ArticlePDF Available

Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory

American Psychological Association
Psychological Review
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors (1977) in a series of experiments. The studies (a) demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; (b) trace the course of the learning of automatic detection, of categories, and of automatic-attention responses; and (c) show the dependence of automatic detection on attending responses and demonstrate how such responses interrupt controlled processing and interfere with the focusing of attention. The learning of categories is shown to improve controlled search performance. A general framework for human information processing is proposed. The framework emphasizes the roles of automatic and controlled processing. The theory is compared to and contrasted with extant models of search and attention. (31/2 p ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological
Review
J
Copyright
©
1977
\^J
by the
American
Psychological Association,
Inc.
VOLUME
84
NUMBER
2
MARCH
1977
Controlled
and
Automatic Human Information Processing:
II.
Perceptual Learning, Automatic Attending,
and
a
General Theory
Richard
M.
Shiffrin
Indiana University
Walter
Schneider
University
of
California, Berkeley
The
two-process theory
of
detection, search,
and
attention presented
by
Schneider
and
Shiffrin
is
tested
and
extended
in a
series
of
experiments.
The
studies demonstrate
the
qualitative
difference
between
two
modes
of
informa-
tion
processing:
automatic detection
and
controlled search. They trace
the
course
of the
learning
of
automatic detection,
of
categories,
and of
automatic-
attention responses.
They
show
the
dependence
of
automatic detection
on at-
tending responses
and
demonstrate
how
such responses interrupt controlled
processing
and
interfere with
the
focusing
of
attention.
The
learning
of
cat-
egories
is
shown
to
improve controlled search performance.
A
general frame-
work
for
human information processing
is
proposed;
the
framework
emphasizes
the
roles
of
automatic
and
controlled processing.
The
theory
is
compared
to and
contrasted with extant models
of
search
and
attention.
I.
Introduction
In
Part
I of
this
paper (Schneider
&
Shiffrin,
1977)
we
reported
the
results
of
several
experiments
on
search
and
attention
that
led us to
formulate
a
theory
of
informa-
tion processing based
on two
fundamental
processing modes: controlled
and
automatic.
In the
context
of
search
studies,
these
modes
took
the
form
of
controlled search
and
auto-
matic detection. Controlled search
is
highly
demanding
of
attentional
capacity,
is
usually
The
research
and
theory reported here were
sup-
ported
by PHS
Grant
12717
and a
Guggenheim
Fellowship
to the first
author,
Grant
MH23878
to
the
Rockefeller
University,
and a
Miller Fellowship
at
University
of
California
at
Berkeley
to the
second
author.
This
report represents equal
and
shared
con-
tributions
of
both
authors.
Requests
for
reprints
should
be
sent
to
Richard
M.
Shiffrin,
Department
of
Psychology, Indiana
University,
Bloomington, Indiana 47401.
'
serial
in
nature with
a
limited comparison
rate,
is
easily established, altered,
and
even
reversed
by the
subject,
and is
strongly
de-
pendent
on
load. Automatic detection
is
rela-
tively well learned
in
long-term memory,
is
demanding
of
attention only when
a
target
is
presented,
is
parallel
in
nature,
is
difficult
to
alter,
to
ignore,
or to
suppress
once learned,
and is
virtually
unaffected
by
load.
In the
present article
we
shall
report
several studies
to
elucidate
further
the
proper-
ties
of
automatic
and
controlled processing
and
their interrelations,
to
demonstrate
the
qualitative
difference
between these processing
modes,
to
study
the
development
of
automatic
detection
and the
role
of the
type
and
nature
of
practice
in
such development,
to
study
the
effects
of
categorization,
and to
examine
the
development
of
automatic attending
and
its
effects. After
the
presentation
of the
studies
we
shall present
a
general theory
of
information
processing,
with
emphasis
on the
127
128
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
CM
-
TRIAL
I
TRIAL
2
rs63
TRIAL
3
TRIAL
4
VM
-
TRIAL
I
TRIAL
2
CJK
TRIAL
3
TRIAL
4
Figure
1.
Examples
of
trials
in the
multiple-frame search paradigm
of
Experiment
1,
Part
I. In all
cases, memory-set
size
=
4 and
frame
size
= 2.
Four varied-mapping (VM) trials
and
four
con-
sistent-mapping (CM)
trials
are
depicted.
The
memory
set is
presented
in
advance
of
each trial,
then
the fixation dot
goes
on for
.5
sec
when
the
subject
starts
the
trial,
and
then
20
frames
are
presented
at a fixed
time
per
frame.
Either
0 or 1
member
of
the
memory
set is
presented during
each
trial.
Frame
time,
memory-set
size,
and
frame
size
are
varied
across
conditions.
roles
of
automatic
and
controlled processing.
Our
theory will then
be
compared
and
con-
trasted with extant theories
of
search
and
attention.
A.
Review
of
Paradigms
and
Results
From
Part
I
The
paradigm
for
most
of the
studies
of
Part
I
(and Part
II
also)
is
depicted
in
Figure
1.
Four elements
are
presented simul-
taneously
in a
square;
and
their joint presen-
tation
for a
brief
period
of
time
is
termed
a
frame.
Each trial consists
of the
presentation
of
20
frames
in
immediate succession.
The
elements
presented
are
characters (i.e., digits
or
consonants)
or
random
dot
masks.
In ad-
vance
of
each trial
the
subject
is
presented
with several items,
called
the
memory set,
and
is
then required
to
detect
any
memory-set
items that appear
in the
subsequent
frames.
The
frame
time
is
kept constant across
the
20
frames
of
each trial,
and the
basic depen-
dent
variable
is the
psychometric
function
relating
accuracy
to
frame
time
for
each
condition.
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
129
Three basic independent variables were
manipulated
in
Part
I/Experiment
1.
The
number
of
characters
in
each
frame
(the
frame
size,
F) was
varied
from
1 to 4
(but
was
constant
for all
frames
of a
given
trial).
The
number
of
characters
presented
in ad-
vance
of a
trial (the memory-set size,
M)
varied
from
1 to 4. The
product
of M and F
is
termed
the
load.
A
memory-set item
that
appears
in a
frame
is
called
a
target;
an
item
in a
frame
that
is not in the
memory
set is
called
a
distractor.
One
half
of the
trials
contained
one
target,
and one
half
contained
no
target. Finally,
and
most important,
the
nature
of the
training procedure across
trials
and the
relation
of the
memory-set items
to
the
distractors were varied.
In the
consistent
mapping
(CM) procedure,
across
all
trials,
memory-set items were never distractors (and
vice
versa).
In
addition, memory-set items
were
from
one
category (e.g., digits)
and
distractors
from
another category (e.g., con-
sonants).
In the
varied
mapping
(VM)
procedure,
memory-set items
and
distractors
were
randomly
intermixed over
trials
and
were
all
from
one
category (e.g.,
consonants).
Figure
1
gives examples
of
trials
in
both
the VM and CM
conditions. Depicted
are
four
consecutive trials
from
a CM
block
and
four
from
a VM
block
in
each
of
which
M
= 4 and F
=
2.
Table
1
gives
the
memory
set,
distractor,
and
target
(if
present)
for
each trial
in
Figure
1.
Note
that
memory-set
items
and
distractors intermix across trials
in
the VM
condition
but do not
intermix over
trials
in the CM
condition.
The
most
important
results
are
shown
in
Figure
2.
These results showed
that
the VM
conditions
were strongly
affected
by
load
and
were
quite
difficult;
the CM
conditions were
virtually
unaffected
by
load
and
were
all
easier than even
the
easiest
VM
condition.
It was
suggested
that
a
controlled, serial
search
was
operating
in the VM
conditions
and
that
a
qualitatively
different
process,
automatic detection,
was
operating
in the
CM
conditions.
The
results
of
Part
I/Experiment
1
were
analyzed
on the
basis
of the
accuracy
of the
detection responses.
Part
I/Experiment
2
utilized comparable conditions
but
presented
Table
1
Examples
of CM and VM
Trials
for
Four
Successive
Trials
Trial
Memory
set
Distractor
set
Target
Consistent
mapping
(CM)
1
2
3
4
1
2
3
4
7481
2583
1739
6S82
Varied
MJDG
CJKH
GMCH
JLKF
KGJCM
CHFLD
KGFDM
CMJKD
mapping
(VM)
CFHKL
LGDFM
DLFKJ
CDGHM
4
none
7
8
D
none
none
L
only
a
single
frame
on
each
trial;
accuracy
was
high
and the
results
were
analyzed
on
the
basis
of the
reaction time
of the
responses.
The
results
confirmed
those
of
Part
I/Experi-
ment
1, and a
quantitative model
was fit to
the VM
results
of
both experiments.
This
model
assumed
that
controlled
search
was
a
serial,
terminating comparison process
in
which
one first
compared
all
frame
items
against
one
memory-set item
before
switching
to
the
next memory-set
item.
Each compari-
son
and
each switch
was
assumed
to
require
some time
to be
executed.
The
success
of the
model
in fitting the
results
of
both
experi-
ments suggests
that
the
same search mech-
anisms underlie search experiments
that
uti-
lize
both accuracy
and
reaction time mea-
sures,
and
suggests
that
the
same search
mechanisms underlie performance
in
both
divided-attention
and
search paradigms.
The
vast
differences
between
results
of the
CM
and VM
conditions provided
a
basis
for
reorganizing
and
classifying
the
results
of
previous search
and
detection studies.
These
studies
fell
into
a
relatively simple organiza-
tion,
and
many perplexing
and
seemingly
contradictory results became explicable.
Part
I/Experiment
3
utilized
a
multiple-
target, multiple-frame procedure.
The
study
was
similar
to
Part
I/Experiment
1,
but the
subject
was
presented either zero, one,
or two
targets
per
trial
and was
required
to
report
the
number
of
detected
targets.
The
condi-
130
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
c/)
H
I
r-
LU
100
80
60
CONSISTENT
MAPPINGS
40
4,2)
.(4,1)
(4,4)
A"
(1,4)
O)
LU
O
LJ
100
80
60
VARIED
MAPPINGS
40 80
120
FRAME
TIME (msec)
120
200
400 600 800
FRAME
TIME (msec)
Figure
2.
Data
from
Experiment
1,
Part
I.
Hits
as a
function
of
frame
time
for
each
of the 12
conditions.
Three
frame
times were
utilized
for
each
condition.
The first
number
in
parentheses
indicates
the
memory-set size
and the
second number
in
parenthesis indicates
the
frame
size.
dons
of
greatest interest were those
in
which
two
targets
per
trial
were presented.
In
these
cases
the
spacing between
the two
targets
varied
from
0 to 4 (0
spacing indicates simul-
taneous
presentation;
spacings
of
1,
2, and 4
indicate
the
number
of
intervening
frame
plus
1).
Target
similarity
also
varied,
the
two
targets
being either physically identical
(II)
or
different
(NI).
The
results
for
Part
I/Experiment
3
showed markedly
different
patterns
for the
VM
and CM
conditions. Consider target
similarity
first. In the VM
conditions
de-
tection
of
identical targets
(II)
was
superior
to
detection
of
different
targets
(NI).
How-
ever,
in the CM
conditions,
the
reverse
was
true:
NI
detection
was
superior
to II
detection. Consider
target
spacing next.
In
the VM
conditions performance
was
lowest
when
the
targets occurred
in
successive
frames
(spacing
1).
However,
in the CM
conditions
performance
was
lowest when
the
targets
were
simultaneous
(spacing
0).
These
results
helped
emphasize
the
qualitative
difference
between
the
automatic detection processing
mode
presumed
to be
utilized
in the CM
conditions,
and the
controlled search mode
presumed
to be
utilized
in the VM
conditions.
In
certain
of the
studies
in
this paper,
we
shall utilize this multiple-target, multiple-
frame
procedure
and
will
infer
the
presence
of
automatic detection
or
controlled search
from
the
nature
of the
spacing
effect
and the
target-similarity
effect.
B.
Rationale
for the
Experiments
We
argued
in
Part
I
that
the
cause
of the
difference
between
the CM and VM
results
was the
consistency
of the
mapping (over
trials)
of the
memory-set items
and
dis-
tractors
to
responses.
We
argued
that
con-
sistent mapping leads
to the
development
of
automatic
detection, which enables auto-
matic-attention responses
to
become attached
to
the
memory-set
items.
The
automatic-at-
tention responses enable
the
serial, controlled
search
to be
bypassed
by a
parallel detection
process
unaffected
by
load. However, these
hypotheses must
be
further
tested
for the
following
reasons.
First,
the
fact
that
mem-
ory-set items were categorically distinct
from
the
distractors
was
confounded
with
the
consistency
of the
mapping. These factors
will
be
separated
in
Part
II/Experiments
1,
2,
and 3.
Second,
the
course
of
development
of
the
hypothesized automatic detection
process
was not
studied
in
Part
I. The
course
of
learning
will
be
traced
in
Part
II/Experi-
ments
1 and 3.
Third, there
was no
demon-
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
131
stration
in
Part
I
that
an
automatic-atten-
tion response
is
learned
in CM
paradigms.
Such
a
fact will
be
suggested
by
Part
II
/
Experiments
1 to 3 and
demonstrated
in
Part
II/Experiment
4.
Finally, although these
goals provide
an
initial justification
for the
present experiments, these studies
will
serve
an
even more important purpose
in
elu-
cidating
the
characteristics
and
development
of
automatic processing
and in
differentiating
automatic
from
controlled processing.
II. The
Development
of
Automatic Processing:
Perceptual Learning
A.
Perceptual Learning
and
Unlearning
in a
CM
Task
Using
Letters
Only
Experiment
1 of the
present series
is
simple
in
conception. With
the use of the
same basic
multiple-frame
paradigm used
in
Part
I/
Experiment
1
(see
Figure
1),
performance
is
examined
as a
function
of the
amount
of
training
under consistent-mapping conditions.
One
change
is
made, however: Both
the
dis-
tractor
set and the
memory ensemble consist
of
consonants.
This
procedure enables
us to
study
the
acquisition
of
automaticity when
the two
sets
are not
already-learned cate-
gories. Furthermore,
it had
been observed
informally
that
the use of
digits
and
con-
sonants
(in the CM
conditions
of
Part
I/
Experiment
1) led to a
relatively rapid
acquisition
of
automatic detection.
It was
felt
that
the use of
letters only
to
make
up
the two
sets
would slow down acquisition.
Values
of M and F
were chosen
so as to
make controlled processing
difficult
(M = 4,
F—2),
and a
frame
time
(/) of 200
msec
was
chosen
so as to
make performance
low
when
controlled processing
was
being utilized.
(These choices
are
justified
by the
data
in
Figure
2.)
Thus
it was
expected
that
per-
formance
would
be
quite poor
at the
start
of
training, when automatic detection
had not
been
learned
and
controlled search
had to be
used,
but
would improve markedly
as
auto-
matic detection developed.
I.
Method
The CM
presentation procedure
of
Part
I/Experi-
ment
1 was
utilized
(see
Figure
1). The
frame
size
was
always equal
to 2 and the
memory-set size
was
always equal
to 4. Two
disjoint
character
sets
were
used
for the
memory ensemble
and the
distractor
set.
One
consisted
of the
following
nine consonants:
B,
C, D, F, G, H, J, K, and L; the
other
con-
sisted
of the
following
nine consonants:
Q, R, S,
T,
V, W, X, Y, and Z.
There
were
four
new
subjects,
two of
each
sex,
all
naive
to our
tasks.1
Two
subjects
began training
with
the
consonants
from
the
second
half
of the
alphabet
'as
the
memory
ensemble,
and two
subjects
began
with
the
consonants
from
the first
half
of
the
alphabet
as the
memory ensemble. There were
five
blocks
of
trials
per
session, each containing
60
test trials. There were
no
practice
trials. Subjects
were
informed
at the
start
of the
experiment
con-
cerning
the
nature
of the
study
and the
composi-
tion
of the
memory ensemble
and the
distractor
set.
Each
trial
began
with
the
presentation
of a
memory
set (M = 4)
selected randomly
from
the
memory
ensemble.
During
the first
1,500
trials
of the
experiment
for
each
subject,
the
frame
time
was 200
msec;
during
the
following
600
trials,
120
msec.
After
these
2,100
trials
the
memory ensemble
and the
distractor
set
were switched
for
each subject
and
the
frame
time
was set
back
to 200
msec.
This
reversal
'Condition
was
then
run at the
frame
time
of
200
msec
for a
total
of
2,400
additional trials.
The
subjects
were
informed
of the
reversal
at the
time
of
the
switch.
The
subjects responded whenever
a
target
was
detected,
or
gave
a
negative response
at the end of
the
trial.
The
accuracy
of the
response
and the
reaction
time
for
hits were both recorded.
Sub-
jects heard
a
tone
signifying
an
error after
each
incorrect
response.
2.
Results
and
Discussion
The
results
are
presented
in
Figure
3. Re-
sults
for
each block, averaged across
sub-
jects,
are
graphed consecutively.
Thus,
the
graphed points
in
each interval
are
based
on
120
observations.
In the
initial group
of
1,500
trials,
the hit
rate
rose
from
just over
50%
to
about
90%,
while
the
false alarm rate dropped
from
about
12% to
about
3%
(and
the
reaction
1
Since
the
experiments
are not
reported
in
their
chronological
order,
we
list here
the
experiments
in
their
original
order
and
indicate
the
subjects
that
were
used
in
each. Experiment
1 was run
with
new
subjects.
Experiment
4 was run
next)
using
the
subjects
who
took
part
in
Experiment
3 of
Part
I.
Experiment
2 was
then
run on
three
of the
four
subjects
in
Experiment
4.
Experiment
3 was run
last,
on new
subjects.
132
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
100
EC/)
ot
80
0:=
60
HO:
2<
LU-J
40
20
2 o
X
850
o:
£
INITIAL LEARNING
REVERSED
LEARNING-
200
MSEC
FRAME
TIME
120
MSEC
FRAME
TIME
_
*tt*^*fi*.
600
1200
1800
2400
I-
2
650
g
550
UJ
CC
600
1200
1800
TRIAL NUMBER
2400
Figure
3.
Data
from
Experiment
1.
Initial
consistent-mapping learning
and
reversed
consistent-
mapping
learning
for
target
and
distractor
sets
taken
from
the first and
second halves
of the
alphabet.
Memory-set
size
=
4J
frame
size
=
2,
frame
times
are
shown. Percentage
of
hits, percentage
of
false
alarms,
and
mean reaction time
for
hits
are
graphed
as a
function
of
trial
number.
After
2,100
trials
the
target
and
distractor
sets
were
switched
with each
other.
time
for
hits
dropped
from
about
770
msec
to
about
670
msec).
It
seems plausible that
the
subjects adopted
a
controlled search strategy
at the
start
of
training. Some support
for
this hypothesis
is
found
in a
comparison
of the
data
from
this
study with
the
data
from
Experiment
3 of
Part
I
(though
different
subjects were
in-
volved
in the two
studies).
The hit and
false
alarm
probabilities
in the first 60
trials
of
the
present study were
.56
and
.13,
respec-
tively.
The
estimated probability
of
detecting
a
single target
and the
observed probability
of
giving
a
false
alarm when
no
target
was
present
were
.60 and
.18, respectively,
in the
VM
condition
of
Experiment
3b of
Part
I in
which
M was
equal
to 4 and F was
equal
to 2
(this condition
and the first 60
trials
of the
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
133
present
study were both
run at a
frame
time
of
200
msec).
These
two
conditions should
give similar results
if
controlled search
is
being
utilized
in
both studies;
in
fact,
the
probabilities
are
quite similar.
It
should
be
noted
that
the
subjects
all re-
ported extensive, attention-demanding
re-
hearsal
of the
memory
set
during about
the
first
600
trials
of
Experiment
1, but
they
gradually became unaware
of
rehearsal
or
other attention-demanding controlled pro-
cessing
after
this point. Both
the
objective
evidence
and
subjective reports, then, sug-
gest
that
the
subjects used controlled search
at the
start
of
Experiment
1 but
gradually
shifted
to
automatic detection.
The hit
rate
and
false
alarm
rate
after
1,500
trials appeared
to be
changing only
slowly,
but
this could have resulted
from
a
ceiling
effect.
The
frame
time
was
therefore
reduced
to 120
msec
for the
next
600
trials.
At
the
conclusion
of
this additional training,
the hit
rate
appears
to
have reached about
82%, while
the
false
alarm
rate
dropped
to
about
5%.
These results
may be
compared
with
the CM
results
from
Experiment
1 of
Part
I
(see Figure
2).
In
that
study, with
/ = 120
msec,
M
- 4, and F = 2, the CM hit
rate
was
about
92% and the
false
alarm rate
(not
shown)
was
about
5%.
Thus,
the
present results indicate
that
subjects
were utilizing
automatic
detection
by the end of the
initial 2,100 trials
of CM
training.
First,
they
performed
at a
level
not
far
from
that
of the
Part
I/Experiment
1
subjects
in the
comparable
CM
condition
with
the
same
frame
time. Furthermore,
the
present study utilized
a
larger memory
en-
semble
and
distractor
set
than
the VM
con-
ditions
of
Part
I/Experiment
1, a
fact
that
could
only have increased
the
task
difficulty.
Second,
from
the
Part
I/Experiment
1 VM
data
we can
estimate
that
a
frame
time
of
600-800
msec would have been required
to
achieve
a
similar level
of
performance
had
controlled search been used.
At
this stage
of the
experiment,
after
2,100
trials
of CM
training,
the
subjects could
be
expected
to
have
developed
a
well-learned
attention response
to the
members
of the
memory
ensemble.
If
such learning
is firmly
planted
in
long-term memory
and if the at-
tention response occurs automatically,
then
it
should prove very
difficult
for the
subject
to
alter
or
unlearn
his
automatic response
in
any
short period
of
time.
