral nerve activity in the brain. This measure is
then used as an indicator of neural function. Lawton et al. (1998) concluded that there is a fairly
widespread decrease in cerebral activity when preparing immediately to perform motor tasks.
Specific cortex areas of interest are the occipital, parietal and temporal lobes of the brain.
Salazar, Landers, Petruzzello, Han, Crews, and Kubitz (1990) reported that when four best and
worst archery shots were compared, increases in alpha activity in the left hemisphere were
related to poorer performance.
Crews, Lutz, Nilsson and Marriott (1998) in their study exploring psychophysiological
indicators during golf putting suggested that EEG measures of automatic, successful
performance confirm that low levels of activity exist in various areas of the brain immediately
prior to initiating the stroke during the golf putt. These results appeared to reinforce previous
findings (Crews & Landers, 1993; Hatfield, Landers, & Ray, 1984) that suggested that increased
alpha activity was related to reduced error, and a quieting of the left hemisphere appears
important for successful performance. Understanding EEG data in relation to the pre-
performance period would offer a greater understanding of the psychological strategies employed
by performers.
Various recent studies have explored the alpha power characteristics exhibited by sports
performers in various environments. Janelle, Hillman, Apparies, Murray, Meili, Fallon, and
Page 18 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
19
Hatfield (2000) reported that expert marksmen exhibited a significant increase in left hemisphere
alpha power (a more relaxed state) compared with the right hemisphere, as well as asymmetrical
patterns of alpha and beta activity in both hemispheres. Landers, Han, Salazar, Petruzzello,
Kubitz, and Gannon (1994) reported that the amounts of alpha activity across both hemispheres
at the beginning of learning were relatively low. However, as the participants become more
skilled, an increase in alpha activity was evident in the left hemisphere while activity in the right
hemisphere remained constant. All of these studies suggest a link between alpha power,
particularly in the left hemisphere, and the pre-performance state.
While attention concerns the selection of input to the organism, intention refers to the
selection of output (O’Connor, 1981). The perception of objects requires attention to external
space with the consequent peripheral feedback, control of action depends on the implementation
of internal plans and can be a feed forward process (Hoff & Arbib, 1992). This implies the
organism is looking ahead towards a new state, the representation of which steers the
transformation until its completion (Jeannerod, 1994). The intention mode of behaviour probably
uses feed-forward control, based on centrally stored instructions, to monitor the relationship
between the actual and predicted trajectories (Loze, Collins, & Shaw, 1999). When a task being
performed is well known to the individual and is performed in a predictable environment there is
a change from attentional control to intentional control. Prior to performance there is a shift from
the external environment to the internal environment. In essence there is a shift from attention to
external sensory stimulus to an intentional state. This intentional state refers to the selection of
motor output prior to skill execution based on an internal representation of the skill and the
appropriate action(s) necessary for a successful performance (Jeannerod, 1994).
Page 19 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
20
The shift to intention is reflected in EEG. Alpha power has been shown to reflect the level
of visual attention to external stimuli. Due to the decrease in processing of external sensory
information there is an increase in alpha frequency and power in the occipital and parietal cortex.
In target sports this shift from attention to intention is generally acknowledged as occurring 2.5
seconds prior to trigger release. This shift however, is individualistic and dependent on task
difficulty. Loze, Collins and Holmes (2001) in a study with elite shooters examined the pre-shot
occipital EEG alpha-power activity of elite air-pistol shooters. Specifically they selected the best
and worst 5 shots for each shooter based on four indicators of shot quality, which were either
outcome based or rated by experts. Loze et al. (2001) found that pre-shot alpha power increased
through epochs 1 to 3 prior to best shots, but decreased prior to worst shots. They also found that
alpha power was also significantly greater during the final pre-shot epochs of the best shots. This
implies that prior to best performance in closed skill tasks there is an increase in occipital alpha
power immediately prior to performance. This indicates that achieving this state is essential to
achieving optimal performance levels. Comparing the preparatory stages of good and poor
performance in golf could offer an insight into exactly what the optimal cognitive state prior to
performance is and possibly how the performer can achieve the required psychological state prior
to performance. A logical next step, in the use of hear rate and EEG methods, is to explore the
potential coupling between psychophysiological indicators and specific routine behaviours and
thought patterns.
Gaps in current knowledge and directions for future research
Pre-performance routines have been well observed and described. Future research needs
to go beyond this point. The first step is to develop a clear understanding of what function
routines actually fulfil. One approach here will be the adoption of qualitative methodologies to
Page 20 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
21
explore the thoughts feelings and understanding of the performers who so readily use pre-
performance routines as a core component of their performance. More mixed study designs are
also required to test some of the hypothesized functions of the routine such as enhancing the
recall of psychological and physiological states (Marlow et al., 1998) and acting as a trigger for
well-learnt movement patterns (Crews & Boutcher, 1987; Moran, 1996).
There has been extensive description of the behaviours that constitute the routines used
by performers, specifically focusing on behaviour frequency, consistency and temporal
components. The associated mental skills or processes underpinning these behaviours have
received much less attention. Are these mental skills as consistent as these physical behaviours?
Do they link / couple with the behaviours or are they separate? How does the use of these skills
and strategies evolve over time, are they taught or do they develop independently? On this last
point, study designs that are more longitudinal in nature would be welcome. Specifically, a
greater insight into the teaching and development of routines from novice through to expert is
needed. Regarding the learning and development of pre-performance routines the views,
perceptions and understanding of the coach / instructor have been widely overlooked. However,
the coach is often cited as a key influence on the performers skill development and consistency.
How do coaches teach and develop routines? Future research should look to answer these
questions and explore the impact that the coach has on the development of routines and
performers’ understanding of their importance and function. Finally, researchers should seek to
explore potential relationships between psychophysiological indicators, behaviour components,
and reported mental components of pre- performance routines.
Page 21 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
22
References
Beauchamp, P. H., Halliwell, W. R., Fournier, J. F., & Koestner, R. (1996). Effects of cognitive-
behavioral psychological skills training on the motivation, preparation, and putting
performance of novice golfers. Sport Psychologist, 10, 157-170.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs
choking under pressure? Journal of experimental psychology: Generalize, 130, 701-725.
