Content uploaded by Françoys Gagné
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Françoys Gagné on Jun 25, 2015
Content may be subject to copyright.
Author Query Sheet
Manuscript Information
Journal
Acronym CHAS
Volume and
issue 21(2)
Author name Gagne
Manuscript
No. (if
applicable)
525341
AUTHOR: The following queries have arisen during the editing of your
manuscript. Please answer the queries by making the necessary corrections
on the CATS online corrections form. Once you have added all your
corrections, please press the SUBMIT button.
QUERY NO. QUERY DETAILS
AQ1.
AQ2.
AQ3.
AQ4.
AQ5.
Please supply full affiliation details (University, City/Town, Country).
Galton does not currently appear in the reference section – please supply details.
Query has been removed as it was not applicable.
This is spelt ‘McCrae’ in the reference section – which is correct?
URL references should include the page/article title and an access date.
High Ability Studies
Vol. 21, No. 2, December 2010, 81–99
ISSN 1359-8139 print/ISSN 1469-834X online
© 2010 European Council for High Ability
DOI: 10.1080/13598139.2010.525341
http://www.informaworld.com
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
Motivation within the DMGT 2.0 framework
Françoys Gagné*
Université du Québec à Montréal, Canada
Taylor and FrancisCHAS_A_525341.sgm10.1080/13598139.2010.525341High Ability Studies1359-8139 (print)/1469-834X (online)Original Article2010Taylor & Francis2120000002010Professor FrancoysGagnefygagne@gmail.com
This article begins with a brief survey of the recent update of the Differentiated
Model of Giftedness and Talent (DMGT). The DMGT defines talent development
as the transformation of outstanding natural abilities (called gifts) into outstanding
knowledge and skills (called talents). Two types of catalysts, intrapersonal and
environmental, actively moderate the talent development process. The concept of
motivation is situated at the core of the intrapersonal catalysts. Borrowing from
action control theory, two goal management sets of processes are differentiated.
The first set targets goal-identification (pre-decisional) activities, whereas the
second set deals with goal-attainment (post-decisional) activities. They are labeled
‘motivation’ and ‘volition’ respectively. The article describes these two sets of
processes, especially the distinction between goals and motives, as well as some
practical impacts of individual differences in motivation and volition.
Keywords: DMGT; goal management; motivation; volition; passion; intrinsic/
extrinsic motives
The present issue of HAS focuses on the phenomenon of motivation as it relates to the
process of talent development. My own objective within this article will be to circum-
scribe the role(s) played by motivation within my theory of talent development, called
the Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent, version 2.0 (DMGT 2.0).
There is no doubt among researchers and professionals interested by the phenom-
enon of talent development that motivation, however it is specifically defined, plays a
crucial role in the long process through which youth and adults attempt to reach excel-
lence in any field of human activity: excellence in academic pursuits, occupational
performance, artistic realizations, or athletic prowess. When Hemery (1986, p. 204)
concluded his book of interviews with famous athletes by saying ‘they tell us all some-
thing about ourselves and what we may be capable of achieving, if we dream and make
the commitment to work hard towards that vision’, he was directly referring to moti-
vation. When Albert Einstein summarized the source of his achievements by saying
‘It was curiosity, obsession, and sheer perseverance that brought me to my ideas’
(Folsing, 1998, p. 7), he was also focusing on motivational concepts. Francis Galton
was also highlighting these factors when he explained the source of eminence by saying
‘I do not mean capacity without zeal, nor zeal without an adequate power of doing a
great deal of very laborious work’ (1892, p. 77). I could add dozens of similar quotes
going all the way back to the musings on human nature by early philosophers.
The main difficulty for any scholar interested by this domain of inquiry resides in
its complexity: dozens of definitions and theories have been proposed since the early
*Email: fygagne@gmail.com.
AQ1
AQ2
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 81 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
82 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
days of scientific psychology. Dozens of terms – if not hundreds – have been more or
less closely associated with this phenomenon: interests, passion, needs, values, will
power, determination, perseverance, intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and countless
others. And this short list doesn’t even begin to survey the vast inventory of human
motives. I do not consider myself an expert in this particular domain. But, as I tried to
further analyze the components of the DMGT, I could not avoid deepening my knowl-
edge of that complex domain. That review of the empirical literature brought me to
assemble a series of ideas on the role of motivation in talent development, which I will
try to summarize in the next pages. Since many readers might not have discovered yet
the DMGT, especially its recent 2.0 version, I judged important to offer a basic over-
view of the theory; it will help them better understand where the concept of motivation
fits within the model. More detailed descriptions of the theory appear elsewhere (e.g.,
Gagné, 2009).1
The Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent (DMGT)
Among current conceptions of giftedness (see Sternberg & Davidson, 2005), the
DMGT stands alone in its clear, distinct, and well-operationalized definitions of two
key concepts in the field of gifted education: giftedness and talent. Most scholars in
the field commonly use these two terms as synonyms, just like in the common expres-
sion: ‘the gifted and talented are…’ Occasional distinctions between the two terms
will take many forms, and give rise to a diversity of views and theories (Gagné, 2004).
But, an in-depth look at these divergent views brings forth one underlying consensus,
namely a distinction between early emerging forms of ‘giftedness/talent’ with strong
biological roots on the one hand, as opposed to fully developed adult forms of ‘gift-
edness/talent’. Scholars will express that distinction through pairs of terms like poten-
tial/realization, aptitude/achievement, and promise/fulfillment. It manifests itself in
statements like these: ‘Talent development is important to achieving one’s full poten-
tial’ (Brody & Stanley, 2005, p. 28); ‘Being gifted means moving beyond potential to
actual performance’ (Cross & Coleman, 2005, p. 53); ‘To be born with high talent
potential and later possibly be talented in some career-oriented field’ (Feldhusen,
2005, p 74).
The DMGT was created to take advantage of that distinction. As shown in
Figure 1, it brings together five components: gifts (G), talents (T), a talent develop-
ment process (D), intrapersonal catalysts (I) and environmental catalysts (E).
Figure 1. Gagné’s Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent (DMGT 2.0; 2008 update).
The basic talent development trio
The first trio includes the three components whose interaction summarizes the essence
of the DMGT’s conception of talent development: the progressive transformation (D)
of gifts (G) into talents (T). Here are formal definitions for the two target concepts.
Giftedness designates the possession and use of untrained and spontaneously
expressed outstanding natural abilities or aptitudes (called gifts), in at least one abil-
ity domain, to a degree that places an individual at least among the top 10% of age
peers.
Talent designates the outstanding mastery of systematically developed competen-
cies (knowledge and skills) in at least one field of human activity to a degree that
places an individual at least among the top 10% of ‘learning peers’ (those who have
accumulated a similar amount of learning time from either current or past training).