To
test
this
hy-
pothesis
we
reversed
the
memory ensemble
and
the
distractor
set for
each subject (and
set the
frame
time back
to 200
msec).
We
hypothesized
that
automatic detection would
prove impossible
and
that
the
subject would
be
forced
to
revert
to
controlled search.
The
results
of the
reversal were quite
dramatic.
The hit
rate just
after
reversal
fell
to a
level well below
that
seen
at the
start
of
training when
the
subjects were com-
pletely unpracticed. Very gradually there-
after
the hit
rate recovered,
so
that
after
2,400
trials
of
reversal training, performance
reached
a
level about equal
to
that
seen
after
1,500
trials
of
original training.
In
summary,
the
original training resulted
in
quite strong
negative
transfer, rather than positive
transfer.
Subject's verbal reports indicated
that
a
shift
back
to
controlled search occurred after
reversal. Initially,
before
reversal,
all
sub-
jects reported rehearsing
the
memory
set
during
each
trial.
After
the 2nd day
(600
trials) subjects reported
that
they were
no
longer
rehearsing
and
only glanced
at the
memory
set. However, subjects reported
that
after
reversal
they
tried
various
methods
to
perform
the
task
and
eventually ended
up
rehearsing
the
memory-set items again, though
this rehearsal also decreased
after
a
week
of
postreversal practice.
This
pattern
of re-
ports could indicate
a
shift
from
controlled
search
to
automatic detection during original
learning,
then
a
shift
after
reversal
to
con-
trolled
search,
and
then
finally a
return
to
automatic detection.
What might
be the
cause
of the
negative
transfer
after
reversal?
If the
reversal caused
subjects
to
revert
to
controlled search, then
it
might have been expected
that
perform-
ance would
fall
to the
level seen
at the
start
of
original learning (when controlled search
was
presumably
utilized).
The
actual results
suggest
that
controlled search
is
hindered
when
the
distractors
are
items
that
subjects
have been previously trained
to
respond
to as
134
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
CM
targets
(i.e., items
that,
due to
previous
training, give rise
to
automatic-attention
responses).
This
hypothesis
is
verified
in
Experiment
4, to be
reported
later.2
Beyond
the
poor performance
at the
start
of
reversal learning, negative transfer
is
also
evidenced
by the
extremely large amount
of
training needed
to
overcome
the
effects
of
orig-
inal learning.
After
about
900
trials
of
original
practice
the hit
rate
reached about 90%.
However,
it
took
about
2,100 trials
to
return
to
this performance level
after
reversal.
Ap-
parently
the
necessity
to
unlearn
the at-
tention responses
to the
previous memory-set
items,
or the
necessity
to
overcome some
learned
inhibition
to the
previous
distractors,
or
both, causes
a
reduction
in the
rate
of
acquisition
of
automatic processing
after
reversal.
It
would
be
interesting
to
know whether
negative
transfer
would obtain
in
several
variations
of our
paradigm.
For
example,
in
the
present study,
the
initial training might
have caused
the
memory
set to
become
a
learned
category,
but
probably
did not
cause
categorization
of the
distractor set.
If the
distractor
set was a
well-known category,
would
reversal
still
cause
a
severe
perform-
ance drop? Furthermore,
it
could
be
argued
that
the
initial training
did not
continue long
enough
for the
negative transfer
to
reach
full
strength.
The
amount
of
negative transfer
might
depend
on the
amount
of
overtraining
during
initial learning. Such
a
view
has
been
put
forth
for
certain verbal
and
animal learn-
ing
situations (see
Handler,
1962, 196S;
Jung, 1965
for two
views
on the
subject).
Thus
it
might
be
asked whether
performance
drops would
be
caused
by
reversal
if the
original
automatic detection responses were
greatly overlearned.
These
two
questions
are
answered
by
Experiment
3.
B. The
Reversal
of
Consonants
and
Digits
for
Well-Practiced
Subjects
Throughout
the
series
of
experiments
in
Part
I, the
memory ensemble
in the CM
condition
was
always
the
same, either digits
or
consonants,
and no
member
of the CM
memory
ensemble
was
ever
a
distractor, even
in
the VM
conditions.
The VM
conditions
always consisted solely
of
items
from
the
distractor
set in the CM
conditions.
Thus
a
subject
at the
conclusion
of the
series
of
Part
I
studies
who had
been searching, say,
for
consonants
in
digit
distractors,
had
never
been given
a
consonant
as a
distractor:
Whenever
a
consonant
had
appeared,
it was
a
target.
These
facts naturally suggested
a
study
in
which
the
roles
of
consonants
and
digits were reversed
for
these subjects.
1.
Method
The
procedure
was
identical
to the
multiple-
frame,
multiple-target paradigm
of
Experiment
3a
of
Part
I
(whose results were described
in the
Introduction)
in
most
respects,
except
that
the
memory
ensemble
and the
distractor
set
were
switched
in
both
the CM and VM
conditions. Thus
in
the CM
condition
the two
subjects
who had
been
searching
for
consonants
in
digit distractors
now
searched
for
digits
in
consonant distractors;
in
the VM
condition these
two
subjects
had
been
searching
for
digits
in
digits
and now
searched
for
consonants
in
consonants.
These
contingencies were
reversed
for the
remaining
two
subjects
who had
previously been searching
for
digits
in
consonant
distractors
in
their
CM
conditions.
The
frame
time
(/) was 60
msec
for the CM
conditions
and 200
msec
for the VM
conditions.
Just
as in
Experiment
3
a of
Part
I, M was
equal
to 2, and F was
equal
to
2;
zero
targets
were pre-
sented
on one
fourth
of the
trials,
one
target
was
presented
on one
fourth
of the
trials
and two
targets were presented
on one
half
of the
trials.
When
two
targets were presented, they were
either
identical (II)
or
nonidentical
(NI),
and the
spac-
ing
between them
was
either
0, 1, 2, or 4.
Each
subject
was
given
12
blocks
of 132
trials
in
each
of
the VM and CM
conditions.
The VM and CM
blocks alternated.
Upon
'Completion
of the
initial
set of 24
blocks,
the
frame
time
was
increased
to 120
msec
and an
additional
18
blocks
of CM
trials were
run
for
each
subject.
2
One
might hypothesize
that
the
basic compari-
son
rate
of
controlled
search
is
altered
and
slowed
after
reversal,
thereby accounting
for the
negative
transfer.
Two
facts argue against this hypothesis.
Experiment
2
shows
that
controlled search proceeds
at an
unchanged
rate
even when
all
items consist
of
targets
from
previous
CM
conditions. Experiment
4
shows
that
even
one CM
target presented
in a
to-be-ignored spatial location greatly reduces detec-
tion
of a
simultaneously presented
target
in a
to-be-
attended location.
This
result suggests that reversal
performance
is
harmed because attention
tends
to
be
drawn
to the
distractors.
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
135
It
should
be
noted
that
the
subjects
for
this
ex-
periment
had had roughly
20,000
trials
of
practice
in
the
various
CM and VM
conditions
in
earlier
studies
(see
Footnote
1).
2.
Results
and
Discussion
The
results
are
depicted
in
Figure
4. A
simple
correction
for
guesses
and
false
alarms
has
been carried
out on the raw
data,
as
described
in
Part
I, but
this
correction
did
not
affect
the
qualitative features
of the re-
sults.
The
circles
in the
left-hand
panel give
the
comparable
VM
prereversal
data
from
Experiment
3a of
Part
I
(/=
200
msec),
while
the
circles
in the
center panel give
the
comparable
CM
prereversal data
from
Ex-
periment
3c of
Part
I (/ = ,60
msec);
the
triangles
in the two
left-most
panels give
the
data
from
the
present experiment (see
Foot-
note
1).
The
most dramatic
findings of
Experiment
2
are
shown
in the
middle panel
of
Figure
4,
which gives
the CM
reversal
results.
It is
evident
that performance drops
off
markedly
after
reversal. Estimated percentage
of
detec-
tion
of two
targets
is
about
80%
prior
to
reversal
and
about
20%
after
reversal. Fur-
thermore,
these data represent
12
blocks
of
training
over which very little,
if
any,
re-
covery
was
taking place.
It is
interesting
to
note
that
all of the
subjects predicted
that
no
change
in
their performance would occur
after
the
consonants
and
digits were reversed,
and the
subjects were
uniformly
startled
and
even dismayed
by the
extreme
difficulty
of
the
reversed
task.
These results resolve
the
confounding
be-
tween
categorization
and the
mapping condi-
tions. Even when there
is a
well-learned
and
well-practiced categorical
difference
between
memory
ensemble
and
distractor set, reversal
in
the CM
paradigm gives rise
to
marked
impairment
of
performance. Thus, categorical
differences
between memory ensemble
and
distractor
set
cannot
be the key
factor under-
lying
the
utilization
of
automatic detection.
Rather
it is the
consistency
of
mapping
that
underlies
the
acquisition
of
automatic detec-
tion.
Consider
next
the VM
data given
in the
REVERSAL CONDITIONS
IUU
u
LU
UJOT
80
01-
,
llJ
I-O
zee
60
UJ<
DU
a!
;J
40
o<
£
1-
h
'
' ' ' '
-
a A
.
Nf^.
i i i i i i
°o
"A
^^°~
A
^A__^
*^A
Ti-
i i i i i i
D
Q
i
v-:
t
i i i i
i
w
0'I24
0124
0124
SPACING
SPACING
SPACING
0 I
01
2
01
NUMBER
OF
TARGETS
Figure
4.
Data
from Experiment
2.
Probabilities
of
correctly
detecting
one
target
when
one was
presented,
or two
targets
when
two
were presented,
as a
function
of
spacing,
for
each
condition.
The
data
shown
have
been
adjusted
to
remove
effects
of
guessing
and
false
alarms.
The
observed
percentage
of
"none"
responses
to
no-target
trials
is
also
shown.
Circles show
the
estimated
percentage
of
detection
prior
to
reversal
(data
from
Experiment
3,
Part
I),
while
triangles
indicate
performance
after
reversal
in
Experiment
2. The
squares
in the
right-hand
panel
show
post-
reversal
detection
from
Experiment
2
after frame
time
was
increased
to 120
msec.
The
open
symbols
and
dashed lines indicate
the II
conditions
(identical
targets);
the
closed
circles
and
solid
lines indicate
the
NI
conditions
(different
targets).
(VM =
varied
mapping;
CM =
consistent
mapping;
f =
frame time,
in
msec.)
136
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
left-hand
panel
of
Figure
4. It is
evident
that
the
switch
from
consonants
to
digits
(or
vice versa
for
other subjects)
had
almost
no
effect.
The
levels
of
performance remain
the
same
as in
Experiment
3a of
Part
I, and
the
qualitative pattern
of
results
is
still about
the
same, showing
the
pattern indicative
of
controlled search.
That
is,
worst
perform-
ance occurred
at
spacing
1, and
performance
was
worst when
the
targets were
different
(NI).
These
effects
indicate
the
presence
of
controlled search.
In
summary,
a
change
of
the
character
set
used
in the VM
conditions
had no
appreciable
effect
under
the
present
conditions.
These
results
are
most
interesting,
be-
cause
after
reversal
the
memory ensemble
and the
distractor
set
both consisted
of
items that
had
come
to
elicit automatic-
attention responses
in
previous training.
In
the
reversal conditions
of
Experiment
1 we
saw
that
making
the
distractor
set but not the
memory ensemble consist
of
such
"automatic"
items resulted
in
considerable negative trans-
fer.
In the
present
experiment
it is
evident
that
performance
is not
worse
after
reversal
when
subjects have been trained
to re-
spond
to
both
the
targets
and
distractors
in
previously
CM
training.
These results might
be
expected
on the
basis
of the
following
reasoning.
Just
after
reversal
in
both Experiment
1 and in the CM
conditions
of
Experiment
2 the
subjects were
using
controlled search.
In
Experiment
1,
after
reversal,
the
automatic-attention responses
to the
distractor items tended
to
draw
at-
tention away
from
target items
to
which sub-
jects
had not
previously been trained
to re-
spond
and
hence
to
order
the
controlled search
in
such
a way
that
the
distractor items were
compared earlier
in the
search. Thus per-
formance
was
impaired. (This hypothesis
is
confirmed
in
Experiment
4.)
However,
in the
present experiment, after reversal,
all
items,
distractors
and
targets alike, gave rise
to
attention responses, which could
be
expected
to
cancel each other
out on the
average.
That
is,
there should
be no
selective bias
to
compare
distractors
prior
to
targets since
both
types
of
items give rise
to
equivalent
attention responses.
Thus,
the
controlled
search operates normally, with targets com-
pared
in a
random order
of
search,
and
per-
formance
is
equivalent
to
that
seen prior
to
the
switch
of
character sets.
In
short,
the use in a
search task
of
stimuli
that
elicit automatic-attention responses (de-
veloped through previous
CM
training)
can
(a)
improve performance over
that
seen
in
the
usual
VM
conditions
if
these stimuli
are
memory-set
items
but not
distractors,
in
which
case automatic detection will
be
uti-
lized;
(b)
leave performance unchanged
from
that seen
in the
usual
VM
conditions
if
these stimuli
are
both memory-set items
and
distractor items,
in
which case normal con-
trolled search will
be
utilized;
and (c) im-
pair
performance
from
that
seen
in the
usual
VM
conditions
if
these stimuli
are
distractors
but
not
memory-set items,
in
which case
subjects
will
utilize controlled search
that
will
be
reduced
in
effectiveness
by
automatic
attending
to
distractors.
Let us now
turn
to the
results
of the final
reversed
CM
training blocks.
We
suspected
that
the
subjects
in the
reversed
CM
condi-
tion
(the
middle panel) were reverting back
to the use of
controlled search,
but
perform-
ance levels were
so low
that
it was
difficult
to
interpret
the
pattern
of
results. Thus
frame
time
in the CM
reversal condition
was
increased
to 120
msec,
and 18
blocks
of
addi-
tional
CM
training were run.
The
results
of the
trials with
/ = 120
msec
are
represented
by the
squares
in the
right-
hand panel
of
Figure
4. It
should
be
noted
first
that
the
pattern
of the
results
is in-
dicative
of
controlled search
in
that
the
worst
performance
occurs
at
spacing
1.
However,
the
results
do not
show
the
target-similarity
effect
that
we
have been
led to
expect
(in
Experiment
3 of
Part
I) for
controlled search:
Results
for
conditions using
II
target pairs
are
not
superior
to
those using
NI
target
pairs. Furthermore,
the
overall level
of
per-
formance
is
higher than
one
would expect
for
the VM
conditions.
At a
frame
time
of
120
msec performance
is
higher than
that
seen
for the VM
conditions
at a
frame
time
of
200
msec
(in the
left-hand panel
of
Fig-
ure
4).
The
results
can be
explained simply
and
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
137
elegantly
by the
hypothesis
that
the
sub-
jects were carrying
out a
controlled search
for
the
category
as a
whole.
The
Experi-
ment
2
reversal condition
differed
from
the
Part
I/Experiment
3 VM
conditions
in
that
the
memory-set items
and the
distractor
items
in the
present instance were cate-
gorically
different
(numbers
and
letters).
This
categorical
difference
obviously
did not
allow
the
subjects
to
utilize automatic detec-
tion
after
reversal. However,
the
categorical
difference
could very well have helped
the
subjects
to
adopt
a
more
efficient
controlled
search. Suppose
the
subject ignored
the
specific
members
of the
memory
set and
searched instead
for any
instance
of the
cate-
gory
"numbers"
(or
"letters"
as the
case
may
be).
Then only
one
comparison would
be
needed
for
each display item
in
each
frame,
regardless
of
memory-set
size.
Compared with
the
case when
a
categoriza-
tion
is
unavailable,
a
controlled search
for
the
category
of
each input saves search time
in
two
ways.
First,
there
is no
need
for
mul-
tiple memory-set comparisons
for
each
frame
item. Second, there
is no
need
for
time-
consuming switches between memory-set
items.
Thus
performance improves consider-
ably.
Furthermore,
the
distinction between
identical
and
nonidentical multiple targets
is
no
longer
meaningful—both
targets
are
sim-
ply
category members, regardless
of
their
physical similarity. Thus
no
difference
be-
tween
the II and
NI
conditions would
be
predicted.
The
results
in
Figure
4
support
these contentions.
As
far as we can
tell, through
a
block-by-
block
analysis
of the
reversal results,
the
sub-
jects
did not
begin
to
recover
any
appreciable
degree
of
automatic detection even
after
30
blocks
of CM
reversal training. Undoubtedly,
the
difficulty
in
relearning
(or
unlearning)
is
related
to the
great
amount
of
overtrain-
ing
with
the
original mapping
of
stimuli
to
responses. Eventually, however, were
the
reversal experiment continued,
we
would
ex-
pect
automatic detection
to
develop once
again.
In
summary, then,
the
results
of
Experi-
ment
2
show
that
a
switch
of
character sets
does
not
affect
VM
performance,
but a
switch
of
memory ensemble
and
distractor
set
causes
a
great decrement
in
performance
in the CM
conditions.
The CM
decrement occurs despite
the
categorical
difference
between
the
mem-
ory-set items
and
distractor items (letters
vs.
numbers).
It is
argued
that
the
subjects,
after
reversal, adopt
a
controlled search
strategy
in
which they search
for the
category
as a
whole
rather than search
for the in-
dividual members
of the
category.
The
results
of
Experiments
1 and 2
could
hardly have provided
a
more dramatic demon-
stration
of the
qualitative
difference
between
automatic detection
and
controlled search,
and of the
dependence
of the
search mecha-
nism
on the
nature
of the
training procedures
(i.e.,
the
consistency
of the
stimulus-response
mapping).
The
results
of
Experiments
1 and 2
also
demonstrate
the
long-term nature
of the
learning underlying
the
automatic process.
Experiment
1
showed
that
even original
learning
of an
automatic response could
take
thousands
of
trials
of
practice
to
develop
if
a
previously known categorization
did not
distinguish
the
memory ensemble
and the
dis-
tractor set. Relearning
after
reversal
was
even
slower
to
develop. Experiment
2
showed
that
relearning
after
reversal could
be
very
retarded even when
the
sets were categorized,
as
long
as the
original automatic detection
process
was
well overlearned.
C. The
Role
of
Categorization
in
Controlled
Search
and in the
Development
oj
Automaticity
The
role played
by
categorization
in
con-
trolled search
and in the
development
of
automatic detection
is
suggested
by
results
of
the
preceding experiments,
but it is an
important
enough
concept
to be
examined
in
a
separate experiment. Furthermore,
the
nature
of the
process
by
which categories
are
learned
is
also worth studying.
These
considerations underlay
the
design
of
Experi-
ment
3. We
decided
to use new
subjects
and
to
train them
in two
conditions,
in
neither
of
which were
the
memory
and
distractor
sets preexperimentally categorized.
The first
condition
was
designed
to
induce controlled
138
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
100
S
80
60
40
20
M=2
M=4
o
Categorical
a
Mixed
f =
350
0-4
5-9
10-14
15-19
20-24
^25
28
30-32
33-35
f
VM
Training
CM
Training
SESSIONS
Figure
S.
Data
from
Experiment
3.
Estimated percentage
of
detection
of
both targets,
as a
func-
tion
of
session number, averaged over spacing
and
target-similarity conditions,
for
each
of the
four
main conditions.
At
Session
25
training
was
switched
to a CM
procedure.
At
Session
30, the
frame
time
was
reduced
to 160
msec.
(VM =
varied
mapping;
CM =
consistent
mapping;
M =
memory-set
size;
f =
frame
time,
in
msec).
search
in
circumstances
such
that
a
cate-
gorization
could
not
develop.
The
second
con-
dition
was
also
designed
to
induce
controlled
search,
but in
circumstances
such
that
a
categorization
of the
memory
sets
could
be
learned.
Both
of
these
conditions
utilized
a
varied-mapping
procedure
to
prevent
auto-
matic
detection,
and
both
were
followed
by
a
series
of CM
trials,
during
which
trials
automatic
detection
developed.
Thus,
the
CM
trials
traced
the
course
of
development
of
automatic
detection
when
a
categoriza-
tion
was,
or was
not,
present
at the
start
of
CM
training.
1.
Method
The
subjects each took
part
in two
conditions
called
mixed
and
categorical,
run in
different
blocks.
In
each condition
the
memory
ensemble
and
dis-
tractor
set
were
drawn
from
a
total
of
eight con-
sonants.
The two
eight-consonant
sets
did not
over-
lap.
The
sets were
{GMFP,
CNHD}
and
{RVJZ,
BWTX}.
The
assignment
of
these
two
sets
to the
two
conditions
was
permuted across subjects.
Both conditions used
a VM
procedure
similar
in
certain respects
to
that
of
Experiment
2 of
this
report
and
Experiment
3 of
Part
I. The
multiple-
target, multiple-frame paradigm
was
used with
/ 250
msec
per
frame.
Only
12
frames were used
on
each
trial,
with
all
targets
appearing
on
frames
3
through
10.
Frame
size
was
equal
to 2 in all
con-
ditions,
and two
memory-set sizes
were
used
in
different
blocks:
M = 2 and
Af
=
4.
In the
mixed condition,
the
members
of the
memory
set
were chosen randomly
on
each trial
from
the
ensemble
of
eight consonants;
the
distractor
set
consisted
of
four
items
randomly
chosen
from
those consonants
not
used
in the
memory set.
These
four
.distractors
were chosen randomly
to fill the
nontarget, nonmask, positions
in the
various frames
of
the
trial.
The key
feature
of the
mixed
condition
was
the
fact
that
memory-set items
and
distractors
were
randomly intermixed
from
trial
to
trial.
In the
categorical
.condition,
the
eight ensemble
items were divided
into
two
sets
of
four
that
re-
mained disjoint throughout
the
experiment
for
each
subject.