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., McMahn, C., & Starks, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes
counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill focused attention on novice and
experienced performers of sensorimotor skills. Journal of experimental psychology:
Applied, 8, 6-16.
Boutcher, S. H. (1990). The role of performance routines in sport. In J. G. Jones, and L. Hardy.
(Eds.), Stress and performance in sport (pp.231-245). New York: J. Wiley.
Boutcher, S. H. (1992). Attentional and Athletic Performance: An integrated pre-performance
routineoach. In T. S. Horn (Ed.). Advances in Sport Psychology,(pp.251-266).
Champaign, ILL: Human Kinetics.
Boutcher, S. H., & Crews, D. J. (1987). The effect of a preshot attentional routine on a well
learned skill. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 18, 30-39.
Boutcher, S. H., & Zinsser, N. W. (1990). Cardiac deceleration of elite and beginning golfers
during putting. Journal of sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 37-47.
Carlstedt, R. A. (2004). Critical Moments During Competition: A mind-body model of sport
performance when it counts most. New York: Psychology Press.
Cassioppo, J. Y., & Tassinary, L. G. (Eds.) (1990). Principles of psychophysiology: Physical,
social and inferential elements. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Page 22 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
23
Cohn, P. J. (1991). An exploratory study of peak performance in golf. The Sport Psychologist, 5,
1-14.
Cohn, P. J., Rotella, R., & Lloyd, J. W. (1990). Effects of a cognitive-behavioral intervention on
the pre-shot routine and performance in golf. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 33-42.
Collins, D. (2002). Psychophysiology and athletic performance. In B. Blumenstein, M. Bar-Eli,
and G. Tenenbaum (Eds.) Brain and body in sport and exercise. Chichester: John Wiley
and Sons.
Collins, D., Powell, G., & Davies, I. (1991). Cerebral activity prior to motion task performance:
an electroencephalographic study. Journal of Sport Sciences, 9, 313 – 324.
Cotterill, S. (2008). Developing effective pre-performance routines in golf. Sport & Exercise
Psychology Review, 4(2), 10-16.
Cotterill, S., & Collins, D. (2003) Exploring the Consistency of Pre-Shot Routines Across Shot-
Type in Golf. Proceedings of the XIth European Congress of Sport Psychology.
Copenhagen: Denmark.
Cotterill, S., & Greenlees, I. (2003). Temporal Consistency of pre-performance routines in
world-class polevaulters. Proceedings from the VIIth IOC World Congress on Sport
Sciences. Athens, GR: September.
Cotterill, S., Sanders, R., & Collins, D. (In-press) Developing effective pre-performance routines
in golf: Why don’t we ask the golfer? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology.
Crampton, J. (1989). Establishing pre-shot routines for tournament golfers: an example of the
use of micro-computers in performance planning. Sports Coach, 12, 9-12.
Page 23 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
24
Crews, D. J., & Boutcher, S. H. (1986). An exploratory observational behavior analysis of
professional golfers during competition. Journal of Sport Behaviour, 9, 51-58.
Crews, D., Lutz, P., Nilsson, P., & Marriott, L. (1998). Psychophysiological Indicators of
Confidence and Habituation During Golf Putting. In M. R. Farrally and A. J. Cochran
(Eds.) Science and Golf III, (pp.159-165), Champaign, Ill: Human Kinetics.
Crews, D. J., & Landers, D. M. (1993). Electroencephalagraphic measures of attentional patterns
prior to the golf putt. Medicine in Sport and Exercise, 25, 116-126.
Czech, D. R., Ploszay, A. J., & Burke, K. L. (2004). An Examnation of the Maintenance of
PreShot Routines in Basketball Free Throw Shooting. Journal of Sport Behavior, 27,
323-329.
Douglas, K., & Fox, K. R. (2002). Performance and practise of elite women european tour
golfers during pressure and non-pressure putting simulation. In E. Thain (Ed.), Science
and Golf IV (pp. 246-256). London: Routledge.
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fairweather, K. G., & Potgeiter, J. R. (1993). The effect of pre-shot strategies on golf putting.
South African Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and Recreation, 16,
35-40.
Foster, D. J., Weigand, D. A., & Baines, D. (2006). The effect of removing superstitious
behavior and introducing pre-performance routine on basketball free-throw
performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18, 167-171.
Gayton, W. F., Cielinski, K. L., Francis-Keniston, W. J., & Heanes, J. F. (1989). Effects of pre-
shot routine on free-throw shooting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 317-318.
Page 24 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
25
Gould D., & Udry, E. (1994). Psychological skills for enhancing performance: arousal regulation
strategies. Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise, 26, 478-485.
Gould, D., Weiss, M., & Weinberg, R. (1981). Psychological characteristics of successful and
non-successful big ten wrestlers. Journal of Sport Psychology, 3, 69-81.
Guillot, A., Collet, C., Molinaro, C., & Dittmar, A. (2004). Expertise and peripheral autonomic
activity during the preparation phase in shooting events. Perceptual Motor Skills, 98,
371-381.
Hall, E. G., & Erffmeyer, E. S. (1983). The effect of visuo-motor behavior rehearsal with
videotaped modeling on free throw accuracy of intercollegiate female basketball players.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 343-346.
Hall, C. R., Rodgers, W.M., & Barr, K. A. (1990). The use of imagery by athletes in selected
sports. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 1-10.
Harle, S. K., & Vickers, J. N. (2001). Training quiet eye improves accuracy in basketball free
throw. The Sport Psychologist, 15, 289-305.
Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding Psychological Preparation for Sport:
Theory and Practise of elite performers, Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Hassmén, P., & Kolvula, N. (2001). Cardiac deceleration in elite golfers as modified by noise
and anxiety during putting. Perceptual and motor skills, 92, 947-957.
Hatfield, B. D., Landers, D., & Ray, W. J. (1984). Cognitive processes during self-paced motor
performance: An electroencephalographic profile of skilled marksmen. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 6, 42-57.