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 82 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 83
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Gifts (G)
The DMGT distinguishes six natural ability domains (see Figure 1), four of them
belonging to the mental realm (intellectual-GI, creative-GC, social-GS, perceptual-
GP), and the other two to the physical realm (muscular-GM, motor control-GR). Each
domain constitutes a sub-component with multiple facets. Natural abilities are not
innate; they do develop, especially during childhood, through maturational processes
and informal exercise. Yet, that development and level of expression are partially
controlled by the individual’s genetic endowment. Gifts can be observed more easily
and directly in young children because environmental influences and systematic learn-
ing have exerted their moderating influence in a limited way. However, they still show
themselves in older children, even in adults, through the facility and speed with which
individuals acquire new competencies (knowledge and skills) in any given field of
human activity. Said differently, ease and speed in learning is the trademark of any
type of giftedness.
Talents (T)
As defined in the DMGT, talents progressively emerge from the transformation of
these outstanding natural abilities or gifts into the well-trained and systematically
developed competencies characteristic of a particular field of human activity. On the
potential–performance continuum, talents represent the performance pole, thus the
outcome of the talent development process. Talent fields can be extremely diverse.
Figure 1 shows nine talent sub-components. Six of them have their origin in John
Holland’s work-related classification of personality types: realistic, investigative,
Figure 1. Gagné’s Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent (DMGT 2.0; 2008 update).
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 83 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
84 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
artistic, social, enterprising, and conventional (RIASEC) (see Anastasi & Urbina,
1997, chapter 14). The three others ensure an almost complete coverage of existing
human occupations, as exemplified in the International Standard Classification of
Occupations (ISCO) (see International Labour Organization, 2008).
Developmental process (D)
In this theory, natural abilities or aptitudes act as the ‘raw materials’ or constitu-
ent elements of talents; they act through the talent development process. The
concept of talent development is formally defined as the systematic pursuit by
talentees, over a significant and continuous period of time, of a structured program
of activities leading to a specific excellence goal. The neologism talentee describes
anyone participating in a systematic talent development program, whatever the
field.
The D component has three sub-components: activities (DA), investment (DI), and
progress (DP), each of them with multiple facets. Talent development begins when a
child or adult gains access (DAA), through identification or selection, to a systematic
program of activities. These activities include a specific content (DAC), the curricu-
lum, offered within a specific learning environment or format (DAF). The investment
(DI) sub-component quantifies the intensity of the talent development process in terms
of time (DIT), money (DIM), or psychological energy (DIE). Finally, the progress
(DP) of talentees from initial access to peak performance can be broken down into a
series of stages (DPS; e.g., novice, advanced, proficient, expert). Its main quantitative
representation is pace (DPP), or how fast – compared to their initially planned
progress rate (ipsative view) or to learning peers (normative view) – talentees are
progressing toward their predefined excellence goal. The long-term developmental
course of a talentee will be marked by a series of more or less crucial turning points
(DPT) (e.g., being spotted by a teacher or coach, receiving an important scholarship,
accidents, death of a close one).
The prevalence question
Any definition of normative concepts must specify how subjects differ from the norm
and what it means in terms of the prevalence of the population subsumed by the label.
In the DMGT, the threshold for both giftedness and talent is placed at the 90th percen-
tile (Gagné, 1998). In other words, those who belong to the top 10% of the relevant
reference group in terms of natural ability (for giftedness) or achievement (for talent)
may receive the relevant label. This generous choice of threshold is counterbalanced
by the use of five successive levels of giftedness or talent based on the metric system.
Thus, within the top 10% of ‘mildly’ gifted or talented persons, the DMGT identifies
the following four progressively more selective subgroups, respectively labeled
‘moderately’ (top 1%), ‘highly’ (top 1:1,000), ‘exceptionally’ (top 1:10,000), and
‘extremely’ or ‘profoundly’ (top 1:100,000).
The ‘supporting cast’
The talent development process is facilitated (or hindered) by the action of two types
of catalysts; intrapersonal and environmental (see Figure 1).
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 84 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 85
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Intrapersonal catalysts (I)
The I component has five sub-components grouped into two main dimensions,
namely stable traits (physical-IF, or mental-IP), and goal management processes
(self-awareness-IW, motivation-IM, and volition-IV). Within the mental or personal-
ity (IP) category, we find an almost infinite list of descriptive qualities. The concept
of temperament refers to behavioral predispositions with a strong genetic component,
whereas the term personality encompasses a large diversity of positive or negative
acquired styles of behavior (McCrea et al., 2000). The dimension called goal-
management was inspired by a theory of self-management proposed by De Waele,
Morval, and Sheitoyan (1993). Two of its subdivisions, IM and IV, are the focus of
this article; consequently, the three sub-components of that whole dimension will
receive detailed attention in the next part of this article.
Environmental catalysts (E)
The E component used to be placed below a central arrow representing the develop-
mental process. In the 2.0 update, the E catalysts have been moved up and behind the
I component. This partial overlap signals the crucial filtering role played by the I
component with regard to environmental influences; the bulk of environmental stimuli
have to pass through the ‘sieve’ of an individual’s needs, interests, or personality
traits. The E component comprises three distinct sub-components: Milieu-EM, Indi-
viduals-EI, and Provisions-EP. The first one (EM) includes a diversity of environmen-
tal influences, from physical or geographical ones (e.g., climate, rural vs. urban living)
to social or cultural ones. The second sub-component (EI) focuses on the psychologi-
cal influence of significant persons in the talentees’ immediate environment. The third
sub-component (EP) covers all forms of talent development services and programs.
The two traditional facets of enrichment and administrative provisions directly paral-
lel the ‘content’ and ‘format’ sub-categories of the DA sub-component earlier
described. Here we adopt a broader outlook rather than examine provisions from the
strict perspective of a given talentee’s talent development course. Administrative
provisions are traditionally subdivided into two main practices: (a) part-time or full-
time ability grouping, and (b) accelerative enrichment (e.g., early entrance to school,
grade skipping).
The Chance (C) factor
Chance used to be introduced as a fifth causal factor associated with the environment,
for instance the chance of being born in a particular family, or the chance of the school
in which the child is enrolled developing (or not) a program for talented students. But,
strictly speaking, chance is not a causal factor. Just like the type of influence (positive
vs. negative), chance qualifies the various causal influences (G, I, D, E) in terms of the
degree of control a person possesses with regard to any one of these causal influences.