The
sets
of
four
are
indicated
by the
posi-
tion
of the
comma
in the
above
listing
of the
con-
sonants making
up
each set. Note
that
the two
sets
of
four
were chosen
to be
highly confusable
with
each other
in the
sense
that
pairs
of
visually con-
fusable
letters were
separated
into
the two
sets.
On
a
given
trial
one of
these sets
of
four
was
chosen
randomly
to be the
distractor set,
and the
memory-
set
items were then chosen randomly
from
the
remaining set. Thus,
in the
categorical condition
there
were just
two
disjoint categories
of
items,
one
from
which
the
memory
set was
drawn,
and
one
that
served
as the
distractor
set. However,
the
category
that
served
as the
distractor
set
varied
randomly
from
trial
to
trial.
Thus,
'the
procedure
still
involved
a
varied mapping,
but the
categories
were never intermixed
and
could eventually become
well learned.
To
make
it
easier
for the
subjects
to
learn
the
categories,
the
members
of the
memory
set
pre-
140
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
the
results
of the M 2 and M
=
4
condi-
tions
converge (indicating category learning
and
the use of a
categorical search
strategy).
In the
mixed condition performance
in the
M
= 4
condition remains much lower than
in
the M = 2
condition,
but
performance
in
both
of
these
is
worse than
in
either
cate-
gorical condition (suggesting
that
the
pres-
ence
of
well-known
categories
reduces
the
effective
memory-set size
to 1).
To
ascertain whether controlled search
was
being
utilized
in
these various
VM
conditions
and to
determine
the
nature
of
that
search,
it
is
necessary
to
consider
the
spacing functions
shown
in
Figure
6.
This
figure
gives
the
spacing
functions
for
Sessions
19-23,
when
performance
had
stabilized, averaged across
subjects.
For
both
M = 2 and M
=
4
con-
ditions,
the
mixed condition shows
the
pat-
tern
we
have come
to
expect
for
controlled
search, with performance worse
at
spacing
1
and
worse
for
different
targets
(NI).
Performance
in the
categorical condition
was
equivalent when
M = 2 and M 4.
The
usual performance impairment
at
spacing
1
occurs
for
identical targets (II),
but
when
the two
targets
differ
(NI),
performance
in
the
spacing
0
condition
is
greatly impaired,
a
result
not
seen
in
previous
VM
conditions.
Furthermore,
the II and NI
conditions show
only
a
small
difference
except
at
spacing
0.
The
results
from
the
mixed condition con-
form
to the
pattern expected
for
controlled
search
in
three respects; When
M
=
4
per-
formance
is
worse than when
M
2;
there
is
a
performance
reduction
at
spacing
1;
and
performance
is
worse
for NI
than
II
con-
ditions.
The
results
from
the
categorical condition
were
unexpected
in
some respects
but are
explicable
in
terms
of an
hypothesis that
categories were learned
and
utilized
in
con-
trolled
search.
Let us
assume that
the
pres-
ence
of a
known category allows
one to
com-
pare
the
category
of the
memory
set to the
category
of any
given
display
item
in a
single
operation. Then
the M = 2
conditions should
not
differ
from
the M 4
conditions,
and in
fact
both conditions should show performance
levels equivalent
to
those expected
for a
memory-set
size
of 1 in a
normal mixed con-
dition.
This
reasoning explains
why the
cate-
gorical conditions
are
both superior
to the
best mixed condition, which uses
a
memory-
set
size
of
2.
The
category hypothesis, however, sug-
gests
that
target similarity should
not
matter,
so
that
the II and NI
functions should
be
identical.
To
explain
the
spacing
0
results
without abandoning
the
category
model,
we
suggest
the
following
hypothesis. Suppose
that when
a
subject locates
a
target category,
he
or she
briefly
switches
to an
item mode,
perhaps
to
check
that
the
input
is
truly
a
category
member.
If the
second item
in the
frame
is
identical, then
it
will
be
found
in an
item-comparison
mode,
but if
nonidentical,
it
will
be
located only
if the
subject reverts
quickly
enough
to a
categorical-comparison
mode.
By the
next
frame,
the
reversion
to a
categorical mode
is
complete,
so the
various
functions
tend
to
converge.
Why
would sub-
jects tend
to
switch
to an
item mode?
The
categories
were constructed
so as to be ex-
tremely
confusable with each other. Perhaps
category
encoding
is
learned
under
these
cir-
cumstances
but
remains somewhat error
prone. Then
it
would
be
logical
to
check
any
target category
by
using
an
item mode.
Whatever
the
explanation
for the
details
of
the
spacing
function,
the
data
as a
whole
make
a
strong case
that
categories have been
learned
in the
categorical condition during
VM
training,
and
that
the
presence
of
cate-
gories
in a VM
situation allows
the
subject
to
adopt
a
simpler
and
more
efficient
form
of
controlled search.
The
argument
that
controlled search
is
operating
in
these conditions would
be
sub-
stantiated
if it
could
be
demonstrated
that
performance
improves when
the
subjects
switched
to CM
training.
We
turn next
to
the CM
procedure.
3.
Method
Following
the
24th session
of VM
training,
a CM
procedure
was
initiated.
In the
categorical condi-
tion,
one of the
categories
was fixed
thenceforth
as the
memory ensemble,
and the
other category
was fixed
thereafter
as the
distractor set.
In the
mixed
condition,
a set of 4 was
chosen
and was
fixed
thereafter
as the
memory
ensemble—the
re-
maining
4
items
were
fixed
thereafter
as the
dis-
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
141
tractor set.
The two
sets
of 4
used
in the
mixed
condition
were
those indicated
by the
position
of
the
comma
in the
listing
of the two
sets
of
eight
consonants:
{GMFP,
CNHD}
and
(RVJZ,
BWTX}.
In
other
words,
these were
the
same
sets that
had
been
used
as
categories
in the
categorical
conditions
for
other
subjects. This
CM
training
procedure
was
identical
for
both
conditions
and
followed
in
other
respects
the
procedures
used
in the
preceding
VM
conditions.
Each
subject
was
given
at
least
10
sessions
of VM
training.
The
frame
time
after
CM
Session
4
(Session
24
overall)
was
reduced
to 160
msec
and
after
CM
Session
11
(Session
35
overall)
was
.reduced
again
to 120
msec
(at the
time
of
this
writing
this study
had not
been
completed).
4.
Results
and
Discussion
of
the CM
Conditions
The
changes over sessions following
the
switch
to CM
training
are
depicted
in
Figure
5.
Consider
first the
results
of
Sessions
25
through
28,
which immediately
followed
the
switch
to CM
training. Quite clearly,
a
very
rapid
and
dramatic rise
in
performance took
place,
and
furthermore,
the
results
of the
mixed
and
categorical
conditions tend
to
converge.
By
Session
28 the
performance
level
in the
mixed condition
had
risen
to
over
90%
and in the
categorical condition
had
risen
to
over 95%. Since
the
subjects were
clearly
approaching
ceiling,
the
frame time
was
then reduced
to 160
msec
and CM
train-
ing
continued
for six
additional
sessions.
Note
that
performance
was
still improving
and
that
the
mixed
and
categorical,
and M
= 2 and M
=
4,
conditions
effectively
con-
verged.
It is
interesting
to
note
that
during
VM
training,
20-25
sessions were necessary
before
the M = 2 and M = 4
performance levels
be-
came
equal
in the
categorical condition. Thus
one
might tentatively conclude
that
category
encoding
in the
present experimental context
required
20-25 sessions
to
develop.
Thus
category
encoding would
be
unlikely
to de-
velop
for the
mixed
condition
in
just
four
CM
sessions.
Yet
after
only
four
sessions
of
CM
training,
the
performance level
in the
mixed
condition rose dramatically
and ap-
proached
that
of the
categorical
condition.
This
result suggests that automatic detection
de-
veloped
in the
mixed condition
in the
absence
of
a
well-learned category
(at
least
during
the first
four
sessions
of CM
training).
In
other
words,
the
learning
of a
category
is
apparently
not a
necessary
prerequisite
for
the
development
of
automatic detection.
Figure
7
shows
the
spacing
functions
for
the six
sessions
(30-35)
of CM
training
run
at a
160-msec
frame
time.
The
results
are
averaged over subjects
and
sessions.
In ad-
dition
the M 2 and M
=
4
conditions
are
lumped together
since
they
did not
differ.
Somewhat
to our
surprise
the
pattern
is al-
most identical
to
that
seen
for the
categorical
condition
during
the
latter
stages
of VM
training,
except
that
the
performance level
is
much higher.
The
comparable
pattern
from
the CM
conditions
of
Part
I/Experiment
3
was
quite
different:
Detection
was
about
equal
in all
conditions except
for a
depressed
detection
rate
when spacing
was 0 and
when
the
targets were identical.
At
spacing
0 the
present data clearly show performance
to be
higher when
the
targets
were
identical.
Disregarding
the
shape
of the
spacing
functions,
the
large improvement
in
perform-
ance when
CM
training commenced does sug-
gest
that
the
subjects were learning auto-
matic detection.
An
hypothesis
that
reconciles
these facts suggests
that
the
subjects were
using
automatic
detection
to
locate
the first
target,
and
were then reverting
to
controlled
search
to
check
the
accuracy
of the
target
detection.
Some time
may be
lost
before
the
subjects
revert
to
automatic detection.
Thus
the
advantage
of II at
spacing
0
would
be
due
to the
temporary
use of a
controlled
search.
This
hypothesis
is
similar
to
that
used
to
explain
the
categorical results
in the
VM
conditions (Figure
6). In
both
cases,
a
tendency
to
recheck
the first
detected item
may
have
led to an
alteration
in
search
strategy.
The
reason
may be the
same
in
both
cases: Rechecking
of
located targets
may
have been induced
by the
stimulus
sets,
which
were chosen
so
that
the
memory
set
and
distractor
set
were maximally visually
confusable.
Thus,
both
automatic
detection
and
category encoding
may
have been some-
what
error
prone,
thereby
requiring
recheck-
ing
in an
item mode.
We are
currently
ex-
ploring this hypothesis.
In
summary,
a
number
of
conclusions
can
142
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
I/)
1
\J\J
«-
**
c
.
i-
80
d
0
•So
Estimate
detection
-P*
cr>
O O
g
-
-
II
N
I
a
*
i
0
°
^x
^p=Z^^
-
.^.^
^^.~-^"m
Categorical
~
Mixed
CM,f
=
160MSEC
i i
i
i
i
1012
4
i
Spacing
of two
targets
t
N
umber
of
targets
Figure
7.
Data
from
Experiment
3.
Estimated
percentage
of
detection
as a
function
of
number,
spacing,
and
similarity
of
targets
for CM
Sessions
30-35,
averaged over
memory-set
size
(which
had
no
effect)
for
all
conditions.
(CM
consistent
mapping;
f =
frame
time;
II =
identical
targets;
NI
=
nonidentical
targets.)
be
drawn
from
the
results
of
Experiment
3.
First,
arbitrary collections
of
characters
can
be
learned
as
categories.
For
this learning
to
occur,
it is
necessary
that
the
categories
re-
main consistently
denned
across trials,
but
not
necessary
that
the
same category
re-
main
the
target
set
across trials. Second,
the
learning
of a
category
in a VM
search para-
digm
enables
the
subject
to
adopt
a
more
efficient
controlled search,
one in
which
the
category
of the
memory
set may be
com-
pared
in a
single
operation
to the
category
of
a
display item, thus eliminating
the
effects
of
variations
in
memory-set size. Third,
a
switch
to a CM
training procedure results
in
fairly rapid acquisition
of
automatic
de-
tection, with
a
concomitant improvement
in
performance
for
both
the
categorical
and
non-
categorical stimuli. These conclusions sup-
port
those
suggested
by the
results
of Ex-
periment
2 in all
important respects.
D.
What
is a
Category?
A
Discussion
and
Selective
Review
1.
Some Hypotheses Concerning Categories
Experiments
2 and 3
showed
how
search
benefits
from
the
learning
of a
categorical
distinction
between memory ensemble
and
distractor set.
But
what
is a
category? Most
generally,
we may
define
any
object
in
mem-
ory
that
refers
to
(stands for, consists
of)
any two or
more objects
in
memory
as a
category. Verbal labels
are one
common
class
of
categories,
but
other types
of
categories
might
also exist.
For
example,
a
visual symbol
might represent several objects.
Of
course,
a
category
in
general need
not
exist
in a
form
similar
to any of the
sensory modalities.
A
completely abstract node could represent
two
or
more objects
in
memory.
With
respect
to
search tasks,
the
import-
ance
of
categories
is
clear. When
all of the
members
of a
memory
set are
members
of
the
same
category
and no
distractors
are in
that
category, then
a
controlled search
can
bypass
the
individual memory-set items
and
utilize
comparisons
that
involve matching
the
single category against
the
category
of
each
display item.
For
category search
to be
uti-
lized
effectively,
however,
the
category must
be
learned well enough
that
each displayed
element will
be
encoded
automatically
and
consistently according
to its
category.
Of
course
other features, such
as the
element's
name
and
shape, will also
be
encoded,
but
only
the
category
feature
is
necessary
for a
category search.
We
suggest
that
the
category
coding
must
be
automatic, because
if it
were
not,
a
search
of
long-term memory would
have
to be
carried
out to
identify
the
cate-
gory;
the
time taken
for
such
a
long-term
search would almost certainly wipe
out any
gains
that
are due to the
reduction
in
num-
ber of
comparisons allowed
by the
category
search
(at
least
for
small memory-set
sizes).
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
143
Note
that
automatic category encoding
is
not
the
same
as
automatic detection. Auto-
matic
detection
refers
to the
case
when
a
stimulus
gives rise
to an
automatic-attention
response that bypasses
the
need
for a
serial
search through either memory
or the
display.
Automatic
category encoding
refers
to the
case when
the
stimulus gives rise auto-
matically
to a
node representing
the
category
of
the
stimulus. However, without
an at-
tention
response attached
to the
stimulus
node
or the
category node,
the
presence
of
the
category would have
to be
deduced
through
a
serial search
of the
displayed
elements.
Our
studies have shown
that
an
automatic
category
encoding
of
arbitrary
collections
of
characters (consonants,
in the
case
of
Experi-
ment
3)
can
develop.
The
cause
of the
learn-
ing
is not yet
clear. Some subjects
in
early
versions
of
Experiment
3
never showed
any
evidence
of
categorical
search
(usually
sub-
jects with
low
levels
of
performance).
It may
be
that
these subjects
did not
notice
the
categorical nature
of the
memory ensemble
and
hence could
not
develop
a
well-learned
category node
in
memory. Alternatively,
these subjects could have developed
an ap-
propriate
category node
but
have failed
to
use
this node
to
facilitate their search.
There
are
several lines
of
evidence
sug-
gesting
that
automatic detection
may
develop
faster
if a
categorization
is
available
at the
start
of
consistent training.
For
example,
it
is
easy
to
learn
to
search
for a set of
stimuli
defined
by a
simple
physical
feature (see
Neisser,
1963, 1967,
and
studies
in the
next
section).
The CM
results
in
Figure
5
show
that
the
categorical condition retains some superi-
ority over
the
mixed condition
for
about eight
sessions
(25-32)
but the
magnitude
of the
difference
is
surprisingly
small.
It is
possible
that
the
high confusability between
the two
categories
of
Experiment
3
makes
the
cate-
gories
and the
automatic response
difficult
to
learn.
If so,
more training prior
to the CM
phase could possibly have
led to a
larger dif-
ference
in the
rate
of
development
for the
two
groups. Alternatively
(or
additionally)
the
small size
of
these
categories
may
have
allowed
automatic responses
to the
individual
members
to
develop
so
quickly
that
advan-
tages
due to a
category response were mini-
mized.
It is
conceptually important
to
keep
in
mind
the
possibility
that
automatic responses
might
be
attached
to
different
stages
of en-
coding
of a
single stimulus.
In
particular,
when
an
automatic-attention response
de-
velops
for a
category, other attention
re-
sponses
may
develop
at a
different
rate
for
the
stimuli making
up
that
category.
Thus,
if
a
categorization
is
available
at the
start
of
CM
training,
the
attention responses
to the
category might develop sooner than
atten-
tion responses
to
some
or all of the
individual
stimuli
in the
category.
This
effect
would
be
expected since
any one
stimulus would
appear
only
occasionally
as a
target, whereas every
target
would
be an
instance
of the
category.
On
the
other hand,
in the
cases
in
which
the
memory
ensemble does
not
form
a
category
at the
start
of CM
training,
there
might
be
at
least some individual stimuli
that
come
to
elicit automatic-attention responses while
the
category
is
still being learned.
Once
an
automatic-attention
or
encoding
response
develops,
we
assume
that
it is no
longer
under control
of the
subject
and
will
occur whenever
its
corresponding stimulus
is
presented.
This assumption will
be
supported
by
Experiment
4.
However, when
a
cate-
gorial
encoding (but
not an
attention
re-
sponse)
is
learned,
it
will
not
necessarily
be
utilized
by the
subject during
a
controlled
search.
We
suppose
a
category response
will
facilitate controlled
search
only
if the
subject
both notices
the
category
and
also decides
to
alter
his
search
to
compare
the
category
rather than
the
individual stimuli. These
considerations
would
no
longer apply
if an
automatic-attention
response were learned
in
response
to the
category;
in
such
a
case sub-
ject
strategy
would
not
matter
since
attention
would
be
directed
to the
category
in any
event.
2.
Search Studies
Using
Categories
In a
number
of
memory-search studies
(in
these
studies
F = 1 and M
varies),
the
mem-
ory set
consists
of
several categories,
and
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
145
set
item. Smith
(1962)
and
Green
and
Ander-
son
(1956)
carried
out
similar
studies
in
which
a field of
colored two-digit numbers
was
pre-
sented
and the
subject
was
asked
to
search
for
a
particular two-digit number
in a
par-
ticular
color.
The
results
showed
that
search
rate depended much more
on the
number
of
items
in the
designated color
than
on the
total
number
of
items presented, though both
effects
were
present.
A VM
procedure
was
utilized
in
their studies
so
that
automatic
detection
could
not
have been used
to
restrict
search
to the
items
of a
particular color.
Probably
a
two-phase
controlled
search
was
utilized
in
this situation.
First,
a
rather fast
serial
search
was
probably
carried
out to
locate
the
next item
of the
designated color;
then
a
slow comparison
of
that
two-digit
number
was
probably made
to
determine
whether
it was the
target.
This
explanation
is
supported
by the
fact
that searching took
a
long time
(up to 20
sec),
so
that
a
30-40
msec
search rate
for
color could have accounted
for
the
increases
in
response time
as
total
size
increased. These increases were small
only
in
comparison with
the
larger
within-
category
effects.
The
main conclusion
to be
drawn
from
the
studies reported
in
this section, based
on re-
peated
findings, is
that
the
presence
of
cate-
gories
in
search tasks
can be
used
to
facilitate,
benefit,
and
modify
controlled search.
III. Experiments
on
Focused Attention
In
part
I we
made
the
case
that
the
processes
utilized
in
attention
experiments
and
those utilized
in
search
and
detection experi-
ments
are
often
the
same.
In
fact,
in
many
cases
it is
purely arbitrary whether
a
given
study
is
referred
to as an
"attention,"
"search,"
or
"detection"
study. Experiment
1
in
Part
I
could
be
described
as a
divided-
attention study
in
which attention
had to
be
divided among
M
memory-set items
and
F
frame
items during each
frame.
The
results
showed
tremendous
deficits
in
dividing
at-
tention
in the VM
conditions,
and
virtually
no
deficit
in
dividing attention
in the CM
conditions.
The
results
of
Experiment
2 of
Part
I, and the fit of the
quantitative model
to
both studies, showed
that
divided-atten-
tion
deficits
in
these paradigms
are due to
the
limitations
of
controlled
processes,
in
particular,
to the
limited rate
of
short-term
search.
Thus,
all our
comments
and
conclusions
concerning search
and
detection
apply
equally
well
to
attention studies.
In
particular,
CM
training should
lead
to the
development
of
automatic detection
that
shoulld bypass
divided-attention
limitations,
while
VM
training should cause controlled search that
should
severely
limit
the
ability
to
divide
attention.
In
discussing
the
development
of
automatic
detection
we
have proposed
that
an
auto-
matic-attention
response
is
learned
in re-
sponse
to the
unchanging members
of the
memory ensemble.
The
present
study
will
test this hypothesis.
The
test
(Experiment
4d)
will
entail asking
the
subject
to
ignore
certain locations
and
then inserting
in
those
locations items
that
subjects
had
been pre-
viously
trained
to
respond
to as CM
targets
(to see
whether these targets attract
atten-
tion).
These studies
will
also answer
the
following
question:
To
what degree
can the
subject
focus
attention
on a
specified
subset
of the
inputs without distraction
from
the
remain-
ing
(irrelevant) inputs. Such studies
are
usually termed
focused-attention
studies,
in
contrast
to the
studies
of
Part
I and
Experi-
ments
1 to 3 of the
present article,
studies
that
would
be
appropriately termed divided-
attention
studies,
A.
Terminology
There
is a
problem
of
terminology
in the
studies
of
this section
that
is
best solved
by
the
introduction
of the
following definitions:
1.
A
foil
refers
to any
input
that
appears
in
a
to-be-ignored display location, whereas
a
distractor
refers
to a
nontarget
that
appears
in
to-be-attended display location.
2.
A CM
foil
is a
foil
that
has
previously
been
used
in CM
training
as a
memory-set
item.
3. A CM
target
foil
is a CM
foil
that
would
have been
a
target requiring
a
positive
re-
sponse
if it had
appeared
in a
to-be-attended
146
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
100
eo
40
20
F-2
F-4/diagonal
F-4
M
~ 0 I I 0 I 2 4
|
I
SPACING
OF 2
TARGETS
|
NUMBER
OF
TARGETS
Figure
8.
