ArticlePDF Available

Enhancing Interest and Performance With a Utility Value Intervention

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

We tested whether a utility value intervention (via manipulated relevance) influenced interest and performance on a task and whether this intervention had different effects depending on an individual's performance expectations or prior performance. Interest was defined as triggered situational interest (i.e., affective and emotional task reactions) and maintained situational interest (i.e., inclination to engage in the task in the future). In 2 randomized experiments, 1 conducted in the laboratory and the other in a college classroom, utility value was manipulated through a writing task in which participants were asked to explain how the material they were learning (math or psychology) was relevant to their lives (or not). The intervention increased perceptions of utility value and interest, especially for students who were low in expected (laboratory) or actual (classroom) performance. Mediation analyses revealed that perceptions of utility value explained the effects of the intervention on interest and predicted performance. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Content may be subject to copyright.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 1
Enhancing Interest and Performance with a Utility Value Intervention
Chris S. Hulleman
James Madison University
Olga Godes, Bryan L. Hendricks, and Judith M. Harackiewicz
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Author Note
Chris S. Hulleman, Department of Graduate Psychology and Center for Assessment
Research Studies, James Madison University; Olga Godes, Department of Psychology,
University of Wisconsin-Madison; Bryan L. Hendricks, Department of Psychology, University
of Wisconsin-Madison; and Judith M. Harackiewicz, Department of Psychology, University of
Wisconsin-Madison.
Portions of this paper were presented at the American Educational Research Association
annual convention, April 2007.
This article is based on a doctoral dissertation submitted by Chris S. Hulleman to the
University of Wisconsin-Madison under the supervision of Judith M. Harackiewicz. Thanks are
extended to Martha Alibali, Geoffrey Borman, Patricia Devine, and Adam Gamoran for their
service on the dissertation committee. This research was supported in part by grants from the
Department of Psychology and the Institute for Education Sciences, U.S. Department of
Education, through Award #R305C050055 to the University of Wisconsin-Madison and Award
#R305B050029 to Vanderbilt University. We wish to thank Brian An for his assistance with data
analysis, and the talented research assistants in the Harackiewicz Research Lab for their
assistance with data collection.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Chris Hulleman, Center
for Assessment and Research Studies, MSC 6806, James Madison University, 821 S. Main St.,
Harrisonburg, VA, 22807. Email: hullemcs@jmu.edu.
Manuscript in press at the Journal of Educational Psychology, November 13, 2009.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 2
Abstract
We tested whether a utility value intervention (via manipulated relevance) influenced interest
and performance on a task, and whether this intervention had different effects depending on an
individual’s performance expectations or prior performance. Interest was defined as triggered
situational interest (i.e., affective and emotional task reactions) and maintained situational
interest (i.e., inclination to engage in the task in the future). In two randomized experiments, one
conducted in the laboratory and the other in a college classroom, utility value was manipulated
through a writing task in which participants were asked to explain how the material they were
learning (math or psychology) was relevant to their lives (or not). The intervention increased
perceptions of utility value and interest, especially for students who were low in expected
(laboratory) or actual (classroom) performance. Mediation analyses revealed that perceptions of
utility value explained the effects of the intervention on interest and predicted performance.
Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Keywords: expectancy-value, utility value, motivation, interest development, educational
intervention
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 3
Enhancing Interest and Performance with a Utility Value Intervention
Things indifferent or even repulsive in themselves often become of interest because of
assuming relationships and connections of which we were previously unaware. Many a
student…has found mathematical theory, once repellent, lit up by great attractiveness
after studying some form of engineering in which this theory was a necessary tool.
(Dewey, 1913, p. 22)
As recognized by Dewey, one way to enhance motivation and achievement may be to
help students find value and meaning in their schoolwork (Brophy, 1999; Hidi & Harackiewicz,
2000; National Research Council and the Institute for Medicine, 2004; Stipek, 2002; Wagner et
al., 2006; Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). Unfortunately, the research literature on student motivation
suggests that such motivation is in short supply in American schools. An alarming trend
indicates that interest in school tends to decrease over time (Anderman & Maehr, 1994; Lepper,
Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005), and that students with lower competence beliefs report lower interest
and motivation than students with higher competence beliefs (Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, &
Wigfield, 2002). Students’ competence perceptions arise, in part, from prior performance
experiences and are predictive of achievement (Bandura, 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2008). We
hypothesize that a value intervention may be particularly effective in enhancing motivation for
individuals with a history of poor performance or low performance expectations
Perceived Value
A useful theoretical framework for understanding the role of perceived value in
achievement contexts is the expectancy-value model (e.g., Atkinson, 1957; Eccles et al., 1983;
Edwards, 1954; Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & Sears, 1944; Tolman, 1955; Vroom, 1964), and the
Eccles expectancy-value model of achievement choices has been particularly influential in
education research (Eccles et al., 1983; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992).
Eccles et al. posit that perceived expectancies for success and task values contribute to
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 4
achievement choices and task performance. Expectancies for success are defined as individuals’
beliefs about how well they will perform on an upcoming task. There are important theoretical
and operational differences in expectancy constructs (e.g., self-efficacy, expectancies for
success, and perceptions of competence) across various theoretical formulations, but the general
principle is that students who believe that they can do well on a task are more likely to be
motivated and persist on the task (c.f. Bandura, 1997; Pintrich, 2003). Furthermore, recent
reviews of the expectancy literature reveal that prior performance experiences are the primary
foundation for expectancies of future success (Usher & Pajares, 2008), and thus students’ history
of performance should also predict motivation and performance.
Task values are more situation-specific types of values than other frameworks that define
values as broader constructs, such as benevolence, religiosity, and power (e.g., Feather, 1995;
Fries, Schmid, Dietz, & Hofer, 2005; Lewin, 1951; Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz, 1992). Eccles et
al. (1983) define task values as the perceived importance of the task because: it is useful or
relevant for other tasks or aspects of an individual’s life (utility value); it is enjoyable and fun to
engage in (intrinsic value); doing well on the activity influences the individual’s self-concept,
self-worth, and identity (attainment value); and because of the perceived negative aspects of
engaging in the activity, such as effort or negative emotional states (e.g., performance anxiety,
fear of failure). In contrast to earlier expectancy-value models of achievement motivation that
conceptualized expectancy and value as inversely related (Atkinson, 1957; Fischoff, Goitein, &
Shapira, 1982; Lewin et al., 1944; Vroom, 1964), the Eccles et al. model suggests that they are
independent constructs that are often positively and reciprocally related. Positive expectancies or
a sense of competence can enable individuals to perceive value in activities. In addition, finding
value and meaning in activities can increase task engagement, and the development of
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 5
competence and positive performance expectations (e.g., Eccles & Harold, 1991; Eccles &
Wigfield, 1995).
Research utilizing distinct measures of expectancy and task value supports the separation
of expectancy and value, finding that they are often moderately and positively correlated, as well
as the differentiation of value into task value factors (Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). However,
despite being conceptually distinct, task values have often been analyzed as a single factor, with
empirical studies most often combining intrinsic, utility, and attainment value (e.g., Anderman,
Eccles, Yoon, Roeser, Wigfield, & Blumenfeld, 2001; Bong, 2001; Jacobs et al., 2002; Wigfield
et al., 1997). A substantial body of research has found that expectancies are correlated with
performance (e.g., course grades, GPA), whereas perceived task values are correlated with
interest and achievement choices (Eccles & Harold, 1991; Updegraff, Eccles, Barber, & O’Brien,
1996; Wigfield, 1994; Xiang, Chen, & Bruene, 2005; see Wigfield & Eccles, 1992 for a review).
When examined separately, both utility and intrinsic task value have been associated with
measures of motivation such as course enrollment decisions (Durik, Vida, & Eccles, 2006;
Updegraff et al., 1996), leisure time activity choices (Durik et al., 2006), and interest in specific
school subjects (Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, Linnenbrink, & Tauer, 2008; Hulleman, Durik,
Schweigert, & Harackiewicz, 2008). In addition, there is evidence that perceived utility value is
associated with performance (Bong, 2001; Cole, Bergin, & Whitaker, 2008; Durik et al., 2006;
Hulleman et al., 2008; Mac Iver, Stipek, & Daniels, 1991; Simons, Dewitte, & Lens, 2004).
Thus, utility value may be of particular importance for both motivation and performance in
educational settings (Simons, Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Lacante, 2004). However, because the
majority of the research regarding utility value effects has been correlational, there is a need to
explore the causal effects of utility value (Maxwell & Cole, 2007).
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 6
The few studies that have manipulated task value demonstrate that value interventions
can promote situational interest in laboratory activities (Durik & Harackiewicz, 2007; Godes,
Hulleman, & Harackiewicz, 2007; Simons, Vansteenkiste et al., 2004) and high school science
classrooms (Hulleman & Harackiewicz, in press), although these effects vary according to
important individual differences. Godes et al. (2007, Study 1) found that emphasizing the utility
value of a math activity (e.g., A nurse may use mental math computations to calculate
medication amounts) actually undermined subsequent interest for individuals with low
perceptions of competence in math. In contrast, the utility manipulation promoted interest in the
math activity for individuals with high perceptions of competence. Durik and Harackiewicz
(2007) used a similar intervention and found the same effect on situational interest for a math
activity for students with low and high initial interest in math, respectively. Thus, simply
informing students of the applications of an activity may not always promote interest. It is
possible that the method used to promote perceived utility value undermined the manipulation’s
effectiveness for some students. Godes et al. (2007) and Durik and Harackiewicz (2007)
emphasized the utility value of a math activity by informing students about the relevance of the
material for their lives. For a student who does not do well in math or find math interesting,
being told that math is important to his future may be threatening and intensify negative
reactions. Rather than increase his engagement in the material, he may withdraw further from the
learning environment.
We believe that a more effective approach would be to encourage students to generate
their own connections and discover the relevance of course material to their lives for themselves.
This method provides students the opportunity to make connections to topics and areas of their
lives of greatest interest. Allowing students to discover the connections between an activity and
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 7
their lives on their own may be a less threatening way to promote the perception of utility value,
and may therefore be particularly beneficial for students with low performance expectations.
Such utility value interventions are intended to enable change in interest over time. Thus,
like Hidi and Renninger (2006), we consider interest to be both a psychological state of activity
engagement and a predisposition to engage with a topic or activity over time. Similar to other
models of interest, the four-phase model of interest (Hidi & Renninger, 2006) distinguishes
between cognitive (i.e., meaning) and affective (feeling) components of interest (Alexander,
Jetton, & Kulikowich, 1995; Harp & Mayer, 1997; Kintsch, 1980; Krapp, 2002; Schiefele,
1996), and short-term or situation-specific interest (i.e., situational interest) and long-term or
enduring personal interest (i.e., individual interest; Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Schiefle, Krapp, &
Winteler, 1992; Silvia, 2001, 2006). Interest is important as both an educational process that
contributes to effective learning (e.g., attention, complexity of knowledge, levels of learning; for
reviews see Alexander & Murphy, 1998; Hidi, 1990; Schiefele, 1991) and outcomes (e.g.,
achievement, educational choices; e.g., Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, & Elliot, 2000;
Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, & Elliot, 2002; Harackiewicz et al., 2008; Schiefele et al., 1992).