Chance’s crucial involvement is well summarized in a statement made by the late John
W. Atkinson, a pioneer researcher in the field of achievement motivation. He believed
that all human accomplishments could be ascribed to ‘two crucial rolls of the dice over
which no individual exerts any personal control. These are the accidents of birth and
background. One roll of the dice determines an individual’s heredity; the other, his
formative environment’ (1978, p. 221). These two impacts alone give a powerful role
AQ4
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 85 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
86 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
to chance in sowing the bases of a person’s talent development possibilities. Because
of its redefined role, the ‘chance’ factor should no longer appear in a visual represen-
tation of the DMGT. Yet, its popularity among DMGT ‘fans’ – as well as my personal
attachment to it – brought me to create some room for it in the background of the
components it influences.
Developmental dynamics
Any significant coverage of the dynamics of talent development within the DMGT
framework well exceeds the scope of this brief overview. Here are a few highlights.
In the DMGT, outstanding natural abilities are treated as the raw materials or the
constituent elements of talents. Because of this basic relationship, the presence of
talent necessarily implies the possession of well above average natural abilities; one
cannot become talented without first being gifted, or almost so. The reverse is not true
however. It is possible for outstanding natural abilities to remain potentialities, as
witnessed by the well-known phenomenon of academic underachievement. The
arrows in Figure 1 indicate that intrapersonal and environmental catalysts typically act
through the talent development process. That moderator role of the learning/practice
process is quite normal: it confirms that talent does not manifest itself overnight. The
skills have to be built over time even when, thanks to very high natural abilities, the
first achievements appear almost immediately and without apparent effort.
Interactions can be very complex. Indeed, it is my conviction that empirical
evidence exists to support causal interactions between any pairing of the five compo-
nents, and in both directions in each case. For instance, the impact – positive or nega-
tive – on one’s self-concept of being labeled ‘gifted’ or ‘talented’ is a typical case of
G → I interaction. Conversely, I factors may exert an impact on the development of
natural abilities. Moreover, interactions can manifest themselves within a given
component. The relationships between parents and teachers regarding a child’s special
educational needs, whether they are collaborative or tainted with conflict, represent
one of the most common instances of direct interactions within the E component.
Similarly, within the I component, personality disorders may affect the goal setting
process. Finally, talented outcomes may themselves enter into a feedback loop and
influence the talentees and-/or influential persons in their environment. No doubt that
the early successes of young students, young artists, or young athletes serve to
heighten their motivation to pursue their training and even increase its intensity.
In summary, no causal component stands alone. They all interact with one another
and with the developmental processes in very complex ways; and these interaction
patterns will differ significantly from one person to the next, as well as at different
stages of that process.
Focusing on the motivation–volition sub-components
The concept of motivation has been a crucial element of the DMGT since its first
English publication (Gagné, 1985). At that time, my implicit definition of that concept
espoused the common view of motivation found in most psychology handbooks,
namely that complex mixture of desires, needs, goals, dedication, effort, will power,
and dozens of related concepts. Its placement within the DMGT, namely as one of the
major intrapersonal catalysts, has remained unchanged over the intervening years; it
simply has been better circumscribed and operationalized. As mentioned above, the
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 86 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 87
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
concept of motivation (IM) belongs to a more general dimension called goal manage-
ment, which also includes self-awareness (IW), and volition (IV).
Goal management first appeared in the DMGT under the label ‘self-management’
(Gagné, 2003). The need to insert some forms of personal management activities orig-
inates in an interview study with a group of multitalented young adults and their
parents (Gagné, 1999). One of the unexpected results from that study was a quasi-
unanimous mention by the parents that their child had shown from a very early age a
high level of autonomy and independence in the management of his/her very heavy
personal schedule of activities. According to these parents, their children never had to
be reminded of the numerous daily tasks involved in the pursuit of their multiple
talents (e.g., study or homework, sport or music practice, preparation for competitions
– even household chores!). That dimension owes its original structure to a theory of
self-management proposed by De Waele, Morval, and Sheitoyan (1993); but it did not
include the IM–IV differentiation. Self-management was soon relabeled ‘goal-
management’ to respect the specific nature of the DMGT as a talent development
theory, and not as a broader personal development theory (see for instance Moon,
2003, for such a broader outlook). In other words, the DMGT’s goal management
dimension focuses on how talentees define their excellence goals, and how they work
at reaching them, whatever the field.
The general idea of personal management is certainly not new in psychology or
education; it has been discussed under a variety of labels, like self-actualization
(Maslow, 1954), self-determination (Deci & Ryan, 1985), self-regulation (Zimmer-
man, 1998), and a few other synonyms. There are of course significant differences
between these various theories, but these subtleties are not relevant here. Goal
management has become the umbrella concept for a group of processes related to the
management of all talent-related conative – from the Latin ‘conatum’, meaning effort-
ful behavior – activities. The term conation brings back a very old, but still useful, tril-
ogy: cognition, affection, and conation. They refer respectively to thoughts, emotions,
and actions, considered in early scientific psychology as the three major forms of
human behaviors. The first of the three sub-components of goal management, namely
self-awareness (IW), covers a series of ‘social’ abilities associated with perceptive-
ness, but focused on oneself as the target. Knowing one’s abilities (GI, GC, etc) and
personal traits (IF and IP) is crucial for an appropriate selection of one’s talent goal(s),
as well as the precise assessment of one’s strengths to pursue any goals, especially
challenging ones, to their successful completion.
The two other sub-components, motivation (IM) and volition (IV), have their source
in action control theory (ACT), a motivational theory developed by two German schol-
ars, Heinz Heckhausen and Julius Kuhl (see Kuhl & Beckman, 1985), in which they
propose a clear differentiation between (a) activities related to the identification of
appropriate – macro or micro – goals, and (b) ulterior activities devoted to reaching
the chosen goal(s). They use the terms ‘motivation’ and ‘volition’ respectively as labels
for these two sets of behaviors, also referred to as ‘pre-decisional’ and ‘post-decisional’.
Lynn Corno (see Corno, 1993; Corno & Kanfer, 1993) adapted the theory to the class-
room context. I was immediately attracted by that theory, and from 1997 onwards it
became a central part of the I component. Corno’s presentation leaves aside the moti-
vation member of that pair to focus on the volition construct. She considers the ACT
to have given renewed – and deserved – importance to a construct that was for too long
subsumed under the encompassing umbrella of the motivation concept. She points out
that ‘prominent psychologists of the 1920s and 1930s effectively removed volition from
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 87 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
88 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
the investigative scene by arguing that it could be accounted for by motivation’ (Corno,
1993, p. 14). In her view, the ACT gives volition a distinct and significant role in every
goal-related endeavor. Let’s examine more closely these two sub-components of the
DMGT.
Motivation (IM)
The motivation sub-component can be subdivided into two distinct elements: goals
(IMG) and motives (IMM). These two facets represent respectively the ‘what’ and the
‘why’ of any motivational process.