Data
from
Experiment
4a.
Estimated per-
centage
of
detection
as a
function
of
number
and
spacing
of
targets
in a VM
procedure with
frame
time
equal
to 200
msec.
The
spacing
0
points
are
non-
identical
targets, while
the
spacings
of
1,
2, and 4 are
identical
targets.
(F
=
frame
size.)
display location (although
it
appears
in
fact
in
a
to-be-ignored display
location).
It is a
member
of the
current memory set.
4. A VM
foil
is a
foil
that
has
previously
served
as
both
a
target
and
distractor
in VM
conditions.
5.
A VM
target
foil
is a VM
foil
that
would
have been
a
target
requiring
a
positive
re-
sponse
if it had
appeared
in a
to-be-attended
display location.
It is a
member
of the
cur-
rent
memory set.
6.
Valid
positions
or
characters
are
to-be-
attended positions
or
characters. Invalid
positions
or
characters
are
to-be-ignored posi-
tions
or
characters.
7.
FII
denotes
a
trial
in
which
two
identical
memory-set
items appear,
one in a
to-be-at-
tended
display position,
and one in a
to-be-
ignored
display position.
8.
FNI
denotes
a
trial
in
which
two
dif-
ferent
memory-set items appear,
one in a to-
be-attended display position,
and one in a
to-be-ignored
position.
B.
Focusing
Attention
in a VM
Multiple-
Frame Task
The first
study
in the
present series
of
experiments
is
designed
to
show
that
"con-
trolled
search"
is not a
misnomer,
that
sub-
jects
can
control their search
to the
extent
that
VM
foils
can be
ignored,
and
that
search
can be
carried
out
through
the
valid display
positions without decrement caused
by the
foils.
1.
Met
hod
The
paradigm
utilizes
the
multiple-target, multiple-
frame
VM
procedure.
There
are
three
main
con-
ditions:
(a)
M
= 2, F = 2; (b) M = 2, F = 4; (c)
M
= 2, F
=
4,
diagonal. Condition (c),
denoted
"diagonal,"
was
designed
so
that
one of the di-
agonals
of the
display
was
always
valid
(to-be-
attended),
and the
other diagonal
was
always
in-
valid
(to-be-ignored).
In
this
condition four
char-
acters were presented
on
each frame,
two
valid,
and two
invalid
foils.
We
expected
that
this diagonal
condition
would elicit performance similar
to
that
for
the M = 2, F = 2
condition,
and
better
than
that
of the
M
= 2, F = 4
condition.
The
four
subjects
in
this study were
the
same
as
those used
as in
Experiment
3 of
Part
I. In
the
diagonal condition, only
the
upper-left
and
lower-right
frame
positions ever contained
a
target.
The
subjects
were
fully
instructed regarding
this
fact
and
were instructed
to
ignore
the
invalid
diagonal.
For
each subject,
the
blocks
of
trials
utilizing
the
diagonal procedure were
run
after
the
other conditions were completed.
The
M
= 2, F = 2
condition
had 14
blocks
per
subject;
the M
=
2,
F
= 4
condition
had 11
blocks
per
subject;
the
diagonal
condition
had 16
blocks
per
subject,
the
first
4
of
which were practice blocks.
There
were
120
test trials
per
block, plus
an
additional
30
trials
of
practice
for the first
block
of a
session
and
15
trials
of
practice
for
each subsequent block
in
a
session.
The
procedure used
in
this study
differed
some-
what
from
that
of
Part
I/Experiment
3 and
from
the
other multiple-target
tasks
reported
in
this
article.
The
primary
difference
lay in the
relation
of
target
similarity
to
spacing.
The
spacing
0
con-
dition utilized
NI
targets
only,
and the
spacing
1,
2,
and 4
conditions utilized
II
targets
only.
In
addition,
the
present experiment allowed
targets
to
reoccur
in the
same frame positions
if
they
were
not
in
successive frames. Also,
in the
present
study,
the
subjects
pushed
a
single response button each time
they thought they detected
the
target.
The
responses
were
to be
made when
the
targets
appeared
rather
than
at the
trial's end.
2.
Results
and
Discussion
On
about
1%
of the
trials
in
each condition
a
response
was
made
before
the
target
ap-
peared,
and
these responses were
not
counted.
The
results
are
presented
in
Figure
8. The
results
of the F = 2 and
diagonal conditions
obviously
do not
differ,
but
results
of
both
conditions
are
clearly superior
to
those
of the
F
= 4
condition. These results were expected
on
the
supposition
that
the
subject should
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
147
have been able
to
control
his
processing
in
the
diagonal condition
so
that
comparisons
would
occur only
for the
characters
on the
valid diagonal.
The
peculiar relationship
of
spacing
to
target similarity
in
this study caused
the
shape
of the
spacing
functions
to
appear
to
differ
from
those
in
previous multiple-target
studies.
In
fact,
however,
the
corresponding
points
from
the
present study
and
Part
I/
Experiment
3 are
quite close
in
value.
It is
interesting
to
note
the
subject's comments
at the end of the
experiment. They noticed
an
excess number
of II
conditions overall,
but
none
noticed
that
the II
pairs
were
not
tested
at
spacing
0, nor
that
the
NI
pairs were
not
tested
at the
longer spacings.
The
implications
of
this study
are
straight-
forward.
Subjects
can
control their search
in
VM
situations
at
least
to the
degree
that
comparisons
can be
limited
to a
specified
diagonal.
It is
possible
that
characters
on the
invalid
diagonal
are
sometimes compared,
but
not
until
the
valid diagonal
is
searched
first
(if
time were taken
to
compare
foils
during
the
search
of the
valid diagonal, then per-
formance
would
be
worse).
Of
course, this
study demonstrates only
a
minimum amount
of
subject control.
In
future
studies
it
would
be
desirable
to
explore this matter more
thoroughly.
For
example,
we
might ask:
Can
search
alternate
between diagonals
in
successive
frames?
Can
search order
be
cued
individually
for
each
frame?
C.
Distraction Caused
by
"Targets"
During
VM
Search
Experiment
4a
showed
no
distracting
effect
of
VM
foils
(on the
invalid diagonal). How-
ever,
none
of
these
foils
was in
fact
identical
to any
member
of the
memory set.
The
present study, then,
is
designed
to
determine
the
distracting
effect
of VM
target
foils:
To
what extent
can
members
of the
memory
set
be
ignored when they appear
in
invalid dis-
play locations?
1.
Method
The
paradigm
is
similar
in
general outline
to
that
of the VM
conditions
of
Part
I/Experiment
3.
g
H
O
UJ
H
UJ
0
1
UJ
cc
\-
\-
UJ
o:
UJ
IUU
90
80
70
en
-
*v
0
Q
0
N.
/
^V
o
o
FII
•—•FNI
i
ii
i
-I
+1
+4
NUMBER
OF
FRAMES FROM
TARGET
TO
FOIL
Figure
9.
Data
from
Experiment
4b. FII =
target
foil
identical
to
target;
FNI
=
target
foil
nonidentical
to
target. Percentage
of
target detection
as a
function
of
the
spacing
and the
similarity between
the
target
and the
target
foil.
Varied-mapping procedures were
used,
and
frame
time
was 200
msec.
The
peculiarities
of
Experiment
4a
were eliminated.
The
multiple-frame procedure
was
utilized with
M
= 2 and F = 4. The
upper-left, lower-right
diagonal
was
valid,
and the
other
diagonal
was
invalid. Either zero
or one
targets appeared
on
the
valid
diagonal,
and an
appropriate
binary
re-
sponse
was
required.
A VM
task
was
utilized
on
the
valid diagonal,
and the
foils
on the
invalid
diagonal
were
chosen
from
the
distractor
set for the
valid
diagonal.
In
addition, there
was on
every
trial exactly
one VM
target
foil
(i.e.,
a
member
of
the
memory set)
on the
invalid diagonal.
On
one-third
of the
trials,
only
a VM
target
foil
appeared,
always
on
frames
8-13.
On
two-thirds
of
the
trials both
a
target
foil
and
target appeared;
on
one-half
of
these trials
the
target
and
target
foil
were identical
(FII),
and on
one-half
of
these
trials
the
target
and
target
foil
were
different
(FNI).
Finally,
the
target appeared
on any of
frames
8-13,
and the
target
foil
appeared equally often
on
frames
—4,
—1,
+1,
and +4
with
respect
to the
target
frame.
That
is, the
spacing between
the
target
and
target
foil
was
systematically varied. Thus,
the
target-foil-only
trials occurred with
a
probability
of
J,
and
each
of the
other eight conditions occurred
with
a
probability
of 8 X i = A. All
these
trials
were
randomly
intermixed within each block.
Each block contained
144
test
trials
preceded
by
15
practice trials. Each subject
ran
through
a
total
of
12 or 13
blocks
in
four
sessions.
2.
Results
and
Discussion
The
results
are
shown
in
Figure
9. The
figure
gives
the
percentage
of
hits
as a
func-
tion
of the
spacing between
the
target
and
148
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
the
target
foil,
when
the
target
foil
is
iden-
tical
(FIX)
and
nonidentical
(FNI)
to the
target.
The
correct rejection rate when
no
target
was
present
was
91%.
The
results
may
be
summarized
briefly:
The
target
foil
had
no
effect
on the hit
rate except when
it was
different
from
the
target
(FNI)
and
preceded
the
target
by 1
frame,
in
which case detec-
tion
probability
was
decreased
by
.11.
In the
present study
a
target
foil
reduced
performance
when
it
preceded,
but not
when
it
followed,
the
target, Suppose
that
this
re-
duction
is due to the
same factors
that
pro-
duce decrements
in
multiple-target
detection
in
VM
conditions (Experiment
3,
Part
I).
Then
one
target probably inhibits detection
of
a
subsequent target,
but not of an
anteced-
ent
target.
That
is, the
reduced detection
at
spacing
1
seen
in the VM
conditions
of
Part
I/Experiment
3 and
also seen
in
Figure
4
and 6 of the
present paper, could have been
caused
by
either
of the two
targets' reducing
detection
of the
other.
The
present results
provide some reason
to
argue
that
it is the
first
of
two
successive
targets
that
reduces
detection
of the
second.
Let us
assume
that
the
decrement
in
per-
centage
of
detection
of two
targets
in VM
paradigms
is in
fact
a
decrement
in
detection
of
the
second
target.
Then
it is
possible
to
compare
the
magnitude
of the
decrements
in
the
multiple-target studies (e.g., Experi-
ment
3,
Part
I) and the
present study.
In
fact,
the
decrements
are
much larger
in the
earlier studies
in
which
all
display locations
were
valid.
For
example,
the
difference
in
detection between spacing
1 and
spacing
4
in
Part
I/Experiment
3 was 30% in the
NI
condition
and
8%
in the II
condition;
the
comparable decrements
in the
present study
are
\\%
and
0%.
Thus
a
target
foil
reduces
detection
of a
target
in the
next
frame,
but
the
reduction
is
much smaller than
that
caused
by an
actual target.
How
can the
present
findings be
reconciled
with those
of
Experiment
4a in
which foils
did
not
impair performance?
The
pattern
of
findings
would
be
explicable
if the
valid
diagonal were always searched
first and
then,
whenever
that
search
finished
early,
one or
more
characters
on the
invalid diagonal were
inadvertently
checked
in
addition.
In
this
event, target
foils
would occasionally
be
noticed
and
might harm subsequent detection
in
a
fashion similar
to
that
caused
by
true
target detection.
Of
course, this hypothesis
is
speculative
and
other explanations
are
undoubtedly
available.
Whatever
the
explanations
for the
details
of
the
results,
it is
safe
to
summarize
the
results
of
Experiments
4a and 4b as
follows:
Subjects
are
able
to
control their search
in
varied-mapping paradigms.
The
degree
of
their control
is
sufficient
to
eliminate
any
distracting
effect
of
nontarget
foils
and to
reduce
the
distracting
effect
of
target
foils
well
below
the
level caused
by
other targets.
Thus, attention
focusing
is
quite successful:
VM
foils
have,
at
most,
a
small distracting
effect
when they appear
in
to-be-ignored
locations
during controlled search.
D. The
Distracting
Effect
of
"Targets"
During
CM
Search
Experiments
4a and 4b
examined
the
ability
to
focus
attention during controlled
search.
We
next ask:
To
what degree does
attention
focusing
affect
automatic detec-
tion?
Our
previous studies have demonstrated
that
automatic
detection
is not
affected
by
frame
size,
so
there would
be no
point
in
carrying
out a CM
counterpart
of
Experiment
4a in
which
the
invalid diagonal would con-
tain distractors only.
We
decided
to
carry
out
a
counterpart
to
Experiment
4b to find out
whether
a CM
target
foil
would interfere
with
automatic target detection
on the
valid
diagonal.
M and F
were
set
equal
to 2.
During each
frame,
each diagonal contained
one
mask
and
one
character.
One
diagonal
was
always
valid,
the
other invalid.
On
every trial
a CM
target
foil
appeared
on the
invalid diagonal
in
exactly
one
frame.
A CM
target appeared
on
the
valid diagonal
on
two-thirds
of the
trials;
on
one-half
of
these
trials
the
target
and
target
foil
were identical
(FII)
and on
one-half,
nonidentical
(FNI).
In
each
of
these cases,
the
target
foil
occurred either
four
frames
before,
in the
same
frame
with,
or
four frames after
the
target
(-J
probability
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
149
each).
There were
162
trials
per
block
and 6
blocks
per
each
1-hour
session.
Two
sessions
were
run
with
frame
time equal
to 60
msec
and two
sessions were then
run
with
frame
time
equal
to 30
msec.
The
results
are
shown
in
Table
2. The
only
significant
effect
was a
slight drop
in
performance
(about
4%)
in the
FII
condition
at / = 60
msec with simultaneous target
and
foil
(0
=
4.45,
p<.QOQl);
performance
dropped considerably when
/
dropped
to
30
msec,
but all
conditions became roughly
equal.
The
results
of
this study demonstrate
that
a
target
foil
hinders detection
of an
identical
simultaneous
target.
The
magnitude
of the
effect
is
small,
but so is the
magnitude
of
the
decrement
that
occurs when
two
simul-
taneous
CM
targets
are
presented.
In
fact
these
decrements
are not
much
different
at
the
equivalent
frame
times
(4% vs.
&%).
It
seems reasonable
to
assume
that
the
same
mechanism
is
causing both
effects.4
It is
interesting
to
compare these
findings
to
those
of
Eriksen
and
Eriksen
(1974).
In
our
terminology, they used
a
single-frame
CM
task with
M 2, F = 1, and
they used
reaction
time
as the
dependent measure.
However,
the
distractor
set and the
memory
ensemble
were both
of
size
2 and
were con-
sistently mapped across trials,
so
that
an
equally strong
but
opposite tendency
to
auto-
matically respond
to the
members
of the two
sets
was
probably learned.
(In our CM
studies, only
the
memory-set items
can
come
to
elicit
automatic responses, because dis-
tractors
are
presented
on
every trial.
The
only
exception
in our
studies occurred when
F
= 1 in the
single-frame
paradigm
of
Part
I,
and
even then
the
distractor
set was
always
larger than
the
memory set.)
In the
Eriksen
and
Eriksen study,
the
relevant item always
appeared directly above
the fixation
point,
but
three invalid items were placed
on
each
side
of the
relevant item.
The
nature
and
distance
of
these invalid items were varied.
When
the
distance
to the
nearest item reached
1
°
of
visual angle,
the
invalid characters
had
little
effect
on
reaction time.
At
closer dis-
tances,
all
reaction times were slowed,
but
flanking
distractors produced
the
greatest
Table
2
Effect
of
Distraction
on A
utomatic
Detection: Experiment
4c
Variable
Session
1
60-msec
frame
time
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
Session
2
60-msec
frame time
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
Session
3
30-msec
frame
time
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
Session
4
30-msec
frame time
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
FII
FNI
Spacing
(target
to
foil)
-4
-4
0
0
+4
+4
-4
-4
0
0
+4
+4
-4
-4
0
0
+4
+4
-4
-4
0
0
+4
+4
%
hits
91.9
94.2
89.4
95.6
96.1
95.
1
95.4
93.5
90.7
93.8
92.8
95.6
75.7
79.1
74.5
77.6
77.1
80.1
77.3
78.9
80.3
81.0
78.7
78.2
%
correct
rejections
90.0
91.2
75.2
71.5
Note.
FII
=
trial
in
which
two
identical memory-set
items
appear,
one in a
to-be-attended display position,
and one in a
to-be-ignored display position.
FNI
=
trial
in
which
two
dif-
ferent
memory-set
items
appear,
one in a
to-be-attended
posi-
tion,
and one in a
to-be-ignored position.
slowing
(since
they automatically produced
a
competing
response);
flanking
characters
that were
not
distractors
or
memory-set
items produced
a
moderate
slowing
(regard-
4
The
results
in
Table
2
give some indications
that
the
disrupting
effect
may
change
with
practice.
Over
the
four
sessions,
the
differences between
FII
and
FNI at
spacing
0
was, respectively, 6.2%, 3.1%,
3.1%,
and
.7%.
If
this trend actually exists,
it may
be
caused
by the
development
of a new
automatic
response
that
restricts search
to the
valid
diagonal.
That
is,
position-specific information might govern
the
automatic-attention response; such
an
auto-
matic process could
be
learned because
the
valid
diagonal never changes over trials.
It
would
be
interesting
in
future
investigations
to
compare per-
formance
in
this condition
with
performance
in a
condition
that
alternates
the
valid
diagonal
from
trial
to
trial,
since
alternation
should
prevent
the
long-term learning
of
position-specific encoding.
150
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
90
o
LU
r~
80
LU
Q
cr
LU
o
OL
LU
0.
70
60
50
NO
FOIL-
-I 0
+1
NUMBER
OF
FRAMES FROM
TARGET
TO
FOIL
Figure
10.
Data
from
Experiment
4d.
Percentage
of
target
detection
in a
varied-mapping procedure
as a
function
of the
spacing
between
the
target
and the
target
foil,
when
subjects
had
previously
been
trained
to
respond
to the
foil
as a
consistent-mapping target.
Frame
time
was 200
msec.
less
of
their visual similarity
to
either set),
and flanking
targets, whether identical
or
nonidentical, produced
the
least
slowing
(since they automatically produced
a
com-
patible
response).
Both
the
Eriksen
and
Eriksen
(1974)
study
and our
study suggest
that
CM
target
foils
can
neither
be
excluded
from
processing
nor
prevented
from
affecting
performance.
In
addition, however,
the
Eriksen
and
Eriksen
result suggests
that
the
spatial
configuration
of
the
inputs
can
determine
the
magnitude
of
these
effects
(see Footnote
4 for a
discus-
sion
of
location-specific automatic
detection).
E. The
Distraction
of
Controlled Search
by
Automatic
Detection
Controlled search depends
on an
apparently
serial process
that
should easily
be
disturbed
if
an
automatic-attention response occurs
that draws attention
to an
invalid location.
Such
a
rationale suggests
a
paradigm
for
our
next study
in
which
CM
foils
appear
on
the
invalid diagonal while
a
controlled search
occurs
on the
valid diagonal.
This
study
is
probably
the
most important
in
this series
(4a-4d)
because
it
provides
a
test
of the
hypothesis
that
an
automatic-
attention response
to
consistently trained
targets
is
learned.
1.
Method
Each
frame
contained
4
characters.
The
upper-
left
to
lower-right diagonal
was
valid. Memory-set
size
was 2, and the VM
conditions were utilized.
Search
on the
valid diagonal
was for
consonants
in
consonants
(or
digits
in
digits
for
other sub-
jects).
The
foils
on the
invalid diagonal were
chosen
from
the
distractor
set
used
for the
valid
diagonal,
except
for the CM
foil.
When
a CM
foil
appeared
it was
chosen
from
the set
that
had
served
as the CM
memory
ensemble
in all
previous
studies
for
that subject. Thus,
if
consonants were being
used
on the
valid diagonal,
a CM
foil
would
be a
digit,
and
vice versa.
On
one-sixth
of the
trials, neither
a
target
nor a
CM
foil
appeared;
on
one-sixth
of the
trials only
a
target appeared;
on
one-sixth
of the
trials only
a CM
foil
and no
target appeared;
on
one-half
of
the
trials both
a
target
and a CM
foil
appeared,
with
the
spacing between them
equally
likely
to
be
—1,
0,
+1.
Each block
of
trials
contained
144
test
trials.
The
first
block
of
each session
had
IS
practice trials
and the
other three blocks
had 5
practice
trials.
Two
sessions were
run for
each
of
the
four
subjects (the same subjects
as
those used
in
Experiments
4a-4c.)
The
frame
time
was 200
msec.
2.
Results
and
Discussion
The
results
are
shown
in
Figure
10.
Each
percentage
is
based
on
7,68 observations.
When
no CM
foil
or
target appeared
the
false
alarm rate
was
4%.
When only
a CM
foil
appeared
the
fajse
alarm rate
was
again
4%.
Thus,
in the
absence
of an
actual target
the
presence
of a CM
foil
caused
no
increase
in
false
alarms.
Since
the
appearance
of CM
foils
was
highly
correlated
with
the ap-
pearance
of
targets,
one
might have supposed
that
a
bias
or
guessing strategy would arise,
such
that
subjects would more often guess
that
targets were present
if a CM
foil
was
seen.
The
present
data
argue against such
a
bias
(as do the
data
discussed below).
The hit
rate when
a
target
but no CM
foil
was
present
was
84%.
The
distraction caused
by a CM
foil
may be
determined through
comparison with
this
performance level.
When
the CM
foil
preceded
the
target
by one
frame
the hit
rate
was
&2<fc,
not
significantly
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
151
lower
than
the 84%
base
rate. When
the
target
and CM
foil
were presented simul-
taneously,
the hit
rate dropped
to
62%.
