ArticlePublisher preview available

Anagram solution as a function of bigram versatility

Authors:
  • Culturefactors
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Established bigram versatility (BV) as measured by the number of different words in which a bigram appeared. Anagram solution as a function of BV and bigram frequency was tested using 24 undergraduates as Ss. An inverse relationship between BV and anagram solution time was found. Results are discussed within the context of a reduced search area for anagram solving. (16 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
oj
Experimental
Psychology
1973,
Vol. 100,
No.
2,
259-262
ANAGRAM
SOLUTION
AS A
FUNCTION
OF
DIGRAM
VERSATILITY1
ROBERT
L.
SOLSO,2
GENE
K.
TOPPER,
AND
WILLIAM
H.
MACEY
Loyola University, Chicago
Bigram
versatility
(BV),
as
measured
by the
number
of
different
words
in
which
a
bigram
appeared,
was
established.
Anagram solution
as a
function
of BV and
bigrain
frequency
was
tested.
An
inverse
relationship
between
BV and
ana-
gram solution
time
was
observed.
The
results
were
discussed
within
the
con-
text
of a
reduced
search area
for
anagram solving.
The
solution
time
required
for un-
scrambling
anagrams
has
been
actively
studied.
Several
significant
variables
have
been
isolated
as
contributing
to
anagram
solution
time
(Kendler,
1961;
Kencller
&
Kendler,
1962).
Mayzner
and
Tresselt
(1958,
1959, 1962, 1963, 1966)
identified
anagram solution
to be a
function
of
)
word frequency
of the
solved word,
(b)
word
length,
(c)
number
of
letter
moves
from
anagram
to
solution word,
and
(d)
multiple
solutions
of
anagrams.
Mayzner
and
Tresselt
(1959, 1966) also reported
that
the
frequency
of a
bigram
in the
solution
word
may
affect solution time.
It is
plausible
that
anagrams containing
Q
and
U
or T and H,
which
are
frequently
combined
in a
bigram
in the
English lan-
guage, would provide
61
with
a
potent
cue
in
solving
an
anagram.
The
significance
of
bigram frequency
(BF)
and
anagram solution
has
also
been
studied
by
Dominowski
and
Duncan
(1964),
who
reported
data
seemingly
in-
consistent
with
that
of
Alayzner
and
Tres-
selt's
(1959, 1966) data.
The
difficulty
in
obtaining consistent
findings may be
due
to the
presence
of an
unisolated
variable.
Dominowski (1967) suggested that
Ss
used
bigrams
to
generate
words
as
pos-
sible
solutions
and the
higher
the
proba-
bility
of
bigram appearance
in the
solution
word
the
higher
the
likelihood
of
rapid
solution.
Dominowski's
logic
is
that
S
selects
one
letter
from
the
anagram
as
the
1
The
authors
express
their
appreciation
to
Carl
P.
Duncan
and
Roger Dominowski,
who
made
helpful
suggestions
on the
manuscript.
2
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
Robert
L.
Solso,
Department
of
Psychology, Loyola Uni-
versity
of
Chicago,
6525
Xorth
Sheridan Road,
Chicago,
Illinois
60026.
first
letter
of a
bigram
and
then matches
it
with
the
other
letters
in the
anagram
on
the
basis
of the
frequency
of the
bigram
(e.g.,
TH)
;
then presumably
the
search
for
the
solution
word
is
restricted
to
words
containing
TH. By
reducing
the
number
of
alternatives
that
S
must
test,
the
solu-
tion
is
expedited. However, Dominowski
noted
that
in
addition
to BF the
number
of
different
words
in
which
a
given
bigram
may
occur could also
affect
anagram solu-
tion
time.
We
propose
that
in
addition
to BF
bigrain
versatility (BV), defined
as the
number
of
different
words
in
which
a
given
bigram
may
occur,
is
also
an
important
variable.
If 5
generates bigrams
and
searches
for
words
that
may
contain
the
bigrams,
then
the
number
of
alternatives
that
he
must
consider,
as
determined
by
BV,
should
affect
his
solution time.
It is
noted
that
BV and BF may be
high cor-
relates
of
each other
but
that
they rep-
resent
significantly
different
dimensions.
To
illustrate
the
difference between
BV
and BF,
consider
the
bigram
OF.
This
has a
high
frequency
in the
English lan-
guage
but its
frequency
is
largely derived
from
the
word "of."
On the
other hand,
BV
tends
to be
relatively low.
The
effect
of BV in
anagram solving
may
be
related
to the
6"s
memory search
in
an
effort
to find a
match
between
bi-
grams (which
may
have
been formed
on
the
basis
of BF) and
words
which
contain
the
bigrams.