Goals (IMG)
When individuals identify a particular goal they want to reach they are pinpointing
WHAT they want to achieve, either at a microscopic level (e.g., I want to complete
this school assignment) or at a macroscopic level (e.g., I want to become a firefighter).
Within the context of the DMGT’s talent development theory, goals exclusively repre-
sent expected or desired outstanding performance outcomes, in other words achieve-
ment goals expressed as talented performances. Of course, these performance goals
are field-related: academic performances, athletic performances, artistic perfor-
mances, and so forth. All other macroscopic or microscopic goals (e.g., wanting to get
married and have children, wanting to take a vacation in a foreign country, wanting to
build a close friendship with another person) have no relevance to the DMGT. Excel-
lence goals vary as much as the breadth of available talent fields. They may appear
when children or adults discover a personal interest in a particular field. Accustomed
as we are of associating talent development with academic excellence, we tend to
forget that most excellence goals have nothing to do with that limited field. We will
see below that goals and motives are closely related through the concept of intrinsic
motivation.
The content of a goal represents its qualitative dimension, whereas its distance
from a baseline situation, usually expressed as a time frame (e.g., I want to complete
the second year of the music course within the next six months), represents its quan-
titative dimension. For instance, if two equally talented young swimmers look at their
next semester’s improvement, with Swimmer A planning to deduct 0,5 second from
her present time and Swimmer B expecting a 0,7 second reduction, then Swimmer B’s
goal will be quantitatively higher than Swimmer A’s goal. In fact, goals can be
expressed through a second quantitative aspect, namely their perceived level of impor-
tance in the eyes of talentees. How much does that talentee want to reach that partic-
ular goal? It can certainly be assessed at least on some ordinal scale (not very
important… extremely important). In theory, the answer to that question should corre-
late strongly with the volitional energy expended in reaching it. In practice, no
certainty exists that we will observe a high correlation; as Corno points out, some
highly desired but low level goals might not require high volitional outputs. For
instance, a majority of high school students with moderate (top 1%) academic talent
may judge it very important – for whatever ‘motive’ – to maintain their top level
achievement; yet, they will have little difficulty reaching that goal, because, sadly, the
regular high school curriculum offers very few occasions for these talented students
to explore the limits of their volitional strengths. In other situations, an originally
highly desired goal might progressively lose its attraction because it requires a much
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 88 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 89
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
higher level of effort than originally anticipated. This is what happened to this author’s
dream of becoming a competent – not even talented – clarinetist, after investing two
years in lessons!
Motives (IMM)
As important as goals are, most of the research in the field of motivation has been
devoted to motives, that is the reasons, conscious or not, which serve as the rationale
for both the qualitative and quantitative dimensions of goal choices. The question that
underlies the analysis of motives is ‘Why do people pursue that specific goal with that
particular, high or low, level of intensity?’ Motives can take many forms, as shown
through examples given in the top part of Figure 2. The most basic dichotomy differ-
entiates approach/attraction motives (the proverbial ‘carrot’) from avoidance motives
(the proverbial ‘stick’). That basic dichotomy applies to all levels of living species,
from homo sapiens down to unicellular organisms. In practice, few specialists use it
because of its excessive broadness; they prefer focusing on more specific instances of
either member of this pair. The top part of Figure 2 also shows a major motivational
role played by needs, interests, and values, as well as intrinsic vs. extrinsic motives
that define the two apparently opposite poles of a self-determination continuum
described in Deci and Ryan’s (1985) theory. Other taxonomies could have been added,
like Maslow’s well-known hierarchy of five basic needs (Maslow, 1954), Holland’s
RIASEC hexagon of occupation-related personal interests mentioned earlier (see the
T component), or Henry Murray’s list of needs and press initially created to analyze
Figure 2. ‘Crossing the Rubicon’ (adapted from Corno, 1993, Figure 2, p. 18).
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 89 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
90 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
the Thematic Apperception Test’s stories (Murray et al., 1943). It is well beyond the
scope of the present article to examine in detail the complex realm of human motives
with regard to talent development; the DMGT framework can embrace whichever
among them will be recognized as the most significant. I will briefly comment here on
a few that appear more frequently in the professional literature or among teachers.
Figure 2. ‘Crossing the Rubicon’ (adapted from Corno, 1993, Figure 2, p. 18).
Intrinsic/extrinsic motives
Intrinsic motivation (IMI) probably qualifies as one of the most talked about motiva-
tional concepts in educational fields; it is usually defined as the pursuit of a learning
activity for its own sake, for the ‘intrinsic’ pleasure of doing it. Some will call its stronger
expressions ‘love of learning’ or ‘passion for learning’. Although IMI has been studied
mainly within the schooling process, the concept applies to any area of interest. Indeed,
I consider the concept of ‘interest’ as a perfect overlap of IMI. People who like/love
tap dancing, science fiction, gardening, kayaking, spelunking, astrology, entomology,
football, or any one of thousands of interest areas available to youth and adults, all exem-
plify intrinsic motivation, namely exploring an interest area for the simple pleasure
gained from that activity. Intrinsic motivations vary immensely at the individual level.
Each person will express qualitatively and quantitatively different reactions to any inter-
est area; some love playing cards, others hate it, some love reading, others don’t like
it, and so forth. Indeed, these individual differences have been the basis for interest
inventories, as exemplified by the famous Strong Interest Inventory (see Anastasi &
Urbina, 1997), whose profile structure is based on the RIASEC types mentioned above.
In the realm of human conduct, any motive other than the pursuit of an activity
(e.g., a learning activity, practicing a trade, a profession, or a hobby) for the pleasure
gained from the practice itself will be considered an extrinsic motive (IMX). Going to
school to get a more prestigious or well-paying job, to please one’s parents, or to enjoy
a nice group of peers, are all extrinsic motives. Consequently, most of the human
needs identified by scholars over the past century, both approach and avoidance needs,
even outside pressures transformed into motives (e.g., doing something under the
pressure of a gun), all members of that vast panoply of motivations will be considered
extrinsic motives to pursue some activity.
The vast majority of educators look down on extrinsic motives as less ‘noble’ than
intrinsic ones. Think of all these professional development activities that aim to
improve teachers’ skills as stimulators of intrinsic motivation in their students. I have
stopped counting the number of parents and teachers asking for my advice on how
best to bring about that IMI for a particular subject matter – or all of them! – in
students who appear little motivated by their school learning. There is a strong belief
among educators that academic success goes hand in hand with high intrinsic motiva-
tion. In point of fact, the authors of the self-determination theory that brought the IMI
and IMX concepts into popularity did present these two constructs as the opposite
poles of a self-determination continuum.