Interest Development
In the Hidi and Renninger (2006) model, interest development is influenced by the
experience of positive affect in relation to an activity, and perceiving value and developing
knowledge in a domain. Further, interest is theorized to develop in four phases. As momentary
interest in a specific situation is activated by some external cue (triggered situational interest:
Phase 1), an individual may perceive value in the activity, and the desire to continue pursuing the
activity may then deepen over time (maintained situational interest: Phase 2). If situational
interest is maintained and an individual continues to engage in the activity and perceive value in
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 8
it, then individual interest may begin to develop (emerging individual interest: Phase 3).
Continuing re-engagement with the task over a period of time, along with increased knowledge
and positive affect, can create an enduring interest in the activity (developed individual interest:
Phase 4). Thus, the interest that develops or deepens in a particular context depends on the extent
to which value, positive affect, and knowledge are experienced in relation to the activity (Hidi &
Renninger, 2006; Mitchell, 1993).
In addition, interest is reciprocally related with other motivational variables such as self-
efficacy and self-regulation. Lipstein and Renninger (2007) hypothesize that interest is a
mediator for the development of self-efficacy and self-regulation skills, as interest maintains
attention and effort required to develop knowledge and continue learning over time. This
reasoning is congruent with some of self-regulation (e.g., Sansone & Thoman, 2005;
Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005) that demonstrate that higher levels of self-regulation are
associated with higher levels of interest (Cleary & Zimmerman, 2001; Kitsantas & Zimmerman,
2002; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1988). Self-regulation skills that help students learn and
stay motivated enable them to know when to connect material to their lives, thereby helping
them be more engaged and interested (Sansone & Thoman, 2005; Zimmerman & Kitsantas,
2005). The development of self-efficacy beliefs is also hypothesized to interact reciprocally with
interest, with interesting activities leading to the development of competence beliefs and
competence beliefs leading children to explore and develop interest in activities (Pajares, 1996;
Renninger et al., 2008; Schunk & Pajares, 2005). Because self-regulation skills co-vary with
self-efficacy and interest, students with lower levels of self-efficacy and competence do not tend
to have these self-regulatory skills (e.g., they do not ask curiosity questions; Renninger, 2000).
These students may have a more difficult time maintaining interest, and thus require external
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 9
support to maintain task engagement (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000; Hidi & Renninger, 2006). In
contrast, students with higher levels of self-efficacy and competence are less in need of
situational supports for interest because their interest is already at a higher level. In fact, these
students may require different supports, such as providing more challenging material or setting
proximal learning goals (Lipstein & Renninger, 2007; Renninger, Bachrach, & Posey, 2008).
Thus, it is plausible that students who are disengaged from school due to a history of poor
performance or low expectations may benefit the most from a utility value intervention
(Hulleman & Harackiewicz, in press). The extrinsic nature of utility value makes it particularly
amenable to situational interventions from teachers or parents, who may be able to help students
discover and appreciate the connections between a task and their lives. In the classroom, one
way to highlight utility value could be to ask students to describe the relevance of course
material to their own lives. For example, a student interested in basketball might apply math to
calculating free throw percentages for her favorite players, whereas a student interested in
nursing might apply his knowledge to calculating the correct dosage of medicine to give patients.
The resultant perception of utility value could then promote active engagement in learning,
which may in turn generate excitement, effort, interest, and achievement (Brophy, 1999;
Cordova & Lepper, 1996; Wagner et al., 2006).
As presented in Figure 1, we hypothesize that a situational intervention that encourages
individuals to make a connection between a task and their lives (i.e., a relevance intervention)
will increase perceptions of utility value for the task. In turn, these perceptions of utility value
should lead to increases in situational interest and possibly performance. These direct effects will
be moderated by perceived or actual competence in the activity (the dashed path in Figure 1).
Specifically, we expect that the relevance intervention to be particularly effective in promoting
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 10
perceived utility value for students with a history of poor performance or low expectations. In
turn, perceptions of utility value will promote both subsequent interest and performance. Because
the utility value intervention is most likely to trigger situational interest, this type of interest is
best captured in terms of affective and emotional responses to the material (Hidi, 1990;
Hulleman et al., 2008). If maintained over time, this externally triggered situational interest
could become the beginning of emerging individual interest. Because students with low levels of
competence will likely need additional supports to enhance interest, such as proximal goal
setting and freedom to pursue curiosity questions (Lipstein & Rennigner, 2007; Renninger,
2000), the utility valued intervention is less likely to enhance their interest.
Current Research
In our research, we explore how features of the situation can trigger situational interest
and promote the transition from situational to individual interest. Although educators have little
influence over the individual interests that students bring to their classrooms, they can play a
pivotal role in designing, implementing, and maintaining the quality of the classroom
environment, and they may be able to influence students’ perceptions of value. The perception of
value is hypothesized to be a key contributor in the progression from situational (Phase 2) to
individual (Phase 3) interest, and to the deepening of existing individual interest (Hidi &
Renninger, 2006). Situations that highlight task value could serve to stimulate engagement for
students who are less engaged with an activity, and facilitate the development and maintenance
of situational interest. Although the relationship between perceived utility value and interest has
been established in previous correlational research, we investigated whether we could increase
perceived utility value with an external intervention, and if so, whether these changes in
perceived utility value fostered interest and performance.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 11
To this end, we tested whether perceptions of utility value can be influenced with an
experimental intervention, and whether changes in utility perceptions trigger situational interest
(i.e., emotional and affective task reactions), maintain this interest over time (i.e., intention to
return to the activity), and improve performance. Because we focus on the role of value in the
development of interest, our measures of situational interest focus on the emotional aspects of
interest and thus are not confounded with our task value measures. In addition, although
knowledge is an important aspect of the four-phase model, we do not measure it within the
research presented herein. In two randomized experiments, one conducted in the laboratory and
the other in a college classroom, personal relevance was manipulated through a writing exercise
in which participants were asked to explain how the activity was relevant to their lives. We
tested whether expected (Study 1) or actual (Study 2) performance moderated the direct effects
of our intervention on the outcomes. In addition, we tested our theoretical model by examining
whether perceptions of utility value mediated the effects of the intervention on interest and
performance.
Study 2 was designed to extend the results of Study 1 from the laboratory into the
classroom. However, there are some important differences between the two studies that need to
be highlighted, mainly due to differences in context. A math activity was used in Study 1 to
provide an ecologically valid learning activity, albeit one that required a low level of processing
skills (e.g., 2-digit multiplication). In Study 2, we tested our intervention in the context of an
introductory psychology course, due to our belief that the relevance intervention is not domain
specific. Because of its design, the introductory course matched the math task as both required
relatively lower levels of processing skills and knowledge complexity compared to more
complex tasks. In addition, because students entered the laboratory activity with little or no
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 12
familiarity with the specific math task they were about to learn, we used their expectations for
performance as the moderator of the relevance intervention. In the classroom, students had half a
semester of experience in the psychology class to develop an understanding of their performance
level in the course. As a result, we used actual performance (in terms of early exam scores) as the
moderator of the relevance intervention.
Study 1 – The Laboratory
In this study, undergraduates were taught a new mental mathematics technique for
solving two-digit multiplication problems. Participants were randomly assigned to the relevance
or control conditions. Participants in the relevance conditions were asked to write a short essay
describing how the math activity could relate to their life or to the lives of college students in
general, whereas participants in the control condition were asked to complete a writing task
unrelated to the math activity. We measured participants’ perceived utility value, situational
interest in the task, maintained situational interest (i.e., inclination to re-engage in the task at a
later time), and performance on the task.
We hypothesized that participants in the relevance condition would be more interested in
the math activity at the end of the session than those in the control condition. We also expected
that these effects would be moderated by participants’ performance expectations, such that
students with low performance expectations would benefit more from the intervention than those
with high performance expectations. In addition, we hypothesized that both of these effects
would be mediated by participants’ utility perceptions, and that utility value perceptions might
also be associated with performance.
Method
Participants
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 13
One hundred and seven undergraduate students (50 males, 57 females) from the
introductory psychology class at the University of Wisconsin, Madison participated in the study.
Participants were 92% Caucasian, 4% Hispanic, 3% Asian, and 1% African American
Participants were run individually and received extra credit upon completion of the 60 minute
session.
Measures
Participants’ initial interest in math was measured using a 4-item scale (e.g., “I find math
enjoyable”). Participants’ performance expectations for answering the multiple problems during
the experimental session were measured with a 3-item scale (e.g., “I think I’ll do well on the
following sets of problems”). Participants’ perceptions of the technique’s utility value were
measured using a 3-item scale (e.g., “This technique could be useful in everyday life,” Perceived
Utility Value). Participants’ situational interest in the technique was measured using a 5-item
scale (e.g., “The left-to-right technique is interesting”). Participants responded to all self-report
scale items in this study on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly
agree). Participant’s maintained situational interest in the technique was assessed by asking, “Do
you think you will use the technique you learned today on your own in the future?” Participant
responses were coded as ‘0’ for “no” and ‘1’ for “yes”. Appendix A lists the individual items and
reliability coefficients for each self-report scale in Study 1. The total number of problems solved
correctly on the official problem set was used as a measure of participants’ performance.
Procedure
Participants were run through the experimental session individually. After completing a
consent form and a measure of initial interest in math, an audio recording guided the participants
through a colorful instructional notebook that taught them a four-step method for solving two-
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 14
digit multiplication problems in their head (adopted from Flansburg & Hay, 1994; see Barron &
Harackiewicz, 2001, for a more detailed description of the experimental procedures). The
instructions also indicated the basic outline of the session, which went as follows. After the
learning period, participants were given three minutes to practice the technique on a problem set.
Following this practice period they reported their performance expectations for the experimental
session. Next, the experimenter handed the participant a folded sheet of paper (to ensure that the
experimenter was blind to condition) that contained instructions for writing either a relevance or
control essay. Based upon pilot testing, all participants were given 10 minutes to type the essay
on a laptop computer. All participants indicated a familiarity with the computer and word
processing program used to record their essays. Participants in the relevance condition were
asked to:
Type a short essay (1 – 3 paragraphs in length) briefly describing the potential
relevance of this technique to your own life, or to the lives of college students in
general. Of course, you’ll probably need more practice with the technique to really
appreciate its personal relevance, but for purposes of this writing exercise, please
focus on how this technique could be useful to you or to other college students, and
give examples.
Participants in the control condition were asked to write about two pictures that were hanging on
the wall of the experimental room. The pictures were of math-related (e.g., a man examining
charts and figures) and art-related scenes (e.g., covers from the New Yorker magazine) that
contained enough objects and detail to describe in a 10 minute essay. Participants in the control
writing were asked to:
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 15
Type a short essay (2 paragraphs) describing the objects that you see in both pictures;
simply describe in detail the objects that you see. First, in one paragraph, simply describe
in detail the objects that you see in the picture on the left. Second, in one paragraph,
simply describe in detail the objects that you see in the picture on the right.
After writing the essay, the experimenter informed the participants that they would have
six minutes to work on the official problem set while using the new technique. After participants
were told how many problems they solved correctly on the official problem set, they then
completed measures of utility value and situational interest. Finally, we assessed their inclination
to use the technique in the future (maintained situational interest).
Results
Manipulation Check
In order to assess the effectiveness of the intervention on the content of participants’
essays, each of the essays was coded by two research assistants who were blind to the
experimental conditions and hypotheses of the study. The essays were coded for the presence
(yes/no) of utility value for the math technique, and the number of examples of utility value in
the essay. The coders read each of the 107 essays independently and assigned a rating.