Thus,
the
higher frequency
bigrams with lower versatilities should
generate
fewer
potential
solution
words
(less
interference) than their
high-fre-
quency,
high-versatility counterparts.
The
present
study
was
designed
to
study
the
259
Article
This research investigated semantic priming of nonwords in a timed lexical-decision task. The nonword letter strings were anagrams of, and orthographically and phonetically similar to, words that were either related or unrelated to previously presented prime words. To compare the nature of priming effects in lexical and nonword decisions, two different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) were used, a short one (250 ms), intended to produce automatic activation of concepts related to the primes, and a long one (1500 ms), intended to produce attentional activation. Priming effects were greater for words at the short SOA than at the long one. Nonwords showed the reverse pattern, with greater priming effects at the long SOA than at the short one.
Article
In the first part of this paper bigram frequency counts are given for the first letter-pairs, last letter-pairs and “other” letter-pairs of words of more than three letters. A short discussion of the use and relevance of such tables is given. In the second part, lists of anagram-pairs of words are given for words of length three or more letters, together with approximate percentages of occurrence of such “anagrammatical” words in the English language.
Article
A sample of 45 student subjects provided solution scores for 80 five-letter anagrams. These scores were analysed as a function of solution word imagery, con-creteness, familiarity, objective frequency, age-of-acquisition and associative meaningfulness using multiple regression techniques. Two bigram measures together with number of vowels, nature of starting letter (vowel or consonant), anagram pronounceability and anagram-solution similarity scores were also entered into the regression equations. The bigram measures, the starting letter and anagram-solution similarity emerged as having significant associations with the solution scores. Previous reports of imagery effects in anagram are discussed in the light of the present results.
Article
This review draws upon available work in English published during the period 1970 through 1973. The reference list was derived from two computer searches.
Article
The effects on cognitive processing of 1 and 5 cumulative nights of partial sleep deprivation (PSD) and 1 and 2 nights of subsequent recovery oversleep was studied using an anagrams test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Seven paid undergraduate volunteers (5 male; 2 female) aged 18–23 who had normative 6.5–8.0 hr sleep patterns participated. They followed an 8-day schedule of morning and afternoon testing sessions consisting of 2 days in each of the experimental conditions: prebase-line, PSD, recovery sleep, and postbaseline. Sleep duration was calculated relative to individual habitual by subtracting 40% per night (PSD) and adding 40% and 20%, respectively (recovery sleep). On the anagrams test, results showed exaggerated time of day differences during both sleep manipulation conditions. Card-sorting performance was characterized by lesser efficiency of cognitive strategies (> non-perseverative errors) during recovery sleep and tendency towards perseveration with sleep loss. Results were discussed with particular reference to hypothesized neural substrates and clinical implications.
Article
Full-text available
While previous research has consistently demonstrated that anagrams with solution-words of high frequency are easier to solve than anagrams with solution-words of low frequency, the results for solution-word imagery have been contradictory. Experiment I involved the use of four six-item lists which varied factorially on imagery and frequency and were equated for meaningfulness and total bigram-position frequency. Eighteen Ss attempted to solve these anagrams under conditions of distributed practice and 18 Ss under massed practice. Data for solution latency and number of solutions revealed that anagrams were easier to solve if the solution-words were of high imagery or high frequency ( ps < .001). Distribution of practice and practice over 24 problems did not influence performance. Experiment II replicated the effects of solution-word imagery and frequency with a different sample of words and with more precise controls ( ps < .025). The results were discussed in terms of the processes involved in the storage and retrieval of words from semantic memory and the role that retrieval processes play in solving anagrams. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A comprehensive count of bigram and trigram frequencies and versatilities was tabulated for words recorded by Kučera and Francis. Totals of 577 different bigrams and 6,140 different trigrams were found. Their frequencies of occurrence and the number of different words in which they appeared are reported in this article.
Article
A comprehensive count of bigram frequencies and versatilities by position was tabulated for two- through nine-letter words recorded by Kucera and Francis (1967). A total of 577 bigrams were found variously distributed throughout words. Such counts should prove useful to workers who wish to determine the orthographic regularity of specific words.
Article
Full-text available
4 groups of 20 Ss each were presented with a set of 21 multiple-solution anagram problems and 2 additional groups of 20 Ss each received 21 single-solution problems, in which the anagram was itself a word. An S-R mediational model or problem-solving behavior was employed to make predictions concerning the effects of 4 variables, viz., anagram letter order, solution-word Thorndike-Lorge frequency, anagram digram frequency, and solution-word digram frequency, on anagram solution times. All predictions of the model were confirmed and the results showed that for multiple- as well as single-solution anagram problems, solution times are faster for easy letter orders, high solution-word Thorndike-Lorge frequencies, low anagram digram frequencies, and high solution-word digram frequencies.