Intrinsically motivated behaviors are engaged in for their own sake – for the pleasure
and satisfaction derived from their performance… Extrinsically motivated behaviors, on
the other hand, are instrumental in nature. They are performed not out of interest but
because they are believed to be instrumental to some separable consequence… Self-
determination theory posits that the four types of extrinsic motivation [external,
introjected, identified, integrated] result from the internalization processes having been
differentially effective. The resulting regulatory styles thus fall at different points along
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 90 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 91
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
an autonomy continuum that describes the extent to which they have been internalized
and integrated. (Deci, Vallerand, Pelletier, & Ryan, 1991, pp. 328–329)
The above quote clearly mentions an ‘autonomous continuum’ whose positive
pole is represented by intrinsic motivations, and negative pole by ‘external’ extrinsic
motivations, with various forms of more internalized IMX somewhere in between
these two extremes. It should follow from that view that intensities in IMI will be
negatively correlated with parallel intensities in IMX. That hypothesis has not resisted
the search for empirical support. For instance, Gagné and St Père (2002) separately
measured these two constructs with a large sample of high school girls using a self-
administered questionnaire developed by Vallerand, Blais, Brière, and Pelletier
(1989); assessments were done both at the beginning and at the end of a fall semester.
Here are their conclusions.
It is also interesting that the IM [Intrinsic] and EM [Extrinsic] constructs, contrary to
theory, are not negatively correlated. The students implicitly see them as independent,
while the parents’ ratings make them positively correlated. These results show that it is
possible for intrinsic and extrinsic motives to be pursued in parallel, for the intrinsic
pleasure of learning to coexist with the desire to find a well-paying job (EM.Ex) or to
prove to oneself that one can attain the goal of a high school diploma (EM.Ij). This is not
the place for a full-fledged reexamination of Deci and Ryan’s self-determination contin-
uum, but our results clearly question that central aspect of their theory. Moreover, other
recent empirical work (e.g., Hoekman, McCormick, & Gross, 1999; Nicholls, 1992)
supports that questioning. (Gagné & St Père, 2002, p. 94)
In brief, it is my conviction that extrinsic motives are as important in any talent
development situation than intrinsic ones. No one would believe that athletes
progressing toward international excellence do not have in mind a possible Olympic
medal (extrinsic), or a huge income (extrinsic), or the power felt from being the best
(see the famous film Chariots of fire on that subject).
Passion
As mentioned earlier, quantitative differences in intrinsic motivation can range from
high levels to total lack (I hate…). Very high levels of interest have made popular the
use of a label originally associated with romantic love: passion. Of course, lay people
have always used the term passion to describe intense involvement in an interest area,
related either to work or leisure. Its exploration as a scientific construct is quite recent.
Robert Vallerand, with his colleagues and students, can be credited for having studied
that construct more than anyone (see Vallerand et al., 2003). These scholars define
passion as follows.
[It is] a strong inclination toward an activity that people like, that they find important,
and in which they invest time and energy. Thus, for an activity to represent a passion for
people, it has to be significant in their lives, something that they like, and something at
which they spend time on a regular basis… Such passions become central features of
one’s identity and serve to define the person. Those who have a passion for playing the
guitar, for reading, or for jogging do not merely play the guitar, read, or jog. They are
‘guitar players’, ‘readers’, or ‘joggers’. (Valerand et al., 2003, p. 757)
Vallerand et al. (2003) distinguish two types of passions: harmonious (HP) and
obsessive (OP). In the first case, ‘individuals are not compelled to do the activity but
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 91 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
92 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
rather they freely choose to do so… the activity occupies a significant but not over-
powering space in the person’s identity and is in harmony with other aspects of the
person’s life’ (p. 757). In the second case, people ‘cannot help but to engage in the
passionate activity. The passion must run its course as it controls the person. Because
activity engagement is out of the person’s control, it eventually takes disproportionate
space in the person’s identity and causes conflict with other activities in the person’s
life’ (p. 757). In order to assess the prevalence of passion among youth and adults,
Vallerand and his coworkers created a 34-item self-administered passion scale to
assess both OP and HP dimensions of the passion construct, to which were added a
few general items about the nature of passion. One key item assessed to what extent
respondents considered an activity ‘dear to their heart’ to be a passion (using a seven-
point scale ranging from ‘1’ = do not agree at all to ‘7’ = completely agree). They
decided that a score of four and above on that specific question would mean a
‘passionate’ level of identification.
In one study, they asked a group of 559 college students to identify such a ‘dear to
their heart’ activity, and use it as the target of their passion scale. All the participants
were able to do so, and only 16% gave a rating lower than four on the key ‘passion’
question. Do 85% of adults really possess a ‘passionate’ center of interest in their life?
Such a large percentage makes me question whether their choice of a 4+ threshold for
that key question represents an appropriate threshold or an overly generous one. I must
point out that this dissatisfaction in no way questions Vallerand’s general theoretical
or empirical approach. It was interesting to observe that 60% of the activities identified
concerned sports (individual or team), that 35% were related to active (e.g., playing
music or painting) or passive (e.g., listening to music, reading) leisure activities, but
that less than 4% concerned work or education-related activities. These researchers’
work opens an interesting door to the empirical study of an important motivational
construct in the context of talent development. Still, much remains to be explored,
especially the presence of that extreme form of intrinsic motivation in talent develop-
ment endeavors, and its relationship with extrinsic motives, like the needs for power
and achievement. This comment brings us to the last of my observations with regard
to the motivation construct.
Talent-related motives
Which motives appear, more so than most others, to foster exceptional achievements?
In his fascinating book on the origins of ‘greatness’, Dean Keith Simonton (1994)
devotes a full chapter to the subject of motivation. Borrowing from the work of Henry
Murray and David McClelland, he targets their famous triad of motives: achievement
(IMA), power (IMP), and affiliation (IMF). Simonton considers two of them, the
needs for achievement and/or power, as the more plausible drivers of exceptional
productivity. If the concept of power seems clear to circumscribe theoretically, the
need for achievement leaves room in my view to definite ambiguity. Simonton
borrows McClelland’s definition of that construct, stating that it is ‘the desire to do
“something for its own sake, for the intrinsic satisfaction of doing something better”.
Such individuals set high standards for themselves, and try to surpass what has been
done before’ (p. 125).
That definition leaves me somewhat dissatisfied. At first glance, it clearly consid-
ers the achievement motive as a form of intrinsic motivation; the first part of the quote
makes that clear. On the other hand, it seems to me that the need to ‘surpass what has
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 92 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 93
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
been done before’ (by others?) has a definite normative component; it sounds like a
desire to show one’s superiority over others. To use a sport analogy, I am not
convinced that winning athletes can easily dissociate their intrinsic – and ipsative –
pleasure at surpassing themselves from that most extrinsic satisfaction of surpassing
others, of gaining power by making others the losers. At low levels of excellence or
at early stages of a talent development process, it might be easy to accept a limited
role for the power motive, but as the competition gets tougher and the excellence goals
approach a maximum level, no doubt that the power motive will increase its presence
as a driver of further learning and practice.
