ArticlePublisher preview available

The role of choice in learning as a function of meaning and between- and within-subjects designs

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Examined the effect of giving Ss the opportunity to choose learning materials in a paired-associate paradigm. 4 studies using high- and low-meaning materials and employing between- and within-Ss designs were conducted with 40 undergraduates in the 1st 2 and 21 Ss in the others. Use of a between-Ss design showed that A-B learning was facilitated by choice with high-meaning but not low-meaning materials. Using a within-Ss design in which Ss chose 5 stimulus-response (S-R) pairs and were forced to learn 5 others, results show performance on the forced pairs was elevated to the level of the choice pairs. These data suggest that allowing Ss the opportunity to choose at least some S-R pairs generally facilitates performance through the operation of a proposed motivational mechanism which may also benefit learning of nonchosen S-R pairs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
of
Experimental Psychology
1972,
Vol.
94, No. 2,
235-238
THE
ROLE
OF
CHOICE
IN
LEARNING
AS A
FUNCTION
OF
MEANING
AND
BETWEEN-
AND
WITHIN-SUBJECTS
DESIGNS1
RICHARD
A.
MONTY
"
Human
Engineering
Laboratories
LAWRENCE
C.
PERLMUTER
Bowdoin
College
The
effect
of
giving
5s the
opportunity
to
choose
the
materials they wish
to
learn
in a
paired-associate paradigm
was
examined with high-
and
low-meaning
materials employing between-
and
within-Ss
designs.
Use of a
between-5s
design showed
that
A-B
learning
was
facilitated
by
choice with high-meaning
materials
but not
with low-meaning materials. Using
a
within-5s design
in
which
5s
chose
five S-R
pairs
and
were forced
to
learn
five
other
S-R
pairs,
the
results
showed
that
performance
on the
forced
pairs
was
elevated
to the
level
of
the
choice pairs.
These
data
suggest
that
allowing
5s the
opportunity
to
choose
at
least
some
S-R
pairs generally facilitates performance through
the
operation
of a
proposed motivational mechanism which
may
also
benefit
the
learning
of
nonchosen
S-R
pairs.
Perlmuter,
Monty,
and
Kimble
(1971)
have
demonstrated
that
if 5s are
allowed
to
choose
S-R
responses
to be
learned
on a
subsequent paired-
associate
task
(Choice
group),
performance
is
better
than
if
they
were given responses
to
learn which
were
not of
their
own
choosing (Force group).
The
strength
of
this
effect
was
found
to be
related
to the
rate
of
presentation
(i.e.,
the
faster
the
rate
the
greater
the
effect)
and to the
meaningfulness
of
>the
materials (the
benefits
derived
from
the
choice
procedure were greater
for the
low-meaning
ma-
terials).
The
differences
with respect
to
meaning,
'however,
were
not
conclusive owing
to the
fact
that
meaning
was
included
as a
within-5
variable.
Thus,
with
a
10-word list,
half
of the
pairs were
of
high meaning
and
half
of
low. With
a
maximum
score
of five on
each
half
list,
there
was
evidence
of a
ceiling
effect
with
the
high-meaning materials.
Stated
differently,
differences
in
performance
be-
tween
the
Choice
and
Force
groups with
the
high-
meaning
materials
may
have been limited
by the
fact
that
both groups approached asymptotic
performance with high-meaning stimuli.
In
addi-
tion, insofar
as
meaning served
as a
within-5
variable,
it is
possible
that
the
presence
of the
high-
meaning materials
influenced
performance with
the
low-meaning materials
or
vice
versa.
Thus
one
purpose
of the
present study
was to
avoid
the
problems inherent
in the
within-5 design
and
1
This
research
was
supported
in
part
by
Grant
MH
15315
from
the
National Institute
of
Mental
Health
to the
second author.
All of
these experi-
ments were conducted
at the
University
of
Delaware.
Thanks
go to F.
Loren Smith
for
making laboratory
space
available. Special
thanks
are
given
to
Margie
:
Rosenberger,
who ran 5s in the
experiments.
Data
reduction
was
performed
by
Mary Bosse
and
Patricia
Reese.
The
paper
may be
reproduced
in
full
or in
part
for any
purpose
of the
United
States
Government.
2
Please
address
requests
for
reprints
to
Richard
A.
Monty, Behavioral Research Laboratory, Human
Engineering Laboratories, Aberdeen Proving
Ground, Maryland 21005.
reexamine
the
extent
to
which
the
opportunity
to
exercise choice facilitates learning with both
high-
and
low-meaning materials.
Since
the
previous experiments have demon-
strated
that
performance
is
facilitated
by
affording
5 the
opportunity
to
choose, presumably
as a
result
of
enhanced motivation,
is it
reasonable
to
expect
that
motivation might generally
benefit
the
learning
of
nonchosen materials
as
well
? The
second purpose
of
the
present paper
was to
examine
the
extent
to
which
the
motivational mechanisms postulated
by
Perlmuter
et
al.
(1971)
would
enhance performance
of
nonchosen materials embedded
in a
list
of
chosen
materials.
