ArticlePDF Available

Do We Think Dreams Are in Black and White due to Memory Problems?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The present study was designed to investigate whether memory processes play a role in why some persons say their dreams are black and white. The findings indicate that the percentages of black and white dreams are related negatively to color memory and dream recall frequency. When colors were recorded immediately after the dream was recorded, the percentage of black and white dream elements dropped to 2.7%. When participants were presented the option that dream colors might not be remembered, the percentage of explicit black and white dreams became very small, and the findings are thus in line with the continuity hypothesis of dreaming. Future studies might use extensive training of color memory and dream recall in order to investigate whether highly trained persons still have some dreams or dream elements that are in black and white. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Content may be subject to copyright.
Do We Think Dreams Are in Black and White due to
Memory Problems?
Michael Schredl, Aylin Fuchedzhieva, Heike Ha¨mig, and Verena Schindele
Central Institute of Mental Health
The present study was designed to investigate whether memory processes play
a role in why some persons say their dreams are black and white. The findings
indicate that the percentages of black and white dreams are related negatively
to color memory and dream recall frequency. When colors were recorded
immediately after the dream was recorded, the percentage of black and white
dream elements dropped to 2.7%. When participants were presented the
option that dream colors might not be remembered, the percentage of explicit
black and white dreams became very small, and the findings are thus in line
with the continuity hypothesis of dreaming. Future studies might use extensive
training of color memory and dream recall in order to investigate whether
highly trained persons still have some dreams or dream elements that are in
black and white.
Keywords: colors, dream content, memory, dream recall frequency
Several studies (Bentley, 1915; Middleton, 1933; Husband, 1936; Tatibana, 1938)
carried out in the first half of the 20th century had large numbers of persons who
reported that they did not see colors in their dreams, for example, 40% (Middleton,
1942). Schwitzgebel (2003) repeated the Middleton study over 50 years later and found
that only 4.4% of the participants reported no colors in their dreams. If participants
were specifically asked whether they dream in color or black and white, the percentage
of persons with black and white dreams was extremely low: 0% (Schwitzgebel, 2003),
4% (N40.000 American online poll, cited in Schwitzgebel, 2002), 6.6% (Okada,
Matsuoka, & Hatakeyama, 2005), 10% to 22% (Chinese students; Schwitzgebel,
Huang, & Zhou, 2006). On the other hand, Stepansky et al. (1998) reported that 47%
of 1000 subjects answered no to the question “Do you have colored dreams?” This
clearly indicates that the wording of the question is important with regard to the results.
Laboratory research (Kahn, Dement, Fisher, & Barmack, 1962; Snyder, 1970; Re-
chtschaffen & Buchignani, 1983, 1992) showed that—even if the participants were
asked immediately upon awakening about colors in their dreams—about 20% of the
dreams did not include any color. So, the question whether there are dreams in black
and white is still unanswered.
Michael Schredl, Aylin Fuchedzhieva, Heike Ha¨mig, and Verena Schindele, Sleep Laboratory,
Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael Schredl, Sleep Laboratory,
Central Institute of Mental Health, PO Box 12 21 20, 68072 Mannheim, Germany. E-mail: Michael.
Schredl@zi-mannheim.de
175
Dreaming Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 18, No. 3, 175–180 1053-0797/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1053-0797.18.3.175
Schwitzgebel (2002) hypothesized that dreams might be neither colored nor
black and white, that the color modality is indefinite. That would explain why
persons in the era of black-and-white movies often said their dreams were black
and white whereas today most people say their dreams are in color. The continuity
hypothesis of dreaming (cf. Schredl, 2003), which states that dreams reflect waking
life, would predict that all dreams include colored elements in the same way our
waking world does (with the exception of black-and-white objects, of course).
According to this hypothesis, objects that are normally colored (e.g., skin, sky,
water) should not appear as black and white within dreams. The marked percentage
of persons who report that they do not remember whether their dreams are colored
or not (Schwitzgebel, 2003) suggests that memory might play a role. If the color is
not outstanding or important for the dream action, it might be difficult to remember
the colors; for example, the clothes’ colors of a person one talked to in the dream.