And
when
the CM
foil
followed
the
target
by one
frame
the hit
rate rose
to
77%,
a figure
still
significantly
lower statistically than
the
84%
hit
rate
when
no CM
foil
was
present.
In
short,
a CM
foil
provides
little
hindrance
to
the
detection
of a VM
target presented
in
the
following
frame,
greatly hinders detection
of
a VM
target presented simultaneously,
and
even reduces slightly
the
detection
of a VM
target presented
in the
preceding
frame.
The
simplest interpretation
of
these
re-
sults
is
that
a CM
foil
interrupts
the
ongoing
controlled
search
and
causes
a
loss
of
process-
ing
time, thereby reducing target detection.
The
interruption
is
caused
by
what
we
have
termed
an
automatic-attention response
to
the CM
foil.
We
have argued that subjects
have been trained
to
respond
to CM
targets
with
an
automatic-attention response. Even
when
such
a
target appears
on a
to-be-ignored
display diagonal,
it
apparently causes
an at-
tention
response
that
interrupts
processing
along
the
valid diagonal
and
directs attention
to the
invalid diagonal.
The
time lost
before
attention
is
returned
to the
valid diagonal
and
the
search
is
resumed causes
a
consider-
able decrement
in
performance
if the
target
is
in
fact
on the
valid diagonal during
that
frame.
Since
target detection
in a
frame
following
a CM
foil
is not
hindered,
the
recovery
from
the
distraction caused
by a CM
foil
must
be
quite
rapid;
that
is,
recovery must take place
in
a
time period under
200
msec. Further-
more,
the
automatic-attention response must
occur
fairly
rapidly, since
a CM
foil
even
reduces
detection
of a
target
in the
preceding
frame.
Perhaps
the
processing
of one
frame
occasionally
overlaps
the
start
of the
next;
for
example,
a
comparison initiated
but not
completed
before
termination
of a
frame
may
be
completed
before
controlled processing
of
the
next
frame
begins. Occasionally,
a
target
might
be the
character undergoing compari-
son
when
the
subsequent
frame
begins,
and
a CM
foil
in
that
next
frame
could interrupt
comparison
of the
target
and
thereby impair
performance.
The
primary
finding of the
present study
is the
demonstration
that
the
responses
to
CM
targets
are
both
well
learned
and
auto-
matic.
CM
targets cannot
be
ignored, even
when
they
are
known
to be
irrelevant
and
occur
in
consistently invalid display loca-
tions
and
even when subjects
are
instructed
to
ignore them.
Our
results,
of
course,
do not
establish
that
it is
impossible
to
control
the
detection
processes
that
we
have labeled
"automatic."
In
fact,
Sperling (Note
1,
Note
2) has re-
ported
some
findings
that
do
tend
to
show
that
at
least some amount
of
control over
some
aspects
of the
search process
in CM
multiple-frame
tasks
is
possible. However,
the
degree
of
control over,
and the
ability
to
ignore,
stimuli
that
subjects have been
trained
to see as
targets
in CM
situations
are
clearly much less than
in VM
situations.
Thus,
for the
processing mode
in the CM
conditions
of the
present tasks,
we
prefer
the
possibly slightly inaccurate,
but de-
scriptive, term automatic
to the
logically
more
accurate,
but
less
descriptive,
term
systemic, which
was
used
in
Shiffrin
(197Sa)
to
refer
to the
same type
of
processing.
F.
Limitations
on the
Ability
to
Focus
Attention
The
present series
of
studies
(4a-4d)
ex-
amined
the
subjects' ability
to
focus
atten-
tion. Experiment
4a
showed
that
attention
during
controlled search could
be
focused
on
specified
spatial locations. Experiment
4b
showed
that
the
focusing
is not
complete,
because
certain types
of
stimuli
in
to-be-
ignored
locations
are
sometimes processed
and
when processed,
can
reduce target detec-
tion. Experiment
4c
showed
that
a CM
target
in a
to-be-ignored
location
hinders
automatic
deletion
by a
small amount.
Ex-
periment
4d
showed
that
attention
is
greatly
distracted
from
the
relevant locations when
a CM
target appears
in a
to-be-ignored loca-
tion
during
a VM
search
task.
This
last
find-
ing
is
particularly interesting because
the
distracting stimulus
differs
from
the
relevant
stimuli
not
only
in
spatial
location
but
also
in
category.
152
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
These
findings are
particularly important
because previous demonstrations
of
difficul-
ties
in
focusing attention have usually utilized
incompatible responses.
That
is, a
stimulus
in
response
to
which
a
subject
has
been
trained
to
emit
response
x is
presented
in a
to-be-
ignored
location
in
conjunction with
a
stimulus
requiring
a
response
y
that
is
incompatible
with
x. In the
Stroop
Color-Word
Inter-
ference
Test
(1935),
for
example,
the
sub-
ject must read names
of
colors
that
are
printed
in ink
colors
that
do not
correspond
to the
names. Reading speed
is
greatly slowed
in
this case (see Jensen
&
Rohwer, 1966).
Keele
(1972)
showed
that
decrements
in
processing stimuli resulted when
a
similar
color-name
incompatibility
was
present.
The
Eriksen
and
Eriksen
(1974)
study discussed
above also showed
that
response time
was
dependent
on the
compatibility
of
responses
between
stimuli
to be
attended
to and
those
to
be
ignored.
Many other examples
of
this
kind
can be
found
in the
literature,
but our
demonstra-
tions
differ
in one
very important respect.
Namely,
the
distracting stimulus
in our
stud-
ies
hurts
performance even
though
subjects
have been trained
to
respond
to it in
exactly
the
same
way as to the
target
stimulus. Both
types
of
studies demonstrate
that
the
to-be-
ignored
stimulus receives processing,
but the
studies utilizing incompatible responses
do
not
demonstrate
any
processing interaction
short
of the
motor responses themselves.
On
the
other hand,
our
studies suggest
that
the
focused-attention
deficit
results
from
a di-
version
of
attention
and an
accompanying
loss
of
processing time, since there
is no re-
sponse incompatibility.
Our
studies have shown
that
focused-at-
tention
deficits
can
arise
due to
distraction
by
either
CM or VM
items
in
appropriate
contexts,
though probably
due to
rather dif-
ferent
mechanisms
in the two
cases.
In
many
studies
in the
literature
that
have shown
focused-attention
deficits,
it is not
clear which
factor
is
responsible.
For
example,
in
shadow-
ing
tasks
the
listener repeats aloud
a
desig-
nated message (usually
in one
ear) while
other
distracting messages
are
also presented.
The
early studies showed
that
distracting
messages barely harm shadowing performance
when
they
are
distinguishable
by an
obvious
physical characteristic (such
as
spatial
origin;
see
Cherry, 1953; Cherry
&
Taylor, 1954).
These
studies
are
probably like
our
Experi-
ment
4a in
which
one
diagonal
is
relevant
during
VM
search
and the
other
can be
ignored.
A
result related
to
Experiment
4b
can be
found
in
studies
by
Treisman
(1964a,
1964c)
in
which items
of
some possible
relevance
are
presented
in the
nonshadowed
ear
(similar
to our
target
foils).
For
example,
if
the
distracting message
is in the
same
voice,
but in
French,
a
considerable distrac-
tion occurs. Many studies have shown
that
the
message
in the
nonshadowed
ear is an-
alyzed
to
some considerable depth (e.g.,
Lewis,
1970; Treisman, 1960, 1964b).
In
these studies
it is not
clear which type
of
processing causes
the
analysis,
but in
other
studies
it is
clear
that
automatic processing
is
responsible
for the
analysis
that
occurs
(Corteen
&
Wood,
1972;
Von
Wright, Ander-
son,
&
Stenman,
1975).
Further reviews
of
such studies will
not
be
undertaken here because
of the
difficulty
in
ascertaining
whether
focused-attention
deficits
are
caused
by
controlled processing
of
invalid
items
or by
automatic processing
of
invalid
items.5
If
nothing else,
our
results
strongly suggest
that
future
research
on
focused
attention should include controls
to
differentiate
deficits
caused
by
controlled
and
automatic processing.
In
summary,
we
have seen
that
subjects
can
divide attention almost without
deficit
when
automatic detection
is
utilized
(Part
I/
6
Controlled
processing
might
be
given
to oc-
casional
inputs
in an
irrelevant
ear,
because
ear of
origin
is a
fairly
difficult
cue to
utilize (compared
with
spatial location
in a
visual task,
for
example).
Whether
automatic
processing
or
controlled proc-
essing
is
used
to
restrict analysis
to the
desired ear,
occasional
failures
of
selection
are to be
expected,
failures
that
will
cause
occasional
items
in the
to-be-
ignored
ear to be
given
controlled
processing.
In
addition,
'Controlled
processing
of
items
in the
to-be-
ignored
ear may
occur
on
occasion when processing
of
the
items
in the
valid
ear is
completed
and a
brief
interval
occurs
before
the
next valid input
arrives
(an
argument similar
to
this
is
used
in the
discussion
of
Experiment
4b).
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
153
Experiments
1 and 2, CM
conditions,
see
Figure
2) and
cannot divide attention with-
out
deficit
when controlled search
is
utilized
(Part
I/Experiments
1 and 2, VM
conditions,
see
Figure
2).
Focused-attention
deficits
are
quite substantial when caused
by
automatic
responses
to
to-be-ignored items (Experi-
ment
4d and
Figure
10) but are
quite
a bit
smaller
when
caused
by
controlled processing
of
to-be-ignored
items,
since
the
subjects'
control
is
usually adequate
to
prevent such
processing (Experiments
4a and 4b, and
Figures
8 and
9).
One
implication
is the
pre-
diction
that
the
type
of
training
procedures,
VM
or CM,
will
determine whether divided-
or
focused-attention
deficits
will occur.
G. The
Nature
of
Automatic
Detection
The
reversal studies, Experiments
1 and 2,
and the
category study, Experiment
3,
make
it
clear
that
long-term learning
is
responsible
for
the
phenomenon
of
automatic detection
that
is
seen
in the CM
conditions.
The
lengthy
training period required
for
acquisition,
and
the
even longer training periods required
to
alter
the
detection process once learned,
testify
to the
permanent nature
of the
learn-
ing. Furthermore,
the
phenomenon
is
power-
ful
enough
to
transcend
the
laboratory
context. Subjects given
CM
training
on
one
group
of
letters
as
memory-set items with
another group
as
distractors,
as in
Experi-
ments
1 and 3,
report
effects
on
reading out-
side
the
laboratory. Despite
the
fact
that
all
the
letters
used
in the
experiments were
capitalized,
the
subjects reported
that
the
memory-set items
from
the CM
conditions
tended
to
"jump out"
from
the
page during
normal reading.
This
effect
was
distracting
enough
that
one
subject would
not
attempt
to
read
for an
hour
or
more
after
an
experi-
mental session.
It is
clear then
that
automatic detection
reflects
a
powerful
long-term process.
We now
ask: What
is it
that
is
learned? Experiments
4a-4d
imply
that
an
attention response
is
learned,
but
many details
of the
automatic
detection
process
remain
to be
specified.
It is our
feeling
that
several factors con-
tribute
to the
process
that
has
been termed
automatic detection.
First
there
is an
auto-
matic-attention response
to the
features
that
are
encoded
from
an
input target;
this
re-
sponse directs attention
to the
representation
in
short-term memory
of the
relevant visual
input
and
also
to the
representation
in
mem-
ory of the
appropriate member
of the
memory
set. Second,
in
addition,
an
automatic "tar-
get" response
is
learned
that
tells
the
subject
that
a
target
is
among
the
inputs. Third,
in
addition,
in
some
situations
an
automatic
overt motor response (such
as a
button press)
is
learned
in
response
to a
target.
The
third factor,
an
automatically pro-
duced
overt
motor
response,
is
probably
limited
to
special situations
in
which
the
same completely consistent response
to all
targets
is
immediately required.
Our
reaction
time
study
(Part
I,
Experiment
2)
may
rep-
resent such
a
situation,
but our
multiple-
frame
tasks
do
not.
In the
multiple-frame
tasks,
no
overt response
is
required until
the
trial's
end.
In
addition, some
of the
tasks
in
our
studies require
that
the
number
of
targets
be
counted, rather than
that
a re-
sponse
be
made
to
each target
as it
appears.
Finally,
the
tasks
in
Experiment
4
occa-
sionally
present
target
foils
in
to-be-ignored
locations,
and the
subject
has
little
difficulty
suppressing
responses
to
such
targets.
Thus,
we
feel
that
automatic overt responses
can
be
learned
but are
probably
not a
major
contributor
to
performance
in
most
of our
tasks.
The first two
factors,
the
occurrence
of
automatic
"attention"
and
"target"
responses,
undoubtedly
play
a
major
role
in our
tasks.
However, such responses must
be
followed
by
some
sort
of
controlled process
or
controlled
decision
to
generate
the
required overt
re-
sponse.
A
number
of
controlled processes
may be
used.
In
most
of our
studies
it
would
not
have been
sufficient
to
simply
use the
occurrence
of an
automatic
"attention"
or
"target"
response
as a
basis
for a
decision
to
respond.
For one
thing, some
of our
studies
require
responses
to be
counted;
for
another,
some
of our
studies present target
foils
to
which responses must
be
withheld. Further-
more,
in
Experiment
3 we saw
that
the
mem-
ory and
distractor
sets
could
be so
confus-
able
that
automatic responses could
not be
154
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
learned
in a
perfectly accurate manner,
so
that
a
switch
to a
controlled item mode
was
necessary
to
check
the
accuracy
of
automatic
responses.
For all of
these reasons,
we
propose
that
following
the
occurrence
of an
automatic-
attention
or
target response,
the
subject
en-
gages
in the
minimal controlled processing
necessary
to
satisfy
the
task
requirements.
Such
controlled
processes
might
consist
of
comparing
the
memory representations
to
which
attention
has
been drawn, counting
the
target instances,
and
checking
the
spatial
location
of
located targets
to see
whether
they
are
foils
or
valid targets.
Before
concluding
this
discussion,
it is
useful
to
consider
briefly
a few
supplementary
hypotheses regarding
the
automatic detec-
tion
process.
Each
of
these hypotheses
as-
sumes
the
occurrence
of an
automatic-atten-
tion response.
First,
consider
the
possibility
that
atten-
tion
is
drawn automatically
to the
representa-
tion
of the
relevant visual input, which
is
then compared serially
to the
memory set.
This
hypothesis
is
easily rejected since
it
implies,
contrary
to
fact,
that
memory-set
size
will have
a
large
effect
under
CM
con-
ditions.
Second,
consider
the
possibility that
an at-
tention
response directs
the
subject
to the
relevant visual input, whose category
is
then
compared
in one
step
to the
category
of the
memory set.
This
hypothesis
is
testable
in
any of
several ways
but is
difficult
to
reject
on
the
basis
of
present evidence.
A
tentative
inference
from
Experiment
3
suggested
that
automatic detection
for
items
in a
four-letter
set
developed much
faster
than category
learning
for
that
set.
If so,
then this category
hypothesis could
be
ruled
out
(because
the
M
= 2 and M = 4 CM
functions converged
quickly
in the
mixed condition
of
Experiment
3).
Nevertheless, additional research
will
be
needed
to
test this hypothesis conclusively.
Assuming
that
an
automatic-attention
re-
sponse underlies automatic detection,
it
seems
clear
why
automatic detection does
not de-
velop
in VM
situations:
An
attention
or
overt
response
that
is
helpful
on one
trial
(when
the
producing stimulus
is a
target) will
be
harmful
on
another (when
the
producing
stimulus
is a
distractor).
A
subject cannot
learn both
an
attending
and a
nonattending
response
to the
same stimulus. While this
argument
seems clear,
an
important theo-
retical question remains. What
is the
under-
lying
mechanism
that
inhibits
the
learning
of
an
automatic-attention response
in the
VM
search situation?
Suppose
that
a
Learning
event
takes
place
each time
a
target
is
found
correctly
and is
therefore
reinforced.
In
consistent (CM)
paradigms there will
be no
impediment
to
the
learning
of
attention responses.
In VM
paradigms,
the
outcome
is
less clear.
It
might
be
supposed
that
every item, whether cur-
rently
a
memory-set item
or a
distractor,
comes
to
elicit attention responses
due to
those trials
on
which
it is a
target. There
are
then several possibilities:
(a)
Learning con-
tinues
until
all
items have acquired attention
responses
of
roughly equal strength. Because
the
responses
are of
equal strength, they tend
to
cancel each other,
and
controlled search
must
be
utilized. Later,
if a
switch
is
made
to a CM
paradigm,
the
strength
of the re-
sponses
to the fixed
memory-set items
be-
comes
greater than
that
for the
distractor
items,
(b) As an
attention response begins
to
develop
it
starts
to
occur
on
trials when
the
input
is a
distractor. Because attention
is
directed
to the
wrong
input, performance
suffers,
and the
response
is
therefore
in-
hibited,
(c)
Each
time
a
distractor
is
com-
pared during
a
controlled search, whether
or
not an
attention response occurs, inhibition
of
any
previously
reinforced
attention
re-
sponse
may
take
place because
a
comparison
is
carried
out but not
reinforced. According
to
explanations
(b) and
(c),
the
inhibition
cancels
any
learning
that
would otherwise
occur,
so
that
attention responses
to any of
the
items never develop
to
significant
degrees.
We
prefer
hypotheses
(b) or
(c),
or any
similar
proposal
that
implies
that
attention
responses
do not
develop
in VM
paradigms.
Is
there
any
evidence
to
distinguish
these
views
from
hypothesis
(a)?
Only indirect
evidence
is
available
at
present,
but the
models
are
easy
to
test.
For
example,
after
VM
training
one
could introduce
new
items
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
155
as
targets, with other
new
items
as
distrac-
tors,
or
with
the
previously
trained
VM
items
as
distractors.
The old VM
distractors
should severely hinder performance
if
they
elicit attention responses developed during
previous
training. Other similar
tests
could
also
be
carried out,
but a
definitive
answer
is not yet
available
at the
time
of
this writing.
IV. A
Framework
for
Information Processing
This
section
of the
paper
will
be
organized
as
follows.
Section
A
will present
an
overview
of
the
system
and an
overview
of the
role
of
controlled
and
automatic processing. Section
B
will elaborate
on the
fundamental nature
of
controlled
and
automatic processing. Sec-
tion
C
will present
a
framework
for
search,
detection,
and
attention. Section
D
will dis-
cuss
how
automatic
and
controlled processing
are
utilized
in
memory storage
and
retrieval.
A. An
Outline
of a
General Theory
Memory
is
conceived
to be a
large
and
permanent
collection
of
nodes, which become
complexly
and
increasingly interassociated
and
interrelated through learning. Most
of
these nodes
are
normally passive
and
inactive
and
termed long-term
store,
or
LTS, when
in
the
inactive
state.
The set of
currently
ac-
tivated
nodes
is
termed short-term
store,
or
STS.
LTS is
thus
a
permanent, passive repos-
itory
for
information.
STS is a
temporary
state;
information
in STS is
said
to be
lost
or
forgotten when
it
reverts
from
an
active
to an
inactive phase. Control
of the
informa-
tion-processing system
is
carried
out
through
a
manipulation
of the flow of
information
into
and out of
STS.
These
control processes
include
decisions
of all
sorts, rehearsal, cod-
ing,
and
search
of
short-
and
long-term
store.
LTS
contains learned sequences
of
informa-
tion
processing which
may be
initiated
by a
control process
or by
environmental
or in-
ternal
information
input,
but are
then
ex-
ecuted
automatically with
few
demands
on
the
capacity
of
STS.
1.
Long-Term Structure
The
structure
of the
nodes making
up LTS
will
be
treated
as a
very general graph with
complex
interrelations among nodes.
An in-
dividual node
may
consist
of a
complex
set
of
information
elements,
including associa-
tive connections, programs
for
responses
or
actions,
and
directions
for
other types
of
information
processing. What then
sets
off
one
node
from
a
group
of
nodes?
One
node
is
distinguishable
from
a
group
of
nodes
be-
cause
it is
unitized,
that
is,
when
any of its
elements
are
activated (i.e., placed
in
STS),
all of
them
are
activated.
One
activated node
may of
course activate another node,
but it
does
not do so in all
situations, only when
the
context
or the
state
of the
information-
processing system
is
appropriate.
2.
Structural Levels
It
seems likely
that
the
structure
of
long-
term
store,
at
least
in
part,
is
arranged
in
levels (perhaps sometimes
in a
hierarchical
tree
structure).
By
levels
we
refer
to a
tem-
poral directionality
of
processing such
that
certain nodes activate other nodes
but not
vice
versa.
In
sensory processing, there
is a
tendency
for
increasing information reduction
as
successive levels
are
activated. Thus,
a
visually presented word could
first be
pro-
cessed
as a
pattern
of
contrast regions, colors,
regular
variations,
and so
forth; then
lines,
angles,
and
other similar features could
be
activated; then letters
and
letter names
and
verbal
or
articulatory codes; then
the
word's
verbal code;
and finally, the
meaning
and
semantic correlates
of the
word.
This
se-
quence
is
meant
as an
example,
and we do
not
wish
to
imply
that
these
are the
relevant
features,
that
this
is the
only possible order-
ing,
or
that
this listing
is
exhaustive. Such
a
sequence
of
feature
encoding should occur
automatically
to a
normally skilled reader.
3.
Automatic
Processes
An
automatic process
can be
defined
within
this
system
as a
sequence
of
nodes
that
nearly
always becomes active
in
response
to a
par-
ticular input configuration, where
the
inputs
may be
externally
or
internally generated
and
include
the
general situational context,
and
where
the
sequence
is
activated without
the
156
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
necessity
of
active control
or
attention
by
the
subject.