Highlen, P. S., & Bennett, B. B. (1983). Elite divers and wrestlers: A comparison between open
and closed skill athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 390-409.
Page 25 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
26
Hill, K. L., & Borden, F. (1993). The effect of attentional cueing scripts on competitive bowling
performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 503-512.
Hoff, B., & Abib, M. A. (1992). A model of the effects of speed, accuracy and perturbation on
visually guided reaching. In R. Caminiti, P. B. V. Johnson and Y. Burnod (Eds.), Control
of Arm Movement in Space, Berlin: Springer-Verlag (pp.285-306).
Holder, T. (2003). Concentration Training for Closed Skills. Pre-performance routine. In I.
Greenlees, and A. Moran (Eds.), Concentration skills training in sport (pp.67-75).
Leicester: The British Psychological Society.
Hung, T. M. (2002). Brain Activity and Sport Performance: Implications for Sport Psychologists.
Published Abstract in Journal of Sport and Exercise Sciences, S16.
Jackson. R. C. (2001). Consistency of pre-performance routines: Analysis of the 1999 Rugby
World Cup. Journal of Sports Science, 20, 21-22.
Jackson, R. C. (2003). Pre-performance routine consistency: temporal analysis of goal kicking in
the Rugby Union World Cup. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 803-814.
Jackson, R.C., & Baker, J. S. (2001). Routines, Rituals, and Rugby: Case study of a
world class goal kicker. The Sport Psychologist, 15, 48-65.
Janelle, C. M., Singer, R. N., & Williams, A. M. (1999). External distraction and attentional
narrowing: Visual search evidence. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 70-91.
Janelle, C. M., Hillman, C. H., Apparies, R. J., Murray, N. P., Meili, L., Fallon, E. A., &
Hatfield, B. D. (2000). Expertise differences in cortical activation and gaze behavior during
rifle shooting. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22, 167-182.
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing Brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery.
Behav. Brain Science, 17, 187-245.
Page 26 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
27
Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (2001). Pre-performance routine training using holistic process
goals. In P. R. Thomas (Ed.) Optimizing performance in golf (pp.264-278). Brisbane:
Australian Academy Press.
Kirstenbaum, D. S. (1987). Self-regulation of sport performance. Medicine and Science in Sport
and Exercise, 19, S106-51B.
Kirschenbaum, D. S., & Bale, R. M. (1980). Cognitive-behavioral skills in golf: Brain power in
golf. In R. M. Suinn (Ed.) Psychology in Sports: Methods and applications (pp. 275-287),
Minneapolis: Burgess.
Kirschenbaum, D.S., Tomarken, A.J., & Ordman, A.M. (1982). Specificity of planning and
choice applied to adult self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 576-
585.
Lacey, B. C., & Lacey, J. L. (1978). Two-way communication between the heart and the brain,
significance of time within the cardiac cycle. American Psychologist, 33, 99-113.
Lamirand, M., & Rainey, D. (1994). Mental imagery, relaxation, and accuracy of basketball foul
shooting. Perceptual Motor Skills, 78, 1229-1230.
Landers, D. M., Han, M., Salazar, W., Petruzzello, S. J., Kubitz, K. A., & Gannon, T. L. (1994).
Effects of learning on electroencephalographic and electrocardiographic patterns in novice
archers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 25, 313-330.
Landers, D., Petruzzello, S. J., Salazar, W., Crews, D. J., Kubitz, K. A., Gannon, T. L., & Han,
M. (1991). The influence of electrocortical biofeedback on performance in pre-elite
archers. Medicine and Sciences in Sports and Exercise, 23, 123-129.
Page 27 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
28
Lawton, G. W., Hung, T. M., Saarela, P., & Hatfield, B. (1998). Electroencephalography and
mental states associated with elite performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
20, 35-53.
Lidor, R. (2007). Preparatory routines in self-paced events. In G. Tenenbaum and R. C. Eklund
(Eds.) The Handbook of Sport Psychology (3rd edn). (pp.445-465) Hoboken, New Jersey:
Wiley.
Lidor, R., Arnon, M., & Bronstein, A. (1999). The effectiveness of a learning (cognitive) strategy
on free-throw performance in basketball. Applied Research in Coaching & Athletics
Annual, 14, 59-72.
Lidor, R., & Tenenbaum, G. (1993). Applying learning strategy to a basketball shooting skill: A
case study report, Bitnu’a, 108-126.
Lidor, R., & Mayan, Z. (2005). Can beginning Learners Benefit From Preperformance Routines
When Serving in Volleyball? The Sport Psychologist, 19, 343-363.
Lidor, R., & Singer, R. N. (2000). Teaching pre-performance routines to beginners. JOPERD,
71, 34-36.
Liu, J., & Zhang, Y. (2003). The effects of two types of preperforamnce routine on the
acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. RQES, 74, A-33.
Lobmeyer, D. L., & Wasserman, E. A. (1986). Preliminaries to free throw shooting: Superstitious
behavior? Journal of Sports Behavior, 9, 70-78.
Lonsdale, C., & Tam, J. T. M. (2007). On the temporal and behavioral consistency of pre-
performance routines: An intra-individual analysis of elite basketball players’ free throw
shooting accuracy. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 259-266.
Page 28 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
29
Loze, G. M., Collins, D., & Shaw, J. C. (1999). EEG Alpha Rhythm intention and occulomotor
control. International journal of psychophysiology, 33, 163-167.
Loze, G. M., Collins, D. J., & Holmes, P. S . (2001). Pre-shot EEG alpha-power reactivity during
expert air-pistol shooting: A comparison of best and worst shots. Journal of Sport
Sciences, 19, 727-733.
Mack. M. G. (2001). Effects of Time and Movements of the PreShot Routine on Free Throw
Shooting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 567-573.
Mahoney, M. J., & Avener, M. (1977). Psychology of the elite athlete: An exploratory study.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 335-342.
Marlow, C., Bull, S., Heath, B., & Shambrook, C. (1998). The use of a single case design to
investigate the effect of a pre-performance routine on the water polo penalty shot.
Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 1, 143-155.
