ArticlePublisher preview available

Cognitive appraisals and transformation in delay behavior

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Investigated the effects of different cognitive representations of the rewards (outcomes) in a delay of gratification paradigm on children's ability to wait for these rewards. Ss were 60 3-5 yr olds in nursery school. It was found that consummatory (arousing) ideation directed at the relevant (contingent) rewards hindered effective delay. In contrast, cognitive transformations of the rewards which focused on their nonconsummatory qualities and associations significantly facilitated delay behavior more than did comparable ideation about similar rewards irrelevant to the delay contingency. Consummatory ideation focused on rewards irrelevant to the contingency also greatly helped to maintain delay. Theoretical implications for the role of fantasy and cognitive appraisal in self-control are examined. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
oj
Personality
and
Social Psychology
197S,
Vol.
31,
No. 2,
254-261
Cognitive
Appraisals
and
Transformations
in
Delay
Behavior
Walter
Mischel
Stanford
University
Nancy
Baker
University
of
California,
San
Diego
The
effects
of
different
cognitive representations
of the
rewards (outcomes)
in
a
delay
of
gratification
paradigm
on
children's
ability
to
wait
for
these
rewards were investigated.
Consummatory
(arousing) ideation directed
at the
relevant (contingent) rewards hindered
effective
delay.
In
contrast, cognitive
transformations
of the
rewards which focused
on
their nonconsummatory
qualities
and
associations significantly facilitated delay behavior more than
did
comparable
ideation about similar rewards irrelevant
to the
delay contingency.
Finally, consummatory ideation
focused
on
rewards irrelevant
to the
contin-
gency also greatly helped
to
maintain delay. Theoretical imph'cations
for the
role
of
fantasy
and
cognitive appraisal
in
self-control were examined.
In
view
of the
central importance
of the
"reinforcement"
concept
in
contemporary
psychology,
it is
surprising that
so
little
is
known
about
how the
mental representation
of
rewards
affects
the
individual's pursuit
of
them. There
is an
enormous discrepancy
be-
tween
the
theoretical
significance
attributed
to
rewards
in the
regulation
of
goal-directed
behavior
and our
lack
of
understanding
of
how
their cognitive representation
by the
subject
influences
his
behavior.
The
present
study
is
part
of a
program
to
explore this
topic
by
focusing
specifically
on how a
per-
son's ideation about
the
contingent rewards
in
a
choice situation
affects
his
ability
to
maintain goal-directed activity until
he
achieves
his
preferred outcomes.
Several experiments have indicated
that
the
capacity
to
sustain
self-imposed
delay
of
gratification
depends
in
part
on the
extent
to
which
the
person avoids cues
that
remind
him
of
the
rewards (outcomes)
that
he
expects
and
wants
but is
prevented
(interrupted,
blocked,
or
delayed)
from
getting
(e.g.,
Mischel
&
Ebbesen, 1970; Mischel, Ebbesen,
&
Zeiss,
1972;
Schack
&
Massari, 1973).
To
increase subjective frustration,
one
would
have
to
focus
cognitively
on the
blocked
goal objects
(e.g.,
by
engaging covertly
in
This study
was
supported
in
part
by
Grant
M
6830
to
Walter Mischel
from
the
National Institutes
of
Mental Health,
U. S.
Public Health Service.
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
Walter
Mischel,
Department
of
Psychology,
Stanford
Uni-
versity, Stanford, California
94305.
anticipatory goal
responses);
to
decrease
frus-
tration,
one
would have
to
suppress
the
goal
objects
by
avoiding them cognitively.
"Frus-
tration tolerance"
in the
delay paradigm
would
depend
on the
subject's ability
to
sup-
press
his
attention
to the
rewards while
re-
maining
in the
frustrative situation until
he
has
attained
the
goal.
While
this interpretation seems reasonable,
it is
probably incomplete. Indeed, Mischel
and
Moore (1973)
found
that
exposure
to
symbolically presented rewards (slides) dur-
ing the
delay period significantly increased
delay
of
gratification.