An
automatic sequence
differs
from
a
single
node
because
it is not
necessarily unitized.
The
same nodes
may
appear
in
different
automatic sequences, depending
on the
con-
text.
For
example,
a red
light might elicit
a
braking response when
the
perceiver
is in
a
car,
and
elicit
a
walking, halting,
or
traffic-
scanning response when
the
perceiver
is a
pedestrian.
Since
an
automatic process utilizes
a
rela-
tively
permanent
set of
associative connec-
tions
in
long-term store, most automatic
processes
will
require
an
appreciable amount
of
training
to
develop
fully.
Furthermore,
once
learned,
an
automatic process will
be
difficult
to
suppress
or to
alter.
When
an
automatic sequence
is
initiated,
its
nodes
are
activated
and
hence
the as-
sociative information enters STS.
This
fact
does
not, however, mean
that
the
various
elements
of the
automatic process must
be
available
to
consciousness
or
recallable
at a
later
time.
The
activation
in STS
could
be
extremely
brief
(in
msec, say)
and
unless
attention
is
directed
to the
process
when
it
occurs
or
unless
the
sequence includes
an
automatic attention-calling response, then
the
information
may be
immediately lost
from
STS,
and the
subject
may be
quite
un-
aware
that
the
process took place. Even
when
an
automatic-attention response
is
part
of
the
sequence,
it
will
not
necessarily
affect
ongoing
controlled processing unless
the
strength
of the
attention response
is
suffi-
ciently
high.
4.
Thresholds
of
Activation
However
an
automatic process
is
initiated,
whether
by
internal
or
external input
or by a
control
process,
it is
presumed
that
the
prob-
ability
that
the
process will
run to
comple-
tion
depends
on the
strength
of the
initiating
stimulation.
The
simplest
and
most common
examples
are
seen
in
studies
of
psychophysical
thresholds.
If a
letter,
for
example,
is
visually
presented
at a
low-enough
intensity
or a
short-enough
duration, then
it may be en-
coded
not as a
letter
but as a
partial
collec-
tion
of
line-like features.
At
lower durations
or
intensities even these features will
not be
activated.
Although
we
have described automatic
processes
as
largely beyond subject control,
some
indirect control
is
possible through
ma-
nipulation
of the
activation threshold
for
auto-
matic processes.
In
particular, according
to
what
we
shall
call
the
contextual
hypothesis,
the
activation threshold
can be
lowered
by
the
inclusion
of
information
in STS (at the
time
of
presentation
of the
activating input)
that
is
associatively related
to the
nodes
making
up the
automatic
sequence.6
Note
that
a
lowering
of the
threshold does
not
imply
that
the
quality
of
processing
is
improved.
One
result
of
threshold lowering
is
that
the
automatic process will
be
triggered
by
inputs
that
normally would
and
should
not do so. For
example,
the
feature
"horse"
might
be
incorrectly triggered
by the
input
"house"
if the
threshold
for
"horse"
is
lowered
sufficiently.
S.
Controlled Processes
A
controlled process utilizes
a
temporary
sequence
of
nodes
activated
under
control
of,
and
through attention
by, the
subject;
the
sequence
is
temporary
in the
sense
that
each
activation
of the
sequence
of
nodes requires
anew
the
attention
of the
subject. Because
active attention
by the
subject
is
required,
only
one
such sequence
at a
time
may be
con-
trolled without interference, unless
two se-
quences each requires such
a
slow sequence
of
activations
that
they
can be
interwoven
serially.
Controlled processes
are
therefore
tightly
capacity-limited,
but the
cost
of ca-
pacity limitations
is
balanced
by the
benefits
deriving
from
the
ease
with which such
processes
may be set up,
altered,
and
applied
in
novel situations
for
which automatic
se-
6
It may
also
be
possible
to
lower
the
threshold
by a
.recent
activation
of the
same automatic
se-
quence.
However, this hypothesis
is
difficult
to
distinguish
from
the
contextual one,
because
an
activation
of a
sequence
is
usually accomplished
by
the
prior,
.recent
presentation
of
information
as-
sociatively
related
to the
sequence,
and
this
related
information
is
still likely
to be in STS at the
time
of
test.
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
157
quences have never been learned. Controlled
processing operations utilize short-term store,
so the
nature
of
their limitations
is
deter-
mined
at
least
in
part
by the
capacity
limita-
tions
of
STS.
6.
Short-Term
Store (STS)
Short-term store
is the
labile
form
of the
memory system
and
consists
of the set of
concurrently activated nodes
in
memory.
The
phenomenological
feeling
of
conscious-
ness
may lie in a
subset
of
STS, particularly
in
the
subset
that
is
attended
to and
given
controlled
processing.7
The
capacity
of STS is
determined sto-
chastically (see
Shiffrin
&
Cook, Note
3)
so
that
a
large amount
of
information
may
be
present (activated)
at any one
moment,
but
only
a
small amount
of
information will
persist
for an
appreciable time period
of
several seconds
or
more. Forgetting
or
loss
from
STS is
simply
the
reversion
of
cur-
rently active
information
to an
inactive
state
in
LTS.
What
determines
the
loss rate?
We
sup-
pose
that
the
rate
of
loss
of any
informational
element
or
node
in STS
depends
on the
num-
ber of
similar elements simultaneously active
in
STS.
By
similarity
we
refer
not
only
to
formal
physical similarity
but
also
to
sim-
ilarity
of
features
at
comparable levels
of
processing (e.g.,
the
typeface
of a
printed
word
will
be
less likely
than
the
word itself
to
cause forgetting
of a
verbally encoded
second word
in
memory).
When
a
large
amount
of
similar information
is
present,
the
loss
rate
will
be
rapid
but
will slow
as the
amount
of
active information decreases.
To
give
an
example, when
a
complicated visual
scene
is
presented
to a
subject
briefly
in a
tachistoscope,
a flood of
visual information
enters
STS
and
initiates
a
series
of
chains
of
automatic processing
that
result
in
higher
level features' also entering STS. However,
most
of the
activated
information
will
have
decayed
and
will
be
lost
from
STS in
just
a few
hundred milliseconds
after
physical
offset
of the
scene (see Sperling
1960);
just
a few of the
features, perhaps
at
higher levels,
will
remain present longer than
a few
seconds.
STS has two
somewhat distinct roles.
The
first
is
the
provision
of a
temporary store-
house
for
information currently important
to
the
organism.
That
is, it
acts
as a
selective
window
on LTS to
reduce
the
amount
of
information
for
processing
to
manageable
proportions.
The
second role
of STS is the
provision
of a
work space
for
decision making,
thinking,
and
control processes
in
general.
7.
Learning:
Transfer
to LTS
Consider
first
what
is
meant
by
transfer
from
STS to
LTS. Transfer implies
the
for-
mation
in
permanent memory
of
information
not
previously
there.
To be
precise,
this
con-
sists
of the
association
(in a new
relation-
ship)
of
information
structures
already
in
LTS.
A
minimal requirement
for
this
new
associative structure
is the
simultaneous
presence
in STS of the
separate
elements
to
be
associated
or
related.
That
is, the
various
nodes
to be
linked
in a new
relationship must
be
activated
in
STS.
Thus,
transfer
to LTS
does
not
imply
the
removal
of the
transferred
information
from
STS,
nor the
placing
of
new
"subunits"
in LTS
that
do not
already
exist
in
either
LTS or
STS. Rather, transfer
means
the
formation
of new
associations
(or
the
strengthening
of old
associations)
be-
tween
information structures
or
nodes
not
previously associated
(or
strengthened)
in
LTS. Most
new
associative structures
will
include
as a
component
the
context
in STS
at the
time
of the
transfer.
The
above remarks specify
the
nature
of
new
learning
in STS but not the
cause under-
lying
the
storage mechanism.
It has
been
7
An
alternative formulation, closer
to
that
sug-
gested
by
Atkinson
and
Shiffrin
(1968),
would
use
the
term
STS to
refer
to
those nodes
that
are
given
attention
and
controlled processing. Other
activated nodes would
be
referred
to by
another
term, such
as
"sensory
register"
in
Atkinson
and
Shiffrin's
terminology.
At
such
a
general level
of
discussion,
it is
doubtful
that
there
are
substantive
differences
between
the two
approaches—versions
of
each could
be
constructed
to be
identical
to
each
other. Each approach
has its own
heuristic
ad-
vantages,
but a
theory
is
better judged
in
terms
of
its
detailed assumptions
and
predictions than
by
its
choice
of
either
type
of
terminology.
158
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
shown
that
rehearsal (and coding rehearsal)
are
strongly implicated
in
learning (see
Shiffrin,
197Sb,
for a
discussion).
We
prefer
an
extension
of the
rehearsal hypothesis.
We
assume
that
what
is
stored
is
what
is at-
tended
to and
given controlled processing.
Rote rehearsal
is
just
one
form
that
attention
can
take.
In
fact, maintenance rehearsal
may
result
in
storage
of
low-level auditory
or
verbal codes
not
helpful
for
long-term recall,
(but
helpful
for
long-term
recognition),
while
coding
rehearsal
may
result
in
storage
of
high-level
codes
useful
for
recall. This model
suggests
that
some degree
of
attention
or
controlled
processing
is a
prerequisite
for
storage.
Thus,
incidental learning situations,
in
which
no
extended rehearsal takes place,
will
still result
in
some learning
to the
degree
that
the
input items
are
attended
to
during
presentation.
These
hypotheses
that
controlled processing
will
underlie learning apply
of
course
to the
development
of
automatic processing,
and
in
particular,
to the
development
of
auto-
matic detection studied
in the
search
and
detection paradigms earlier
in
this paper
and
in
Part
I. In
those paradigms
it was
seen
that
automatic
detection
developed
only with
consistent training. Consistent training
is
crucial
when
the
learned sequences
in LTS
contains
an
internal
or
overt response
that
will
be
harmful
to
performance
on
trials
that
are
inconsistent.
For
example,
an
attention
response
to an
item will harm performance
on
trials when
that
item
is a
distractor. Purely
informational
sequences (i.e., sequences
that
do
not
include responses
that
direct internal
processes
or
overt actions) will
be
stored
in
LTS
when attended
to,
regardless
of
con-
sistency
of
training.
In all
cases,
however,
consistent training
and
large numbers
of
repetitions
should lead
to
stronger automatic
encodings
and
processes.
An
implication
of the
hypothesis
that
trans-
fer
to LTS is
engendered
by
controlled pro-
cessing
is the
important
concept
that
con-
trolled
processing,
and
hence attentional
limitations,
will
be
involved during
the ac-
quisition
of
automatic processing.
We
have
largely
been
identifying
attentional limita-
tions
with controlled processing (e.g., con-
trolled search)
and
have shown
how
auto-
matic processing (e.g., automatic detection)
can
bypass attentional limitations.
It
should
not
be
overlooked
that
the
initial learning
of
automatic processing
may
require
a
variety
of
control processes
and
hence
will
be
sub-
ject
to
various limitations
that
may
dis-
appear when learning
has
progressed
to a
high
level.8
8.
Retrieval
of
Information
from
Short-Term
Store
Several retrieval modes
from
STS are
pos-
sible.
An
automatic process might direct
the
retrieval process
to
just
a
subset
of the ac-
tivated information (e.g., only
the
letters,
not
the
masks,
are
compared
in our VM
search
studies).
The
various controlled search strategies
that
are
available
are
normally serial
in na-
ture
but a
variety
of
search orders
is
possible.
Search
order
may
depend upon instructions,
strength
of
short-term traces,
the
nature
of
categories
of the
traces,
the
modality
of the
information,
and the
structure
of
STS.
This
last
point
is
worth emphasizing: Since
STS
is
embedded
in
LTS,
it
partakes
of the
struc-
ture
of LTS and is not a
totally
undiffer-
entiated mass
of
information.
Thus
the
order-
ing
of a
search
can
utilize
this
existent struc-
ture.
In
addition,
the
comparison process
may
be
exhaustive
or
terminating,
and the in-
formation
located
in one
phase
of the
search
can
be
used
to
redirect
a
later
phase
of the
search.
It is the
control
of
search order
that
is
responsible
for
many
of the
selective atten-
tion
effects
that
are
observed. Information
in
locations
to
which attention
is first
directed
(i.e.,
the first
locations
searched)
will
in
general
receive
faster
and
more accurate
processing
for the
obvious reasons.
8
This distinction
is
helpful
in
understanding
the
relation
of our
work
to
studies
of
attention
in
infrahuman
organisms:
the
traditional
studies
of at-
tention
in
discrimination
learning
and
blocking (e.g.
see
Mackintosh,
1975)
are
involved with those
limitations
that
occur
during
acquisition, while
a
number
of
newer studies
are
involved with limita-
tions
in
steady state situations when learning
is not
possible
(e.g.
see
Riley
and
Leith,
1976).
160
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
following
general properties:
(a)
They
are
limited-capacity processes
requiring
attention.
Because
these limitations prevent multiple
control
processes
from
occurring simulta-
neously,
these processes
often
consist
of the
stringing
together
in
time
of a
series
of
singly
controlled unitary
operations,
(b) The
limitations
of
control
processes
are
based
on
those
of STS
(such
as the
limited-comparison
rate
and the
limited amount
of
information
that
can be
maintained without
loss),
(c)
Control
processes
can be
adopted
quickly,
without
extensive training,
and
modified
fairly
easily (though
not
always
by
verbal
instruction),
(d)
Control processes
can be
used
to
control
the flow of
information within
and
between levels,
and
between
STS and
LTS.
In
particular, they
can be
utilized
to
cause permanent learning (i.e.,
transfer
to
LTS)
both
of
automatic sequences
and of in-
formation
in
general,
(e)
Control processes
show
a
rapid development
of
asymptotic per-
formance.
For a
given control process,
if
automatic processing does
not
develop (due,
say,
to
varied-mapping procedures)
and if
the
constituent elements
of the
process
do
not
otherwise change
due to
long-term learn-
ing,
then performance level will stabilize very
quickly
at an
asymptotic value. When per-
formance
improves over trials,
it
does
so be-
cause
the
control process
is
changed,
or be-
cause automatic processing develops,
or be-
cause
the
constituent nodes
that
are
linked
by the
control process
are
themselves altered
by
long-term learning.
Common
examples
of
control processes
in-
clude
maintenance
or
rote rehearsal, coding
rehearsal, serial search, long-term memory
search,
and
decisions
and
strategies
of all
kinds.
These
will
be
discussed
in
more detail
in
the
sections below.
2.
Automatic
Processing
Automatic processes have
the
following
properties,
(a)
They
are not
hindered
by the
capacity limitations
of STS and do not re-
quire
attention.
Thus
automatic processes
often
appear
to act in
parallel with
one an-
other
and
sometimes
appear
to be
indepen-
dent
of
each
other,
(b)
Some automatic
processes
may be
initiated under subject con-
trol,
but
once initiated
all
automatic processes
run
to
completion automatically (though
some
indirect control
is
possible through
manipulation
of the
contents
of STS at the
time
of the
inciting
input),
(c)
They
require
considerable
training
to
develop
and are
most
difficult
to
modify,
once
learned,
(d)
Their
speed
and
automaticity
will
usually
keep
their constituent elements hidden
from
con-
scious
perception,
(e)
They
do not
directly
cause
new
learning
in LTS
(though they
can
indirectly
affect
learning through forced
al-
location
of
controlled
processing),
(f)
Per-
formance
levels
will
gradually improve over
trials
as the
automatic sequence
is
learned.
In
many
cases
asymptotic
performance
levels
may not be
reached
for
thousands
of
trials.
It is
particularly important
to
specify
care-
fully
what
we
mean when
we say
that
an
automatic process does
not
partake
of the
capacity limitations
of
STS.
To
make this
clear,
let us
suppose
that
X, Y, and Z are
nodes
that
are
automatically activated
in
turn
by
input
I in the
presence
of
general
context
nodes
C. We may
depict
the
sequence
as
follows:
C C
As
each
of X, Y, and Z is
activated,
it
enters
STS and its
future
residence
in STS
will
be
affected
by the
limitations
of
STS.
In
fact,
if
the
nodes
X, Y, and Z do not
include
an
attention-attracting response, then
it is
quite
conceivable
that
all
three
nodes will
decay
and be
lost
within
a few
hundred milliseconds,
and
the
subject
will
be
quite unaware
that
such
an
automatic sequence ever occurred.
Nevertheless,
the
sequence itself
is not
governed
by the
limitations
of STS in the
sense that
the
probability
of its
being
ac-
tivated
and the
rate
of its
occurrence
will
not
be
affected
by
other concurrent automatic
and
conrolled processes taking place
in STS
(at
least
if
context
C is
present
and the
other concurrent processes
do not
also utilize
nodes
X, Y, or
Z).
3. The
Development
oj
Automatic
Processing
The
tendency
for one
node
to
activate
another will
be
increased
if
both nodes
are
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
161
present simultaneously
in STS and if a
con-
trol process
and
attention
are
directed toward
these nodes, thereby increasing their salience
in
STS.
Before
we can
describe
efficacious
training
conditions,
we
must distinguish between
two
types
of
automatic processes.
One
type
of
automatic sequence
may be
called
actional,
because
it
includes
phases
that
direct internal
processes (like calls
for
attention)
or
phases
that
produce overt responses (such
as
button
presses).
A
second type, called
informational,
contains
no
directions
for
actions.
The
dis-
tinction
is
crucial because
an
informational
sequence strengthened
on one
trial will
not
have deleterious consequences
for
perform-
ance
on
other types
of
trials.
On the
other
hand
an
actional sequence
may
give rise
to
a
response antagonistic
to a
response
re-
quired
on
another trial.
Thus,
to be
useful
in
a
given task, actional sequences require
special, consistent, training conditions.
To
make this point clear, suppose
that
node
B
produces
a
response antagonistic
to
that produced
by
node
C,
while nodes
D and
E
produce
no
responses.
If any of
A-B, A-C,
A-D,
or
A-E
is
trained alone, then
that
sequence
can be
learned.
If
training
on A-D
is
mixed
from
trial
to
trial with training
on
A-E, then
A
will come
to
elicit
both
D and E.
However,
if A-B
trials
are
mixed with
A-C
trials then learning
of
both
will
be
impossible,
since
A
cannot lead simultaneously
to two
antagonistic responses.
The
automatic
de-
tection
system discussed
at
length
in
this
paper
is
just
an
actional sequence, since
an
automatic-attention response
to one
stimulus
is
incompatible with
a
simultaneous atten-
tion
response
to
another stimulus; therefore
automatic detection requires consistent train-
ing
to
develop.
Thus,
in the
varied-mapping
conditions
of any of our
studies automatic
detection could
not
develop
and
controlled
processing
had to be
utilized.
4.
Combinations
of
Automatic
and
Controlled
Processing
Although
sensory inputs
are first
encoded
with
the
automatic processing system, with
the
results being made available
to
con-
trolled
processing,
it
must
not be
concluded
that
automatic processing invariably precedes
controlled
processing.
In
fact,
automatic
and
controlled processes
can
proceed
in
parallel
with
one
another
(as in
Experiment
4d
when
automatic processing
of the
elements
on the
invalid diagonal proceeded
in
parallel
with
controlled processing
of the
valid
diagonal).
Even more important, controlled processing
is
often
used
to
initiate automatic processing.
Particularly
in
complex processing situations,
(such
as
reading),
an
ongoing mixture
of
con-
trolled
and
automatic processing
is
utilized.
The
next
steps
in the
serial, controlled
processing sequence
are
based
on the
output
of
automatic processes initiated earlier; then
new
automatic sequences
are
initiated
and
these
run to
completion
in
parallel with
the
ongoing
controlled processing.
5.
The
Benefits
of
Automatic
and
Controlled
Processing
A
system based
on the two
basic processing
modes
with
the
characteristics
we
have
de-
scribed
has
many advantages.
In
novel situa-
tions
or in
situations requiring moment-to-
moment
decisions, controlled processing
may
be
adopted
and
used
to
perform
accurately,
though slowly.
Then
as the
situations become
familiar,
always requiring
the
same sequence
of
processing operations, automatic processing
will
develop, attention demands
will
be
eased,
other controlled operations
can be
carried
out in
parallel with
the
automatic processing,
and
performance will improve. Some
of the
advantages
of
such
a
system
are (a) It
allows
the
organism
to
make
efficient
use of a
limited-capacity processing
system.
The de-
velopment
of
automatic processing allows
the
limited-capacity system
to be
cleared
and
devoted
to
other types
of
processing neces-
sary
for new
tasks,
(b) It
allows attention
to be
directed (through automatic-attention
responses)
to
important stimulation, what-
ever
the
nature
of the
ongoing controlled
processing,
(c) It
allows
the
organism
to
adjust
to
changes
in the
environment
that
make previously learned
activity
patterns
useless
or
harmful,
(d) It
allows
the
organism
to
deal with novel situations
for
which auto-
162
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
Long
term store
s
N
S
0
R
Y
I
N
P
U
T
S
Short
term store
Level
*=K
"T*
i
i
i~
(^-
Level
Automatic
2
Encoding
v~
^f
s*
Automatic
attention
response
4
^
^"'
/ /
"V
*~
'
/
Level
N-1
*t=3
K=3—
\,
V.
,<!^
YAttentioiT(
\
director
)
Level
N
•^,
t
r
/
Controlled processing
M=)
/""
he
ku
es
C
b
omatic
ponse
Dntr
oiled
response
R
E
S
P
0
N
S
E
P
R
0
D
U
C
T
I
0
N
0
U
T
P
U
T
S
Figure
11.
A
model
for
automatic
and
controlled processing during tasks requiring detection
of
certain input stimuli. Short-term store
is the
activated subset
of
long-term store.
N
levels
of
auto-
matic encoding
are
shown,
the
activated nodes being depicted within each level.