Maynard, I. W. (1998). Improving concentration. Leeds: National Coaching Foundation.
McCann, P., Lavallee, D., & Lavallee, R. M. (2001). The effect of pre-shot routines on golf
wedge shot performance. European Journal of Sport Science, 1, 231-240.
Moore, W. S. (1986). Covert-overt service routine and play better, Golf Digest, 115-117.
Moore, W. E., & Stevenson, J. R. (1994). Training a pre-shot routine for golf. Applied research
in coaching and athletics annual, 161-167.
Moran, A. P. (1996). The psychology of concentration in sports performers: a cognitive analysis.
Hove: Psychology Press.
Moran, A. (2004). Sport and Exercise Psychology: A Critical Introduction. Hove: Routledge.
Page 29 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
30
Murphy, S. (1994). Mental preparation for golf: achieving optimal performance. In A. J.
Cochrane and M. R. Farrally (Eds.) Science and Golf II: Proceedings of the world
scientific congress of golf. London: E and FN SPON.
Nicholls, A. R. (2007). A longitudinal phenomenological analysis of coping effectiveness among
Scottish international adolescent golfers. European Journal of Sport Science. 7, 169-
178.
Nicholls, A. R., Holt, N. L., & Polman, C. J. (2005). A phenomenological analysis of coping
effectiveness in golf. The Sport Psychologist, 19, 111-130.
O’Connor, K. (1981). The intentional paradigm and cognitive psychology. Psychophysiology,
18, 121-128.
Orlick, T. (1986). Psyching for Sport: Mental training for athletes. Champaign, Ill: Human
Kinetics.
Ost, L. G. (1988). Applied Relaxation: Description of an effective coping technique.
Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 17, 83-96.
Predebon, J., Docker, S. B. (1992). Free-throw shooting performance as a function of pre-shot
routines. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 167-171.
Radlo, S. J., Steinberg, G. M., Singer, R. N., Barba, D. A., & Melnikov, A. ( 2002). The
influence of an attentional focus strategy on alpha brain wave activity, heart rate, and dart
throwing performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 33, 205-217.
Ram, N., & McCullagh, P. (2003). Self-modeling: Influence on psychological responses and
physical performance. The Sport Psychologist, 17, 220-241.
Page 30 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
31
Rotella, R. J., & Bunter, L. K. (1981). Mind mastery for winning golf. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Salazar, W., Landers, D. M., Petruzzello, S. J., Han, M., Crews, D. J., & Kubitz, K. A. (1990).
Hemispheric asymmetry, cardiac response, and performance in elite archers. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 61, 351-359.
Schack, T. (1997). Ängstliche Schler im Sport – interventionsverfahren zur Entwicklung der
Handlungskontrolle. Schorndorf. Germany: Hofmann.
Shaw, D. (2002). Confidence and the pre-shot routine in golf: A case study. In I. Cockerill (Ed.),
Solutions in Sport Psychology (pp.108-119). London: Thomson.
Siedentop, D. (1983). Developing teaching skills in physical education (2nd edn.). California:
Mayfield.
Singer, R. N. (1988). Strategies and meta-strategies in learning and performing self-paced
athletic skills. The Sport Psychologist, 2, 49-68.
Singer, R. N. (2002). Pre-performance state, routines, and automaticity: what does it take to
realize expertise in self-paced events? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24,
359-375.
Southard, D., & Amos, B. (1996). Rhythmicity and preperformance ritual: Stabilizing a flexible
system. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 288-296.
Southard, D. L., & Miracle, A. (1993). Rhythmicity, ritual and motor performance: A study of
free-throw shooting in basketball. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 64, 287-
290.
Thomas, P. R., & Over, R. (1994). Psychological and Psychomotor Skills Associated With
Performance in Golf. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 73-86.
Page 31 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Pre-performance routines in sport
32
Tremayne, P., & Barry, R. J. (2001). Elite pistol shooters: physiological patterning of best vs.
Worst shots. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41, 19-29.
Vealey, R. (1988). Future directions in psychology skills training. Sport Psychology, 7, 318-336.
Vergeer, I., & Hanrahan, C. (1998). What modern dancers do to prepare: content and onjectives
of preperformance routines. AVANTE, 4, 49-71.
Weinberg, R. S. (1988). The Mental Advantage. Champagne, Ill: Leisure Press.
Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. K. (1999). Perception and Action in Sport. London:
E and FN Spon.
Wrisberg, C. A., & Pein, R. C. (1992). The pre-shot interval and free throw shooting accuracy:
An exploratory investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 14-23.
Yancey, R. (1977). Develop a pre-shot routine and play better, Golf Digest, 115-117.
Zinsser, N., Bunker, L., & Williams, J. M. (1998). Cognitive techniques for building confidence
and enhancing performance. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied Sport Psychology:
Personal Growth to peak performance (3rd edn.) (pp.270-295). Mountain View, CA:
Mayfield.
Page 32 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
1
Focusing
Imaging Executing
Readying Evaluating
Step 1
Step 2
Step 3
Step 4
Page 33 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
1
PRACTICE PREPARATION PERFORMANCE ANAYSIS
goal setting imagery attentional control self-talk
relaxation automaticity emotional
control
Page 34 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Table 1. Summary of experimental studies exploring pre-performance routines in sport.
___________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________________
Study Participants Routine Conditions / task Results
Hall & Erffemeyer (1983) 10 female inter Use of visuomotor behaviour rehearsal Use of Visuomotor behaviour rehearsal
collegiate basketball relaxation phase, visualization of a stressful enhanced free-throw performance.
players. event, and performance in a simulated stressful
situation.
Crews & Boutcher (1986) 30 Undergraduates Routines included two practice swings, visualization Trained males had higher post training scores
(17 male, 13 female) Of a line from target to the ball, setting the club next to than the control group, but this was not
in a beginners golf class. the ball, one glance to the target, setting grip, setting feet, replicated for females.
one more glance at the target, then executing the swing.
Lobmeyer & Wasserman University basketball 2 conditions: i) normal responses and routines before each shot Free-throw accuracy was higher for the normal
(1986) players (12 male,15 ii) without preparatory routine. condition compared to the without condition.
female), High School
basketball players
(10 male, 6 female).
