ArticlePublisher preview available

Variable Attention Facilitates Creative Problem Solving

American Psychological Association
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Traditionally, researchers have associated creativity with defocused attention. Recent experimental evidence contradicts this notion by demonstrating that in creative people, defocused attention is a variable state rather than a stable trait. Specifically, creative people are better at adjusting their focus of attention as a function of task demands. When the task is ill defined and ambiguity is high, attention is defocused, resulting in slower processing on the task. In contrast, when the task is well defined and ambiguity is low, attention is focused, resulting in faster processing on the task. This flexibility can confer distinct advantages to creative people in the course of problem solving as changes in the structure of the problem necessitate corresponding adjustments in solution strategy.
Variable Attention Facilitates Creative Problem Solving
Oshin Vartanian
DRDC Toronto
Traditionally, researchers have associated creativity with defocused attention. Recent experimental
evidence contradicts this notion by demonstrating that in creative people, defocused attention is a variable
state rather than a stable trait. Specifically, creative people are better at adjusting their focus of attention
as a function of task demands. When the task is ill defined and ambiguity is high, attention is defocused,
resulting in slower processing on the task. In contrast, when the task is well defined and ambiguity is low,
attention is focused, resulting in faster processing on the task. This flexibility can confer distinct
advantages to creative people in the course of problem solving as changes in the structure of the problem
necessitate corresponding adjustments in solution strategy.
Keywords: attention, creativity, problem solving, reaction time
There has been longstanding interest in the role of attention in
the creative process (see Runco & Sakamoto, 1999). This is not
surprising, given that many researchers define the creative process
as one involving the combination of concepts previously thought
to be unrelated (Poincare´, 1913). Because combining two or more
concepts necessitates that they fall within the focus of attention,
variations in the focus of attention can have a direct impact on
one’s ability to engage in this combinatorial activity. By and large,
researchers have argued that in creative people defocused attention
or “broad attentional capacity” is a stable trait (Eysenck, 1995;
Kasof, 1997; Mendelsohn, 1976). Presumably, this stable charac-
teristic enables creative people to attend to more concepts at any
given point in time, thus increasing the likelihood of combining
them into novel and useful products—the accepted definition of
creativity (see Sternberg, 1999).
However, this description of creative people does not ring true
based on what we know from biographical and personality studies
of the problem-solving behavior of eminent scientists (see Feist,
1999; Martindale, 2001). Specifically, the problem-solving behav-
ior of eminent scientists can alternate between extraordinary levels
of focus on specific concepts and playful exploration of ideas. This
suggests that successful problem solving may be a function of
flexible strategy application in relation to task demands. In the
earlier phases of problem solving when the problem space is
relatively ill defined and ambiguous, a state of defocused attention
would appear advantageous as it would enable the problem solver
to explore more concepts in the problem space, potentially increas-
ing the likelihood of sampling relevant building blocks for con-
structing creative solutions. In contrast, in the later phases of
problem solving when the problem space is relatively well defined
and unambiguous, a state of focused attention would appear to be
advantageous as it would enable the problem solver to focus
specifically on those concepts that appear more relevant for further
scrutiny, discarding other less relevant concepts.
Indirectly, such variation in the focus of attention should be
captured by variation in processing latency. When attention is
defocused and the problem solver is attending to more concepts
simultaneously, processing should slow down. In contrast, when
attention is focused and the problem solver is attending to fewer
concepts simultaneously, processing should speed up. Based on
this line of reasoning, we set out to conduct a series of experiments
to test the hypothesis that creative participants would be slower
than noncreative participants on tasks that are ambiguous and
involve interference, but that they should be faster than noncre-
ative participants on tasks that are unambiguous and do not involve
interference. A pattern of findings consistent with this prediction
would provide the necessary ammunition to support the idea that
far from being perpetually defocused, creative people are more
flexible than noncreative people in their focus of attention. What
follows is a summary of the findings of our studies.
Creativity and Speed of Information Processing
Our first study involved a sample of undergraduate men from an
American university (Vartanian, Martindale, & Kwiatkowski,
2007). We measured creative potential using fluency scores from
the Alternate Uses Task. Each participant completed four reaction
time tasks in randomized order, two of which did not involve
interference or ambiguity (Hick Task, Concept Verification Task)
whereas the other two did (Negative Priming, Global-local). As
predicted, there was a significant negative correlation between
creative potential and response latency on the Hick Task and the
Concept Verification Task, whereas there was a significant posi-
tive correlation between creative potential and response latency on
Negative Priming and Global-local. Furthermore, within Global-
local, the positive correlation between creative potential and re-
sponse latency was particularly strong when interference was
maximal, as would be predicted by our hypothesis. These results
demonstrated that participants with higher creative potential are
more flexible along the attention continuum, and that their focus of
attention varies more as a function of the characteristics of the task.