EXPERIMENT
I
In the first
experiment,
the
effect
of
choosing
the A-B
responses
on
learning
of the A-B
list
was
examined using
a
relatively easy (high-meaning)
word
list.
It was
hoped
that
use of the
10-word
list
would
avoid
the
problems
of the
ceiling
effect
noted
in the
earlier experiments (Perlmuter
et
al.,
1971)
and
hence allow
for a
more direct assessment
of
the
role
of
meaning.
Method.—The
5s
were
40
male
and
female
students, aged
19-25,
from
the
University
of
Delaware,
who
were paid $2.00 each
for
their
participation
in the
experiment.
They
were
alternately assigned
to two
groups
of 20 5s
each
in
order
of
their appearance.
The
apparatus
and
general procedures were
identical
to
those employed
by
Perlmuter
et al.
(1971).
Briefly,
the
words
and
paralogs used
in
this
study were
the
last
70 five-letter
items
taken
from
a
list generated
by
Taylor
and
Kimble (1967)
and
were designated
as
high-meaning stimuli.
These
70
items were assembled into
10
groups
of 7
each. Within each group,
1
item
was
designated
arbitrarily
as the
stimulus word
and 1
item desig-
nated
arbitrarily
as the A-C
response word.
The
remaining
5
items served
as the five
potential
response
words
on the A-B
list.
The
Choice group
was
first
presented with
10
slides,
1 at a
time. Each
slide contained
a
stimulus word centered
on the
235
... To account for these results the authors suggested that the opportunity to exercise choice results in heightened motivation, thereby facilitating performance. To further examine this possibility, Monty and Perlmuter (1972) permitted 5s to choose 5 out of 10 of the PA items and forced them to learn the remaining 5 items. With this procedure it was found that the differential effect of choice was not observed. ...
... Two components of the choice variable were manipulated : amount of choice allowed during the selection and the locus of choice, i.e., where in the procedure choice took place. Quantity of choice (as defined by the ratio of chosen to nonchosen items) was manipulated primarily to investigate further the findings of the earlier experiments (Monty & Perlmuter, 1972), while locus of choice was manipulated to determine when choice operates. That is, if the beneficial effects of choice occur during the A-B trials when 5s are learning the materials to which they have been exposed 16 previously, then the locus of choice may be an irrelevant variable. ...
... level of confidence. In short, then, the observation that the opportunity to exercise choice led to better performance than when no such opportunity was provided is consistent with the findings of Perlmuter et al. (1971) and Monty and Perlmuter (1972). Monty and Perlmuter (1972) also reported that when Choice vs. Force was treated as a within-Ss variable, there was some evidence that the chosen and forced materials were learned equally well. ...
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the effect of giving 120 undergraduates the opportunity to choose the response materials they wish to learn in a paired-associate task. A paradigm was used which manipulated the amount of choice allowed during the selection procedure and the locus or point at which choice took place. Results show that locus of choice was an extremely powerful variable and that if properly manipulated, choosing only 3 responses in a 12-item list elevated performance to a level near that of a 12-item list in which all responses were chosen. Motivational mechanisms are postulated to account for these findings. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
... Recent investigations (Monty & Perlmuter, 1972;Monty, Rosenberger, & Perlmuter, 1973;Perlmuter, Monty, & Kimble, 1971) have demonstrated that if 5s are allowed to choose their own responses from among alternatives prior to learning a paired-associate list, learning is better than if they are required to learn S-R pairs not of their own choosing. This effect, however, required that when 5s choose their responses, this choosing occur in the presence of their respective stimuli. ...
... In the first experiment the effect of choosing stimuli prior to learning of a paired-associate list was examined using the relatively easy (high-meaning) word list employed by Monty and Perlmuter (1972). The phenomenon was examined first using a fixed number of trials and second using a trials-to-criterion paradigm. ...
... The general procedure was the same as that employed by Monty and Perlmuter (1972). Briefly, the stimuli consisted of words typed in elite capitals on Kodak Ektagraphic Write-On Slides, presented with a Kodak 850 Carousel projector at a viewing distance of 1.8 m. ...
Article
Full-text available
Notes previous experiments showing that if Ss choose responses from among alternatives, subsequent paired-associate learning is facilitated if the choosing occurs in the presence of the respective stimuli. Choosing in the absence of the stimuli does not facilitate performance. In experiments with university students (N = 122), choosing the stimuli in the presence of the respective responses facilitated performance; however, choosing the stimuli in the absence of the respective response hindered performance. This lack of symmetry between choice of stimulus and choice of response is consistent with the finding that the effect of some variables may be different when either the stimulus or response items have been differentially familiarized. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
... Previous experiments have defined a number of conditions in which performance can be improved by permitting subjects to choose either the stimulus or the response side of S-R pairs immediately prior to paired-associate (PA) learning (Monty & Perlmuter, 1972; Monty, Rosenberger, & Perlmuter, 1973; Perlmuter, Monty, & Kimble, 1971). Basically, in some of these experiments one group of subjects was permitted to choose from among alternative responses and thereby compose S-R pairs to be subsequently learned in a PA paradigm. ...