The present study was designed to investigate whether memory processes play
a role in explaining why some persons state that their dreams are black and white.
First, a new item eliciting color in dreams was developed, explicitly explaining that
dream colors might not be remembered, so that the participants can estimate the
percentage of dreams without remembered colors—in addition to colored dreams
and black and white dreams. Second, it was tested whether the percentage of black
and white dreams are related to color memory tasks carried out during the day.
Third, the participants were asked to write down a dream immediately upon
awakening at home and report the colors of all major dream elements. If memory
plays a role, one would expect that the number of black-and-white dream elements
would be smaller in this condition.
METHOD
Participants
The sample included 49 persons whose mean age was 21.8 years (SD 3.4).
These 40 women and 9 men were mainly psychology students. The participants
were recruited at the University of Mannheim.
Materials
Dream Questionnaire
Overall dream recall frequency was measured by a 7-point rating scale, 0
(never), 1 (less than once a month), 2 (about once a month), 3 (twice or three times
a month),4(about once a week),5(several times a week), and 6 (almost every
morning). The retest reliability of this scale for an average interval of 70 days is high
(r.85, N196; Schredl, 2004a). In order to obtain units of mornings per week,
the scale was recoded using the class means (0 30, 1 30.125, 2 30.25,
330.625, 4 31.0, 5 33.5, 6 36.5).
A brief description regarding colors in dreams was given before presenting three
rating scales (0% to 100% in 10% steps) eliciting the percentage of colored dreams,
176 Schredl, Fuchedzhieva, Ha¨ mig, and Schindele
black and white dreams, and dreams without explicit memory of colors. The descrip-
tion included comments regarding the three types of dreams. The color dreams could
include colors as they were in waking life but also colors that are bizarre, that is, those
of a red-white-dotted bird. The perception in black and white dreams was, compared
to looking at a black and white film. If no explicit color perception was remembered the
dreams were to be labeled as ones without explicit memory of colors. The total of the
three percentages should amount to 100%. The same three questions were given to
evaluate the color perception of dream elements.
Visual Memory
The first color memory task was a picture from a children’s book displaying a
large, complex circus scene with different objects in different colors. After looking
at the picture for three minutes, the participants had to answer nine questions about
the color of specific objects, for example, the color of the musicians’ hats. The sum
score ranged from 0 to 11 because two questions elicited two colors.
The second memory task consisted of 24 cards displaying different objects: 12
were black and white and 12 were colored. Six of the colored pictures showed
objects with a typical color like bananas, a strawberry, and a fire engine whereas the
other six pictures showed objects that might have a variety of colors (car, house
etc.). The cards were presented to the participants for 2 min. They were allowed to
scan them one by one but were not allowed to sort the cards or to look at more than
one picture at once. Between presentation and recall, the participants were asked
to read a text (about 15 min) and answer several questions about its content to
ensure that the participants had concentrated on the text. For measuring color
memory, the number of correctly recalled colored objects without typical colors was
included in the analysis.
Dream Record Sheet
Immediately upon awakening in the morning the participants were to record
the next dream they recalled as completely as possible. Next, they should list the
objects of the dream and state the color—if any—or whether the object was black
and white (like watching a black and white movie) or whether they are not able to
recall the color of the object. The sum of all objects per dream was computed as
well as the number of colored objects, black and white objects, and objects without
remembering the color.
Design and Procedure
First, the participants completed the dream questionnaire. Then the two visual
memory tasks were carried out. At the end of the session the participants received
the dream record sheet which they were to return to one of the experimenters.
Of 49 participants, 32 persons returned their dream record sheets.
Dreams in Black and White 177
Statistical analyses were carried out with the SAS 9.1.3 software package for
Windows.
RESULTS
Mean dream recall (recoded) for the total sample was 3.43 (SD 2.10) mornings
with explicit dream recall per week. The percentages of colored dreams or dream
elements (questionnaire) and dream elements (dream report), respectively, are de-
picted in Table 1. The participants rated most of their dreams and dream elements as
being colored but for a large number of dreams they could not remember the colors.