Simonton ends his chapter on motivation with a section called ‘the ultimate motive
for greatness?’ Note the question mark. He begins by showing that extraordinary scien-
tific or artistic productivity, both in quality and quantity, results from a huge amount
of work: long weeks, month after month, and year after year. He gives examples of
many giants’ enormous career productivity, like Edison’s close to 1100 patents,
Balzac’s 85 novels, or Picasso’s 20,000 works. As he says:
It takes work to become a renowned genius. These individuals are driven by huge moti-
vational forces that far eclipse the impetus behind less accomplished colleagues… Where
does this drive come from? The answer to this question is one of the great mysteries of
psychology… The genius may be in the highest percentiles on the achievement motive,
or the power motive, or maybe both together… Moreover, we certainly must be willing
to credit the genius with some intrinsic motives as well. Geniuses cannot spend so many
hours without an inherent passion for what they do. (Simonton, 1994, pp. 140–141)
This is a fascinating quote, and so for many reasons. First, Simonton confounds
elements of Heckhausen and Kulh’s motivation (the urge and passion) and volition
(the determination, the drive, and the hard work). In other words, it confirms my
earlier statement to the effect that few scholars acknowledge this differentiation.
Second, when Simonton says ‘or maybe both together’, he confirms my own observa-
tion that power and achievement cannot be easily distinguished, at least in practice.
Finally, his statement that ‘we must be willing to credit the genius with some intrinsic
motives as well’ (my emphasis) directly places the power and achievement motives
among extrinsic (IMX) motives, thus contradicting his earlier definition of the
achievement motive. As a final touch, it is worth quoting Simonton’s conclusion: ‘In
all likelihood, there can be no ultimate motive behind success’ (p. 141). That is a
statement I fully endorse.
Volition
The authors of action control theory labeled the point of commitment ‘crossing the
Rubicon’, a reference to Julius Cesar’s fateful decision in 49BC to bring his army
south of that northern border and attack the Roman government. He is said to have
uttered there that famous sentence ‘alea jacta est’ (the dice are cast), meaning that his
decision meant a point of no return. In practice, as shown in Figure 2, that commit-
ment need not be as final as the metaphor implies; when individuals experience major
– in their views, of course – difficulties, they will not hesitate to re-cross that virtual
Rubicon in order to reassess their initial goal. Still, that Rubicon-like commitment
means, in Corno’s words, that ‘goals are typically protected and fostered by self-
regulatory activity rather than reconsidered or changed, often even when challenged’
(1993, p. 15).
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 93 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
94 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Components
Even though Corno (1993) introduces ACT at a microscopic level, that of day-to-day
student decisions in the classroom, it remains quite easy to extrapolate the theory at
the macroscopic level typical of the DMGT’s approach to talent development. The
main function of the volitional processes consists in directing and controlling intellec-
tual, emotional, and behavioral activities in order to maximize goal attainment in situ-
ations when such goal(s) is/are perceived as hard to reach; an easy goal requires no
volition at all. For instance, I confess needing no volition to keep reading a good
thriller; in fact, volition will be harnessed to put it aside! Figure 2 illustrates two types
of strategies that individuals will utilize during the implementation period, especially
when ‘the going gets tough’: (a) goal-related cognitions, and (b) action control
processes. Corno (1993) describes them as follows.
Action control processes refer to knowledge and strategies used to manage cognitive
and non-cognitive resources for the purpose of goal attainment. How students allocate
and control their attention, as well as enlist techniques for self-motivation and for
handling intrusive emotions, are examples of processes included in this cluster. The
second cluster consists of goal-related cognitions that form the basis for adaptive use of
learning strategies (or mindful effort investment) in tasks (Salomon, 1983, p. 13). These
include (a) the well-timed application of deep or elaborative processing and (b) the
monitoring and appraisal processes that help determine the extent to which effort invest-
ments are sustained. (Corno, 1993, p. 17)
Note the looping arrow in the bottom center of Figure 2; it conveys the
dynamic idea of recurring volitional strategies through the long period – at least
within the DMGT’s macroscopic perspective – between commitment to a goal and
reaching it. The other dotted arrow at left identifies occasional dropping out of the
implementation loop in order to re-cross the Rubicon and reassess the relevance of
a goal. Most students, artists, and athletes need to do that once in a while during
the course of their talent development process. The last item in Figure 2 is called
‘volitional styles’. The expression refers to relatively stable individual differences
in temperament that affect goal choices and striving through the action-control
processes. The concept of will power as a human trait, one of the five personality
dimensions in the Big Five theory of personality (Digman, 1990), comes immedi-
ately to mind. This particularity of the ACT’s volition construct creates an auto-
matic link with the IP sub-component of the DMGT, exemplifying the complex
interactions between components and sub-components in the dynamic process of
talent development.
IM–IV relationships
The most common approach to assess volitional intensity or strength uses self-report
items, usually embedded in broader ‘motivation’ questionnaires (see Gagné & St Père,
2002, for a literature review). But, there is another way to examine volition more
directly, namely through the investment (DI) sub-component (see Figure 1). The time,
energy, or money that talentees do invest in the pursuit of their excellence goal(s) can
tell a lot about their determination not to re-cross the Rubicon. How talentees will
react to occasional failures or setbacks can also be used as a volitional index, although
that must be done with caution, because of the parallel impact of other personality
characteristics (e.g., optimism, emotional stability) or outside pressures.
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 94 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 95
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
The distinction between IM and IV measures confirms the conceptual differentia-
tion between the two constructs. And that differentiation has practical impacts. When
talentees express their motivational intensity – how important the goal is, how impor-
tant the motive is – we remain within the confines of the motivational phase, even
though that information announces volitional ‘intentions’. These intentions do not
guarantee their actual manifestation throughout the implementation phase of the goal-
management process. As New Year promises and yo-yo dieting have shown again and
again, the road to hell is paved with good intentions! Said differently, we cannot
expect a close relationship between quantitative assessments of motivation and voli-
tion. Some young people easily ‘fall in love’ with a new activity, while others will
slowly develop their liking, keeping it at a lower level. Similar individual differences
can be observed in the strength of will power or level of investment in a given talent
development process. These disparities lead to interesting differential profiles, with
high-M behavior being sometimes associated with high-V behavior, but also with
moderate-V or even low-V behavior; and vice versa.