This
facilitative
effect
of
exposure
to
slides
of the
rewards
was di-
rectly opposite
to the
earlier
finding
that
visual exposure
to the
rewards themselves
greatly decreased delay time (Mischel
&
Ebbesen, 1970; Mischel
et
al.,
1972).
The
earlier
findings had
been obtained
in
basically
the
same subject population (i.e., preschool
children
in the
same nursery school)
and
from
an
essentially similar delay paradigm.
Nevertheless,
the
Mischel
and
Moore
study
showed
that
exposure
to
slides
of the
relevant
rewards
enhanced delay behavior more than
did
exposure
to
comparable distractions
(slides
of
similar
but
reward-irrelevant
ob-
jects
and
blank slides).
The
critical
difference
between
the
reward-relevant attention manip-
ulations
in the
earlier experiments
and in the
Mischel
and
Moore study
was
that
previously
children
had
been exposed
to the
actual
re-
ward
objects,
but in the
Mischel
and
Moore
experiment
they were exposed
to
symbolically
254
... For example, in classic delay-of-gratification experiments, children were taught to delay gratification in a given situation by imagining marshmallows, not as food, but as puffy white clouds, or imagining pretzels as long, brown logs (W. Mischel & Baker, 1975;W. Mischel & Moore, 1973). ...
... These include teaching children to execute specific cognitive strategies (W. Mischel & Baker, 1975;W. Mischel & Moore, 1980), developing self-regulatory skills through practice on curricular activities (Rybanska et al., 2018), labeling children with an identity as a "patient" person (Toner et al., 1980), influencing children's belief in the potential for exertion to be energizing (Haimovitz et al., 2020), and providing an environment that is reliable and trustworthy (and therefore one in which delay of gratification can be counted on to actually produce the promised larger reward), among other changes to children's social environments (Kidd et al., 2013;Ma et al., 2020;Michaelson & Munakata, 2016). ...
... Our first, preregistered experiment sought to establish the effect of manipulating a strategic mindset on the classic, single delay-ofgratification task (e.g., W. Mischel & Baker, 1975 Ebbesen, 1970;W. Mischel et al., 1972). ...
Article
Full-text available
Overcoming challenges to achieve success involves being able to spontaneously come up with effective strategies to address different task demands. Research has linked individual differences in such strategy generation and use to optimal development over time and greater success across many areas of life. Yet, there is surprisingly little experimental evidence that tests how we might help young children to spontaneously generate and apply effective strategies across different challenging tasks. We test this in an area important to development: delaying gratification. To do this, we developed a “strategic mindset” storybook that encouraged children, when waiting felt hard, to ask themselves strategy-eliciting questions, such as: “What can I try to be better at this?” In two experiments (N = 237), 5- to 6-year-old children who read the strategic mindset storybook with an experimenter (vs. a control storybook) waited significantly longer to receive desirable treats (Experiments 1 and 2) and to watch an appealing YouTube video (Experiment 2). Moreover, they were able to wait longer because they spontaneously generated and applied a greater number of effective waiting strategies. Going beyond classic research that taught children specific strategies to delay gratification, our results suggest that our new “metacognitive” approach can empower children’s self-regulation.