The percentage for colored objects, however, was significantly higher for the dream
reports where the colors were elicited directly upon awakening (d0.837, t4.7, p
.0001), whereas the percentage of black and white objects (d0.560, t⫽⫺3.2, p
.0034) and objects without remembering the color (d0.537, t⫽⫺3.0, p.0049)
decreased. Overall, 178 objects were reported in the 32 dream reports (word count:
M93.8, SD 70.4). All dreams included at least one colored element. None of the
participants reported that all of his or her dreams were black and white; the highest
percentage of this questionnaire item was 70%.
The mean score for the circus picture task was 4.84 (SD 1.85) and correlated
substantially (r.366, p.0096) with the score of the colored objects without
typical colors of Test 2. The mean number of correctly recalled objects was 3.92
(SD 1.24). In Table 2, the correlations between percentages of black and white
dreams/dream elements, dream recall frequency, and color memory scores are
shown. Dream recall frequency and the score of the circus picture task were related
significantly to two of the percentages regarding black and white dreams whereas
the memory task including colored objects did not correlate with the percentage of
black and white dreams.
DISCUSSION
The findings of the present study indicate that memory processes might play a role
in explaining why some persons say that some of their dreams are black and white.
First, the application of a new questionnaire item that included explicit in-
structions about color perception in dreams showed that the colors of dreams or
dream elements were often not remembered. None of the participants stated that
all of his or her dreams are in black and white, thus confirming the results of
Schwitzgebel (2003). Asking retrospectively about colors in dreams, only about
Table 1. Percentages of Colored Dreams and Dream Elements
Color perception
Dreams Dream elements
Elements of
dream reports
MSDMSDMSD
Colored (%) 55.1 33.2 59.2 29.2 82.8 22.4
Black and white (%) 9.4 15.6 9.2 12.0 2.7 5.8
Without color memory (%) 35.5 29.7 31.0 27.4 14.6 20.8
178 Schredl, Fuchedzhieva, Ha¨ mig, and Schindele
10% of the dreams and dream elements were rated as black and white. This figure
decreased significantly to 2.7% if the colors have to be reported immediately after
recall and recording of the dream. It seems very plausible that memory processes
affect the retrospective estimates elicited with the questionnaire. The percentage
of 82.8% of colored elements within the dream is comparable to the laboratory
studies (Kahn et al., 1962; Snyder, 1970; Rechtschaffen & Buchignani, 1983, 1992)
explicitly asked for colors after the dream was reported to the experimenter. The
present study indicates, however, that the previously reported figure of 20% for
black and white dreams/dream elements was not valid because the participants
were not given an option for stating that they did not remember the colors of the
dreams or dream elements. The actual number of black and white dreams or dream
elements (seeing a colored object in shades of gray) seems to be quite small.
The findings also indicate that the percentage of black and white dreams/
dream elements is related to waking-life color memory if a task that is similar to
remembering dreams is applied (complex scene with a lot of colored elements).
Due to a relatively small sample size, future studies are needed to corroborate this
finding. In addition, the percentage of black and white dreams was related to dream
recall frequency. Persons with high dream recall estimated their dreams and dream
elements as being black and white less often. Previous research has shown that high
recallers remembered more details of a dream, for example, more words per dream
report (Schredl, 2000; Schredl, 2004b; 2004c). A specific training program enhanced
dream recall frequency and the ability to remember dream details, too (Reed,
1973). This link between dream recall frequency and recall of dream details further
support the notion that memory processes play a role in explaining the occurrence
of black and white dreams since their percentage decreases in trained persons.
Overall, the findings do not support the hypothesis formulated by Schwitzgebel
(2002) that dream elements might not have distinctive colors but rather are in line
with the continuity hypothesis (Schredl, 2003) which assumes that colors in dreams
are comparable to the colors in waking life.