Article
Full-text available
STUDIED THE EFFECTS OF LANGUAGE VARIABLES ON ANAGRAM SOLVING. IN ADDITION TO VARYING THE FREQUENCY OF SOLUTION WORDS, THE BIGRAM-RANK (BR) TOTALS OF ANAGRAMS AND OF SOLUTION WORDS WERE MANIPULATED. THE RANK OF A BIGRAM (-1) INDICATES THE NUMBER OF MORE FREQUENT BIGRAMS WITH THE SAME INITIAL LETTER; TOTALS ARE OBTAINED BY SUMMING THE RANKS OF SUCCESSIVE BIGRAMS IN A LETTER SEQUENCE. PERFORMANCE IMPROVED WITH INCREASING WORD FREQUENCY. ANAGRAMS WITH HIGH BR WERE EASIER TO SOLVE THAN THOSE WITH LOW BR. IN EXP. I, EACH S SOLVED FOR WORDS WITH SIMILAR BR, AND PERFORMANCE 1ST DETERIORATED, THEN IMPROVED AS WORD BR INCREASED. IN EXP. II, EACH S SOLVED FOR WORDS WITH WIDELY VARYING BR, AND PERFORMANCE DECREASED LINEARLY WITH INCREASING WORD BR. SEVERAL INTERACTIONS AND PRACTICE EFFECTS WERE ALSO OBTAINED. (19 REF.)
Article
Full-text available
A study was made of the effect of letter order and word frequency variables on anagram solution times. Hard letter orders and low word frequency counts both produced marked increases in anagram solution times.
Article
Full-text available
A study was made of the effect of word transition probability on anagram solution times. Word transition probability was varied by selecting words with either relatively high or low summed digram frequency totals, while holding constant anagram letter order, word frequency, and anagram transition probability variables. The results, predicted on the basis of an S-R mediational model, showed that anagrams whose word solutions have high transition probability totals are solved significantly faster than anagrams whose word solutions have low transition totals.
Article
Subjects were given, as stimuli, either letters or bigrams and asked to think of a word beginning with those stimuli. Words emitted were to be 3, 4, or 5 letters in length. Each of the six groups contained 100 Ss. Subjects were given 60 sec per stimulus to produce a word. Response measures were the number of words emitted, latency of response, and language frequency of emitted words. It was found that responses were fewer, latencies were longer, and word frequencies were lower for bigram than for letter stimuli, and for longer vs. shorter word lengths. It was pointed out that the effective pool of acceptable words is smaller for bigram than for letter stimuli and is probably smaller for longer than for shorter words. The findings with all response measures were interpreted leager terms of a greater difficulty in thinking of words as pool size is reduced.
Article
In three experiments on anagram solving, the variables were high vs. low total bigram frequency both of the anagrams and the solution words. In two of the experiments, spacing between letters of the anagrams was varied. Subjects were run in groups with all anagrams presented at once in two experiments and individually with one anagram at a time in the third experiment. The time allowed for solving was varied from experiment to experiment.With the exception that more anagrams were solved as more time was alowed, no main variable had any consistent effect across experiments. The consistent finding over all three experiments was a highly significant interaction between bigram frequency total of anagram and word. Anagrams with low totals were solved more easily for words with low than for words with high totals. Anagrams with high totals were solved more easily for words with high than with low totals. Certain interpretations of this interaction were discussed.
Article
Anagram solution may be considered to be a process in which an S permutes in an algorithmic fashion all letters in the anagram. However, Ss may find it possible to "rule out" certain permutations because attempting these permutations is highly unlikely to yield an English word. 20 adult Ss were presented with 20 5-letter anagrams in a 20 X 20 Latin square. Ss were randomly assigned to sequences. The anagrams were chosen from 2 levels of frequency on the basis of the Thorndike-Lorge (1944) count. At each frequency level 5 of the words were "high" ruleout, i.e., permitted elimination of 90-94 of the possible 120 permutations and 5 were "low" ruleout, permitting elimination of 70-76 of the permutations. Anagrams with "high" ruleout totals were solved significantly more rapidly (p < .01) at both frequency levels.
Article
This paper references some methodological problems with which a researcher in the field of reasoning must contend. Then a simple pretheoretical model of problem solving is described, followed by a report of research which the model generated, and which in turn is shaping the model itself. It is hoped that the present paper demonstrates that it is possible to investigate problem solving in a systematic fashion. If more psychologists accepted this possibility and were willing to expend their research energies in the field of problem solving, progress in this area would be greater than it is today. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Problems in problem solving research
  • H Pi Kendler
KENDLER, H. PI. Problems in problem solving research. In, Current trends in psychological theory: A bicentennial program. Pittsburgh, Pa.: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1961.