Differences were resolved through discussion (84% initial agreement). The two scales (number
of types, number of examples) were each standardized and averaged to create a composite index
of the degree of Observed Utility Value that participants mentioned in their essays (M = 0.00, SD
= 0.98; α = .98).
In order to test whether or not the relevance condition caused participants to write about
more personal relevance in their essays than those in the control condition, we conducted an
independent samples t-test using the coders’ ratings of Observed Utility Value as the dependent
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 16
variable. The t-test indicated that participants in the relevance condition mentioned significantly
more utility value in their essays (M = 1.74, SD = 0.61) than those in the control condition (M =
0.00, SD = 0.00), t(105) = 19.91, p < .01. We also examined the number of sentences that
participants wrote in their essays. The t-test indicated that participants in the relevance condition
(M = 9.52, SD = 2.87) wrote fewer sentences than those in the control condition (M = 12.87, SD
= 3.50), t(105) = -5.43, p < .01. Thus, we could be assured that any effects of our intervention
were due to participants mentioning more utility value in their essays and not because they wrote
more in that condition.
Analytic Approach
The data were analyzed using hierarchical multiple regression in two steps. First, we
examined the direct effects of the relevance intervention and performance expectations on the
outcomes of situational interest, maintained situational interest, and performance. Second, we
tested whether perceptions of utility value mediated the direct effects of the relevance
intervention on the outcomes.
Prior to conducting analyses we standardized all continuous variables. Interaction terms
were created by multiplying the variables together. Preliminary analyses revealed that the main
and interactive effects of gender were not significant predictors of any outcome, and were thus
trimmed from the regression models. The basic regression model consisted of four terms: initial
interest in math, performance expectations, the relevance intervention contrast (-1 = control, +1
= relevance), and the two-way interaction between performance expectations and the relevance
intervention. The interaction was included to test whether the intervention functioned differently
for individuals with low and high levels of performance expectations. Significant interactions
were examined by computing predicted values based on estimates for one standard deviation
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 17
below and above the mean on performance expectations (Aiken & West, 1991). Descriptive
statistics and zero-order correlations for all Study 1 measures are presented in Table 1, and the
results of the regression analyses are presented in Table 2.
Direct Effects on Situational Interest, Maintained Situational Interest, and Performance
The basic model accounted for a significant portion of the variance in situational interest,
F(4, 102) = 8.12, p < .001, R2 = .21. There was a significant main effect of the relevance
contrast, t(102) = 2.69, p = .01, (β = .24), indicating that participants in the relevance condition
(Ŷ = 5.09) became more interested in the technique than participants in the control condition (Ŷ
= 4.61). This main effect were qualified by the significant interaction between performance
expectations and the relevance contrast, t(102) = -2.30, p = .02, (β = -.22). As shown in the
upper-left panel of Figure 2, participants low in performance expectations found the technique to
be more interesting in the relevance condition (Ŷ = 4.93) than in the control condition (Ŷ =
4.17). In contrast, individuals with high performance expectations found the new technique to be
equally interesting in both the relevance (Ŷ = 5.26) and control conditions (Ŷ = 5.06). There was
also a significant main effect of initial interest, t(102) = 2.30, p = .05, (β = .21), and performance
expectations, t(102) = 3.77, p < .01, (β = .48), indicating that participants with higher initial
interest and/or performance expectations became more interested in the technique than those
with lower initial interest or performance expectations.
Logistic regression was used to analyze the results from the dichotomous (0 = no, 1 =
yes) measure of maintained situational interest. Regressing inclination on the basic model
revealed that the four variables in the model accounted for approximately 33% of the variation in
interest (Nagelkerke R2 = .33)1. There was a significant main effect of the relevance contrast,
Wald χ2(1, N = 107) = 7.23, p < .01, odds ratio (OR) = 8.29, indicating that participants in the
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 18
relevance condition were more inclined to use the technique in the future than those in the
control condition. There was also a significant main effect of performance expectations, Wald
χ2(1, N = 107) = 6.48, p = .01, odds ratio (OR) = 2.24, indicating that participants with higher
performance expectations were more inclined to use the technique in the future than those with
lower performance expectations. As shown in the middle-left panel of Figure 2, these main
effects were qualified by a significant interaction between performance expectations and the
relevance contrast, Wald χ2(1, N = 107) = 10.10, p < .01, odds ratio (OR) = 0.23, indicating that
participants with lower performance expectations were more inclined to use the technique in the
relevance condition than in the control condition. Participants with higher performance
expectations reported similar levels of inclination in the two conditions.
The basic model accounted for a significant portion of the variance in performance, F(4,
102) = 6.31, p = .01, R2 = .20. There were significant main effects of initial interest, t(102) =
1.97, p = .05, (β = .18), and performance expectations, t(102) = 3.77, p < .01, (β = .37),
indicating that participants with higher initial interest and/or performance expectations scored
higher on the problem sets than those with lower initial interest or performance expectations. No
other effects were significant.
Mediation Analyses
We examined whether perceived utility value mediated the effect of the relevance
intervention on situational interest and maintained situational interest, and whether perceived
utility value was related to final performance. We first regressed perceived utility value on the
basic model. We then added perceived utility value to the basic model, resulting in a 5-term
mediation model. We followed procedures outlined by Kenny, Kashy, & Bolger (1998) to test
the mediated effects. In this case, the direct effect of the relevance intervention, and the
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 19
interaction between the intervention and performance expectations, were multiplied by the
effects of perceived utility value on the focal outcome (i.e., the alpha-beta term, or the indirect
effect). The new product term was divided by its standard error to produce a significance test for
mediation. This technique has been shown to be robust to Type I errors (MacKinnon, Lockwood,
Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002).
The basic model accounted for a significant portion of the variance in perceived utility
value, F(4, 102) = 3.69, p < .01, R2 = .14. As predicted, the main effect of the relevance
intervention was significant, t(102) = 2.03, p = .05, (β = .19), indicating that participants in the
relevance condition (Ŷ = 5.33) found the technique to be more useful at the end of the session
than participants in the control condition (Ŷ = 4.92). This effect was qualified by a significant
interaction between the intervention and performance expectations, t(102) = -3.06, p < .01, (β = -
.29). As shown in the bottom-left panel of Figure 2, participants with lower performance
expectations found the technique more useful in the relevance condition (Ŷ = 5.21) than in the
control condition (Ŷ = 4.43). In contrast, participants with higher performance expectations
found the technique equally useful in the relevance (Ŷ = 5.32) and control conditions (Ŷ = 5.32).
The mediation model on situational interest accounted for significantly more variance
than the basic model (R2change = .38, p < .001). Perceived utility value was a significant
predictor of situational interest, t(101) = 9.57, p < .01, (β = .65), indicating that participants who
perceived more utility value in the math technique found it more interesting than those who
perceived less utility value in the technique. Importantly, the main effect of the relevance
intervention, the main effect of performance expectations, and their interaction were no longer
significant. The formal test of mediation revealed that perceived utility value mediated the
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 20
effects on situational interest for the relevance intervention, z = 1.86 (p = .06), performance
expectations, z = 3.19 (p < .01), and their interaction, z = 2.93 (p < .01).
The mediation logistic regression model on maintained situational interest accounted for
more variance than the basic model (Nagelkerke R2-change = .41). There was a significant main
effect of perceived utility value, Wald χ2(1, N = 107) = 9.95, p < . 01, odds ratio (OR) = 80.38,
indicating that participants who perceived more utility value in the math technique were more
inclined to use the technique in the future. The direct effect of the relevance intervention was
reduced in size but still significant, Wald χ2(1, N = 107) = 5.37, p = .02, odds ratio (OR) = 15.53,
but its interaction with performance expectations was not, Wald χ2(1, N = 107) = 3.35, p = .07,
odds ratio (OR) = 0.26. The formal test of mediation revealed that perceived utility value
mediated the effects on maintained situational interest for both the relevance intervention, z =
1.84 (p = .06), and its interaction with performance expectations, z = 2.86 (p < .01). The top
panel of Figure 3 presents the path model results for Study 1.
Regressing performance on the meditational model did not account for significantly more
variance than the basic model (R2change = .01, p = .22), and the effect of perceived utility value
was not significant, t(101) = 1.23, p = .22, (β = .12).
Discussion – Study 1
The results of Study 1 demonstrated that an experimental intervention designed to
influence perceptions of the utility of a math task was successful in doing so. The relevance
intervention triggered situational interest in the math technique, as indicated by their reports of
interest at the end of the experimental session. In addition, the intervention maintained
participants’ interest beyond the experimental session, as indicated by their inclination to use the
math technique in the future. These direct effects were particularly strong for participants with
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 21
lower performance expectations. In addition, the direct effects of the relevance intervention on
interest were mediated by the degree of utility value participants perceived in the math
technique. These findings are unique in several ways.
First, the findings reveal direct evidence of the causal role of utility value in the interest
development process outlined by Hidi and Renninger (2006). This extends prior correlational
research by demonstrating, through a randomized experiment, that utility value can play a causal
role in triggering and maintaining interest in a topic. Second, the results support also our
theoretical model, in particular that the relevance intervention was more effective for students
with low performance expectations. However, because the study was conducted within an
experimental laboratory, it remains to be seen whether they generalize to actual classroom
environments. In addition, the relevance intervention did not impact performance, neither
directly nor indirectly through utility perceptions. It is possible that the effects of the intervention
require more time to impact actual achievement, and thus a longitudinal field experiment may
reveal performance effects.
Study 2 – The College Psychology Classroom
Study 2 was a randomized experiment that extended the laboratory experiment (Study 1)
into an undergraduate psychology classroom. Students were randomly assigned to one of four
conditions at mid-semester: two were relevance conditions and two were control conditions. The
primary questions addressed in Study 2 were: 1) Can a relevance intervention increase
perceptions of utility value within the context of a college class? 2) Can the same intervention
affect students’ subsequent situational interest? 3) Does the relevance intervention trigger and
maintain situational interest in psychology by increasing perceptions of utility value, thus
supporting our hypothesized model? and 4) Does the relevance intervention impact performance
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 22
directly, or indirectly through utility perceptions? We hypothesized that students in the relevance
condition would be more interested in the course at the end of the semester, and more inclined to
major in psychology, than those in the control condition. We again expected that these effects
would be more pronounced for students with lower performance expectations. However, in
Study 2 we examined students’ early performance in the class instead of their expected
performance. Thus, we predicted that the effects of the relevance intervention would be greater
for students who were not performing well in the course at mid-semester. In addition, we
hypothesized that the effects of the intervention would be mediated through students’ perceptions
of utility value.
Method
Overview
This study took place during the course of a 15-week semester at a large, Midwestern
university and consisted of three waves of data collection during the semester. Students’ initial
interest in the course topic and inclination to major in psychology were assessed on the second
day of the semester (Time 1); their initial perceptions of utility value for the course were
measured two weeks into the semester and before the first exam (Time 2); and final measures of
utility value, interest in the course, and inclination to major in psychology were collected during
the 13th week of the semester (Time 3). We also obtained students’ final course grades from
department records. The relevance intervention occurred during the second half of the semester
(weeks 9-12).
Participants and Setting
Participants were recruited from an Introductory Psychology class that consisted of
approximately 350 students. Only students who were taking the course for graded credit and who
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 23
agreed to complete our surveys were included in the sample (N = 318; 91% of the students in the
course). Classes were primarily in lecture format and students’ grades were determined by their
performance on several multiple-choice exams given throughout the semester. Final grades were
assigned based upon a normative curve. Students completed the surveys during class time and
received extra credit for completing all three surveys.