To summarize, the present study indicates that memory plays an important
role in explaining why some persons say their dreams are black and white. It would
be very interesting to carry out studies including extensive training of color memory
in waking life and in dream recall in order to investigate whether highly trained
persons still have some dreams or dream elements that are in black and white. In
order to control for all possible biases, it would be necessary to elicit whether the
participants have ever watched black and white films (since the age of first regular
Table 2. Correlations Between Percentage of Black and White Dreams, Dream Recall Frequency,
and Color Memory
Variables
Color memory
(Test 1)
Color memory
(Circus picture)
Dream recall
frequency
Black and white dreams .164 .321
.243
Black and white dream
elements (questionnaire)
.075 .253
.200
Black and white dream
elements (dream report)
.075 .224 .368
Note. One-tailed test of the correlation coefficient.
p.05.
Dreams in Black and White 179
exposure to colored TV and movies affect the percentage of reported black and
white dreams—cf. Schwitzgebel, Huang, & Zhou, 2006). The continuity hypothesis
(cf. Schredl, 2003) would predict that at least a small portion of dream elements
might be black and white if related experiences were present in the waking life of
the participant. In the same way wearing red goggles affected the colors of the
dream elements in the experimental study of Roffwarg, Herman, Bowe-Anders,
and Tauber (1978).
REFERENCES
Bentley, M. (1915). The study of dreams. American Journal of Psychology, 26, 196–210.
Husband, R. W. (1936). Sex differences in dream contents. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
ogy, 30, 513–521.
Kahn, E., Dement, W., Fisher, C., & Barmack, J. E. (1962). Incidence of color in immediately recalled
dreams. Science, 137, 1054–1055.
Middleton, W. C. (1933). Nocturnal dreams. Scientific Monthly, 37, 460464.
Middleton, W. C. (1942). The frequency with which a group of unselected college students experiences
colored dreaming and colored hearing. Journal of General Psychology, 27, 221–229.
Okada, H., Matsuoka, K., & Hatakeyama, T. (2005). Individual differences in the range of sensory
modalities experienced in dreams. Dreaming, 15, 106–115.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Buchignani, C. (1983). Visual dimensions and correlates of dream images. Sleep
Research, 12, 189.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Buchignani, C. (1992). The visual appearance of dreams. In J. S. Antrobus & M.
Bertini (Eds.), The neuropsychology of sleep and dreaming (pp. 143–155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reed, H. (1973). Learning to remember dreams. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 13, 33–48.
Roffwarg, H. P., Herman, J. S., Bowe-Anders, C., & Tauber, E. S. (1978). The effects of sustained
alterations of waking visual input on dream content. In A. M. Arkin, J. S. Antrobus, & S. J. Ellman
(Eds.), The mind in sleep: Psychology and Psychophysiology (pp. 295–349). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schredl, M. (2000). The relationship between dream recall and dream content: Negative evidence for the
salience hypothesis. North American Journal of Psychology, 2, 243–246.
Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematical model.
Sleep and Hypnosis, 5, 38–52.
Schredl, M. (2004a). Reliability and stability of a dream recall frequency scale. Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 98, 1422–1426.
Schredl, M. (2004b). Traumerinnerung: Modelle und empirische Untersuchungen [Dream recall: Models
and empirical studies]. Marburg, Germany: Tectum.
Schredl, M. (2004c). Dream report length and dream recall frequency: Are they affected by the same
factors? In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 28; S. 15–25).New York, NY:
Nova Science Publisher.
Schwitzgebel, E. (2002). Why did we think we dreamed in black and white? Studies in History and
Philosophy of Science, 33, 649660.
Schwitzgebel, E. (2003). Do people still report dreaming in black and white? An attempt to replicate a
questionnaire from 1942. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96, 25–29.
Schwitzgebel, E., Huang, C., & Zhou, Y. (2006). Do we dream in color? Cultural variations and
skepticism. Dreaming, 16, 3642.
Snyder, F. (1970). The phenomenology of dreaming. In L. Madow & L. H. Snow (Eds.), The psychody-
namic implications of the physiological studies on dreams (pp. 124–151). Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Thomas.