... Kopp's (1989) research on self-regulation suggested that young children required external regulation from parents before intrinsic self-regulatory abilities fully develop. However, some studies indicated that, without guidance, parents may lack sufficient knowledge of effective waiting strategies: they often encouraged children to focus on waiting or the delayed reward, which were found to be associated with a decrease in children's ability to wait (Hom & Knight, 1996;Mauro & Harris, 2000;Mischel & Baker, 1975;Mischel & Ebbesen, 1970). In contrast, children with parents who explained, helped them cope with waiting-related stress and frustration, or taught distraction strategies performed better in delay-of-gratification tasks (LeCuyer-Maus & Houck, 2002;Peake, Mischel, & Hebl, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is commonly attributed to neuro‐cognitive deficits of genetic and/or prenatal/perinatal environmental origins. Sonuga‐Barke proposed an alternative formulation, suggesting that ADHD behaviors are functional expressions of delay aversion—a strong motivational disposition to avoid or escape negative affective states evoked by delay. It is hypothesized that the strength of this disposition, though neuro‐biologically rooted, is exacerbated by early negative social interactions during waiting‐related encounters. This paper reports findings from an initial proof‐of‐concept study that specifically tests this hypothesis in a nonclinical sample. Methods Preschoolers (n = 112; mean age = 46.2 months) and their parents from London, UK, and Hong Kong participated in a longitudinal study. The Parent–Child Delay Frustration Task (PC‐DeFT) and two nonwaiting control tasks were administered at baseline. Children's performance, behavioral and emotional responses, and parents' reactions were observed. Teachers rated children's ADHD behaviors and delay aversion at baseline and follow‐up (12–18 months later). Results At baseline, children's maladaptive performance and parental negative reactions during the PC‐DeFT were correlated with each other and with teacher ratings of ADHD and delay aversion. Negative parental reactions during the PC‐DeFT at baseline predicted an increase in teacher‐rated ADHD behaviors at follow‐up, but similar associations were not observed for baseline parental responses in the nonwaiting tasks. The increase in child ADHD symptoms associated with negative parental reactions at baseline was statistically mediated by delay aversion. These longitudinal effects were consistent across the UK and HK samples. Conclusions The findings provide the first evidence that parent's negative reactions to preschooler's attempts to manage delay are associated with increases in ADHD behaviors overtime, and linked to delay aversion increases. They underscore the potential significance of the early social environment as a contributor to developmental trajectory of ADHD behaviors. Future studies with clinical samples over an extended time‐frame using a range of different aversive environments (i.e. difficult tasks to complete) are indicated.
... Conflicts over goals are not based solely on prioritization, since it is impossible for everything to happen at the same time, conflicts sometimes arise between goals over time, and this is something like conflicts between different selves, the "present self" and "future self" [26,27]. Such a conflict manifested itself in Walter Michel's famous experiment in 1972, where he presented four-year-old children with two offers: one marshmallow now, or two marshmallows 15 minutes later [28]. This experiment determined the challenge of rationality in the dimension of time. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This paper is a review of the goal-based definition of "Reason" that is rooted in David Hume's views and deals with reasons and conflicts arising from it in the form of "Goal and Time" conflicts, where the difference between the goals of "Metaphorical" and "Figurative" plays a vital role in the correct explanation of this concept. On the other hand, the paradoxes of rationality have long been considered in the form of "Cognitive Biases" in studies, but this paper presents a concept of "Rational Ignorance" and "Rational Irrationality", "Threats", "Madman Theory", "Taboo" and "Morality" that have not received much attention so far and explains why many political and religious beliefs are characterized not only by low information but also by systematic bias and high certainty.
Chapter
The virtues of endurance—perseverance, patience, resilience, constancy, longsuffering, and so on—are central to success in any goal-oriented challenging activity that a person undertakes. Gaining a rich understanding of these virtues requires insights from multiple areas of study, but most treatments of the virtues of endurance to date have remained within narrow disciplinary boundaries. No longer. In this volume, philosophers, psychologists, legal scholars, educational theorists, medical practitioners, military theorists, and theologians have labored together to advance our understanding of endurance. Written with scholars in mind, each essay is nevertheless accessible to students and general readers. This volume extends our understanding of the virtues of endurance.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Unlike the negative consequences of food craving such as overconsumption, in this study it is rather our aim to extract the positive aspects of craving by increasing it for healthy foods. We explain how the key components of craving (visual, gustatory and olfactory sensory cues) accompanied by some enhancer and suppressor variables, determine the magnitude of craving and how it will affect consumers’ price sensitivity, their willingness to pay, and finally their desire for food intake.