Relevance Intervention
At mid-semester (after the second exam), students were randomly assigned to one of two
sets of writing conditions – one set was intended to help students see the relevance of the course
material to their lives (relevance), and the other set was intended to serve as a control (control).
The writing exercises were part of the course syllabus and were completed for 5 points of course
credit. Each student was asked to complete their assigned essay twice during the second-half of
the semester – once in the 10th week and again in the 12th week. All students (N = 318)
completed at least one of the essays, and 92% (N = 295) completed both of the required essays.
The instructor, who was blind to students’ condition, graded each essay on a scale from 0 to 5
(essay 1: M = 4.16, SD = 0.86; essay 2: M = 4.24, SD = 4.24).In each condition, students were
asked to select a topic that was currently being covered in class (e.g., the effect of sleep loss on
cognitive functioning) and write a 1-2 page essay. From this point, the instructions for each
condition varied as follows.
In the relevance conditions, students were randomly assigned to either write a letter to a
significant person in their lives (e.g., friend, relative, partner) describing the relevance of their
topic to this person (letter, N = 78), or to find a media report (e.g., magazine, newspaper,
internet, etc.) that pertained to their topic and write an essay that discussed the relevance of the
media report to information they were learning in class (media, N = 82). The letter and media
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 24
assignments asked students to connect the course material to their lives through their social
connections or the popular media. Our preliminary testing of these conditions showed no
differences on our outcomes, and thus they were combined into one relevance condition for the
analyses reported herein.
In the control conditions, students were randomly assigned to either write an outlined
summary of the topic they selected (outline, N = 78), or to search the PsycINFO database for two
abstracts relating to the topic they selected and discuss how the abstracts expanded upon the
material they were learning in class (PsycINFO, N = 80).The purpose of the outline condition
was to control for increases in knowledge that could occur by summarizing the material in
written form. Prior research has demonstrated that knowledge development can occur through
such knowledge consolidation exercises (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 2003), and the outline
condition would control for this effect. The PsycINFO condition was used to control for the
triggering of interest that can occur from the opportunity to explore a particular topic in-depth
(Flum & Kaplan, 2006; Hidi & Renninger, 2006). Compared to the relevance conditions, which
asked students to make connections between the course material and their lives, the PsycINFO
condition asked students to make connections between the course material and the psychological
research from which it is derived. In combination, the outline and PsycINFO conditions control
for knowledge consolidation (and increased learning) and in-depth topic exploration (and
increased interest). Our preliminary testing of these conditions showed no differences in the
control conditions on our outcomes, and thus they were combined into one control condition for
the analyses reported herein.
Measures
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 25
Students’ initial interest in the course topic was assessed with five items (e.g., “I think I
will like learning about psychology in this course”) and were based on prior research
(Harackiewicz et al., 2002; Harackiewicz et al., 2008; Hulleman et al., 2008). Students’
situational interest in the course was assessed with a five-item scale (e.g., “I think the field of
psychology is very interesting”). Students’ maintained situational interest in psychology (initial
and final) was measured with a single item (e.g., “I am interested in majoring in psychology”).
Students’ perceptions of utility value (initial and final) were assessed with a 3-item scale (e.g., “I
think what we are studying in Introductory Psychology is useful for me to know”). Participants
responded to all self-report items in this study on 7-point Likert-type scales from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Appendix B lists the individual items and reliability coefficients
for each self-report scale in Study 2.
Student course grades were based on four multiple-choice exams given throughout the
semester, with each exam given equal weight. Students’ early performance in the course was
calculated by summing their scores on their first two exams (mid-term exams). Students’ final
course grades were obtained from departmental records. Each student could receive one of seven
possible grades, based on the university’s 4-point scale. The average grade for students in this
study was 2.75 (SD = .90). Grades were distributed as follows: A = 17.3%, AB = 13.1%, B =
18.5%, BC = 17.3%, C = 25.3%, D = 5.7%, and F = 3.0%.
Results
Attrition and Missing Data
The initial sample of 318 students included individuals who did not complete all three
waves of the survey: 292 students completed Time 1, 272 completed Time 2, and 272 completed
Time 3. The attrition was primarily due to students missing class on the day the surveys were
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 26
administered: Eight students did not complete any of the three waves of data collection, 20
missed two waves of data collection, and 81 students (26%) missed at least one wave of data
collection. In sum, of the original sample of 318 students, 237 students (74%) had complete data
on all 3 waves.
Manipulation Check
As in Study 1, each of the student essays was coded by two research assistants who were
blind to the experimental conditions and hypotheses of the study. The essays were coded on two
categories. The degree of utility value that students wrote about was coded on a scale from 0 to
3, with more points indicating an increased number of applications and/or a better description of
how the material was useful or applicable to life (observed utility value). To rate the extent to
which participants connected the material to their lives in particular, the coders counted the
number of personal pronouns used in the essay (e.g., I, me, mine, us, our, ours; personalization).
The coders read each of the 624 essays independently and assigned a rating. Cronbach’s alphas
were acceptable for ratings of utility value (0.72 for essay 1 and 0.82 for essay 2) and number of
pronouns (0.99 for essay 1 and 0.95 for essay 2). Differences were resolved through discussion.
The ratings for essay 1 and essay 2 were averaged to create an overall index of observed utility
value (M = 0.64, SD = 0.76) and number of personal pronouns (M = 9.91, SD = 13.23). Because
these two measures were highly correlated (r = .76, p < .001), the values were standardized and
averaged to form a composite rating of observed relevance (M = 0.01, SD = 0.95; α = .86).
In order to test whether or not the relevance conditions caused participants to find more
personal relevance in the material than those in the control conditions, we conducted t-tests using
the coders’ ratings of observed relevance as the dependent variable. The results indicated that
students in the relevance conditions mentioned more utility value and used more personal
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 27
pronouns in their essays (M = 0.50, SD = 1.10) than those in the control conditions (M = -0.44,
SD = 0.44), t(235) = 8.79, p < .01 (d = 1.25). We also tested whether students’ essay grades
varied by condition. The results indicated that there were no differences in essay grades between
students in the relevance conditions (M = 8.16, SD = 1.54) and those in the control conditions (M
= 8.41, SD = 1.54), t(235) = -1.25, p = .22 (d = .16).
Analytic Approach
The same analytic approach was used in Study 2 as in Study 1 except as noted below.
Actual competence (mid-term exams) was entered in the regression models instead of
performance expectations. In addition to initial interest, the regression models also controlled for
initial inclination and initial utility value perceptions. Thus, the basic model contained six terms:
initial interest, initial inclination, initial utility value, mid-term exams, the relevance contrast, and
the interaction between the relevance contrast and mid-term exams. Descriptive statistics and
zero-order correlations for all Study 2 measures are presented in Table 3, and the results of the
regression analyses are presented in Table 4.
Direct Effects on Situational Interest, Maintained Situational Interest, and Course Grades
The basic model for situational interest was significant, F(10, 236) = 32.13, p < .01 (R2 =
.46). There was a significant main effect of the relevance contrast, t(236) = 3.24, p < .01, (β =
.16), indicating that participants in the relevance conditions reported more interest in psychology
at the end of the course than participants in the control conditions. This direct effect was
moderated by a significant interaction with mid-term exams, t(236) = -3.54, p < .01, (β = -.18).
This interaction is presented in the upper-right panel of Figure 2. The predicted values indicated
that students with lower exam scores in the relevance conditions reported more interest in the
course (Ŷ = 4.91) than those in the control conditions (Ŷ = 4.03, β = .34, p < .01). Students with
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 28
higher exam scores reported equivalent levels of interest in the course in the relevance (Ŷ = 4.83)
and control conditions (Ŷ = 4.88, β = -.02, p = .62). In addition, there were direct effects of mid-
term exams, t(236) = 2.90, p < .01, (β = .15) initial interest, t(236) = 6.35, p < .01, (β = .44), and
inclination, t(236) = 2.19, p = .03, (β = .13), indicating that students with higher levels of early
performance, interest in psychology, and inclination to major in psychology at the beginning of
the semester were more interested in the course at the end of the semester than those students
with lower levels.
The basic model accounted for a significant amount of variance in inclination to major in
psychology F(10, 236) = 18.47, p < .01 (R2 = .47). Students who initially took the course
because they were interested in majoring in psychology reported being more interested in
majoring in psychology at the end of the course than those who were not initially interested,
t(236) = 11.69, p < .01, (β = .66). Students who performed better early in the course also reported
being more interested in majoring in psychology at the end of the course, t(236) = 2.21, p = .03,
(β = .11). This effect was qualified by the significant interaction between mid-term exams and
the relevance contrast, t(236) = -2.06, p = .04, (β = -.10). As presented in the middle-right panel
of Figure 2, the predicted values indicated that students with lower exam scores in the relevance
conditions reported more interest in majoring in psychology (Ŷ = 2.40) than those in the control
conditions (Ŷ = 1.92, β = .13, p = .09). In contrast, students with higher exam scores reported
equivalent levels of interest in majoring in psychology in the relevance (Ŷ = 2.43) and control
conditions (Ŷ = 2.70, β = -.07, p = .24).
The basic model did not account for a significant amount of variance in final grade, and
there were no significant direct effects on final grade.
Mediation Analyses
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 29
The same procedures used to test mediation in Study 1 were used in Study 2. We first
tested whether the basic model predicted final perceptions of utility value, and then whether
perceptions of utility value accounted for the effects of the relevance intervention on the
outcomes. The mediation model contained seven terms: initial interest, initial inclination, initial
utility value, mid-term exams, the relevance contrast, the interaction between the relevance
contrast and mid-term exams, and final perceptions of utility value.
The basic model accounted for a significant amount of variance in perceived utility value,
F(10, 236) = 14.96, p < .01 (R2 = .46). Although the direct effect of the relevance contrast was
not significant, t(236) = 1.55, p = .12, (β = .08), the interaction between the relevance contrast
and mid-term exams was significant, t(236) = -2.66, p < .01, (β = -.15). This interaction is
presented in the bottom-right panel of Figure 2. The predicted values indicated that students with
lower exam scores in the relevance conditions (Ŷ = 4.52) perceived more utility value in the
course than those in the control conditions (Ŷ = 3.98, β = .23, p < .01). Students with higher
scores perceived equivalent levels of utility value in the course in the relevance (Ŷ = 4.75) and
control conditions (Ŷ = 4.86, β = -.07, p = .24). The significant direct effects of mid-term exams,
t(236) = 5.02, p < .01, (β = .24), initial utility value, t(236) = 5.82, p < .01, (β = .36), and initial
interest, t(236) = 3.44, p < .01, (β = .30), indicated that students who performed well on the mid-
term exams, and/or reported higher levels of initial utility value and interest subsequently
perceived more utility value in the course than those with lower levels. No other effects were
significant.
Situational interest was regressed on the mediation model and it accounted for
significantly more variance than the basic model (R2-change = .21, p < .01). The significant
direct effect of perceived utility value, t(236) = 12.00, p < .01, (β = .61), indicates that students
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 30
who perceived higher levels of utility value in the course reported more interest in psychology at
the end of the semester. Although the direct effects of the relevance contrast and its interaction
with mid-term exams remained significant in the mediation model, the formal test of mediation
revealed that perceived utility value partially mediated the direct effect of the value contrast on
situational interest, z = 1.66 (p = .096), and fully mediated the interaction between the value
contrast and mid-term exams on situational interest, z = 2.60 (p < .01).