Stepansky, R., Holzinger, B., Schmeiser-Rieder, A., Saletu, B., Kunze, M., & Zeitlhofer, J. (1998).
Austrian dream behavior: Results of a representative population survey. Dreaming, 8, 23–30.
Tatibana, Y. (1938). Grundtypen der Traumfarben [Basic types of dream colors]. Tohoku Psychologica
Folia, 6, 127–144.
180 Schredl, Fuchedzhieva, Ha¨ mig, and Schindele
... Methodological procedures might also contribute to differences between studies: spontaneously reported colors were relatively scarce. In about 20% of the dream reports (Schredl, 2008) colors were explicitly mentioned, whereas when probing for color in dreams, colors were reported by 80% of the participants (Kahn, Dement, Fisher, & Barmack, 1962;Rechtschaffen & Buchignani, 1983, 1992Schredl, Fuchedzhieva, Hämig, & Schindele, 2008). In addition, memory processes played an important role as the percentage of black and white dreams was related to dream recall frequency: High dream recallers estimated their dreams as being black and white less often . ...
... To summarize, based on the continuity hypothesis (Schredl, 2003) one would expect all dreams to be colored since our daily world is colored; a hypothesis that is supported by laboratory and diary study results reporting that actual dream reports very often include colors -if the participants are questioned explicitly directly after reporting the dream (Rechtschaffen & Buchignani, 1992;Schredl et al., 2008). So there is a question as to why some persons report that their dreams are mainly black and white. ...
... In the present study, the participants rated the color modality of their dreams on a questionnaire with three options: percentage of colored dreams, percentage of black and white dreams, and percentage of dreams without color memory. They estimated a considerably large percentage of their dreams with no recall of colors (40%) -similar to 35% found by Schredl et al. (2008). This finding supports the idea that memory processes, i.e., difficulties in remembering dream colors, play an important role. ...
Article
Visual elements are important ingredients of dreams, so dream objects should be - based on the continuity hypothesis of dreaming - as colorful as the waking world. However, the percentages of recalled colored versus black and white dreams as estimated by the participants varied considerably across studies. In the present online study, 2701 persons completed a question about recalling colors in their dreams with three options: percentage of black and white dreams, percentage of colored dreams, and percentage of dreams with no memory of colors. The older participants who most likely had watched black and white TV reported higher recall of black and white dreams than younger persons while the younger group with access to colored TV estimated that their dreams include more often colors compared to the older group. Since the attitude towards dreams and dream recall frequency were positively associated with the reporting of colored dreams, one might hypothesize that dreamers may attribute colors to a dream even if they are do not remember the colors of their actual dreams. In order to validate the present findings, future studies should include the amount of media consumption (TV, cinema etc.) over the life span of the individual and elicit possible confounding factors like age-related memory changes, attention to colors in waking life, and emotional valence of colored dream elements.
... A study was conducted to determine whether the memory plays a role in the recollection of whether people say their dreams are black and white or in color. Research findings demonstrated that the memory of dreaming in color is related to when the dream is remembered, and the frequency of such recollections [40]. Dream recollection is subjective and context-dependent. ...
... By tracing the structural-functional interrelationship between epithelial-mesenchymal cell-cell interactions governed by soluble growth factors and the composition and regulation of surfactant production by the alveoli, the cellular-molecular evolution of the alveolus can be followed from the swim bladder of fish to the mammalian lung [41]. That interrelationship is causal since the production of surfactant is necessary for the step-wise decrease in alveolar diameter from amphibians to reptiles, birds, and mammals [37][38][39][40][41][42]-on the one hand, the only way in which gas exchange can become more efficient is by reducing the alveolar diameter, increasing the surface area-to-volume ratio, thus enhancing gas exchange [41][42][43][44][45]. On the other hand, as evolutionary selection pressure for reduction in the alveolar diameter increases, the only way to prevent alveolar collapse due to increased surface tension based on the Law of Laplace is to produce more and/or more efficient lung surfactant. ...