Article
Many policy makers and governmental organizations have started using generative artificial intelligence (AI) to provide advice to individuals. However, prior research paints an unclear picture of individuals’ receptiveness to the outputs generated by AI, relative to those from human advisors. While some studies (e.g., Longoni, Bonezzi, & Morewedge, 2019) show that individuals prefer outputs generated by humans over AI, others (e.g., Logg, Minson, & Moore, 2019) present an opposite pattern. To reconcile these mixed findings, this research differentiates two perspectives where relative preferences have been widely examined: (1) a bystander perspective, where consumers evaluate the content generated by human versus AI agents, and (2) a decision-maker perspective, where consumers accept recommendations made by the agents. We find that although there is a general trend of preferring human advice over AI advice in individual decision-making—exhibiting a “human superiority effect”—there is no significant difference between human and AI content preferences during bystander evaluations. Additionally, psychological distance constitutes an important contextual moderator explaining the relative preference for human versus AI recommendations. Specifically, when decision-making circumstances are perceived to be psychologically distant (e.g., low personal relevance), the human superiority effect is attenuated. Theoretical contributions are discussed, along with practical implications for businesses and governmental organizations.
Article
Effective emotion regulation is critical for establishing and maintaining positive relationships, and it has previously been linked to several indicators of social competence. Theories agree that one core characteristic of adaptive emotion regulation is the ability to flexibly adapt emotion regulation strategies to situational demands (i.e., strategy flexibility). However, little is known about the development of this ability and its association with social competence at preschool age. We hypothesized a positive association of preschoolers’ strategy flexibility with age, with overall regulation effectiveness as well as with different indicators of their social competence. To test these assumptions, we examined the emotion regulation strategies (i.e., distraction, gaze aversion, reappraisal, self-instruction, and self-soothing) of 60 preschoolers (M = 59.67 months; SD = 8.18; 45% girls) in two standardized situations, namely a frustration-eliciting persistence task and a challenging waiting situation, in which attentional deployment strategies have opposing effects, requiring flexible strategy use across situations. Children’s social competencies in interaction with caregivers, peers, and task demands were assessed through teacher reports. Results reveal that the effectiveness of attentional deployment strategies changed as predicted. Furthermore, preschoolers’ ability to adapt their emotion regulation strategies to the corresponding task demands was positively associated with age and overall regulation effectiveness. Concerning teacher-reported facets of children’s social competence, strategy flexibility was significantly associated with children’s integration into their peer group.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this article is to present, delimit, and integrate the proposals of economic behavior and behavioral economics, including their axioms, heuristics, and biases, to advance research in this field. The article recommends applying first economic and then psychological issues, in that strict order, for epistemological, legal and practical reasons. Based on human action or praxeology, the process of economic decision making and doing is graphed, these in turn frame systems of thought with their different heuristics, the latter of which are the cause of systematic errors called biases.
Article
Deciding how long to keep waiting for uncertain future rewards is a complex problem. Previous research has shown that choosing to stop waiting results from an evaluative process that weighs the subjective value of the awaited reward against the opportunity cost of waiting. Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) tracks the dynamics of this evaluation, while activation in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and anterior insula (AI) ramps up before a decision to quit is made. Here, we provide causal evidence of the necessity of these brain regions for successful performance in a willingness-to-wait task. 28 participants (20 female and 8 male) with lesions to different regions of the frontal lobe were tested on their ability to adaptively calibrate how long they waited for monetary rewards. We found that participants with lesions to the vmPFC waited less overall, while participants with lesions to the dmPFC and anterior insula were specifically impaired at calibrating their level of persistence to the environment. These behavioral effects were accounted for by systematic differences in parameter estimates from a computational model of task performance.Significance Statement Achieving positive outcomes in education, health or personal finance often involves pursuing larger future rewards at the cost of smaller, more immediate ones. Most neuroscience research on future reward pursuit has focused on the initial discrete choice between a smaller reward that will arrive quickly or a larger reward that will arrive later. However, once the choice has been made, persisting in the initial choice of the later reward through the waiting period is perhaps even more critical to success. Here, we identify specific and dissociable causal roles for different regions of prefrontal cortex in determining people's ability to adaptively persist. This finding extends our understanding of how the brain supports subjective value maximization in the context of delayed rewards.