Crews & Boutcher (1987) 12 Lady Pro tour 2 routines used: i) Full-swing routine (stand behind the ball, move All golfers very consistent in their behaviours.
Players. beside the ball, setting the club behind the ball and glance at target, The more successful players had longer
Setting stance, three waggles and two glances at the target, execute pre-shot routines.
The swing. ii) putting routine (stand behind ball, move beside ball, two
Practice swings, setting the club behind ball with a glance at the target
Setting stance, two glances at the target, execute the shot.
Boutcher & Crews (1987) 12 collegiate golfers 2 conditions: routine & control. Routines involved focusing on specific Both male and female routine groups
(6 male, 6 female). cues and actions, such as practice swings. significantly increased time but decreased
variability.
Southard, Miracle & 10 University 2 conditions: i) ritual (unlimited time and freedom of movement prior to Results suggested that the duration of
Landwer (1989) basketball players. free-throw attempt. Ii) Non-ritual (shooting without any movements behaviours was most important to free throw
Other than performing the action under a restricted time period. shooting success.
Wrisberg & Anshel (1989) 40 junior athletes Participants used either i) mental imagery, ii) an arousal adjustment Mental imagery combined with arousal
Male (10.2-12.4yrs). strategy, iii) a combined imagery and arousal adjustment strategy. Adjustment enhanced free-throw performance.
Gayton, Cielinski, 25 male high school 2 conditions: i) pre-shot routine (players used their normal routines) A greater number of baskets were scored in the
Francis-Keniston & basketball players. ii) no routine (shot with no preparation). routine condition when compared to the no
Page 35 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
Hearns (1989) routine condition.
Boutcher & Zinsser 30 golfers (15 elite, Total duration of pre-shot behaviours, duration of time club was Elite golfers had longer more complicated
(1990) 15 beginner). stationary behind the ball, number of practice swings and glances pre-shot behaviours, but were more
at the hole were recorded. consistent when compared to the beginners.
Cohn, Rotella & Lloyd 3 elite collegiate golfers. Behavioural routine: alignment with target, posture, consistent ball No immediate improvements were recorded,
(1990) positions. Cognitive routine: decision on, and commitment to club however, participants did feel the intervention
selection, type of shot to be played, position of the target. had a positive effect on performance.
Wrisberg & Penn (1992) 224 intermural and The length of the pre-shot interval scores and free throw percentages Higher percentage shooters maintained a higher
Collegiate basketball were recorded. level of temporal constancy in executing their
players. PPRs than lower percentage shooters.
Predebon & Docker 30 baskeball players 3 conditions: i) No routine (asked just to shoot with no preparation The imagery group performed the best, followed
(1992) minimum 5 years ii) routine (asked to sight the basket, bounce the ball three times by the routine group, and the no-routine group.
experience. and shoot). iii) Imagery / physical condition (asked to imagine the shot
sequence first without the ball, then with the ball.
Southard & Miracle (1993) 8 female University 4 conditions: i) standard free-throw ritual, ii) maintaining the relative Relative timing of behaviours (rhythmicity) is
basketball players. timing of ritual but reducing overall duration by half, iii) maintaining more important to performance than the
timing but doubling overall duration, iv) Same behaviour and overall absolute timing of pre-performance rituals.
duration but relative durations were altered.
Hill & Borden (1995) 31 male league bowlers. Pre-test – post test design 2 conditions, i) control, ii) experimental who Attentional cueing group scored significantly
were exposed to a generic attentional cueing script, which was to be higher than the control group.
used whilst delivering the ball.
Southard & Amos (1996) 7 male varsity sportsmen. All participants participated in golf, basketball, and tennis rituals and Significant correlations were determined to
performances. exist between relative timing and success.
Beauchamp, Halliwell, 65 junior college 3 conditions: i) Cognitive-behavioural group (sport analysis phase, Cognitive-behavioural group demonstrated
Fourier & Koestner students on an intro individual assessment phase, motivation phase, integration of mental improved performance, motivation, and routine
(1996) golf class. skills and evaluation. ii) Physical Skill group (emphasized the physical usage.
skill and mechanics of putting.
Marlow, Bull, Heath & 3 male water polo Each player developed a PPR including a concentration cue, a Results suggested a positive change
Shambrook (1998) players. relaxation cue, imagery cue and cue word. immediately following PPR introduction.
Jackson & Baker (2001) 1 elite rugby kicker. Physical preparation time and concentration time measured. Physical Both concentration time and physical prep time
prep time from when ball left the kickers hand to the end of physical increased with task difficulty. A number of
Page 36 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
prep period, concentration time then the time from the end of physical different psychological skills were also used to
prep time to initiation of the kick. prepare.
Calmels & Fournier 12 elite female 2 conditions: i) execution of gymnastic floor routine, ii) rehearsal of the Mental movement time was longer than physical
(2001) gymnasts. routine mentally only. movement time as task difficulty increased.
Mack (2001) 17 male intercollegiate 4 conditions: i) normal routine and duration, ii) normal routine and Altering the movements had a significant effect
basketball players. altered duration, iii) altered routine and normal duration, on performance, but duration did not.
iv) altered routine and altered time.
McCann, Lavallee, & 28 male golfers & 28 6 experimental groups both golfers and non golfers allocated to: Only significant improvements in performance
Lavallee (2001) male non-golfers. i) no practice, ii) physical practice, iii) physical practice and a for non-golfer group (iii).
Cognitive-behavioural routine.
Radlo, Steinberg, Singer, 20 male novice dart Participants split into 2 groups: i) internal attentional focus (thinking Results indicated that an external attentional
Barba & Melnikov (2002) throwers. about the feel of the dart, drawing the dart back, the bend in the elbow, strategy is associated with more ideal
The feel of the dart leaving the fingers), ii) External attentional focus psychophysiological responses than an
(focus on the dart board, and throw the dart at the focus). internal focus.