In a follow-up study we sought to replicate our results to test the
robustness of the effect. Testing the same hypothesis, we collected
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Oshin
Vartanian, DRDC Toronto, 1133 Sheppard Avenue West, P. O. Box, 2000,
Toronto, ON, M3M 3B9 Canada. E-mail: oshin.vartanian@drdc-rddc.gc.ca
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2009 by the Crown in Right of Canada
2009, Vol. 3, No. 1, 57–59 1931-3896/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014781
57
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Creative problem-solving requires both flexibility and persistence to generate novel yet useful and valuable ideas [1][2][3][4][5][6]. Given the dynamic nature of creative tasks, whether flexibility (that is, transitioning to a different item, subgoal or strategy) or persistence (continuing with the current item, subgoal or strategy) is optimal for performance often depends on the individual's current or momentary progress towards task goals and the nature of the task [7,8]. In creative tasks-as well as during cognitive search and goal-directed behaviour more generally-neither stubbornly maintained unyielding persistence (sticking with a current goal, subgoal or stimulus) nor excessive flexibility (distractedly or impulsively continually switching attention to new stimuli, goals or subgoals) is likely to lead to best performance [2,5,9,10]. ...
... That is, both shift-count (switching one's attention and ideational search efforts away from the current item to the second of two concurrently available prompts) and dwell-length (persevering in working on the current item, continuing to direct one's focus, cognitive effort, and ideational search towards the current task item) contributed to the number and originality of ideas generated on the divergent thinking tasks. This finding closely coheres with several earlier (and newly emerging) theoretical accounts of creative problem-solving that emphasize the contributions of both flexibility and persistence to the process of generating novel yet useful and valuable ideas [2][3][4][5][6][7][8]65,66]. Whereas shifting may help to overcome or minimize cognitive fixation [40][41][42], and can provide external cues that stimulate thinking in a new direction [52], overly frequent shifting may lead to output interference (good ideas may get lost before they can be output), interfere with deeper search and concentrated attentional effort [53], or preclude extended perceptual-conceptual simulation [55]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adaptivity allows individuals to flexibly execute cognitive control to meet dynamically changing task demands while adhering to task goals. Such adaptivity is crucial for navigating complex problem spaces such as creative problem-solving. Recent theoretical and empirical investigations of individuals' tendencies towards flexibility versus persistence have begun to address the questions of how adaptivity may be jointly shaped by general (across-situational) predispositions and by task requirements. However, such investigation is hampered by the lack of comparable ways to quantify trait-like tendencies across different task contexts. Using a Self-Guided Transitions paradigm, in which participants are allowed to autonomously choose whether to continue, to switch or to return to working on either of two concurrently presented problem-solving items, this preregistered study provides evidence for both clear within-individual consistency in the proclivity towards flexibility versus persistence, and adaptive modulation of flexibility versus persistence for tasks that predominantly call on divergent versus convergent idea search. Both shifting and dwelling were associated with the generation of more numerous and more original ideas on divergent-thinking tasks—underscoring the creative and ideational rewards to be found both by sometimes staying the course (persistence/exploitation) and sometimes choosing to shift our efforts in a different direction (flexibility/exploration).
... Effective idea generation requires that the ideas are both novel and appropriate, which requires identifying and implementing different strategies (Gilhooly et al., 2007;Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011), executive control (Vartanian, 2009;Zabelina et al., 2012;Zabelina & Robinson, 2010), idea evaluation (Finke et al., 1996;Gabora, 2005;Vartanian, 2011), and rejecting obvious and inappropriate ideas (Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011). Additionally, many studies have found links between creativity and intelligence Karwowski et al., 2021;Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011;Silvia, 2015). ...
... Effective idea generation requires that the ideas are both novel and appropriate, which requires identifying and implementing different strategies (Gilhooly et al., 2007;Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011), executive control (Vartanian, 2009;Zabelina et al., 2012;Zabelina & Robinson, 2010), idea evaluation (Finke et al., 1996;Gabora, 2005;Vartanian, 2011), and rejecting obvious and inappropriate ideas (Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011). Additionally, many studies have found links between creativity and intelligence Karwowski et al., 2021;Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011;Silvia, 2015). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Overinclusive thinking, characterized by flexible category boundaries and broad associative networks, is closely linked to creativity. However, there has been a lack of rigorous psychometric evaluation of tasks measuring overinclusive thinking. In this paper, we introduce the Categorical Overinclusive Thinking Task (COverTT) and evaluate its reliability and validity. Across three studies involving 749 participants, we explore the relationship between overinclusive thinking, creativity, openness, creative behaviors, intelligence, and schizotypy. Our findings demonstrate that the COverTT is a reliable tool for assessing overinclusive thinking. Furthermore, we discuss the relationship between the COverTT and the criterion validity measures. To enable other researchers to readily use the COverTT for measuring overinclusive thinking, we provide open access to our data, code, and materials at OSF (https://osf.io/c3phq/?view_only=2973ca020429414db1ea069ede02bed5).