Maintained situational interest was regressed on the mediation model and it accounted for
significantly more variance than the basic model (R2-change = .05, p < .001). The significant
direct effect of perceived utility value, t(236) = 5.05, p < .001, (β = .31), indicates that students
who perceived higher levels of utility value in the course reported more interest in majoring in
psychology at the end of the semester. The interaction between the relevance contrast and initial
exams was no longer significant, and perceived utility value mediated the interaction effect on
maintained situational interest, z = 2.35 (p = .02).
Final grades were regressed on the mediation model and accounted for significantly more
variance than the basic model, (R2-change = .07, p < .01). There was a significant direct effect of
perceived utility value, t(236) = 3.55, p < .01, (β = .33), indicating that students who perceived
higher levels of utility value in psychology received higher grades than students who perceived
lower levels of utility value. The overall mediation path model for Study 2 is presented in the
bottom panel of Figure 3.
Study 2 Discussion
The results of Study 2 replicate and extend those of Study 1 in several important ways.
First, Study 2 demonstrated that the relevance intervention triggered and maintained students’
situational interest in psychology over the course of the semester, particularly for students who
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 31
had performed more poorly on the first two exams. These results replicated the effect in Study 1
– that the intervention works better for some students than others – and extended the results to
include participants with poor performance histories. We found the same pattern of results for
students’ perceived utility value of psychology that we found in Study 1 for the math activity: the
relevance intervention increased perceptions of utility value, which in turn mediated the direct
effects of the intervention on situational interest and interest in majoring in psychology.
Study 2 also replicated prior research that has demonstrated an association between
perceived utility value and performance (Hulleman et al., 2008; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, in
press; Simons, Dewitte & Lens, 2003, 2004). In particular, Study 2 revealed that increases in
utility value predicted students’ final course grades, controlling for utility value at Time 1. By
controlling for initial utility perceptions, this analysis demonstrates that changes in utility value
(as predicted by the relevance intervention) lead to increases in graded performance. Although
there was no direct effect of the intervention on performance, students performed better when
they perceived value in the course material, and our intervention was successful in promoting
those perceptions. In sum, we were able to extend our laboratory results to the college classroom
and document that our relevance intervention promoted interest and performance by enhancing
students’ perceptions of utility value.
Summary of Relevance Intervention Effects Across Studies
In both studies, the relevance intervention had significant direct effects on situational
interest and perceived utility value. Significant interactions between the intervention and
performance expectations or prior performance on triggered and maintained situational interest,
and perceived utility value, were also present in both studies. The intervention also had a
significant direct effect on maintained situational interest in the laboratory. As summarized in
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 32
Table 5, the average effect for participants with low performance expectations or prior
performance was β = .52 in Study 1 and β = .23 in Study 2. In contrast, the average intervention
effect for students with high performance expectations or prior performance was nearly zero, β =
-.03 in Study 1 and β = -.05 in Study 2.
General Discussion
The primary purpose of this research was to conduct an experimental test of a utility
value intervention by encouraging students to discover the relevance of what they were learning.
Across two randomized experiments – one in the laboratory and one in the classroom – we
demonstrated that our intervention, a writing exercise in which we encouraged students to apply
the task or course material to their own lives, increased perceptions of utility value. In turn, these
utility perceptions predicted increases in triggered situational task interest and maintained
interest to re-engage in the math task in the future (Study 1), situational interest and maintained
interest in majoring in psychology (Study 2), and performance (Study 2). The effects of the
intervention were strongest for participants with low performance expectations (Study 1) and
prior performance (Study 2). Although the intervention did not benefit students with high
expectations or prior performance, it did not undermine their subsequent interest or performance.
These results support our hypothesized model outlined in Figure 1, and afford some insight into
the mechanisms of the intervention effects documented here.
A unique aspect of this research is that we manipulated utility value in addition to
measuring it. This combination of experimental and survey approaches allowed us to draw firmer
conclusions regarding the causal nature of utility value (Schneider, Carnoy, Kilpatrick, Schmidt,
& Shavelson, 2007). We focused on utility value for two reasons. From a theoretical perspective,
we wanted to understand how perceived task value contributed to the development of interest
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 33
over time. In their model of interest development, Hidi and Renninger (2006) proposed that
perceived value for a task can contribute to both situational and individual interest. From a
practical perspective, we wanted to know how we could address the documented decline in
student interest over time (Jacobs et al., 2002; Lepper et al., 2005). The task value that seemed to
be the most amenable to a classroom intervention was utility value, given its more external
nature (Brophy, 1999; Heckhausen, 1977; Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000; Pintrich & De Groot,
1990; Schiefele, 1991; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). In addition, prior correlational research has
identified utility value as a potentially important antecedent of both interest and performance.
Theoretical Implications
At first glance, the interaction revealed in our research seems opposite to what is
predicted by most expectancy-value models. That is, most models predict that the combination of
high expectancy and high value will be the most motivating. In contrast, we found that the value
intervention worked most effectively for those with low expectancies. However, the effect of our
intervention was to raise perceptions of utility value for individuals who did not otherwise
perceive value, and so our findings are consistent with the perspective that increases in perceived
value can promote motivation and performance. In addition, the consistency of the interaction
between expectancies and value in our research supports including it in theoretical and empricial
models (e.g., Atkinson, 1957; Edwards, 1954; Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & Sears, 1944; Tolman,
1955; Vroom, 1964; see Mitchell, 1974, for a review). In addition, the relationship between
expectancies and value could not have been uncovered unless task values were considered as
conceptually distinct constructs as proposed by Eccles et al. (1983)..
Our theoretical model provides one means of incorporating these novel results into the
expectancy-value framework. Considering the variability in tasks and contexts, the consistency
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 34
in predictive strength of our intervention and perceptions of utility value suggests that our
theoretical model is viable. However, this model should be considered as an initial step in
understanding how perceived utility value influences motivation and performance. There are
likely other processes, in addition to utility perceptions, that may be influenced by our
intervention and associated with interest and performance. For example, finding an application
for an activity (e.g., math and engineering) may create the possibility of making connections to
goals or aspirations that are personally important to the individual (e.g., a career as an engineer).
As outlined in other models that specify an internalization process (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1985;
Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci, 2006), an individual can identify with an extrinsic motivator such
that it effectively becomes intrinsic in nature. As defined, utility value is a more extrinsic type of
task value: The task is important, not for task-intrinsic reasons (i.e., enjoyment), but for task-
extrinsic reasons (i.e., as a tool for accomplishing a goal). In contrast, intrinsic value – which
focuses on the enjoyment of doing the activity – is associated with more internal processes:
enjoyment of the activity arises from both the task (task-intrinsic) and person (person-intrinsic).
However, even task-extrinsic values can be person-intrinsic (Hulleman et al., 2008; Simons,
Dewitte et al., 2004), and the intrinsic relation to the self may be the critical variable. That is, a
task’s utility can be important for the individual’s sense of self, such as accomplishing a
personally meaningful goal. As a result, we hypothesized that perceiving utility value in a task
could lead to processes that are both internally and externally motivating.
Asking students to think about the applications of a topic may promote active and
involved task engagement which leads them to appreciate the utility value of the topic. In their
process model of intrinsic motivation, Harackiewicz and Sansone (1991) proposed that becoming
involved during task engagement (i.e., task involvement) is a precursor of intrinsic motivation.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 35
Research has demonstrated that feelings of involvement are associated with subsequent interest
and performance on laboratory tasks (Harackiewicz, Barron, & Elliot, 1998; Barron &
Harackiewicz, 2001; Durik & Harackiewicz, 2003; Mitchell, 1993) and with positive life
outcomes such as happiness and academic performance (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). In addition,
the literature on student engagement in the classroom reveals positive relationships between
measures of emotional and behavioral engagement and outcomes such as intrinsic motivation for
learning, hope for the future, and academic achievement (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Patrick,
Skinner, & Connell, 1993; Skinner & Belmont, 1993; Van Ryzin, Gravely, & Roseth, 2009). It is
therefore possible that by making connections between the material and their lives that students
become more involved and engaged in learning – both emotionally and behaviorally – which
promotes learning and interest outcomes.
This study also extends prior research and theorizing on the role of value in interest
development. In their model of interest development, Hidi and Renninger (2006) proposed that
perceived value for a task is a component of both situational and individual interest. In other
words, value for a task can be perceived during task engagement or over time as (possibly) a
more enduring characteristic of the person. We focused on task value as triggering and
maintaining situational interest, and found that utility value played a causal role in the
development of interest over the course of a laboratory session or academic semester. Thus, our
results provide support for Hidi and Renninger’s (2006) model by demonstrating one pathway
through which value can promote interest development.
Limitations
There are several noteworthy limitations to our research. First, the math activity, and to
some extent the introductory psychology course, did not require deeper level processing or
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 36
complex thinking. Thus, any generalizations we could make about the impact of our educational
intervention on learning with more complex tasks may be limited. In addition, the samples in
both studies were undergraduate students and this also has potential to limit generalizations
about our findings to younger students. Second, our analysis of interest was constrained to an
emotion-focused situational interest scale and a single-item indicator of inclination to re-engage
in the task or topic in the future. Future research will need to clarify the effects of the relevance
intervention on other measures of interest that may also include measures of knowledge and
value. In doing so, it will be essential to ensure that interest measures are conceptually and
empirically distinct from other key dependent or independent variables.
Third, knowledge activation – which is an important aspect of interest in the Hidi and
Renninger (2006) model – could also explain the effects of the relevance intervention. Making a
connection between a course topic and real-life could facilitate deeper processing of the material,
encourage reorganization of the material to facilitate recall and future application, or increase the
amount of material encoded into memory (Bransford et al., 2004; Hidi & Renninger, 2006).
Although we did not directly test this possibility in our research, one of our control groups in
Study 2 was intended to increase knowledge consolidation and depth of processing (the outline
condition). The fact that we found effects of the relevance intervention in Study 2 contradicts this
possibility, albeit indirectly, and this possibility can be addressed by future research.
Practical Applications
Our results demonstrate that careful theoretical work can pay dividends in terms of real-
world implications. The development of the relevance intervention began with a theoretical
grounding in expectancy-value and interest development models of motivation. As a result, an
often overlooked aspect of expectancy-value theory – the interaction between expectancy and
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 37
value – proved valuable in understanding student motivation, particularly in response to
classroom interventions. Practically-speaking, the relevance intervention is easy and inexpensive
to implement, produces effects in as few as one or two trials, can be flexibly implemented during
class or on the students’ own time, and is applicable to a diverse array of topics or activities.
Although the participants in our research only wrote about two topics (mental math and
psychology), there is no reason to assume that similar results cannot be obtained in other
domains, such as history, English, or chemistry. In fact, some recent research indicates that these
results also obtain in high school science and college statistics classes (Hulleman, An, Hendricks,
& Harackiewicz, 2007; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, in press). Importantly, the students who most
often concern teachers – those who perform poorly and have low performance expectations –
benefited the most from our intervention, and those with high performance expectations were not
harmed by it. These results parallel the positive effects of other psychological interventions
intended to diminish the racial achievement gap in school performance (e.g., Cohen, Garcia,
Apfel, & Master, 2006), and demonstrate the positive potential of motivational interventions. In
other words, this research demonstrates that “…there is nothing so practical as a good theory
(Lewin, 1951, p. 169).”