... A study was conducted to determine whether the memory plays a role in the recollection of whether people say their dreams are black and white or in color. Research findings demonstrated that the memory of dreaming in color is related to when the dream is remembered, and the frequency of such recollections [40]. Dream recollection is subjective and context-dependent. ...
... By tracing the structural-functional interrelationship between epithelial-mesenchymal cell-cell interactions governed by soluble growth factors and the composition and regulation of surfactant production by the alveoli, the cellular-molecular evolution of the alveolus can be followed from the swim bladder of fish to the mammalian lung [41]. That interrelationship is causal since the production of surfactant is necessary for the step-wise decrease in alveolar diameter from amphibians to reptiles, birds, and mammals [37][38][39][40][41][42]-on the one hand, the only way in which gas exchange can become more efficient is by reducing the alveolar diameter, increasing the surface area-to-volume ratio, thus enhancing gas exchange [41][42][43][44][45]. On the other hand, as evolutionary selection pressure for reduction in the alveolar diameter increases, the only way to prevent alveolar collapse due to increased surface tension based on the Law of Laplace is to produce more and/or more efficient lung surfactant. ...
Article
Full-text available
A point source is the central and most important point or place for any group of cohering phenomena. Evolutionary development presumes that biological processes are sequentially linked, but neither directed from, nor centralized within, any specific biologic structure or stage. However, such an epigenomic entity exists and its transforming effects can be understood through the obligatory recapitulation of all eukaryotic lifeforms through a zygotic unicellular phase. This requisite biological conjunction can now be properly assessed as the focal point of reconciliation between biology and quantum phenomena, illustrated by deconvoluting complex physiologic traits back to their unicellular origins.
Chapter
This article surveys some of the major milestones of scientific dream research, often in response to sleep research, as well as main points of exchange and disagreement with philosophy, with a focus on Western philosophy and non-lucid dreams. It argues that the increasing establishment of dreaming as a target phenomenon for the interdisciplinary study of consciousness, alongside recent advances in sleep and mind wandering research, is leading to revisions in our understanding of sleep, wakefulness, and consciousness.
Chapter
Dream-related behaviors like sharing dreams are best measured via questionnaires whereas dream content analysis provides information about dream content, for example, bizarreness of dreams, emotional tone of dreams. The main focus of this chapter is the so-called continuity hypothesis of dreaming, stating that dreams reflect the waking-life experiences of the dreamer. Research had identified factors that affect this continuity between waking and dreaming, for example, emotional intensity of the waking-life experience or the type of the waking-life activity (studying vs. meeting with friends). Also, gender differences in dream content seem to be continuous to gender differences found in waking life.
Chapter
In order to analyze the verbal reports of the dreamer the method of dream content analysis was developed. The chapter will present the basic idea of this method, different methods to elicit dream reports, the most commonly used coding systems, the methodological challenges, and the reliability and validity of this approach.
Chapter
Das wissenschaftliche Interesse an Träumen wird vorwiegend von zwei Strömungen gespeist: zum einen ist dies die neurowissenschaftliche Seite, die sich für die physiologischen Vorgänge während des Schlafens und des Träumens interessiert, und zum anderen die akademische Psychologie, die sich Trauminhalten und anderen Traumvariablen wie Traumerinnerungshäufigkeit, Umgang mit Träumen usw.