Jackson (2003) 20 elite rugby kickers. Physical preparation time and concentration time measured. Physical Players had longer concentration times and
prep time from when ball left the kickers hand to the end of physical shorter physical prep times when scores were
prep period, concentration time then the time from the end of physical close, no difference between best and worse
prep time to initiation of the kick. kickers on routine time, consistency or
rhythmicity.
Lidor & Mayan (2005) 60 novice female Participants taught 2 variations of PPRs when learning to serve in Motor-emphasized learners were more
volleyball players volleyball: i) motor-emphasized, ii) cognitive emphasized. accurate than cognitive-emphasized learners
in retention trials.
Foster, Weigand, & 22 male basketball 2 conditions: i) control (superstitious behaviour), ii) experimental very little difference reported between the
Baines (2008) players. (developed PPR consisting of a concentration cue, a relaxation cue, superstitious behaviour and PPR groups.
imagery, and a cue word.
Lonsdale & Tam (2007) 15 pro basketball Total routine duration was recorded for each free-throw. Recorded No difference based on the duration of the
players. from when the player received the ball from the referee until release. routine, but differences were observed
depending on behavioural consistency.
Haddad & Tremayne 5 talented junior 2 conditions: baseline and experimental. Experimental group Results suggested the use of centering may
(2009) basketball players. received training in centering. be beneficial as part of the PPR.
PPR – pre-performance routine
Page 37 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
For Peer Review Only
1
Table 2. Guidelines for pre-performance phases (Lidor & Mayan, 2005).
Phase 1 Phase 2 Phase 3
Psychological
foundation
Readying (mechanical ,
mental, and emotional)
Focusing attention Evaluating
Emphasis Directing thoughts and
emotions to create an optimal
state for learning and/or
performing
Selecting a relevant external
cue and focusing intently on it
Using self-feedback for
analysis and subsequent
improvement
Guidelines • Establishing a consistent
basic position
• Feeling comfortable
• Generating +ve thoughts and
emotions about performance
expectations
• Developing confidence
Developing awareness
• Visualising best
performance
• Feeling the movements
• Developing awareness of
body parts.
• Focusing attention on one
situational cue or thought
• Maintaining this focus
• Blocking out internal
distractions
• Blocking out external
distractions
• Executing with a quiet mind.
• Judging the outcome of
the performance
• Assessing the processes
and strategies that produced
the movement
• Determining better ways
to perform in the future
Page 38 of 38
URL: http:/mc.manuscriptcentral.com/rirs
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
... More specifically, PR are learned and practiced in order to direct the athlete's attention, help regulate psychological and physiological responses to stress, and allow motor processes to run with minimal conscious interference (Jackson, 2014;Bompa et al., 2019). Research has suggested that PR can focus an athlete's attention, helping them concentrate on the relevant aspects of the task and block out distractions (Cotterill, 2010;Rupprecht et al., 2021). In addition, PR help athletes regulate their emotions, thoughts, and behaviors so that they can minimize distractions and focus on performing motor tasks optimally (Jackson, 2014). ...
... In addition, PR help athletes regulate their emotions, thoughts, and behaviors so that they can minimize distractions and focus on performing motor tasks optimally (Jackson, 2014). Finally, these routines can help trigger automatic performance, which can facilitate a task's accuracy, especially self-paced tasks (Lidor, 2007;Cotterill, 2010). During this period the athlete puts together relevant psychological skills/strategies (i.e., relaxation, imagery, concentration, positive self-talk) with physical activities, which allows the athlete to achieve an optimal level of arousal/activation (i.e., flow), and, finally, performance best. ...
... In general, PP-R is a ritual scenario of what an athlete/team does, thinks, and feels to get into the "zone" prior to the actual performance. Many examples of PP-R in various sports are described in the psychological literature (e.g., Lidor, 2007;Cotterill, 2010;Gropel and Beckmann, 2017). For example, PP-R for a free throw in basketball may include holding the ball, self-talking, dribbling, or focusing attention on the basket before the actual throw. ...
Article
Full-text available
Preparatory routines (PR) are a necessary tool for achieving optimal emotional states and effective performance, especially in self-paced motor skills. Numerous studies in the area of applied sport psychology have found a positive effect of PR on blocking out distractions and regulating performance thoughts, actions, and emotions. PR contain behavioral and mental components that can be applied in different time periods before and after the event itself: pre-competitive activity routines (PCA-R), pre-performance routines (PP-R), and post-performance activity routines (PPA-R). The aim of this manuscript is to present an effective way to practice psychological techniques and their combinations as part of the PR. The periodization principle and the Learning-Modification-Application (LMA) model with biofeedback training can provide a conceptual framework and means of application for performance enhancement. It will allow the transfer of psychological skills from lab to field and the integration of PR into the athlete’s preparations for peak performance.
... Ensuite, malgré qu'elle ne soit mentionnée que par un entraîneur, l'éducation de la psychologie du sport devrait effectivement être mise davantage de l'avant, notamment en raison du manque de connaissances des jeunes athlètes sur le sujet soulevé par Bell et ses collaboratrices (2020). L'utilisation de routines ou de resets peut en effet être bénéfique, en servant d'échauffement, en aidant l'athlète à garder une bonne concentration, en ne s'attardant pas sur le négatif, tout en facilitant la sélection de schémas moteurs appropriés lorsque nécessaire (Cotterill, 2010). Cotterill (2010) soulève également que les techniques de relaxation et d'imagerie mentale vont souvent être utilisées lors de routines ou de resets. ...
... L'utilisation de routines ou de resets peut en effet être bénéfique, en servant d'échauffement, en aidant l'athlète à garder une bonne concentration, en ne s'attardant pas sur le négatif, tout en facilitant la sélection de schémas moteurs appropriés lorsque nécessaire (Cotterill, 2010). Cotterill (2010) soulève également que les techniques de relaxation et d'imagerie mentale vont souvent être utilisées lors de routines ou de resets. ...