The present lack of good IV measures limits an in-depth analysis of IM–IV rela-
tionships. In an attempt in that direction, Gagné and St Père (2002) separately assessed
high school students’ intrinsic (IM) and extrinsic (EM) motivation for school, as well
as their level of volition (labeled P for perseverance). They observed among other
things that ‘the students’ EM scores entertain no relationship whatsoever with either
IM or P, but these last two are partially correlated (.45/.44)’ (p. 85). The IM–P corre-
lations, obtained both at the beginning and end of a fall semester, indicate a moderate
relationship between students’ self-assessments of their school-related intrinsic moti-
vation and volition, but not between extrinsic motivations and volition. As interesting
as they may be, such exploratory results need to be replicated, not only within the
general education system, but also in other major fields of talent development.
Closing comments
Because of space constraints, it is not possible to explore in more detail the motivation-
volition pair of constructs that play such a central role within the I component of the
DMGT. Still, I couldn’t end this short article without discussing briefly two questions
I consider crucial for a better understanding of the role of motivation in talent devel-
opment: (a) nature’s involvement in goal-directed behavior, and (b) the IM–IV ranking
among causal influences on talent emergence.
Biological underpinnings
The subject of the hereditary or biological underpinnings of common motivational
constructs is rarely discussed in the talent development literature. Most discussions
assume that, except for occasional unconscious influences, goals, motives, or volition
are under the total control of talentees. Yet, a large and growing pool of empirical
research has shown significant hereditary roots for most facets of both IM and IV sub-
components: interests, values, needs, motives, or volitional styles. For example,
Plomin, De Fries, and McClearn (1990) cite a study of Roberts and Johansson (1974),
which ‘included over 1500 twin pairs [both fraternal and identical] and indicated that
all of the vocational types – called realistic, intellectual, social, enterprising, conven-
tional, and artistic – show approximately the same level of genetic influence’ (p. 387),
namely identical twin correlations of about .50 as opposed to correlations of about .25
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 95 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
96 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
for fraternal twins. These were later confirmed through the data accumulated within
the renowned Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart (see Bouchard et al., 1990).
Plomin et al. (1990) also mention a significant genetic component for the achievement
motive.
We can also mention heritability data associated with the Big Five personality
factors mentioned above, also called the Five Factor Model (FFM). These five
factors are commonly labeled extraversion (E), agreeableness (A), conscientiousness
(C), neuroticism (N), and intellect/openness (O). Recall that I identified the C
element as a clear volitional style. Concerning all members of the FFM, there is
growing evidence for a close relationship between temperament dimensions and
adult personality traits (Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000); this relationship probably
explains why all FFM dimensions have significant genetic underpinnings (Rowe,
1997). Finally, I can direct readers to two massive surveys of hundreds of studies on
the biological bases of most social and affective characteristics (e.g., altruism, impul-
sivity, shyness, aggressiveness, empathy); they can be found in Daniel Goleman’s
two bestsellers: Emotional intelligence (1995) and Social intelligence (2006),
especially the last one.
The fact that most IM and IV facets have their roots in a person’s genotype
significantly reduces individual control over the expression of these characteristics.
Recall the Atkinson quote mentioned earlier about one of the two crucial ‘rolls of the
dice’ being dealt by Nature, what he called ‘the accident of birth’. Those who
promote a variety of interventions by parents and teachers aimed at increasing
student motivation and volition should keep in mind the constraints they will
unavoidably face from their subjects’ I characteristics. On the other hand, as the new
science of epigenetics – the influence of environmental agents on gene expression –
has shown, genes are not destiny; there is much more to human behavior than the
outmoded ‘Nature vs. Nurture’ dichotomy. Yet, genes do create predispositions to act
in a certain way, and the strength of these predispositions varies considerably from
one person to the next. Strong negative predispositions require much stronger envi-
ronmental controls to ensure that they will not lead to behavioral dysfunction; and, in
some instances, the required environmental support will exceed most available
resources (see Ridley, 2003). In a nutshell, much remains to be discovered with
regard to the complex interactions between genes and environments, but we should
always keep in mind that significant biological constraints do act on our daily goal-
oriented behavior.
What makes a difference?
Even though all four causal components are always active in any talent development
process, it does not mean that they are equally powerful as agents of talent emergence;
could it be that some of the components and/or sub-components of the DMGT play a
more important causal role than others? Of course, this ‘what makes a difference’
(WMD) question makes little sense at the individual level, and so for many reasons,
the most significant being that any answer given in the case of Subject X has little
chance of applying to Subject Y’s talent development trajectory. Extreme individual
differences abound, and I have accumulated over the years dozens of such examples.
One of the more recent of such counter-intuitive examples targets the famous tennis
player Andre Agassi. In his recent autobiography he states on the very first page: ‘I
play tennis for a living, even though I hate tennis, hate it with a dark and secret passion,
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 96 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 97
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
and always have’ (Agassi, 2009, p. 3) What does this say about the importance of
intrinsic motivation as a driver of exceptional achievements!
But what about averages? Are some factors generally recognized as more powerful
predictors of outstanding performances? For all those involved in identifying youth
with high potential and planning the development of their talents, this is the ultimate
question. Unfortunately, that question has yet to receive a clear answer in the empirical
literature. In a target article published in this journal a few years ago (Gagné, 2004), I
devoted over 20% of my text to that specific question. I summarized my tentative
answer with the acronym C.GIPE. The GIPE part meant that I gave first rank to
outstanding natural abilities (G), with successively lower ranks given in turn to intrap-
ersonal catalysts (I), the developmental process (then labeled P), and finally to envi-
ronmental catalysts (E). The chance factor (C) received a special priority (with a ‘dot’
separating it from the rest) to acknowledge its prominent role in creating individual
differences in natural abilities and intrapersonal catalysts – Atkinson’s two crucial
rolls of the dice. Space does not allow a discussion as extended as the one in the target
article cited above. I will only briefly comment on the G vs. I ranking.
To explain the superiority given to G over I, I pointed out that virtually every
comparative study of the unique explanatory power of motivational constructs over
and beyond IQ measures had shown a clear superiority of the latter. I then quoted
Gagné and St Père’s (2002) exhaustive literature review as follows.
Motivation’s independent contribution to the prediction of scholastic or occupational
achievement appears limited. It is frequently non-existent… or much less powerful
that the independent contribution of cognitive abilities… The 4:1 and 6:1 ratios
respectively extracted from Walberg’s (1984) and Schmidt and Hunter’s (1998)
syntheses, probably upper-limit estimates, are more or less equidistant from the two
extremes. (Gagné & St Père, 2002, p. 10)
This quote means that IQ scores ‘explain’ on average about five times more
achievement variance. As for other constructs included in the I component, there is
little scientific literature on their unique contribution to talent development. Of course,
that analysis applies strictly to academic talent development within the general K-12
schooling process. If we consider that the school system is the only field in which
unmotivated talentees do not have the option to drop out – at least not until late high
school – this might modify any comparison of various predictors of outstanding
achievement. That special constraint does not exist in other fields, like arts or sports.