Article
p>Dreams can sometimes incorporate external sensory stimuli (e.g. sounds, smells and physical sensations) into their course and content, either directly or indirectly. This shows that the brain is still able to monitor, process, and perceive what is happening in the surrounding environment during sleep. This study aimed to examine stimulus incorporation in dreams using two auditory stimuli of different languages – one semantically meaningful to participants and one non-meaningful. We hypothesised that participants exposed to the semantically meaningful language would all report similar experiences to each other, and different from those exposed to the non-meaningful language. All participants first spent several weeks improving their dream recall abilities, and then came to the Sleep and Cognition Laboratory at the University of Lincoln for a two hour morning nap, during which a stimulus was played to them in Rapid-Eye-Movement (REM) sleep. They were awoken shortly after to provide a dream report. All dreams contained conversation or speech of some description, but due to participants’ poor verbal memory for dream speech, we could not conclusively say that the stimuli were responsible for this effect. There were, however, at least two dreams with strong evidence to suggest that the stimuli were directly incorporated. Q-Methodology was used to assess similarity of dream experience. This resulted in three distinct factors: (1) calm, consistent, slightly emotional dreams; (2) emotional, normal, understandable dreams; and (3) unstable, inconsistent, unrealistic dreams. The configuration of factors amongst participants did not fully meet the predictions of the hypothesis; however, positive participant feedback on Q-sorting their dream experiences gives promise and potential for the use of Q-Methodology in future dream research. Recommendations and limitations are discussed. </p
Article
Full-text available
A series of studies was performed on the occurrence of color in dream reports, in order to determine if such reports simply reflect our waking visual experience, or if the colors reported might be influenced by neurological or psychological factors, or both. Content analyses were performed for the presence of dream color in 15,245 dream reports from the large diverse population in the DreamBank.net database, plus 12,841 dream reports from eight long-term dream journals. A third analysis was performed on individual long-term journal data, to determine how the reports for individuals compared to those of the composite averages. The analysis revealed a nearly identical pattern in the colors reported for both the large composite databases and for the individual long-term journal data, but with a wide variation among individuals. The results suggest that dream color recall may be influenced by a common factor, either neurological or psychological, which, in turn, is influenced at an individual level by personal factors. A preliminary test for the influence of personality and emotion on color recall provided a promising indication that emotional factors do influence the colors either dreamed or recalled.
Article
The historical and individual differences in reporting color or greyscale dreams have been previously investigated as factors of age, color memory and experience with black and white media. The present study introduced visual imagery style as a potential factor (Kozhevnikov, Kosslyn and Shephard, 2005), using the Object Spatial Imagery Questionnaire and a custom color imagery task. It was found that individual imagery preferences - the tendency to visualise in pictorial detail or in spatial sketches - was related to questionnaire reported dream color, with object imagery skills being related to more frequent colored dreaming. Incidental memory for color was also examined, but neither recall nor recognition of color properties was related to dream color. Finally, age and childhood experience of black and white media were investigated, but were not found to be significant factors. The implications are discussed in the context of memory effects and continuity of cognitive functions.
Article
Full-text available
Although a large number of studies investigating factors affecting dream recall frequency (DRF) have been carried out, research investigating the reliability and stability of DRF is scarce. Dream diaries of 196 participants kept over at least 28 days were analyzed. The results of the present study indicate that a time period of 2 weeks was sufficient to obtain reliable measurements of interindividual differences in DRF. Despite the high dayto-day fluctuations of dream recall, the stability of this variable was very high. Studies that investigate the stability of DRF by means of other methodological approaches (e.g., questionnaire scales, laboratory awakenings) and over longer time periods (e.g., 1 year) should be carried out to complement the present findings.