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 38
Appendix A – Scale items for Study 1
Participants responded to all self-report items in this study on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Initial Interest (α = .93)
I find math enjoyable
Math just doesn’t appeal to me. (Reversed)
I enjoy working on math problems.
I like learning new math concepts.
Performance Expectations (α = .74)
I think I’ll do well on the following sets of problems.
I felt that I was using the technique correctly.
I felt that I was doing poorly on these problems. (Reversed)
Utility Value (α = .84)
This technique could be useful in everyday life,
I don’t think this technique would be useful to me in the future. (Reversed)
To be honest, I don’t think this technique is useful. (Reversed)
Situational Interest (α = .89)
The left-to-right technique is interesting.
Using this multiplication technique is fun.
It was a waste of time to learn this technique. (Reversed)
I enjoyed using the left-to-right technique.
The learning program was enjoyable.
Maintained Situational Interest
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 39
Do you think you will use the technique you learned today on your own in the future? (Yes/No)
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 40
Appendix B - Scale items for Study 2
Participants responded to all self-report items in this study on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Initial Interest (α = .91)
I think psychology is an interesting subject.
I am not interested in psychology. (Reversed)
I think I will like learning about psychology in this course.
I think psychology will be interesting.
I’ve always wanted to learn more about psychology.
Utility Value (Initial, α = .78; Final, α = .88)
What I am learning in this class is relevant to my life.
I think what we are studying in Introductory Psychology is useful for me to know.
I find the content of this course to be personally meaningful.
Situational Interest (α = .93)
I think the field of psychology is very interesting.
I think what we're learning in this class is fascinating.
To be honest, I just don't find psychology interesting. (Reversed)
I think the material in this course is boring. (Reversed)
Psychology fascinates me.
Maintained Situational Interest
I am interested in majoring in psychology.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 41
Footnotes
1 The Nagelkerke R2 is an approximation of the R2 typically used in ordinary least squares (OLS)
regression (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003).
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 42
References
Aiken, L. S. & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Alexander, P. A., Jetton, T. L., & Kulikowich, J. M. (1995). Interrelationship of knowledge,
interest, and recall: Assessing a model of domain learning. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 87, 559-575.
Alexander, P. A., & Murphy, P. K. (1998). Profiling the differences in students’ knowledge,
interest, and strategic processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 435–447.
Anderman, E. M., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Roeser, R., Wigfield, A., & Blumenfeld, P. (2001).
Learning to value mathematics and reading: Relations to matery and performance-
oriented instructional practices. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26(1), 76-95.
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological
Review, 64, 359-372.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals and optimal motivation:
Testing multiple goal models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706-
722.
Bong, M. (2001). Role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college students' course
performance and future enrollment intentions. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
26, 553-570.
Bransford, J., Brown. A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (Eds.). (2003). How people learn: Brain, mind,
experience, and school. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 43
Brophy, J. (1999). Toward a model of the value aspects of motivation in education: Developing
appreciation for particular learning domains and activities. Educational Psychologist, 34,
75-85.
Cleary, T. J., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2001). Self-regulation differences during athletic practice by
experts, non-experts, and novices. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 61-82.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation
analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A
social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307-1310.
Cole, J. S., Bergin, D. A., & Whitaker, T. A. (2008). Predicting student motivation for low stakes
tests with effort and task value. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 609-624.
Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning:
Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 88, 715-730.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday life.
New York, NY: Basic Books Inc.
Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Cambridge, MA: Riverside Press.
Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2003). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation:
Coherence, concordance, and achievement orientation. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 39, 378-385.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 44
Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How personal
interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 99, 597-610.
Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and ability beliefs as predictors of
high school literacy choices: A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 98, 382-393.
Eccles, J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L. & Midgley, C.
(1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement
and achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches (pp. 75–146). San
Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1991). Gender differences in sport involvement: Applying the
eccles’ expectancy-value model. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3(1), 1533-1571.
Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents’
achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 21, 215-225.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 109-132.
Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51, 380-417.
Feather, N. T. (1995). Values, valences, and choice: The influence of values on the perceived
attractiveness and choice of alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
68, 1135-1151.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 45
Fischoff, B., Goitein, B., & Shapira, Z. (1982). The experienced utility of expected utility
approaches. In N. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and actions: expectancy–value models in
psychology (pp. 315–339). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Flansburg, S., & Hay, V. (1994). Math magic. New York: Harper Collins.
Flum, H., & Kaplan, A. (2006). Exploratory orientation as an educational goal. Educational
Psychologist, 41, 99-110.
Fries, S., Schmid, S., Dietz, F., & Hofer, M. (2005). Conflicting values and their impact on
learning. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 20, 259-273.
Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children’s academic
engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 148–162.
Godes, O., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Boosting students’ interest in math
with utility value: Two experimental tests. Paper presented at the annual American
Educational Research Association conference, Chicago, IL.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When
are they adaptive for college students and why? Educational Psychologist, 33, 1-21.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Predicting success in
college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of
interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 94, 562-575.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M. & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term
and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and performance
over time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316-330.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 46
Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink, E. A., & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The
role of achievement goals in the development of interest: Reciprocal relations between
achievement goals, interest and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
105-122.
Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can get there
from here. In M. L. Maehr, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and
achievement (vol. 7, pp. 21-49). Greenwich, Ct: JAI Press.
Harp, S. F., & Mayer, R. E. (1997). The role of interest in learning from scientific text and
illustrations: On the distinction between emotional interest and cognitive interest. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 89, 92–102.
Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model.
Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283-329.
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource for learning. Review of
Educational Research, 60, 549-571.
Hidi, S. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical
issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70, 151-179.
Hidi, S. & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational
Psychologist, 41, 111-127.
Hulleman, C. S., An, B., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Interest development,
achievement, and continuing motivation: The pivotal role of utility value. Poster
presented at the Institute for Education Sciences 2007 Research Conference, Washington,
DC.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 47
Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). The importance of
utility value in predicting interest and performance in academics and sports. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 100, 398-416.
Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (in press). Promoting interest and performance in high
school science classes. Science.
Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in
children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one
through twelve. Child Development, 73, 509-527.
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In D. T.
Gilbert, S. T. Fiske & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology, 4th ed. (pp.
233-265), New York: McGraw-Hill.
Kintsch, W. (1980). Learning from text, levels of comprehension, or: Why anyone would read a
story anyway. Poetics, 9, 87-98.
Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Comparing self-regulatory processes among novice,
non-expert, and expert volleyball players: A microanalytic study. Journal of Applied
Sport Psychology, 14, 91-105.
Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of interest and its relation to SDT. In E.
L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 405-427).
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational
orientations in the classroom: Age differences and academic correlates. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 97, 184-196.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 48
Lewin, K. (1951). Problems of research in social psychology. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Field
theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers (pp. 155-169). New York: Harper &
Row.
Lewin, K. , Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In J. McV. Hunt
(Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders. New York: Ronald Press.
Lipstein, R. L., & Renninger, K. A. (2007). Putting things into words: The development of 12-15
year olds’ interest for writing. In P. Boscolo & S. Hidi (Eds.), Motivation and writing:
Research and school practice. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Mac Iver, D. J., Stipek, D. J., & Daniels, D. H. (1991). Explaining within-semester changes in
student effort in junior high school and senior high school courses. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 83, 201-211.
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A
comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects.
Psychological Methods, 7, 83-104.
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation.
Psychological Methods, 12, 23-44.
Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted structure in the secondary school
mathematics classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 424-436.
Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, occupational preference and
effort: A theoretical, methodological, and empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81,
1053-1077.
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. (2004). Reducing underage drinking: A
collective responsibility. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 49
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs and mathematical problem solving of gifted students.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 325-344.
Patrick, B. C., Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1993). What motivates children's behavior and
emotion? Joint effects of perceived control and autonomy in the academic domain.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 781-791.
Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in
learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 667-686.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components
of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33-40.
Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic
motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 373-404). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.
Renninger, K. A., Bachrach, J. E., & Posey, S. K. E. (2008). Learner interest and achievement
motivation. In M. Maehr, S. Karabenick, & T. Urdan (Eds.), Advances in Motivation and
Achievement, Vol. 15 (pp. 461-491). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing, Ltd.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 299-323.
Schiefele, U. (1996). Topic interest, text representation, and quality of experience.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 3–18.
Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., & Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement:
A meta-analysis of research. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of
interest in learning and development (pp. 183-211). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 50
Schneider, B., Carnoy, M., Kilpatrick, J., Schmidt, W. H., & Shavelson, R. J. (2007). Estimating
causal effects using experimental and observational designs (report from the Governing
Board of the American Educational Research Association Grants Program). Washington,
DC: American Educational Research Association.
Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2005). Competence perceptions and academic functioning. In A. J.
Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 85-104). New
York: Guilford Press.
Schwartz, S. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical test
in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 25,
S. 1-65). New York: Academic Press.
Silvia, P. J. (2001). Interest and interests: The psychology of constructive capriciousness. Review
of General Psychology, 5, 270–290.
Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York: Oxford University Press.
Simons, J., Dewitte, S. & Lens, W. (2003). “Don't do it for me. Do it for yourself!” Stressing the
personal relevance enhances motivation in physical education. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 25, 145-160.
Simons, J., Dewitte, S. & Lens, W. (2004). The role of different types of instrumentality in
motivation, study strategies, and performance: Know why you learn, so you’ll know what
you learn! British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 343-360.
Simons, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W. & Lacante, M. (2004). Placing motivation and future
time perspective theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 16,
121-139.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 51
Skinner, E. A., & Belmont, M. J. (1993). Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of
teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 85, 571-581.
Stipek, D. (2002). Motivation to learn: Integrating theory and practice (4th edition). Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Tolman, E. C. (1955). Principles of performance. Psychological Review, 62, 315-326.
Updegraff, K. A., Eccles, J. S., Barber, B. L., & O’Brien, K. M. (1996). Course enrollment as
self-regulatory behavior: Who takes optional high school math courses? Learning and
Individual Differences, 8, 239-259.
Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Sources of self-efficacy in school: Critical review of the
literature and future directions. Review of Educational Research, 78, 751-796.
Van Ryzin, M. J., Gravely, A. A., & Roseth, C. J. (2009). Autonomy, belongingness, and
engagement in school as contributors to adolescent psychological well-being. Journal of
Youth Adolescence, 38, 10-12.
Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal contents in
self-determination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation.
Educational Psychologist, 41, 19-31.
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Wagner, T., Kegan, R., Lahey, L., Lemons, R. W., Garnier, J., Helsing, D., . . . Thurber
Rasmussen, H. (2006). Change leadership: A practical guide to transforming our
schools. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: A developmental
perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 6(1), 49-78.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 52
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1992). The development of achievement task values: A theoretical
analysis. Developmental Review, 12, 265-310.
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of competence beliefs, expectancies for
success, and achievement values from childhood through adolescence. In A. Wigfield &
J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 91-120). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, S. Y., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J. A., Freedman-Doan, C.,
Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in childen’s competence beliefs and subjective task
values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 89(3), 451-469.