... Similarly, cognitive inhibition in a dichotic listening task was found to be positively related to DT ability (Rominger et al., 2017). Other studies reported that creative people show a more adaptive or flexible engagement of inhibition (Dorfman et al., 2008;Kwiatkowski, Vartanian, & Martindale, 1999;Vartanian, Martindale, & Kwiatkowski, 2007;Vartanian, 2009;Zabelina & Robinson, 2010). For example, higher creativity predicted a more effective trial-to-trial modulation of cognitive control within the Stroop task (Zabelina & Robinson, 2010). ...
... Similarly, in VR interactive training environments, individuals demonstrate prolonged response times and elevated error rates due to the need for visual searching and challenging cognitive resource allocation (Olk et al., 2018). Multimedia and immersive technologies can increase the cognitive load (Hsu, 2017), and excessive load may inhibit creative thinking (Carissoli et al., 2023;Vartanian, 2009). VR interactive technology integrates multisensory information, and if poorly designed, it may contribute to an increase in cognitive load (Paas et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to systematically investigate the impacts of virtual reality (VR) technology integrated with electroencephalogram (EEG) vibrotactile feedback on undergraduate students’ creativity performance, attention value, and cognitive load. 4P’s creativity model was adopted as the macro strategy, with cognitive load theory and constructivist theory serving as a micro strategy. A 2 x 2 quasi-experimental factor design was utilized. A total of 149 undergraduate students with varying spatial abilities were randomly assigned to an experimental group (using a VR interactive system with EEG vibrotactile feedback) and a control group (without feedback). Covariance analysis (ANCOVA) and analysis of variance (ANOVA) were employed to identify significant differences among the systems and examine interaction effects among variables. The quantitative results provided empirical evidence that the experimental group exhibited significantly higher levels of creativity performance and attention value than the control group. Furthermore, undergraduate students with higher spatial ability demonstrated lower cognitive load, superior creativity performance, and attention value compared to those with lower spatial ability. The study also revealed that the system positively impacts learning in university creativity education and holds promising potential for improving traditional VR interactive training systems. These findings contribute valuable insights for future research on the application of VR interaction technology and EEG technology in cognitive training for creativity.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Chapter
This volume provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date compendium of theory and research in the field of human intelligence. Each of the 42 chapters is written by world-renowned experts in their respective fields, and collectively, they cover the full range of topics of contemporary interest in the study of intelligence. The handbook is divided into nine parts: Part I covers intelligence and its measurement; Part II deals with the development of intelligence; Part III discusses intelligence and group differences; Part IV concerns the biology of intelligence; Part V is about intelligence and information processing; Part VI discusses different kinds of intelligence; Part VII covers intelligence and society; Part VIII concerns intelligence in relation to allied constructs; and Part IX is the concluding chapter, which reflects on where the field is currently and where it still needs to go.
Article
Creativity is a cornerstone of human evolution and is typically defined as the multifaceted ability to produce novel and useful artifacts. Although much research has focused on divergent thinking, growing evidence underscores the importance of perceptual processing in fostering creativity, particularly through perceptual flexibility. The present work aims to offer a framework that relates creativity to perception, showing how sensory affordances, especially in ambiguous stimuli, can contribute to the generation of novel ideas. In doing so, we contextualize the phenomenon of pareidolia, which involves seeing familiar patterns in noisy or ambiguous stimuli, as a key perceptual mechanism of idea generation—one of the central stages of the creative process. We introduce “divergent perception” to describe the process by which individuals actively engage with the perceptual affordances provided by ambiguous sensory information, and illustrate how this concept could account for the heightened creativity observed in psychedelic and psychotic states. Moreover, we explore how divergent perception relates to cognitive mechanisms crucial in creative thinking, particularly focusing on the role of attention. Finally, we discuss future paths for the exploration of divergent perception, including targeted manipulation of stimulus characteristics and the investigation of the intricate interplay between bottom‐up and top‐down cognitive processes.