The
dashed arrows
going
from
higher
to
lower levels indicate
the
possibility that higher level features
can
sometimes
influence
the
automatic
processing
of
lower level features.
The
solid arrow
from
a
node
in
Level
2
to the
attention system indicates that this node
has
produced
an
automatic-attention response,
and
the
large arrow
from
the
attention system
to
Level
2
indicates that
the
attention system
has
responded.
The
arrow
from
level
N to the
Response Production indicates
that
this node
has
called
for
an
automatic overt response, which will shortly
be
executed.
The
arrow
from
Controlled
Processing
to the
Response Production indicates
the
normal mode
of
responding
in
which
the
response
is
based
on
controlled comparisons
and
decisions. Were
it not for the
automatic responses
indicated, detection would have proceeded
in a
serial, controlled search
of
nodes
and
levels
in an
order chosen
by the
subject.
matic
sequences have
not
previously been
learned,
(e) It
allows
the
organism
to
learn
increasingly complex modes
of
processing
by
building
upon automatically learned sub-
systems.
C.
A
Framework
jor
Processing
in
Detection,
Search,
and
Attention
Tasks
The
framework
we
have
in
mind
is
built
within
the
general theory already presented.
It is
illustrated
in
Figures
11
and
12.
When
a
set of
inputs
is
presented (let
us
suppose
for
convenience
that
the
inputs
are
visual)
then each begins
to
undergo automatic
processing.
The
system automatically encodes
each stimulus input
in a
series
of
stages
and
activates
a
series
of
features
in the
process.
For
example,
the
letter
"M" may first be
encoded
in
features indicating contrast,
color,
and
position; then curvature, con-
vexity,
and
angles; then
a
visual letter
code
and a
verbal,
acoustic-articulatory
code,
then
the
codes
"letter,"
"consonant,"
"capital;"
and finally,
perhaps,
semantic
and
conceptual codes like "followed
by
'N',"
"middle
of the
alphabet,"
and the
like.
(We
do
not
necessarily
imply
that
these
features
are
correct
or
exhaustive;
if,
say, amplitude
components
of the
spatial-frequency analysis
of
the
inputs prove
to be
relevant
features
encoded
by the
system,
the
theory
we de-
scribe
would
be
unchanged
in all
important
respects.) What features
will
become
ac-
tivated depends
on the
physical nature
of the
nervous
system
that
was
predetermined gen-
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
163
etically,
the
degree
and
type
of
prior
learning,
the
physical characteristics
of the
display
(like
duration
and
contrast),
and the
general
context, both
that
in the
environment
and
that generated
in STS by the
subject.
To the
extent
that
the
subject directs
the
sensory
receptor orientation
and to the
extent
that
internally generated information
can
alter
the
context
in
STS,
the
subject will have
at
least
some
indirect
control
over
automatic
sensory
coding.
The
automatic
processing
as
described
above
takes
place
in
parallel
for
each
of the
input stimuli.
The
processing
of
each stimulus
is
often
independent, except
for
lateral
and
temporal interactions
at
early stages, called
masking,
and
except
for
learned relationships
between
items
that
may
affect
processing
at
later
stages
(if
adjacent
letters
form
a
word,
for
example).
The
various features
that
are
activated
are all
placed
thereby
in STS
where
they reside
for a
short period
before
being
lost
(i.e.,
before
returning
to an in-
active
state
in
LTS).
We
propose
that
some
of the
features
or
nodes
that
are
automatically
activated
may
initiate
a
response
that
will direct subsequent
processing
or
subsequent
actions.
For ex-
ample,
an
attention response might
be ac-
tivated
by a
feature;
the
attention response
might direct controlled processing
to the
cor-
responding
set of
features representing
that
input stimulus,
so
long
as
other
competing
attention responses
do not
occur simulta-
neously.
As
another
example,
an
overt
re-
sponse might
be
engendered
by a
particular
stimulus
(such
as a
startle
response
to a
sudden loud noise,
a
galvanic skin response
to
an
aversively conditioned word,
a
ducking
response
to a
missile thrown
at the
head).
If
the set of
inputs contains
a
target stimulus
that
gives rise
to a
relevant
nonconflicting
attention response,
or to an
overt response,
then
we say
that
automatic
detection
is
operating. Note
that
the
attention response
could
be
attached
to
features
at any
level
of
processing
and to
more than
one
feature
at a
time.
In
Figure
12, as an
example,
at-
tention
responses
are
attached both
to the
feature
"8" and to the
feature "number."
Inputs
^\
IV
h"\S
I
Visual
featuresVisual
character
code
Category
code
Higher
level
codes
Controlled
processing
Son's
age"
'Model
railroad
track"
etc.
Figure
12.
Another
view
of the
model shown
in
Figure
11.
This
figure
depicts
a
conceivable (but
abbreviated)
series
of
feature
encodings
for a
frame
in
which
two
characters
and two
masks
are
presented.
The
'Consistent-mapping
condition
is
shown
in
which numbers
are
memory-set items
and
letters
are
distractors.
The
arrows skipping levels indicate that
a
given
feature
can
help
activate
features
at
several
different
levels.
The
stimulus
8 is a
consistent-mapping
target,
and
hence
both
the
visual
and
category codes produce automatic-attention responses.
The
attention
response
has
attracted
attention
to the
information deriving
from
the
input
8.
164
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
It is
possible
that
many
of the
inputs will
give rise
to
attention
or
other responses
and
that
these responses will
conflict.
For ex-
ample, every input might automatically
engender
an
attention response; since
it is
impossible
to
direct attention
to
every item
in
the
display
at
once,
the
various attention
responses
will
conflict
and
cancel each other.
In
such
cases,
the
subject will
not be
governed
by the
output
of the
automatic system
and
will
base subsequent actions
and
responses
on
the
results
of a
controlled search. These
hypotheses were supported
in
Experiment
2
(see Figure
4,
left
panel
for the
results).
In
this study,
all
inputs were items
that
sub-
ject
had
previously been trained
to
respond
to
with automatic-attention
responses.
The
results showed that
the
subjects reverted
to
the use of
controlled search;
furthermore,
the
controlled search
was
almost identical
to
that seen
in the
usual
VM
conditions
in
which
none
of the
inputs
had
been trained
to
elicit
automatic-attention responses.
In
cases when
the
input stimuli
do not
activate automatic responses
that
govern
the
detection process, then
a
controlled search
must
be
used.
In
this event,
the
subject must
attempt
to
search through
the set of
features
that
is
activated
and to use a
search strategy
that
is as
efficient
as
possible.
For
example,
in
our
visual
search
studies,
letter
codes
are
normally
compared
in
preference
to
(and
prior
to)
sets
of
angles
and
lines;
a
category
code
representing
a set of
letters
is
often
compared
in
preference
to
(and prior
to)
individual
letter codes,
as
shown
in
Experi-
ment
3.
Furthermore,
the
order
of
search
and the
placement
of
decisions within
the
search
are
also controlled
in an
attempt
to
increase
efficiency.
To
give
an
example, com-
parisons
in our
studies
in
Part
I
took place
in
an
order
that cycled through
the
frame
for
a
given memory-set item
before
switching
to a new
memory-set item,
and a
matching
decision
was
made
after
every comparison.
To
give another example, matching decisions
in
many memory search studies
(M
varying,
F
1)
are
withheld until
all
comparisons
are
completed (Sternberg,
1975).
Note
finally
that
many attention tasks
utilize
search
or
detection paradigms. When-
ever
this
is the
case,
the
framework
described
here applies equally
to
attention
tasks.
Divided-attention
tasks
study
the
increments
in
performance
that
may
occur when
the
load
is
decreased. Decreased loads
are
usually
specified
through advance instructions
that
certain inputs
are
irrelevant, thereby
de-
creasing
the
size
of the
memory set,
the
frame
set,
or
both.
According
to our
framework,
experiments
that
do not
show dividing attention
to re-
duce
performance
fall
into
two
classes.
One
class includes those studies
in
which auto-
matic detection
is
operating. These
are
usually
studies
utilizing
a CM
paradigm
and
high
degrees
of
practice. Divided-attention deficits
will
not be
seen because
the
target
stimulus
will
be
detected automatically,
in
parallel
with
the
other stimuli,
and
often
indepen-
dently
of
other stimuli.
An
exception
to
this
general
rule
may
apply when
two (or
more)
targets
are
presented simultaneously
on a
trial.
Even
if
both targets generate attention
responses, there
may be a
difficulty
in
dis-
criminating
a
double
from
a
single occurrence.
In
such
an
event,
the
controlled comparison
system
may
have
to be
called into
play
to
count
the
targets.
(A
detection decrement
may
then occur, because
the
second item
may
have decayed
from
STS by the
time
the
comparison
of the first
item
is
complete;
see
Moray,
1975,
Shiffrin,
1976,
and the
discussion
of the 0
spacing
effect
in the CM
condition
of
Experiment
3,
Part
I.)
The
second
class
of
studies
in
which
di-
vided-attention
deficits
do not
occur
is
that
in
which controlled search
is
utilized
but the
capacity
of STS is not
stressed. Examples
are
seen
in the
series
of
studies
by
Shiffrin
and his
colleagues, summarized
in
Shiffrin's
(1975a)
article.
In
such studies either
the
load
in STS is
kept low,
or a cue
informing
the
subject
which input
is
relevant appears
at
about
the
same time
as the
inputs.
It is,
however,
only
in
specially
designed
situations that capacity limits
are not
stressed.
In
most studies requiring controlled search,
the
extra time required
to
search
a
larger
number
of
relevant inputs will impair per-
formance,
whether measured
by
reaction
time
or
accuracy. This
was the
case
in the
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
165
VM
conditions
of the
experiments reported
in
this paper,
and in
Part
I: In all
these
cases increases
in
load reduced performance.
There
are two
basic reasons
for the
perform-
ance
reduction
in
controlled search studies.
First,
some
of the
features
in STS may
decay
and
revert
to LTS
before
the
comparison
process reaches them. Second,
new
inputs
may
arrive
and
require processing,
forcing
the
comparison process
to
switch away
from
the
previous inputs.
Probably
only
this
second
factor
operated
in the
studies
we
have
re-
ported
in
this paper.
For
either
of
these
reasons, divided-attention
deficits
can be ex-
pected
in
detection tasks requiring controlled
search.
The
situation
is
quite
different,
however,
in
focused-attention studies.
In
these studies,
certain inputs
are
known
to be
relevant,
and
others
are to be
ignored.
If
controlled search
is
being utilized, there
is
little reason
to ex-
pect
that
any
substantial
deficit
will
be
caused
by the
presence
of
to-be-ignored stim-
uli,
at
least
if it is
clear
to the
subject well
in
advance which stimuli
and
locations
are
relevant
and if
there
are no
location con-
fusions.
In
such cases
the
subjects should
direct
the
search order
so
that
the
relevant
locations
are
searched
first;
thus
perform-
ance deficits should
not
arise.
Evidence sup-
porting these propositions
is
found
in
Experi-
ment
4a:
Subjects were able
to
search
one
diagonal
and
ignore
the
other.
The
tasks
in
which large
deficits
in
focus-
ing
attention will occur
are
those
in
which
irrelevant items give rise
to
attention
re-
sponses during automatic encoding.
In
such
cases, attention will
be
attracted
to the to-
be-ignored
item
and a
loss
of
time will occur
before
the
controlled search
can be
redirected
to the
relevant inputs.
The
time lost will
impair
performance. Such focused-attention
deficits
were seen
in the
reversal results
of
Experiments
1 and 2, and in the
attention
results
of
Experiment
4.
Thus,
our
framework
can be
applied
to a
wide
variety
of
search, detection,
and at-
tention
tasks, although additional assump-
tions
must
be
made
to
generate
precise
models
to
deal with
the
particulars
of
each task.
In
Part
I we
presented
a
quantitative serial,
terminating
search model
and
discussed
some
alternative models.
Our
discussion
was
limited,
however,
to our
basic
single
or
multiple-frame
search task with small values
of
M and
F.
We
will
now
consider
a few
alternative paradigms, along with special
assumptions they might require.
1.
Tasks Utilizing Large Memory Sets
Suppose
there
are too
many items
in the
memory
set to be
maintained
in
STS.
Of
course, then,
the
memory
set
must
be
learned
in
LTS in
advance
of the
test.
There
are
then
two
basic search modes.
In the first,
the
test item
is
automatically encoded
and
accesses
a
node
in
memory
that
contains
the
desired
information.
For
example,
in
deciding
whether
an
item
is any
word,
the
input item,
if
a
word,
is
encoded
to the
nodes represent-
ing
the
word
and the
information
in
those
nodes
is
usually
sufficient
to
classify
the
item
as a
word;
on the
other hand,
a
non-
word
obeying appropriate orthographic con-
straints
is
encoded
to a
lesser degree
and the
failure
of the
automatic
encoding
process
could
itself
serve
as a cue for
"nonwordness."
The
second type
of
search mode would con-
sist
of
entering successive parts
of the
mem-
ory set
from
LTS
into STS, using controlled
search
to
examine
each
subset
in
STS.
For
example,
if
asked whether there
is a flower,
country,
or first
name whose
fourth
letter
is
"u"
a
subject might successively generate
members
of
each category
and
serially check
them.
The
above comments make
it
clear
that
the
case
of
large memory sets
is not
different
in
principle
from
the
cases
in
which
the
memory
set may be
held
in
STS,
though
the
models
may
need additional assump-
tions.
For
example, Atkinson
and
Juola
(1974)
carried
out a
study
in
which sub-
jects
had a
large memorized
set in
LTS.
They
proposed
a
hybrid
model
in
which
a
judgment
based
on
familiarity enabled
the
subject
to
decide
on
some trials without
a
search
that
the
test item
was
definitely
in,
or
definitely
not in, the
memorized set.
On
trials
when
this
initial familiarity judgment
was
ambiguous, then
a
controlled search
through
the set was
made.
166
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
2.
Large Display Sets
When large numbers
of
items
appear
in the
display, then
it may
turn
out
that
acuity
varies considerably across
the
display
for a
given
fixation
point.
In
turn, acuity changes
are
likely
to
lead
the
subject
to
adopt
a
strategy
in
which
eye
movements
are
used
to
bring subsets
of the
display successively
into
a
high-acuity region.
The
location
of
each
new fixation can be
governed
by a
controlled
search
strategy
or by an
automatic
process,
but in
either event
the
necessity
of
obtaining good acuity will
force
this process
to be
serial across successive
eye
movements.
Within
each
fixation,
furthermore, either
automatic detection
or
controlled search could
be
utilized.
The
search studies
in
which sub-
jects scan rows
of
characters
for
targets
have
shown
both modes
of
search.
For
example,
the
Neisser
(1963)
results, reviewed
in
Part
I,
showed
that
automatic detection developed
for
subjects well trained
in
responding
to
letter
sets
presented
in CM
fashion
(memory-
set
size
had no
effect),
while controlled search
was
utilized
for
sets
that
had
been given
only small amounts
of
training.
An
interest-
ing
intermediate case occurred when
the
sub-
ject
was
instructed
to
locate
a row of
char-
acters
that
did not
contain
a
given character.
In
this
case,
even with
CM
training,
the
task
proved quite
difficult.
In our
view
the
sub-
ject would have
to
adopt
a
serial process
from
row to row
(rather than
from
fixation
to fixation).
Within each
row
automatic
de-
tection might
be
used
to
locate
the key
character,
but
then
a
decision would have
to be
made
before
the
next
row
could
be
considered.
3.
Categorical Partitioning
oj
Display Sets
Partitioning
of the
display
set has
usually
been accomplished
by a
categorization
de-
pendent
on a
relatively gross simple physical
feature,
such
as
color, shape,
or
size. When
displays
are
segregated into
two or
more
groups
by
such features, processing
may be
affected
in two
ways.
First,
the
patterning
of
the
input
can
govern
the
nature
of
auto-
matic
processing—controlled
processing
may
be
directed automatically
to a
subset
of the
inputs (assuming
that
one
particular subset
is
consistently
relevant).
Second,
the
per-
ceptual categorization could
influence
the
order
and
perhaps
the
nature
of
controlled
search.
To
give
one
simple example,
a
dis-
play arranged
in two
separate rows will tend
to
be
processed
one row at a
time,
in
reading
order.
A
second example occurs
in
situations
where
controlled processing
can
enable
one
to
more quickly decide about
the
category
of
the
input than about
the
memory-set
membership
of the
input.
In
these cases
a
two-phase controlled search might
be
adopted,
with
the first
phase locating
the
relevant
inputs
by
category,
and the
second phase
matching these inputs
to the
memory set.
This
is
illustrated
by
Green
and
Anderson's
(1956)
and
Smith's
(1962)
studies
in
which
a
two-digit number
of a
particular color
had
to be
found
in a
display
of
many two-digit
numbers
of
differing
colors.
Automatic processing based
on
perceptual
categorization
may
have played
an
important
role
in the
studies
of
search
and
detection
reported
in
this paper.
We
have assumed
throughout
that
only
the
display positions
with
characters,
and not
those with masks,
are
considered
in the
search.
How is
search
restricted
to
character positions?
It is
con-
ceivable that
a
preliminary controlled search
is
used
to
identify
character positions,
but
the
results
of
Experiment
4a
argue against
such
a
view—knowing
the
relevant diagonal
of
a
four-character
frame
leads
to
perform-
ance identical
to
that
seen when
two
masks
and two
characters appear randomly
on
each
frame.
Thus,
a
preliminary controlled search
would
have
to be
extremely
fast
relative
to
the
time
for
each character comparison. More
likely,
an
automatic process develops
that
directs
the
controlled search away
from
mask
positions
and
toward character positions.
Since
such
a
response would
be
consistently
trained
and
reinforced,
it
should
be
learned
in
a
fashion similar
to
that
for
automatic-
attention responses.
It
should
be
noted
that
the
development
of
automatic responses
to
direct controlled
search
is
very similar
to the
process termed
by
Neisser
(1967)
as
"pre-attentive."
Neisser
was
trying
to
explain
how
search could
be
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
167
directed
to a
perceptually segregated subset
of
the
inputs.
We are
suggesting
that
an
automatic process might develop
and
direct
the
search
in
situations where
the
search-
directing response
is
consistently trained
(as
is
usually
true
in
such
studies).
We do not
feel,
however,
that
the
segregation
of the
relevant inputs must
be
based
on
simple
physical
features.
With
enough
consistent
training,
an
automatic search-directing
re-
sponse could develop
for a set of
stimuli
defined
by
quite complex features.
For ex-
ample, Gould
and Cam
(1973)
reported
results suggesting
that
an
automatic process
was
directing
search
to the
locations
of
items
from
a set of 10
potentially relevant letters
and
away
from
the
locations
of
items
from
a
set of 8
other
letters
that
never included
a
target. (Any target
was
always drawn
from
the
potentially relevant
set of 10
letters,
but
on
most
trials
distractors
were chosen
from
this
set of 10 as
well
as
from
the set of
8).
Although
the
evidence
is not yet
available,
we
raise
the
possibility
that
the
converse
of
the
argument
in the
preceding paragraph
might also hold: Even with perceptual seg-
regation
of the
display
into categories deter-
mined
by
simple physical
features,
auto-
matic restriction
of the
search
to the
relevant
category might
not be
possible unless
the
relevant category
is
consistent across trials.
4.
Attention
Tasks
The
models applying
in
attention studies
that
utilize
detection
or
search
paradigms
are
just those discussed
in the
preceding
sections.
The
general
rule
is
that
instructional
manipulations
affect
the
order
of
controlled
search; increases
in
load cause
the
time
needed
for
controlled search
to
increase;
and
consistent
training
leads
to the
development
of
automatic responses
that
allow attentional
limitations
to be
bypassed.
We
will
say a few
words, however, con-
cerning
attention studies that
do not
utilize
detection
or
search paradigms.
One
class
of
studies
utilizes
a
memory paradigm.
The
inputs
are
manipulated
in
ways similar
to
those used
in
detection studies,
but the
sub-
ject
is
asked
to
recall
or
recognize
the
items
at
some time
after
presentation.
One
example
is
the
"split-span"
technique
reviewed
by
Broadbent
(19S8, 1971). Other examples
are
reported
by
Sperling (1960)
and Von
Wright
(1970),
who
studied cued partial
report following
tachistoscopic
exposure
of
character displays.
In
these memory paradigms, essentially
the
same underlying mechanisms
apply
as
in
the
detection paradigms, though memory
storage rather than detection
is the
processing
goal. When
controlled
processing
is
utilized,
the
subject
has
considerable control over
storage:
The
items
that
are
rehearsed
or
coded
are the
items
that
will
be
recalled.
If
an
input
is
presented
that
generates
an
automatic-attention response, then this
in-
put
will receive
attention
and
tend
to be
recalled
regardless
of the
nature
of the
con-
trolled process utilized
to
store
the
other
items.
An
excellent
demonstration
of
these
points
may be
found
in
Kahneman
(197S).
5.
Threshold
Detection
Tasks
An
input
in a
threshold
detection
task,
by
definition,
is
presented
in
such
a way
that
automatic encoding
on
most
trials
will
be
incomplete
and
therefore ambiguous.
That
is,
in
Section IV.A.4
we
discussed
the
fact
the
input stimulation must
be
above some thresh-
old
level
for
automatic encoding
to run to
completion
in an
accurate
fashion.
In
Figure
12,
for
example,
the
"M,"
if
presented
near
threshold, might cause activation
of
some
of
the
features
at the
visual
feature
level,
but
no
features
at
higher
levels.
This
set of
partial features
will
in
general
be
ambiguous
—several
letters
and
numbers might
be
con-
sistent
with
the
activated
feature set.
Thus,
a
target will
on
some trials give rise
to
feature
sets consistent with either targets
or
distrac-
tors.