Article
Full-text available
The possibility of learning to remember dreams was explored with the cooperation of a group of participant-observers who maintained dream diaries for 12 weeks and were selected on the basis of their desire to experience improved dream recall. A self-administered dream recall rating scale was devised which provided measures of several aspects of memory for dreams. The participants apparently increased their ability to retrieve dream memories, but their actual level of dream recall performance was subject to motivational fluctuations. On the other hand, although the participants claimed to have developed no control over the vividness of their memory for dreams, this aspect of their dream recall nevertheless evidenced substantial improvement. The possibility of improving memory for dreams being confirmed, future research should provide (a) more information concerning the control processes governing dream recall, and (b) information concerning how to increase volitional access to these processes.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated the relationship between the recalled frequency of sensory modalities experienced in dreams and waking imagery abilities. Five hundred thirty-one undergraduate students completed 4 imagery ability tests and a dream recall questionnaire containing a question on the frequency of experience of the sensory modalities in dreams. A k-means cluster analysis of the Dream Sense Modality Scale indicated that the participants fell into 3 groups: the major modalities group, whose dreams were entirely visual, auditory, and kinesthetic; the all modalities group, who experienced all sense modalities in dreams; and the no modalities group, whose dreams were without sensory content. The participants' scores on the imagery tests in the 3 groups showed significant intergroup differences. The all modalities group showed the highest levels of ability in evoking vivid imagery and controlling visual imagery and the most frequent use of visual imagery compared with the other two groups. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In the United States, the rise and fall of the opinion that we dream in black and white coincided with the rise and fall of black and white film media over the course of the 20th century, suggesting that our opinions about the coloration of our dreams are subject to cultural influences. This study generalizes that conclusion cross-culturally. Three groups of Chinese respondents, similar in age but differing in history of colored media exposure, were given questionnaires replicating those of Middleton (1942) and Schwitzgebel (2003). As expected, the groups with longer histories of colored media exposure reported more colored dreaming. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
FROM PHENOMENOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT OF 635 REM SLEEP (S) REPORTS FROM 58 NORMAL SS, THE FOLLOWING ARE OUTSTANDING IMPRESSIONS: LABORATORY DREAMS GENERALLY INVOLVE CLEAR, COHERENT ACCOUNTS OF REALISTIC SITUATIONS IN WHICH THE SELF IS INVOLVED IN MUNDANE ACTIVITIES AND PREOCCUPATIONS. 90% WOULD HAVE BEEN CONSIDERED CREDIBLE DESCRIPTIONS OF EVERYDAY WAKING EXPERIENCE. FORM OF VISUAL IMAGERY IS ALMOST INVARIABLY "REPRESENTATIONAL," MORE ELABORATE REPORTS USUALLY INCLUDING COLOR AND SHARPLY FOCUSED DETAIL. AS EVIDENCED BY VERBAL INTERACTION, AUDITORY IMAGERY IS UBIQUITOUS, TOGETHER WITH SUCH MENTAL PROCESSES AS VOLITION, REFLECTION, AND INFERENCE. BOTH THE PROSAIC NATURE OF CONTENT AND THE RICH PHENOMENOLOGICAL SIMILARITY TO WAKING EXPERIENCE FOUND ARE IN KEEPING WITH THE RESULTS OF EARLY INTROSPECTIONIST STUDIES, BUT DISCREPANT FROM THE USUALLY ACCEPTED CONCEPTION OF DREAMING. LABORATORY DREAMS MAY BE A GREATLY DISTORTED SAMPLE, BUT THOSE SPONTANEOUSLY RECALLED MAY BE A HIGHLY SELECTED ONE. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Sex differences in dream contents were studied in 25 men and 25 women by an interview method. Women's dreams are much more vivid and emotional and contain more elements of fear. Women tend to transfer their worries, love and otherwise, into dreams much more than do men. Women dream about their "boy friends," while men do not so often dream about the girls they care for. Dreams of married people are fewer and involve less sex content than those of single persons. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The so-called continuity hypothesis of dreaming states that waking experiences are reflected in dreams. The formulation of the continuity hypothesis is very broad and vague, however, so that it seems necessary to investigate factors which might affect the incorporation rate of waking-life experiences. A review of the different research paradigms, e. g. assessing temporal references of dream elements, studying the effects of the pre-sleep situation on dreams, will be presented. Various methodological issues which limit the generalizability of the findings in this area will also be addressed. After this overview, several factors such as (a) the time interval between waking-life experience and dream occurrence, (b) emotional involvement, (c) the type of waking-life experience, (d) personality traits and (e) the time of the night (time interval between sleep onset and dream onset) for which empirical data indicates an influence on incorporation rates of waking-life experiences will be listed. A mathematical model is proposed which should enable researchers to identify influencing factors and their interactions making a more precise formulation of the continuity hypothesis possible. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Questionnaires were distributed to 277 college sophomore men and women to secure data on the occurrence of colored dreaming and colored hearing. 99.97% of the subjects reported having dreams; 40% reported a lack of color in their dreams. Various comments made by the subjects about their colored dreams are presented; sex differences were apparent. 50% of the cases reported that they experienced colored hearing; numerous comments by these subjects are presented. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)