After re-reading that long analysis in the 2004 HAS target article, I felt comfortable
in maintaining the same GIDE hierarchy. The only small modification that came to
my mind was to suggest that if I had to rank order the IM and IV sub-components, I
would not hesitate to give priority to IV, especially in two situations: (a) K-12
academic talent development, and (b) the pursuit of exceptional or extreme levels of
talent in any field.
In conclusion, allow me to ‘loop the loop’ by bringing back the very first quote I
introduced in this article: ‘[famous athletes] tell us all something about ourselves and
what we may be capable of achieving, if we dream and make the commitment to
work hard towards that vision’ (Hemery, 1986, p. 204). It clearly differentiates the
IM component – if we dream – from its volitional counterpart. And I believe that
these two distinct sets of goal-management processes offer a very useful analytical
framework to dissect the goal-oriented behavior of talentees in all fields of talent
development.
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 97 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
98 F. Gagné
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Note
1. Interested readers can also find on the web an eight-page overview of the updated
version. Just search for ‘DMGT 2.0 overview’. That overview is also available from the
author (fygagne@gmail.com) in four other languages: French, Spanish, Portuguese, and
German.
References
Agassi, A. (2009). Open: An autobiography. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice-Hall.
Atkinson, J.W. (1978). Motivational determinants of intellective performance and cumulative
achievement. In J.W. Atkinson, & J.O. Raynor (Eds.), Personality, motivation, and
achievement (pp. 221–242). New York: Wiley.
Bouchard, T.J., Lykken, D.T., McGue, M., Segal, N.L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of
human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science,
250, 223–228.
Brody, L.E., & Stanley, J.C. (2005). Youths who reason exceptionally well mathematically
and/or verbally: Using the MVT:D4 model to develop their talents. In R.J. Sternberg, &
J.E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 20–37). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Corno, L. (1993). The best-laid plans: Modern conceptions of volition and educational
research. Educational Researcher, 22, 14–22.
Corno, L., & Kanfer, R. (1993). The role of volition in learning and performance. In L. Darling-
Hammond (Ed.), Review of research in education (Vol. 19, pp. 301–341). Washington, DC:
American Educational Research Association.
Cross, T.L., & Coleman, L.J. (2005). School-based conception of giftedness. In R.J. Sternberg,
& J.E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 52–63). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E.L., Vallerand, R.J., Pelletier, L.G., & Ryan, R.M. (1991). Motivation and education:
The self-determination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26, 325–346.
De Waele, M., Morval, J., & Sheitoyan, R. (1993). Self-management in organizations: The
dynamics of interaction. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Digman, J.M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. In M.R.
Rosenzweig, & L.W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 41, pp. 417–440).
Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in
the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Feldhusen, J.F. (2005). Giftedness, talent, expertise, and creative achievement. In R.J. Stern-
berg, & J.E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (pp. 64–79). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Folsing, A. (1998). Albert Einstein. New York: Penguin.
Gagné, F. (1985). Giftedness and talent: Reexamining a reexamination of the definitions.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 29, 103–112.
Gagné, F. (1998). A proposal for subcategories within the gifted or talented populations.
Gifted Child Quarterly, 42, 87–95.
Gagné, F. (1999). The multigifts of multitalented individuals. In S. Cline, & K.T. Hegeman
(Eds.), Gifted education in the twenty-first century: Issues and concerns, (pp. 17–45).
Delray Beach, FL: Winslow Press.
Gagné, F. (2003, November). Self-management: A crucial catalyst. Paper presented at the
50th annual conference of the National Association for Gifted Children, Indianapolis, IN.
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as a developmental theory.
High Ability Studies, 15, 119–147.
Gagné, F. (2009). Building gifts into talents: Detailed overview of the DMGT 2.0. In B.
MacFarlane, & T. Stambaugh, (Eds.), Leading change in gifted education: The festschrift
of Dr Joyce VanTassel-Baska. Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 98 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM
High Ability Studies 99
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
Gagné, F., & St Père, F. (2002). When IQ is controlled, does motivation still predict achieve-
ment? Intelligence, 30, 71–100.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Goleman, D. (2006). Social intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Hemery, D. (1986). The pursuit of sporting excellence: A study of sport’s highest achievers.
London: Willow Books.
Hoekman, K., McCormick, J., & Gross, M.U.M. (1999). The optimal context for gifted
students: A preliminary exploration of motivational and affective considerations. Gifted
Child Quarterly, 43, 170–193.
International Labour Organization. (2008). www.ilo.org/public/english/bureau/stat/isco/
index.htm.
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1985). Action control: From cognition to behavior. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row.
McCrae, R.R., Costa, P.T. Jr., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hrebickova, M., Avia, M.D.,
Sanz, J., Sanchez-Bernardos, M.L., Kusdil, M.E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P.R., &
Smith, P.B. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality, and life span devel-
opment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 173–186.
Moon, S. (2003). Personal talent. High Ability Studies, 14, 5–21.
Murray, H.A. et al. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test: Manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Nicholls, J.G. (1992). Students as educational theorists. In D. Schunk, & J. Meece (Eds.),
Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 267–285). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J.C., & McClearn, G.E. (1990). Behavioral genetics: A primer. New
York: Freeman.
Ridley, M. (2003). Nature via nurture. New York: Fourth Estate.
Roberts, C.A., & Johansson, C.B. (1974). The inheritance of cognitive interest styles among
twins. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 4, 237–243.
Rothbart, M.K., Ahadi, S.A., & Evans, D.E. (2000). Temperament and personality: Origins
and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.
Rowe, D.C. (1997). Genetics, temperament, and personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S.
Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 367–386). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Simonton, D.K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes history and why. New York: Guilford Press.
Sternberg, R.J., & Davidson, J.E. (2005). Conceptions of giftedness. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Vallerand, R.J., Blais, M.R., Brière, N.M., & Pelletier, L.G. (1989). Construction et validation
de l’échelle de motivation en éducation [Construction and validation of the School
Motivation Scale]. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 323–349.
Vallerand, R.J., Mageau, G.A., Ratelle, C., Léonard, M., Blanchard, C., Koestner, R., Gagné,
M., & Marsolais, J. (2003). Les passions de l’âme: On obsessive and harmonious passion.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 756–767.
Zimmerman, B.J. (1998). Academic studying and the development of personal skill: A self-
regulatory perspective. Educational Psychologist, 33, 73–86.
AQ5
CHAS_A_525341.fm Page 99 Tuesday, October 19, 2010 10:10 PM