Similarly,
distractors
will
on
some
trials
give
rise
to
feature
sets consistent with either
targets
or
distractors. Under these conditions
it is
clear
even
in CM
situations
that
auto-
matic-attention
responses
and
automatic
de-
tection
can
only develop very slowly,
if at
all,
because
the
internal
representations
of
targets
and
distractors will
be
mapped con-
sistently
to
responses only
on
those
few
trials
when
the
encoding
happens
to be
complete.
PERCEPTUAL
LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC
ATTENDING
169
tern.
Rote rehearsal
is of
course only
one of
many control processes
that
cause items
to
have
an
extended residence
in
STS; coding,
deciding, retrieving,
and the
like also cause
a
similar result, though these processes
are
primarily intended
to
serve other purposes.
Iq
general, items given attention
of any
sort
are
maintained
in
STS, including items
to
which
attention
is
drawn
by an
automatic
process.
Items
from
which attention
has
been
re-
moved
do not
necessarily leave
STS
quickly,
however. When
the
load
is
small
and new
inputs
are
minimized, items
can
remain
in
STS for
extended
periods
(up to 40 sec in
the
Shiffrin,
1973, study). Another example
is
seen
in the
overt forced-rehearsal study
reported
in
Atkinson
and
Shiffrin
(1971).
A
long
list
of
items
was
presented
and
subjects
rehearsed aloud
a
continually updated list
of
the
three
most
recent
items.
As a
result,
the
last
three items presented were always
recalled
on an
immediate
test.
However,
the
probability
of
recall
for
items prior
to the
last
three
decreased
systematically
as a
func-
tion
of the
spacing
from
last
rehearsal
to
test.
Thus
an
item
removed
from
controlled
processing still requires
a
period
of
time
before
it
becomes
lost
from
STS
(i.e.,
be-
comes
inactive).
This
persistence
in the
absence
of
attention
might
be
called
the
auto-
matic component
of
short-term maintenance.
It is
this
automatic component
of
persistence
that
was
studied
by
Shiffrin
and
Cook (see
Note
3) and
that
determines
the
basic
ca-
pacity
and
persistence
of
STS.
In
brief,
Shiffrin
and
Cook propose
that
the
loss
process
is a
stochastic mechanism whose
rate
increases
as the
amount
of
similar
ma-
terial concurrently
in STS
increases.
2.
Coding,
or
Transfer
to LTS
The
role
of
control processes
in
long-term
storage
is
well
established.
Rote
rehearsal
appears
to
transfer low-level auditory-verbal
codes
to
LTS; these
are not
very useful
for
long-term recall
but can
facilitate long-term
recognition. Coding
rehearsal
appears
to
facilitate long-term retrieval
of all
kinds.
In
general, what
is
attended
to and
rehearsed
is
what
is
stored
in
LTS. (See Bjork,
1975;
Craik
&
Jacoby,
1975;
Craik
&
Lockhart,
1972;
and
Craik
&
Tulving, 1975,
for a
dis-
cussion
of
these issues.)
The
other side
of the
storage question con-
cerns
the
nature
of LTS
transfer when
at-
tention
is not
directed
to an
item
or
sequence
of
items. Studies
of
incidental learning tend
to
show that subjects learn what they attend
to, not
what they
are
told
to
learn (see Craik
&
Tulving, 1975; Hyde
&
Jenkins, 1969,
1973;
Postman,
1964; Schneider
&
Kintz,
1967).
If
controlled
processing
is
necessary
for
long-term
learning then automatic processing
without controlled processing should
not
lead
to
appreciable retention.
One
source
of
evi-
dence
is
found
by
examining retention
for
distractors
in CM
tasks. Such items pre-
sumably
have
received
almost
no
controlled
processing. However,
it may be
assumed
that
due
to
prior exposures
and
prior learning
the
distractors
are
given automatic encoding
at
least
up to the
"name"
level (for evidence
see
Corteen
&
Wood,
1972;
Keele,
1972;
Von
Wright
et
al.,
1975, among others).
Does
the
automatic encoding that these dis-
tractors receive during
the
many
trials
of a
CM
task
lead
to any
retention? Moray
(1959)
had
subjects repeat aloud
a
prose
passage
in one ear
while
a
seven-word list
was
repeated
35
times
in the
other ear. Later
recognition
for the
unattended words
was at
the
chance level. Gordon (1968) showed that
a set of
four
distractors
in a CM
search task
was
recognized near
the
chance level even
after
10
days
of
practice.
Gleitman
and
Jonides
(1976)
showed
that
distractors were
more poorly recognized
in a CM
search
task
than
in a VM
search task (though,
due to
the low
levels
of
practice used
in
their study,
the
effect
could have been
due to the use of
a
controlled search
for
categories
in the VM
condition).
Evidence
of a
rather
different
sort
is
found
in
reading
tasks.
If
automatic processing does
not
lead
to
retention,
then
it
might
be ex-
pected
that
a
skilled reader
who
automatically
processes
surface-structure features
and who
gives
controlled processing
to
conceptual
properties
of a
passage
would
retain
little
information
regarding
the
surface-structure
170
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER
SCHNEIDER
details. Bransford
and
Franks
(1971)
have
collected
data
that
may be
interpreted
in
this fashion.
Of
course,
if
controlled process-
ing
is
directed toward levels
of
analysis
normally carried
out
automatically, then
the
features
attended
to
will
be
remembered (see
Postman
&
Senders,
1946).
To
summarize, whether
or not
some
nominal
storage manages
to be
eked
out in
the
absence
of
controlled
or
attentive process-
ing,
it
seems clear
that
any
phenomenon
that
would
be
appropriately called
"automatic
storage"
is a far
less important determinant
of
LTS
learning than controlled processing.
Before
leaving
the
general topic
of
learn-
ing
it is
interesting
to
speculate
on the de-
velopment
of
complex information-processing
skills.
Since controlled processing
is
limited,
only
a
small part
of the
memory system
can
be
modified
at any one
time.
After
invariant
relations
are
learned
at one
stage,
the
pro-
cessing becomes automatic
and
controlled
processing
can be
allocated
to
higher levels
of
processing.
For
example,
the
child learning
to
read
would
first
give control processing
and
then
give
automatic processing
to
various units
of
information,
The
sequence
of
automatically
learned units might
be
foreground-back-
ground,
features, shapes,
letters,,
words,
and
meanings
of
phrases
or
sentences.
The
child
would
be
utilizing controlled processing
to
lay
down
"stepping
stones"
of
automatic
processing
as he
moves
on to
more
and
more
difficult
levels
of
learning.
The
transition
from
controlled
to
automatic processing
at
each stage would result
in
reduced discrimina-
tion
time, more attention
to
higher order
features,
and
ignoring
of
irrelevant
infor-
mation.
Gibson (1969, chap.
20)
describes
these
effects
to be
three
of the
major trends
in
perceptual
development.
In
short,
the
staged development
of
skilled automatic per-
formance
can be
interpreted
as a
sequence
of
transitions
from
controlled
to
automatic
processing.
3.
Retrieval from
LTS
Retrieval
from
LTS has a
large automatic
component. Sensory
inputs
result
in an
auto-
matic series
of
stages
of
encoding
that
ac-
tivate many features (and perhaps responses)
and
place them
in
STS. Internally generated
inputs
also
tend automatically
to
activate
associated information
in LTS and to
place
it in
STS.
In
general,
the two
primary controlled
phases
of LTS
retrieval consist
of the
pro-
cesses
the
subject uses
to
search
and
process
the
information
in STS
that
has
just been
activated,
and the
processes used
to
alter
the
probe information
from
step
to
step
of the
LTS
search.
It is
important
to
note
that
the
selection
of
probe information
is not
entirely under
subject
control.
In
fact,
the
probe
informa-
tion includes
the
general
contextual
informa-
tion
present
in STS at the
time
of
retrieval,
in
addition
to the
specific
cues
the
subject
manipulates
to
facilitate retrieval.
The
gen-
eral context
is
only
partly
under subject con-
trol,
since much
of it is
environmentally
de-
termined
and
even
the
internally generated
context (e.g., what
the
subject
is
currently
"thinking
about")
may be
difficult
to
alter
radically. Thus, changes
in
probe cues will
be
under subject control
to
only
a
degree,
and
this
fact
is one of the
factors underlying
retrieval
failure.
This
extremely brief
and
superficial over-
view
should
not be
allowed
to
give
the
reader
an
impression
that
distinguishing
the
auto-
matic
and
controlled phases
of
retrieval
is
generally
a
simple
matter.
Furthermore,
the
controlled
phase
can be a
most complex
and
many-faceted
process.
To
give just
one ex-
ample,
Anderson
and
Bower
(1973)
consider
how
subjects compare test sentences with
stored
sentences
in
long-term memory.
In
our
model, there
are two
phases
to
this
ex-
periment:
(a) a
retrieval
of the
appropriate
informational
nexus
from
long-term memory
through
the use of
probe information (in-
cluding
the
test question)
and (b) a
com-
parison
in
short-term memory
of the
test
question
and the
retrieved search set.
The
Anderson
and
Bower model,
in the
present
view,
is
primarily
a
model
of the
second
of
these
processes—the
comparison within
short-term memory
of the
test sentence
and
the
retrieved information.
See
Shiffrin's
(1970,
1975b, 1976) studies
for a
more
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
171
elaborate
discussion
of
long-term
retrieval
processes.
V.
Models
of
Search
and
Detection
In
this
section
the
theory
we
have
pro-
posed will
be
compared with some
of the
previous models
that
have been proposed
for
detection
and
search
tasks.
A.
The
Sternberg
Model:
Serial, Exhaustive
Search
The
serial, exhaustive search model
is
pre-
sented
in
Sternberg (1966). Extensions,
further
evidence,
and
reviews
of the
litera-
ture
are
given
in
Sternberg (1969a, 1969b,
197S).
This
model
is
meant
to
apply
to
memory
search tasks
(F
1,
M
varies)
with
small memory-set sizes.
It has
been
confirmed
impressively
often
but is
limited
in
scope,
as it is
meant
to
apply only
to a
small range
of
paradigms.
In our
present
theory, serial, exhaustive search
is
considered
to be one of the
controlled
search
strategies,
one
that
is
very commonly adopted.
The
factors
that
lead subjects
in our
situ-
ation
to
adopt terminating, controlled search
remain uncertain
at
present. Possibly
the
much
larger
search
loads
in our
study
led to
a
terminating strategy.
One
other minor
dis-
crepancy
in
results
involves
the
Sternberg
(1966)
finding
apparently showing
fixed
sets
(CM)
and
varied sets
(VM)
to
give equiva-
lent set-size
functions.
In
retrospect
we can
see
that
Sternberg's subjects
in the fixed set
procedure were given
far too
little
training,
and the fixed
sets
were
varied
too
often
for
automatic detection
to
have developed.
1.
The
Relation
Between
Sternberg's Model
and
Our
Framework
Reaction time
was
proposed
by
Sternberg
(1969a,
1969b)
to be the sum of
motor
re-
sponse time
and the
times
for
four
additive
stages:
(a)
encoding
(affected
by
stimulus
legibility),
(b)
serial comparison
(affected
by
size
of the
memory
set),
(c)
binary
de-
cision
(affected
by
whether
a
positive
or
negative
trial
has
occurred),
(d)
translation
and
response organization
(affected
by
rela-
tive
frequency
of
positive
and
negative
trials).
In
Figure
11,
in
which
our
more
general
framework
is
depicted, these
stages
proposed
by
Sternberg
may be
placed
as
follows:
"Encoding"
is
equivalent
to
auto-
matic encoding;
"serial
comparison
and bi-
nary decision" both
are
part
of
controlled
processing;
"response
organization
and mo-
tor
response execution"
are
part
of
response
production.
In
conclusion, then,
the
Stern-
berg model
differs
from
the
detailed quan-
titative model
we fit to our
data
in
Part
I,
but
nevertheless
may be
viewed
as a
special
case
of our
general framework.
2.
Problems
for the
Serial, Exhaustive
Comparison
Model
and
Their
Resolution
The
serial, exhaustive model
is
particu-
larly
important
on
account
of the
research
it
has
generated
that
give results
not
consistent
with
the
theory
in its
simplest
form.
These
inconsistencies have
led to
many
new
models,
some
of
which will
be
discussed below.
The
apparent
inconsistencies
include
the
follow-
ing
results.
1.
Reaction times depend
on the
serial
presentation position
of the
tested item
within
the
memory
set (in VM
procedures
with
memory sets changing every trial).
The
most pronounced
effects
occur when
the
memory-set items
are
presented quickly
and
the
display item
is
presented very soon
after
the
last
memory-set items (Burrows
&
Okada,
1971;
Clifton
&
Birenbaum,
1970;
Corballis,
1967;
Klatzky,
Juola,
&
Atkin-
son,
1971;
Klatzky
&
Smith,
1972).
2.
Reaction times depend
on the
relative
frequency
of
presentation
of
items within
the
memory
set in CM
procedures
(see Bie-
derman
&
Stacy;
Krueger,
1970;
Miller
&
Pachella,
1973;
Shiffrin
&
Schneider,
1974;
Theios
et
al.,
1973).
3.
Reaction times
can be
speeded
for
cued
items
in VM
paradigms (Klatzky
&
Smith,
1972)
or
speeded
for
expected items
in CM
paradigms
(Shiffrin
&
Schneider,
1974;
but
note
that
the fixed
sets
changed
every
1,60
trials
so
that
only
low
degrees
of
practice
were
involved),
or
speeded
in a VM
para-
digm
for an
item repeated during
the
pre-
172
RICHARD
M.
SHIFFRIN
AND
WALTER SCHNEIDER
sentation
of the
memory
set
(Baddeley
&
Ecob,
1973).
4. The
outcomes
in CM
paradigms
often
differ
in
fundamental ways
from
those
in
VM
paradigms,
especially when some simple
physical
basis
separates
the
memory
and
dis-
tractor
sets,
or
when
the
degree
of
training
is
high. Many such
findings
have been dis-
cussed extensively
in
earlier sections
of
this
paper
and
will
not be
reviewed again here.
5.
Subjects typically give incorrect
re-
sponses
on
1-10%
of the
trials, depending
upon
the
condition.
The
serial search models
do
not
generally posit explicit mechanisms
to
predict errors.
Before
turning
to
alternative models,
it is
useful
to
consider
how
these phenomena
are
dealt with
in the
framework
of our
theory,
and
also
to see how
Sternberg explains
the
results.
The
various
findings in CM
paradigms
are
easiest
to
explain: Automatic detection
de-
velops
that
enables
the
serial search
to be
bypassed. Much
of the
research
in
this
paper
was
directed toward establishing this fact.
Sternberg
(1975)
is
less
specific
but
also
sug-
gests
that
some alternative, more
efficient,
search process
is
used
in
such situations.
Within varied-mapping paradigms,
re-
course
to the
hypothesis
of
automatic detec-
tion cannot
be
used
to
explain
the
results.
It
would
be
parsimonious
if
each
of the
above
findings
(1)
to
(3)
proved
to be the
result
of a
common mechanism.
Shiffrin
and
Schneider
(1974)
proposed
one
possible mech-
anism—namely,
that
when
information
con-
cerning
test
probabilities
is
available
to the
subject, then
one
item
is
placed
in a
special
state prior
to
each
test
display (called
a
state
of
"expectancy"),
and
that
a
test
of
the
expected item results
in a
faster
response
time than
tests
of
nonexpected items. Stimu-
lus
probability, serial position, cueing, stim-
ulus repetition,
or
differential
importance
could
all be
expected
to
determine
the
prob-
ability with which stimuli will
be
expected.
The
effects
of
expectancy could possibly
act at
several
different
stages
en
route
to the
execution
of the
response.
One
possibility,
also mentioned
by
Sternberg
(1975),
is
that
the
comparison process
is
carried
out in a
speeded
fashion
for an
expected item.
An-
other possibility
is
that
the
encoding process
or
the
response production stage
is
speeded
when
an
expected item
is
tested.
Shiffrin
and
Schneider
(1974)
attempted
to
carry
out a
test discriminating among
the
possible mod-
els. While
the
general notion
of
expectancy
was
supported
by the
results,
the
study
was
not
conclusive
in
determining which version
of
the
model
was to be
preferred.
The
important point
to be
emphasized
is
that
we
(and Sternberg also) suggest
that
findings
(1)
to (3)
listed above
are not in-
compatible with
a
controlled search process
that
is
serial
and
exhaustive.
We
suggest these
findings can be
explained
by
factors
that
af-
fect
other stages
of the
response process
than
the
comparison stage
or
perhaps
by a
factor
that
affects
the
comparison process only
on
certain trials. Many other researchers have
preferred
to
discard
the
hypothesis
of a
serial,
exhaustive
comparison process. Some
of the
models
proposed
as
alternatives will
be
dis-
cussed
below.
The final
problem
for the
serial, exhaustive
search
model
is
posed
by the
occurrence
of
errors.
The
explanations
of
controlled search
processes
by
Sternberg (and
by us) for re-
action time tasks
do not
propose
an
explicit
mechanism
by
which errors
may
occur. Many
investigators have simply ignored errors
as
long
as the
error rate
rate
is
low, under
5%,
say.
This
is
perhaps justifiable
if
errors
are
anomalous
events
that
do not
interact with
any of the
other variables being studied.
In-
deed
the
stability
of findings
across numerous
studies that
do not
attempt
to fix
error rates
at any
given value
and
that
vary consider-
ably
in the
observed
rates
(in the
range
0-
10%)
lends some support
to the
view
that
ignoring
errors will
not
distort
the
conclusions
drawn
from
the
reaction time
data.
In
principle, however, error rates cannot
be
ignored. Pachella
(1974)
has
shown
that
instructions used
to
change error rates
by
just
a few
percent
can
cause considerable
changes
in the
level
and
pattern
of
reaction
times. There
are
many ways
in
which error-
producing
mechanisms
can be
appended
to
controlled search models.
Two of the
most
PERCEPTUAL LEARNING
AND
AUTOMATIC ATTENDING
173
important
are as
follows:
First,
the
subject
might terminate,
or be
forced
to
terminate,
the
controlled search
before
the
search
is
completed.
Then
a
response would have
to
be
made
as a
guess based
on
partial
informa-
tion
at
most,
or
perhaps
no
response
at all
would
be
made
(an
omission).
These
types
of
error resulting
from
early search termina-
tion account
for
almost
all
errors
in
search
and
attention
tasks
using above-threshold
stimuli with accuracy
as a
measure;
in
par-
ticular, most
of the
errors
in the
accuracy
tasks
reported
in the
present paper
and in
Part
I are
errors
of the
type. Second,
the
search process might
be
carried
out
incor-
rectly;
that
is, a
comparison might
be
car-
ried
out
incorrectly owing
to
confusions
among
items, forgetting, misperception,
and
the
like. When error mechanisms
are ap-
pended
to the
basic search model
a new ex-
panded theory results, which must
be
tested
through joint consideration
of
error
data
and
reaction time
data.
Two
basic
approaches
may be
utilized
to
test
search theories incorporating error pre-
dictions.
One
approach involves manipulat-
ing
the
error rate systematically within each
condition;
the
manipulation
may be
carried
out
through instruction, deadline training,
or
signals-to-respond
(Reed,
1973).
This
ap-
proach
has the
advantage
of
making
full
use
of
both
the
error
and
reaction time
data
from
a
single experiment.
It has the
dis-
advantage
that
the
manipulations
of
error
rates
may
affect
the
nature
of the
controlled
search strategy adopted
by the
subject.
For
example, Reed (1976) carried
out a
remark-
able series
of
tests
of a
wide variety
of
models
using
data
collected
in the
signal-to-respond
procedure.
Unfortunately, though
one
model
could
be
singled
out in
preference
to the
others,
the
basic
data
differed
in
important
respects
from
those ordinarily
found
in the
simpler version
of the
same paradigm.
The
alternative approach involves carrying
out
two
separate studies involving
the
same
subjects.
One
study utilizes
the
usual
re-
action time methodology, with instructions
to
keep error rates low.
The
second study
utilizes
a
paradigm
in
which
the
available
search time
is
systematically varied
and the
error
rates corresponding
to
each amount
of
available search time
are
collected.
The
model
derived
from
one
study
can
then
be
used
to
predict
the
results
of the
other.
This
is the
method
that
was
adopted
in
Experiments
1
and
2 of
Part
I.
This
approach
has the ad-
vantage
of
relating
two fields of
inquiry
that
are
normally treated separately.
In the
present paper,
for
example, attention tasks
using
accuracy
as a
measure were linked
to
search tasks
using
reaction time
as a
measure.
It
should
be
noted,
by the
way,
that
the
type
of
error predicted
for the
results
of
Part
I/Experiment
1 is
that
caused
by
premature
termination
of
controlled search whose char-
acteristics were derived
from
the
reaction
time
results
of
Part
I/Experiment
2.
How-
ever,
the
errors seen
in
Part
I/Experiment
2
were
not
necessarily caused
by the
same
mechanism.
In
fact
many,
if not
most,
of
them
may
have resulted
from
confusions,
forgetting,
or
anomalous condition-indepen-
dent factors.
B. The
Theios
Model
There
are
really
two
separate
treatments
to
be
discussed here:
the
specific
micromodel
used
by
Theios
et
al.
(1973)
to
predict
re-
sults
that
would
otherwise
be fit by a
serial,
exhaustive
model,
and the
general theory
used
by
Theios (1973, 197S)
to
describe
the
production
of
response times
in a
variety
of
tasks.
1.
Serial, Terminating Search Through
a
List
of
Memory
Items
and
Distractors
In
this model
a
list
is
constructed
in
mem-
ory,
a
list
on
which appear
all
items
that
might
be
displayed
for
test.
Each
of
these
items
has an
associated response
cue
(posi-
tive
or
negative) attached
to it. The
memory-
set
items
and
distractors
may in
general
be
intermingled
in
this list,
but a
variety
of