Article
Full-text available
Describes 3 experiments with a total of 92 3-5 yr. olds. Exp. I compared the effects of external and cognitive distraction from reward objects on the length of time which Ss waited for a preferred delayed reward before forfeiting it for a less preferred immediate one. In accord with predictions from an extension of frustrative nonreward theory, Ss waited much longer for a preferred reward when they were distracted from the rewards. Exp. II demonstrated that only certain cognitive events (thinking "fun things") served as effective ideational distractors. Thinking "sad thoughts" produced short delay times, as did thinking about the rewards themselves. In Exp. III the delayed rewards were not physically available for direct attention during the delay period, and Ss' cognitive attention was manipulated by prior instructions. While Ss waited, cognitions about the rewards significantly reduced, rather than enhanced, the length of their delay of gratification. Overall, attentional and cognitive mechanisms which enhanced the salience of rewards shortened the length of voluntary delay, while distractions from the rewards, overtly or cognitively, facilitated delay. Results permit a reinterpretation of basic mechanisms in voluntary delay of gratification and self-control. (16 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Explored the role of attentional processes in voluntary delay of reward by manipulating children's attention to the rewards for which they were waiting in a delay-of-gratification paradigm. 32 preschool children waited for a preferred but delayed reward while facing either the delayed reward, a less preferred but immediately available reward, both rewards, or no rewards. The dependent measure was the amount of time they waited for the preferred outcome before forfeiting it for the sake of the less desired but immediately available one. Results contradict predictions from psychodynamic theory and from speculations concerning self-instructions during time binding. Unexpectedly, but in accord with frustrative nonreward theory, voluntary waiting time was substantially increased when Ss could not attend to rewards during the waiting period. Implications are discussed for a theory of the development of delay of gratification. (22 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Examined the effects of temporal aids and frustration level on the voluntary delay behavior of 60 lower-class, black, 1st-grade children. 3 types of temporal aids (sandglass timer plus instructions about its relevance, timer without instructions, and no timer control) and 2 frustration levels (visual presence or absence of rewards) were compared. In general, Ss waited longer when rewards were absent. However, Ss in the toys-present-time-instructions group waited longer than Ss in the other 2 toys-present groups and did not differ from Ss in the toys-absent groups. Results are discussed in terms of the facilitatory effect on waiting of the perception of a decrease in temporal distance from a goal.
Article
Full-text available
Proposes that diverse data challenge and undermine the central assumptions of the traditional trait approach to personality. The implications for conceptions of individual differences and situations in the study of personality are examined. The issues discussed include the nature of behavioral "specificity," the acquired meaning of stimuli, the uses and misuses of traits, and the construction of personality. The following cognitive social learning variables are proposed as basic units for the study of individuals: cognitive and behavioral construction competencies, encoding strategies and personal constructs, behavior-outcome and stimulus-outcome expectancies, subjective stimulus values, and self-regulatory systems and plans. The specific interactions between these person variables and psychological situations are analyzed within the framework of a cognitive social learning approach. (4 p. ref.)
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the effects of attention to symbolic presentations of the contingent rewards (in the form of slide-presented images) on children's ability to wait for the delayed reward. 69 female and 54 male preschool children served as Ss. In sharp contrast to the effect of attention to the actual rewards, attention to the symbolic contingent rewards greatly increased the duration of the Ss' delay of gratification. This unexpected result was obtained both in conditions of waiting and working and both for continuous and intermittent presentation of the slides. Overall findings reveal the opposite effects of attention to real vs symbolic contingent rewards in the delay of gratification paradigm. Theoretical implications of different cognitive functions served by attention to real and symbolic reward stimuli are discussed.
Article
The projective measurement of experimentally induced levels of sex motivation was studied. The TAT protocols were analyzed for manifest sex imagery and sex-involved guilt. Under normal conditions the experimental Ss expressed significantly less sex and guilt in the TAT stories than did the control Ss. Under conditions of alcohol, these results were just the reverse. This is interpreted by assuming that under normal conditions the guilt evoked by sexual arousal is sufficient to inhibit the expression of sex with a consequent lowering of guilt. Under alcohol, the guilt over sexual arousal is reduced enough to permit the expression of sex with a resulting increase in expressed guilt.