Article
Coming up with creative ideas is not easy. In this conceptual article, we integrate organizational behavior, cognitive psychology, and neuroscience literatures to propose that different forms of attention may be a key to maximizing creative usefulness and novelty. Specifically, we develop a neurocognitive framework of attentional control to propose differential pathways from creative goal‐directed attention (a narrow and selective focus) to deliberate information processing, and from undirected attention (a wide and unconstrained focus) to spontaneous information processing. These propositions have implications for creative usefulness and novelty, respectively—namely, that creative goal‐directed attention should facilitate the usefulness of creative outputs to a greater extent than their novelty, whereas undirected attention should promote the novelty of creative outputs to a greater extent than their usefulness. Our framework further suggests that time spent experiencing creative goal‐directed attention followed by undirected attention is the optimal sequence for maximizing both the usefulness and novelty of creative outputs. In combination, our framework advances theoretical understanding of attentional pathways to creative outcomes and offers practical implications for maximizing creative potential at work.
Article
Full-text available
EINLEITUNG: Dieses Kapitel konzentriert sich auf unmittelbare, zusammenhängende, direkte Einflüsse auf Kreativität. Experimentelle Untersuchungen überprüfen die Wirkung von Manipulation auf Wahrnehmung und Bilder, auf Affekte, auf Erregung und Aufmerksamkeit und auf intrinsische Motivation. Die Zusammenfassung der Ergebnisse und Ausblick für zukünftige Forschung erfolgt am Schluss. Zu Beginn einige Anmerkungen zu Kreativität; was ist problematisch in der Kreativitätsforschung, was ist wichtig und was ist zu beachten, welche Beschränkungen gibt es. WAS IST KREATIVITÄT Kreativitätstheorien haben versucht die innere Komplexität anzuerkennen und Kreativität als Syndrom oder Komplex definiert. Sie  gehört zum größten Komplex menschlichen Verhaltens;  wird beeinflusst durch entwicklungsmäßige, soziale u. pädagogische Erfahrungen;  manifestiert sich in unterschiedlicher Art und Weise in einer Vielfalt von Bereichen;  ist von Spontaneität geprägt;  Originalität ist das am meisten beachtete Merkmal EXPERIMENTELLE FORSCHUNG IM RAHMEN DER NATURWISSENSCHAFTEN VS. KREATIVITÄTSFORSCHUNG.  Beeinflussung und Kontrolle sind die festgelegten Eigenschaften von experimenteller Forschung im Rahmen der Naturwissenschaften.
Article
Full-text available
This study tested the hypothesis that creative performance is facilitated by wide breadth of attention. Sixty-one participants completed a measure of trait breadth of attention and wrote a poem while alone in a quiet setting. On a random basis, 40 of these participants were then assigned to write a second poem while being subjected to attention-narrowing stimulation (noise) that was predictable or unpredictable and intelligible or unintelligible; the remaining 21 participants wrote the second poem in the quiet setting. All poems were judged for creativity and were measured for word originality. Results showed that (a) trait breadth of attention was correlated modestly and positively (r =. 20) with creative performance; (b) creative performance was impaired by exposure to noise, especially noise that was unpredictable or intelligible; and (c) noise impaired creative performance more in participants whose trait breadth of attention was wide than in those whose trait breadth of attention was narrow. The relation between creativity and breadth of attention is discussed in terms of the high complexity level of creativity tasks and the need for parallel processing and broad attentional capacity required for successful performance on highly complex tasks.
Article
Thomas Young is arguably one of the greatest geniuses who ever lived, but most people have never heard of him, though he was renowned in his own era. He did important work in a large variety of scientific disciplines, but that was his downfall. Given the specialization of the present era, physicists do not appreciate how important his work in linguistics was, linguists do not appreciate the importance of his work in psychology, and so on. Despite his obscurity today, Young nicely exemplifies the traits that one finds in a genius of the first order: tendency toward analogical thinking, high intelligence, an amazing capacity for hard work, extremely wide interests, distaste for traditional dogmas, and very high self-esteem.
Book
The goal of this handbook is to provide the most comprehensive, definitive, and authoritative single-volume review available in the field of creativity. The book contains twenty-two chapters covering a wide range of issues and topics in the field of creativity, all written by distinguished leaders in the field. The volume is divided into six parts. The introduction sets out the major themes and reviews the history of thinking about creativity. Subsequent parts deal with methods, origins, self and environment, special topics and conclusions. All educated readers with an interest in creative thinking will find this volume to be accessible and engrossing.
Article
Despite the conceptual overlap between intelligence and creativity, little systematic work exists on the link between creativity and speed of information processing. We hypothesized that differential focusing of attention determines the relationship between creative potential and speed of information processing. Supporting our predictions, we found negative correlations between creative potential and reaction time on tasks not involving interference (Hick Task, Concept Verification Task), but positive correlations between creative potential and reaction time on tasks requiring the inhibition of interfering information (Negative Priming, Global Precedence). This pattern of results suggests that the relation between creative potential and reaction time is a function of the differential focusing of attention.