Xiang, P., Chen, A., & Bruene, A. (2005). Interactive impact of intrinsic motivators and extrinsic
rewards on behavior and motivation outcomes. Journal of Teaching in Physical
Education, 24, 179-197.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2005). The hidden dimension of personal competence: Self-
regulated learning and practice. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of
competence and motivation (pp. 509-526). New York: Guilford.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1988). Construct validation of a strategic model of
student self-regulated learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 284-290.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 53
Table 1
Zero-order Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Major Variables in Study 1
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Initial interest 0.93
2 Performance expectations 0.27 0.74
3 Initial performance 0.24 0.51 --
4 Relevance intervention -0.03 0.20 0.09 --
5 Observed utility value -0.13 0.11 0.05 0.88 --
6 Perceived utility value 0.07 0.23 0.15 0.21 0.31 0.84
7 Situational interest 0.24 0.36 0.22 0.27 0.28 0.72 0.89
8 Maintained interest 0.04 0.21 0.11 0.30 0.37 0.68 0.57 --
9 Final performance 0.25 0.42 0.83 0.07 0.06 0.18 0.28 0.17 --
Minimum 1.00 1.33 0.00 0.00 -0.99 1.75 1.00 0.00 0.00
Maximum 7.00 7.00 24.0 1.00 1.89 7.00 7.00 1.00 39
Mean 4.12 5.04 10.2 0.57 0.00 5.10 4.89 0.78 22.5
SD 1.43 1.10 5.09 0.50 0.98 1.10 1.04 0.42 7.46
N = 107. Scale reliabilities are presented along the diagonal where applicable. Correlations greater than 0.19 are significant at p < .05.
Correlations greater than 0.23 are significant at p < .01. Relevance contrast= + 1 (Relevance conditions), - 1 (Control conditions).
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 54
Table 2
Standardized Regression Coefficients for Study 1
Situational Interest Maintained Interest Performance Utility Value
Step 1
Relevance contrast 0.24 ** 0.12 0.30 ** 0.18 * 0.01 -0.02 0.19 *
Performance expectations 0.11 0.11 -0.05 -0.05 0.37 ** 0.37 ** 0.00
Relevance X Expectations -0.22 * -0.02 -0.32 ** -0.14 -0.01 0.03 -0.29 **
Initial interest 0.21 * 0.18 ** 0.03 0.00 0.18 * 0.17 0.05
Step 2
Utility value 0.66 ** 0.60** 0.12
R2 0.21 ** 0.59 ** 0.19 ** 0.50 ** 0.20 ** 0.21** 0.14 **
change R2 0.38 ** 0.31 ** 0.01
Note: Values are standardized regression coefficients.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 55
Table 3
Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Major Variables in Study 2
12345678910
1 Initial interest 0.91
2 Initial inclination 0.50 --
3 Initial utility value 0.59 0.40 0.78
4 Mid-term exams -0.07 -0.13 0.02 0.80
5 Relevance contrast 0.09 0.00 0.02 0.04 --
6 Observed relevance 0.03 -0.01 -0.07 0.08 0.50 0.86
7 Situational interest 0.59 0.39 0.49 0.10 0.19 0.11 0.93
8 Maintained interest 0.37 0.67 0.31 0.02 0.03 0.01 0.48 --
9 Final grade -0.05 -0.12 0.00 0.90 0.02 0.06 0.07 -0.01 --
10 Final utility value 0.53 0.35 0.56 0.20 0.10 0.06 0.78 0.47 0.18 0.88
Minimum 1.63 1.00 1.33 39.00 -1.00 -0.78 1.00 1.00 0.00 1.00
Maximum 7.00 7.00 7.67 117.00 1.00 3.65 7.00 7.00 4.00 7.00
Mean 5.60 2.63 4.60 92.12 -0.04 0.01 4.67 2.38 2.90 4.55
Std. Deviation 0.96 1.83 1.05 13.32 1.00 0.95 1.31 1.83 0.86 1.16
N = 237. Scale reliabilities are presented along the diagonal where applicable. Correlations greater than 0.13 are significant at p < .05.
Relevance = + 1 (Relevance conditions), - 1 (Control conditions).
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 56
Table 4
Standardized Regression Coefficients for Study 2
Situational Interest Maintained Interest Final Grade Final Utility Value
Step 1
Relevance contrast 0.16 ** 0.11 ** 0.03 0.01 0.02 0.00 0.08
Mid-term exams 0.15 ** 0.00 0.11 * 0.04 -- -- 0.24 **
Relevance X Exams -0.18 ** -0.09 * -0.10 * -0.06 -- -- -0.15 **
Initial interest 0.44 ** 0.25 ** 0.03 -0.06 -0.03 -0.13 0.30 **
Initial inclination 0.13 * 0.06 0.66** 0.63 ** -0.13 -0.15 * 0.11
Initial utility value 0.20 ** -0.02 0.03 -0.08 0.07 -0.05 0.36 **
Step 2
Final utility value 0.61 ** 0.31 ** 0.33 **
R2 0.46 ** 0.67 ** 0.47 ** 0.52 ** 0.02 0.09 ** 0.46 **
change R2 0.21 ** 0.05 ** 0.07 **
Note: Values are standardized regression coefficients.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 57
Table 5
Summary of Relevance Intervention Effects on Study 1 and Study 2 Outcomes
Performance Expectations or
Prior Performance
Overall Low High
Study 1 0.24 0.52 -0.03
Study 2 0.10 0.23 -0.05
Note: Values represent the average standardized regression coefficients from separate multiple
regressions. The “Overall” column represents the average standardized regression coefficient of
the relevance intervention predicting perceived utility value, situational interest, and maintained
situational interest in Study 1 and Study 2. The “Low” and “High” columns represent the
standardized regression coefficient for the relevance intervention and one standard deviation
below and above the mean of performance expectations or prior performance, respectively.
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 58
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Path model of utility value effects on interest and performance. Solid paths represent
hypothesized direct effects. The dotted path represents the hypothesized interaction between
performance expectations and the relevance intervention on perceived utility value.
Figure 2. Interactive effects of the relevance intervention and performance expectations or prior
performance on interest and performance. Study 1 results are presented in the left-hand column
and Study 2 results are presented in the right-hand column.
Figure 3. Path models for utility value effects on interest and performance. Study 1 is presented
in the top panel and Study 2 in the bottom panel. Solid paths represent standardized regression
coefficients significant at p < .05 (except for the path from the Relevance Intervention to
Perceived Utility Value in Study 1, p = .06). Dashed paths are not significant (p > .10).
Superscripts (a, b, c) indicate that the direct effect is moderated by a significant interaction with
performance expectations (Study 1) or mid-term exams (Study 2), and are represented with “Lo”
(Low mid-term exams) and “Hi” (High mid-term exams). Non-mediated values are in
parentheses. The regression models on which these effects were based controlled for additional
variables (see text for details).
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 59
Figure 1
PERCEIVED
UTILITY VALUE
INTEREST
PERFORMANCE
RELEVANCE
INTERVENTION
PERFORMANCE
EXPECTATIONS
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 60
Figure 2
Study 1 Study 2
4
4.5
5
5.5
Low High
PerformanceExpectations
SituationalInterest
Control Relevance
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
Low High
MidtermExams
SituationalInterest
Control Relevance
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 61
4
4.5
5
5.5
Low High
PerformanceExpectations
PerceivedUtilityValue
Control Relevance
3.5
4
4.5
5
5.5
Low High
MidtermExams
PerceivedUtilityValue
Control Relevance
0
0.5
1
Low High
PerformanceExpectations
MaintainedInterest
Control Relevance
1.5
2
2.5
3
Low High
MidtermExams
MaintainedInterest
Control Relevance
RUNNING HEAD: Enhancing Interest 62
Figure 3
Relevance
Intervention Perceived
Utility Value
Situational
Interest
Maintained
Interest
.66
.60
Lo .48
Hi -.10
.19
b
.18 (.30
c
)
.12 (.24
a
)
Total Correct
.12
Relevance
Intervention Perceived
Utility Value
Situational
Interest
Maintained
Interest
.61
.31
Lo .23
Hi -.07
.11
a
(.16)
.08
b
Grades
.33
.01
c
(.03)
... Research suggests that psychological interventions that help students find coursework useful in achieving their goals and relevant to their lives (i.e., utility-value interventions; Hulleman & Cordray, 2009;Hulleman et al., 2010Hulleman et al., , 2017 improve students' motivation to learn, increase their effort invested in the course, and, consequently, promote their achievement and persistence (Hulleman et al., 2022;Rosenzweig et al., 2020). More recent evidence also points to the benefits of these interventions for the community college population (e.g., Kosovich et al., 2019;, which serve a large number of racially marginalized and first-generation students. ...
... Utility-value interventions are designed to help students make relevant connections between course topics and their own current or future goals (Hulleman et al., 2010;Hulleman & Harackiewicz, 2021). Utility-value interventions are grounded in the expectancyvalue-cost model of motivation (Barron & Hulleman, 2015;J. ...
... Consistent with past research (e.g., Harackiewicz et al., 2016;Hulleman et al., 2010), we expected to find positive overall effects of the intervention on students' motivation and academic outcomes. Further, based on existing research Rosenzweig et al., 2020;, we hypothesized that expectancies for success, relevance of math, and perceived costs would serve as mechanisms for the effects of the utility-value interventions on achievement outcomes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has established the benefits of utility-value interventions in improving students’ motivation and achievement outcomes. However, further investigation is needed to understand the heterogeneity of intervention effects and identify the contexts within which these interventions are more effective. Accordingly, in the present study, we tested the efficacy of a utility-value intervention in contexts with varying degrees of perceived instructor support in the development of values and relevance. Participants were 2,240 community college students enrolled in one of the four math courses taught by 78 instructors. The results of multilevel models with Bayesian estimation suggested that the utility-value intervention increased students’ relevance for mathematics, which in turn positively predicted their math achievement. Notably, the intervention predicted these outcomes only in contexts where students perceived less support from instructors for the development of values, highlighting its effectiveness in contexts where it was most needed. Additionally, the results revealed an unintended consequence of the utility-value intervention: a slight decrease in students’ expectations for success in their math course compared to the control group. We propose that minor adjustments to the writing activities embedded within the intervention in future research could mitigate any negative effects on students’ expectations for success. This study underscores the importance of examining the heterogeneity of intervention effects, highlighting that interventions do not produce uniform outcomes across all contexts. The results have implications for professional development opportunities for instructors seeking to foster value-supportive environments for their students.
... In turn, such experiences may increase their perceptions of the workshop's value. Indeed, numerous studies at the college level have shown that value interventions have improved students' interest and course performance (e.g., Canning et al., 2018;Harackiewicz et al., 2016;Hulleman et al., 2010). ...
... Additionally, interventions such as motivational messaging could be designed to target the needs of various subgroups such as the value differences highlighted in Table 4 and 5 above. Such efforts have been previously shown to enhance participants' utility values (e.g., Gaspard et al., 2015;Hulleman et al., 2010), as well as competency-based beliefs (Brisson et al., 2017;Canning & Harackiewicz, 2015;Rosenzweig et al., 2022), which in turn have contributed to enhanced STEM student performance (e.g., Canning et al., 2018;Harackiewicz et al., 2016;Rosenzweig et al., 2022). ...
... 2. For the intervention to be perceived as relevant, intervention content about growth mindset should be closely linked to the situation of the users and how they can use the knowledge for change [56]. ...