... The main function of a PPR is to enhance concentration by directing attention to task-specific cues and minimizing internal or external distractions (Cotterill, 2010). It typically comprises task-specific motor actions, such as when a dancer marks parts of the choreography with their hands. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Not being able to manage performance anxiety and subsequently experiencing a decline in performance have been called “choking under pressure”. High trait anxiety and fear of negative evaluation, as well as low self-efficacy or self-confidence, can put performers especially at risk of experiencing choking. This study, therefore, examined the effects of psychological choking interventions tailored to “choking-susceptible” performing artists individually in a coaching setting. Methods We conducted a mixed-methods (QUANT + QUAL) collective case study with nine performing artists, who each received five individual coaching sessions. The tailored choking interventions comprised acclimatization training, goal setting, and pre-performance routines, including elements such as imagery, self-talk, and relaxation techniques. Before and after the 10-week intervention phase, they filled in questionnaires on trait performance anxiety, fear of negative evaluation, and self-efficacy, performed in front of a jury, and were interviewed about their experiences. Transcripts of interviews and coaching sessions were analyzed using thematic analysis. Heart rate measurements, weekly performance videos, and expert evaluations were also part of our comprehensive data. Results Quantitative data showed reductions in performance anxiety and fear of negative evaluation, and increases in self-efficacy and performance quality, from before to after the intervention phase. Most participants also had a lower heart rate when performing for the jury. Themes from qualitative analysis comprised managing nervousness and feeling more relaxed, becoming more self-confident, satisfaction with artistic and mental performance, feeling good and enjoying performing, and general positive effects. Conclusion Tailoring psychological interventions may provide several benefits for choking-susceptible performing artists.
... En relación con la estructura de las rutinas utilizadas en intervenciones de este tipo, Cotterill et al. (2010) indican que el condicionamiento para seguir un conjunto prescrito de pasos conductuales y psicológicos administrados artificialmente podía tener un efecto positivo en un entorno experimental, pero podría no ser efectivo en el entorno competitivo real, por lo que indican que las rutinas deberían desarrollarse en función de la personalidad, los recursos de afrontamiento y las evaluaciones situacionales de cada deportista individualmente. De todas formas, esto puede ser compatible con el uso de una plantilla genérica para la rutina, basada en las demandas psicológicas (Cotterill, 2010). Liu y Zhang (2003) sugieren que las rutinas específicas (genéricas) previas a la actuación mejoran la adquisición de habilidades, mientras que las rutinas auto-desarrolladas promueven la transferencia del aprendizaje de habilidades. ...
Article
Full-text available
Los efectos de las rutinas competitivas sobre el rendimiento han sido ampliamente estudiados. En deportes con pausas tras cada punto jugado, la gestión óptima de las rutinas entre puntos ayuda a los deportistas a mantenerse emocionalmente estables durante todo el partido. Con el objetivo de conseguir una aplicación consistente de las rutinas entre punto, se diseñó una intervención para integrarlas como hábito de entrenamiento. Se trabajó con el grupo de jugadores de tenis de mesa del CAR Sant Cugat Vallès, Barcelona (7 chicas, 4 chicos; M = 15.45 años). Se realizó un taller previo sobre rutinas entre punto y se fijaron éstas de forma individual; durante 12 semanas, los jugadores practicaron y registraron la aplicación de las rutinas; los psicólogos hicieron seguimiento y ofrecieron feedback inmediato a la acción en un entrenamiento por semana; finalmente se comparó la información recogida pre-pos intervención (cuestionario IPED y entrevista semiestructurada sobre uso y efecto de las rutinas). Los resultados a nivel de grupo mostraron una evolución positiva en la aplicación de las rutinas a lo largo de las 12 semanas. Hubo más conciencia en el uso de rutinas y más efectos positivos derivados de ellas en competición que en entrenamiento. El control del lenguaje corporal fue el aspecto incluido en las rutinas que más inconsistencia mostró. En cuanto al análisis pre-pos, se observó una valoración positiva del grupo en relación con el uso y efectividad de las rutinas, especialmente en competición, pero sin diferencias en las subescalas IPED. La utilidad percibida de las rutinas es consistente con hallazgos previos en cuanto a los efectos sobre el rendimiento, y la intervención puede haber contribuido a generar el hábito de usar las rutinas en el entrenamiento, aunque sería necesario seguir explorando formas más objetivas de evaluar el proceso de configuración y el efecto de las rutinas.
... PPRs are believed to improve on-task focus (Gould & Udry, 1994) and may protect against choking (Mesagno & Mullane-Grant, 2010). Most research into the makeup of PPRs (in golf and other self-paced sports) has concentrated on behavioral elements (Cotterill, 2010;Davies et al., 2014), hence few studies have documented the subtle details of athletes' psychological preparations. ...
Article
There are numerous studies of expert golfers’ thought processes, but few have examined thinking during both shot preparation and execution. This study had skilled golfers ( N = 95, mean handicap = 1.5) complete a mixed-methods survey about their preparation/execution thoughts (a) in usual competitive circumstances and (b) during past experiences of choking. The results provided rare documentation of the ways that highly skilled golfers occupy their minds throughout the whole shot-making process. Moreover, the data allowed comparison of what golfers prefer to focus on and what the sport psychology literature recommends as optimal. The clearest gap that emerged was widespread use of deliberate or multifaceted thought during execution, against classical recommendations to swing with a quiet mind. The examination of choking implied that conscious interference was a more common rationalization for choking than previously reported. Implications for practice are discussed.
Article
With emerging evidence suggesting that professional esports players experience a range of psychological demands relating to their performance (e.g., Leis et al., 2022; Poulus et al., 2020, 2022b; Smith et al., 2019), there is a need for evidence-based stress management strategies in esports (e.g., Cottrell et al., 2019; Leis & Lautenbach, 2020). Esports can be defined as an individual or team-based gaming activity that includes competition and ranking systems across amateur and professional levels (Pedraza-Ramirez et al., 2020). Given the competitive demands of esports (e.g., maintaining focus and adaptive coping; Himmelstein et al., 2017; Nagorsky & Wiemeyer, 2020), and the increasing interest from practitioners working on these issues (e.g., Watson et al., 2021), empirical evidence on applied practice in esports is needed (e.g., Leis et al., 2021). This knowledge could help to inform stress management strategies that are oriented on the specific demands of esports (e.g., Cottrell et al., 2019; Leis et al., 2021; Leis & Lautenbach, 2020) and ensure that ethical and professional standards, including competent and conscientious behavior, are being met (e.g., Association for Applied Sport Psychology, 2011; German Society for Sport Psychology, 2003). A pertinent first step to obtain this knowledge is (a) to explore the factors perceived by practitioners to negatively impact the performance of esports players and (b) the stress management strategies these practitioners use to support esports players.
Chapter
Although many different emotions colour our experience of sport, on and off the pitch, anxiety sticks out because of its ubiquitous presence. Focusing on anxiety as a “state”, i.e. a momentary feeling, this chapter uses a cognitive model of emotion to develop an understanding of anxiety in sport. Triggers of anxiety in sport are identified, and the cognitive processes leading to the emotional experience, including body, mind and behaviour, are described. This chapter further describes a core topic of sport psychology, the examination of the relationship between anxiety and performance and the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Understanding of the processes involved in the anxiety experience allows the development and application of strategies to regulate and manage anxiety. This chapter concludes with showing how sport and physical activity may help persons to better deal with anxiety, anxiety disorders and stress.
Article
Over the past 10 years there has been a significant increase in the number of sports adopting a more formalised, and increasingly well-funded, approach to talent development through professional sports clubs. As a result there are increasing opportunities for sport psychologists to work at this level. The purpose of this article is for the author to both share and reflect on experiences of providing sport psychology support in a professional cricket academy over a four-year period. In particular, the approaches adopted, and the effectiveness of the profiling techniques and tools used are explored. This article also seeks to explore the challenges that face a practitioner working in this developmental environment and the importance of developing good working relationships with other members of the support staff and coaching teams.
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary coaching practices such as the Constraint-Led Approach (CLA) have gained traction in recent years within academic literature and applied practice. However, despite the growing popularity of the CLA there remains challenges with the successful implementation across applied settings. Formal education that adopts top-down hierarchical approaches and is decontextualised from coaches' sociocultural contexts has been attributed to this challenge. To investigate this problem further, the purpose of this study was to explore the feasibility of implementing a CLA curriculum within British Diving's Learn to Dive programme. Twenty-one British Diving coaches (10 male and 11 female) were interviewed for this study. The coaches' insights indicated that a new model of coaching was needed to improve the learning and development experiences of the Learn to Dive participants, however, factors such as opposing philosophies about learners and the learning process, path dependency, and operational priorities were identified as potential barriers to implementation. These findings suggest that the implementation of alternative coaching methods should be considered in the context of the sport and the coaches' needs, experience, and socio-cultural-historical biases. Documenting context specific learning experiences through qualitative research designs can start to address the challenges and provide potential solutions to successfully implementing contemporary learning designs.
Article
Full-text available
The results of a qualitative study examining the preperformance routines of professional modern dancers are presented. Using an open-ended format, eleven dancers were interviewed regarding the methods they used to prepare themselves for performance. Qualitative analyses revealed that the dancers employed a variety of techniques and strategies to address four different components of preperformance preparation: creating necessary conditions (time and energy management), preparing to dance (establishing a mind-body connection), preparing to perform (disconnecting from everyday life and regulating nervousness) and preparing to communicate (connecting with the content and intent of the choreography). Results are discussed in relation to precompetitive routines in sport and to the potential usefulness of the dancers’ preparatory techniques in the athletic context.
Article
Full-text available
Explored the effects of a 14-wk cognitive-behavioral teaching program (CBP) vs a physical skills intervention vs no training on the motivation, preparation, and putting performance of 65 adult novice golfers. The Sport Motivation Scale was used to measure intrinsic vs introjected forms of self-regulation. Pre-putt routines and actual putting performance were measured by observer ratings. Ss completed all dependent measures prior to training and at 3 additional times over 4-wk intervals. Ss in the CBP displayed enhanced intrinsic motivation, more consistent use of pre-putt routines, and improved putting performance relative to Ss in the other 2 groups. CBP Ss also showed a significantly reduced use of introjection, which reflects a harsh, self-evaluative form of self-regulation similar to ego involvement. Cognitive-behavioral approaches seem to be effective for performance enhancement and can produce positive motivational effects. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
To execute flawlessly and automatically in sports competition is the goal of any serious athlete. Automaticity suggests nonconscious attention to the act itself while executing, and not being aware of and therefore vulnerable to external and internal distractors. Self-paced sports and events in sports allow time for preparing to perform in a stable and predictable situation. More recently, cognitive, behavioral, and psychophysiological measures associated with developing and realizing proficiency in such acts have become increasingly identified. Many themes associated with these topics appear in the scholarly literature: conscious vs. nonconscious, controlled vs. automatic, voluntary vs. involuntary, explicit vs. implicit, systematic vs. heuristic, willed vs. nonwilled, aware vs. unaware, internal vs. externally oriented, and intentional vs. unintentional behaviors. Implications are being made about ways to influence the learning process by modeling expertise behaviors, as well as enhancing the performanc...
Article
Although self-modeling has been effective in modifying behaviors in a variety of settings, little research has been completed in the physical domain. The purpose of this study was to test the effectiveness of self-modeling on performance and self-efficacy using a sport skill and to explore the cognitive processes underlying self-modeling. A multiple baseline single-subject design was used wherein five intermediate level Volleyball players were given a self-modeling intervention. Performance outcome results indicated that self-modeling may contribute to increases in serve accuracy. Performance form and self-efficacy results were inconclusive. Using a think-aloud protocol, it was noted that although the participants found the images of themselves "shocking," the images command cognitive resources. Postintervention interviews revealed that participants found the self-modeling intervention useful and that it led to changes in behavior and motivation.
Article
This book presents first-time research findings and new empirically based perspectives and applications in sport psychology.Highly provocative data derived from the largest single study ever on athletes is used to advance an original model of peak performance centering on a clearly emerging athlete's profile. Central to the book is the Theory of Critical Moments, which proposes that mental factors are most crucial during specifically delineated psychologically significant periods of competition and helps explain when and why intrusive cognitions influence performance.