Article
Background Engaging young adults in the labor market is vital for economic growth and well-being. However, the path to employment often presents setbacks that impact motivation and psychological functioning. Research suggests exploring positive psychology interventions in job-seeking and scaling the delivery of these using technology. However, dropout rates are high for self-administered psychological interventions on digital platforms. This challenge needs to be addressed for such platforms to be effective conveyors of psychological interventions. This study addresses this challenge by exploring user-oriented methods and proposes persuasive features for the design and development of a new web-based intervention targeting young unemployed adults. Objective This study aims to provide an overview of a new positive psychology wise intervention, including its theoretical underpinnings and human-centered design methodology, targeting young, unemployed adults. Methods Researchers collaborated with designers, developers, and stakeholders to design a web-based positive psychology intervention that leverages evidence-based wise interventions. Key improvements and adaptations were explored through formative usability testing with 13 unemployed young adults aged between 18 and 25 years (the target population). Qualitative usability testing data were collected, analyzed, and integrated into the ongoing design process as iterative improvements. Results The result of this study is a modular intervention web application named RØST, designed to align with the user needs and the preferences of the specific end-user group of unemployed young adults. During the project, this application evolved from early concept sketches and prototypes into a developed solution ready for further testing and use. Insights from both end-user feedback and rich user observation gained in the study were used to refine the content and the design. To increase targeted end users’ motivation, persuasive design features including praise, rewards, and reminders were added. The web application was designed primarily to be used on mobile phones using text messaging for reminders. The development process included technical and data protection considerations. Conclusions This study offers valuable insights into developing psychological or behavioral interventions to support unemployed young adults by documenting the design process and the adaptation and combination of diverse theoretical and empirical foundations. Involving stakeholders and end users in the development enabled relatable content development and resolved potential usability problems. An essential implication is the finding that end-user feedback and insights are crucial in shaping interventions. However, we experienced tensions between the evidence-based interventions and the human-centered design approaches. These tensions were not resolved and highlighted a need for ongoing user motivation support through monetary rewards, which were incorporated into the final web app design.
... Expectancy-Value Theory (Atkinson, 1964;Eccles & Wigfield, 2020;Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010;Wigfield, Tonks & Klauda, 2009;Wigfield & Cambria, 2010;Flake et al., 2015;Durik & Harackiewicz, 2007;Trautwein et al., 2009;Gaspard et al., 2015;Hulleman et al., 2010;Simpkins, Davis-Kean & Eccles, 2006) The educational goal-directed actions are guided by expectations that certain behaviors are likely to lead to a desired outcome and by the extent to which the outcome is valued. ...
Conference Paper
This study investigates the nuanced view of parental expectations regarding their children’s educational objectives in Latvia, with a focus on the perspective of teachers. As parental involvement is an important determinant of a child’s educational journey, understanding the dynamics of these expectations becomes paramount for educational stakeholders. The study aims to provide a comprehensive analysis of teacher observations and experiences related to parental expectations, shedding light on the intricate interplay between home and school environments. The methodology employed for this research includes mixed methods: literature context analysis and surveys for teachers (n = 1150). Preliminary findings reveal a spectrum of parental expectations, ranging from a desire for academic excellence to broader developmental goals. The study identifies commonalities and variations in teacher experiences, highlighting cultural influences and socio-economic factors that may impact the nature and intensity of parental expectations. This research contributes to the broader field of education by providing a localized understanding of the role parental expectations play in shaping children’s educational trajectories. Insights gained from teacher perspectives can inform educational policies, interventions, and practices aimed at fostering a collaborative and supportive partnership between parents and educators.
... Moreover, variability in student characteristics (e.g., ability, preparation, self-efficacy, distractibility) may require diverse teaching approaches and preclude a one-size-fits-all approach. It is clear from the literature that Yacek's assertion that intimation of value is a critically important first step toward sustaining short-term and maintaining long-term interest across the transformative process (Hidi & Renninger, 2006;Hulleman et al., 2010;Pugh et al., 2023;Silvia, 2001). High levels of interest may mediate the self-regulation skills that maintain motivation and connection to material (Lipstein & Renninger, 2007;Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this article, I explore methods that psychology instructors can use to integrate the philosophical principles proposed in the Guidelines for the Undergraduate Psychology Major 3.0 (American Psychological Association, 2023) into their courses and curricula. I apply Yacek’s (2021) aspirational transformative model to expand upon the first philosophical principle that psychology is a transformative pursuit and provide direction for incorporating several other philosophical principles into teaching psychology. The aspirational model can be applied to course-specific objectives or more broadly to course and curricular design to foster student transformation, preparing students for the work force and leading a flourishing life. Application of the aspirational model is one approach to using the philosophical principles in the Guidelines and course-specific values as the lens through which students process their experiences to the betterment of themselves, others, communities, and society. Support for the rationale, methods, and outcomes of the aspirational model draws from a diverse array of empirical and theoretical sources.
Article
Objective While much research has been conducted on retention, attrition, and college completion among those who voluntarily withdraw from college, few studies have focused on involuntary withdrawal in the form of academic dismissal. Even less scholarship has examined the subsequent restoration of academic momentum for returning students. Furthermore, the lack of appropriate theoretical models directed at readmitted students with past records of academic failure has created a dilemma for institutions. Particularly, when trying to assess readmission policies and establishing programs or interventions to promote academic success and persistence to graduation. This study explores factors that influence restoration of academic momentum among students returning from academic dismissal. Method Using a sequential, partial mixed methods approach, descriptive and qualitative data was collected through a survey ( N = 171) and student interviews ( N = 11) at a large, public community college. Results Findings disconfirmed past research that academically dismissed students are primarily underprepared scholastically while confirming life events as a key contributor to dismissal. Results also demonstrated the importance of faculty support and active learning as well as the value of advisors as contextualized change agents in the academic restoration process. Finally, it was evident that returning students possessed motivational attributes conducive to restoring academic momentum, including a strong growth mindset, academic resilience, and high self-determination. Contributions This study affords a Reconceptualized Restorative Academic Momentum Model of Success to guide and understand the academic and psychosocial experiences of students returning from academic dismissal centered on pre-college influences, classroom teaching and learning experiences, support services, and student motivational attributes.
Technical Report
Full-text available
This report examines the unique challenges to and opportunities for school belonging for students in special education, along with best practices to support school belonging for special education students.
Article
Non-traditional students in higher education have been increasing in numbers, and blended learning is considered a learning environment which often meets their special needs. However, dropout seems to be a major challenge. The goal of the current research was to investigate motivational beliefs in the framework of the expectancy–value model that might lead this population to persist in blended learning. Persistence intentions, as well as expectancies and perceived task value, were measured during the first semester of blended learning amongst non-traditional Swiss students. The results showed that intrinsic values, expectancies (competence-related beliefs), and two facets of cost played a role in predicting perseverance intentions. Moreover, subjective task value, as well as expectancy (competence-related beliefs), remained stable over time. These findings fill a gap in the existing literature by quantitatively investigating motivational beliefs of non-traditional students in blended learning and their short-term changes. The authors conclude with a discussion of these findings, their limitations, and their implications.
Preprint
Full-text available
Previous research has shown that students’ interest typically declines across secondary school, likely resulting from a mismatch between their needs and the school environment. Technology-enhanced adaptive teaching may allow teachers to better tailor their instruction to students’ needs, including students’ interests; thus, this approach may be promising in this regard. The objective of this study was to gain insight into the associations between equipping students and their teachers with tablet computers (1:1 technology), student-perceived adaptive teaching, and students' situational interest. We used questionnaire data (i.e., from two measurement points: t 0 = baseline; t 1 = 4 months later) from the longitudinal project tabletBW meets science from 2,601 students (Grade 7, Gymnasium, Germany) attending 28 schools. Fourteen schools were randomly chosen to be given 1:1 technology (i.e., tablet computers for teachers and students; intervention condition). The other 14 randomly chosen schools were not given such equipment (control condition). We assessed how students' situational interest in mathematics was associated with the use of tablet computers (intervention vs. control) and student-perceived adaptive teaching. Results from multilevel models showed that the 1:1 technology had a statistically significant effect on students' situational interest, partly mediated by student-perceived adaptive teaching. Moreover, knowing that students' interest in math declines in adolescence, our results indicate that equipping classrooms with technology might offer a promising way to implement more adaptive lessons that have the potential to provide the appropriate degree of challenge to students and thus spark their interest in mathematics.
Article
Full-text available
The factors that trigger lapses of attention (e.g., mind-wandering) during new learning remain unclear. The present study investigated whether the likelihood of experiencing an attentional lapse depends on (a) the importance of the material being studied and (b) the learner’s age. In two experiments, younger and older adults completed a delayed free recall task in which to-be-remembered words were paired with point values. Thought probes were embedded into the encoding phase of each list to provide an index of one’s ability to maintain attention on task and prevent recurrent lapses of attention (i.e., the consistency of attention). Experiment 1 revealed all individuals better remembered high-value information at the expense of low-value information, and older adults were more frequently focused on the task than younger adults. Participants were also less likely to remember an item at test if they experienced an attentional lapse while learning said item, and they were more consistently focused on the task when studying high-value information than when studying low-value information. Age did not moderate either of these effects. Experiment 2 replicated the findings from Experiment 1 and further revealed that the positive association between age and attentional consistency was explained by age-related differences in affect, motivation, personality, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptomology. Once these factors were accounted for, older age was associated with increased attentional inconsistency (less on-task focus). While future replication of this finding is needed, implications for education and theories of both mind-wandering and aging are discussed.
Article
This review examines recent developments in research on social-cognitive theories of motivation during adolescence and the ways in which such research can be applied to the reform of middle grade schools. While there is ample evidence that the environments in many middle grade schools are antithetical to the needs of early adolescents, few reform efforts have emerged which consider the motivational and developmental needs of youth. This article suggests that effective reform must consider the multiple contexts in which students interact. Recent examples of reform at the classroom and school level using a goal theory perspective are presented.
Article
It is argued that interest is central in determining how we select and persist in processing certain types of information in preference to others. Evidence that shows that both individual and text-based interest have a profound facilitative effect on cognitive functioning and learning is reviewed. Factors that contribute to text-based interest are discussed, and it is suggested that interest elicits spontaneous, rather than conscious, selective allocation of attention. It is further proposed that the psychological and physiological processes associated with interesting information have unique aspects not present in processing information without such interest. Current advances in neuro-cognitive research show promise that we will gain further knowledge of the impact of interest on cognitive functioning and that we will finally be in a position to integrate the physiological and psychological aspects of interest.
Article
This literature review of decision making (how people make choices among desirable alternatives), culled from the disciplines of psychology, economics, and mathematics, covers the theory of riskless choices, the application of the theory of riskless choices to welfare economics, the theory of risky choices, transitivity of choices, and the theory of games and statistical decision functions. The theories surveyed assume rational behavior: individuals have transitive preferences ("… if A is preferred to B, and B is preferred to C, then A is preferred to C."), choosing from among alternatives in order to "… maximize utility or expected utility." 209-item bibliography. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This chapter addresses three questions: (a) What is the relation among students' interest for writing and their conceptual competence, goals, and strategies as writers? (b) What is the relation among students' interest for writing and their perceptions of their effort, self-efficacy, and feedback preferences in their writing? (c) What conditions support students to be effective writers? Portraiture is used to depict students in each of the four phases of interest development identified by Hidi and Renninger (2006): triggered situational interest, maintained situational interest, emerging individual interest, and well-developed individual interest. Data from questionnaires and structured in-depth interviews with middle school students informed portrait development. Discussion centers on the relation among students' phase of interest for writing and their corresponding motivational strengths and needs as writers. Pedagogical implications are considered.