... The magnitude of the superiority of the choice group tended to rapidly increase with trials and subsequently diminish as performance of both groups neared asymptote. Thus, the beneficial effects of choosing responses in the presence of stimuli that were apparent immediately following the act of choosing (Monty et al, 1973; Monty & Perlmuter, 1972; Perlmuter et ai, 1971) also endure a 24-h interval. Further. ...
Article
Four experiments are reported in which half of the subjects were permitted to choose either the stimulus or response members of S-R pairs from groupings of alternative stimuli or responses following the procedures of Perlmuter, Monty, and Kimble. The other subjects were exposed to the materials and were forced to learn the material chosen by their yoked partners. Approximately 24 h later, both groups learned PA lists comprised of either the chosen or forced material. Choosing either the stimulus or the response facilitated performance irrespective of whether the other member of the S-R pair was present at the time of choosing. This finding ruled out an associative hookup interpretation in favor of a purely motivational hypothesis and demonstrated the temporal durability of the effect of choosing.
... It can be seen that the force group performed consistently better than the choice group, and that the magnitude of the difference tended to increase generally as a function of trials. This finding is in sharp contrast with the earlier findings (Monty & Perlmuter, 1972;Monty et al., 1973;Perlmuter et al., 1971), all of which indicated that when A-B learning immediately follows the choice procedure, the choice group performs significantly better than the force group, presumably as a result of enhanced motivation stemming from having had the opportunity to choose. However, these results are supportive of Perlmuter et al. (1971), who found that performance on a nonchosen A-C list that was learned following A-B was poorer for choice 5s than for force 5s. ...
Article
Full-text available
Conducted 2 experiments in paired-associate learning in which 20 undergraduates were permitted to choose their responses from alternatives following the procedure of L. C. Perlmuter, R. A. Monty, and G. A. Kimble . The other 20 Ss were forced to learn the responses chosen by their yoked masters. Both groups of Ss were forced to learn an interposed A-C list following the choice-force procedure but prior to learning the A-B list. The performance of force Ss was reliably superior to that of choice Ss on A-C; however, when both groups learned A-B, the typical facilitative effect of choice was disturbed. In Exp II with 40 undergraduates, interposing a C-D list produced equivalent performance in both groups on C-D. Similarly, the facilitative effect of choice was not observed on the subsequent A-B trials. The degraded performance of choice Ss is discussed in terms of frustration as an additional source of motivation.
Chapter
With the exception of the pioneering interventional studies of Langer and Rodin (1976; Rodin & Langer, 1977; Rodin, Timko, & Harris, 1985), most of the research that has investigated the relationship between perceptions of control and aging have utilized self-reports to determine the extent to which advancing age is associated with changes in perceived control (Blanchard-Fields & Robinson, 1987). Although the self-report procedure is sound in itself, it may have peculiar limitations with respect to assessing age-related changes in perceived control; that is, when individuals are asked to respond to specific questions or scenarios that depict control, older individuals may respond differently than younger individuals to these inquiries, not because their perception of control has changed, but rather because the particular scenario or question may be lacking in appropriateness for either group. Thus, the requisite standardization procedures involved in test construction (i.e., presenting the respondent with a topic for judgment) may limit test validity. In other words, if young and old respondents answer a given question differently, is it because they perceive more or less control, or is the stem of the test item differentially appropriate to young or old individuals? As a result of the potential limitations surrounding exclusive reliance on self-reports, we primarily utilize experimental paradigms that examine how choice and control affect motivation and performance in younger and older individuals. We examine laboratory as well as field studies and propose some principles that may be useful in enhancing the perception of control in the elderly.
Article
This study investigated the relationship between internal versus external locus of control of reinforcement and counter control or reactance in subjects in a verbal learning experiment. Internal and external controllers were given either a choice or no choice of material to be learned in a paired associate, anticipation task (after Monty & Perlmuter, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1972, 94, 235–238). As hypothesized, the lack of freedom of choice was associated with counter control (decreased recall) in internals. Being able to choose material lead to faster learning for both internals and externals after first trial recall.
Article
The self-choice effect, that is the superior memory performance observed when participants are allowed to choose the item at the study phase than when they are not (lack of choice), has been explained by the encoding-strategy hypothesis (Takahashi, 1997). This hypothesis distinguishes between organizational processing, which focuses upon the relationship between chosen items, and item-specific processing, which itself focuses on the elements that make the chosen item distinctive. The encoding-strategy hypothesis predicts that the ability for the successive recall of chosen items can be affected by organized list (Experiment 1). The results of our experiments ran counter to the prediction and were interpreted by a new concept called connective processing, which would emphasize the relationship of paired items. Connective processing was examined through orienting tasks (Experiment 2). The results were more suitably interpreted by connective than organizational processing.
Article
There has been an increasing emphasis in psychology on the perception of control over one''s environment and its effect on behavior and performance. In the present experiment, subjects were allowed to choose material they would like to learn if given the opportunity, or allowed to choose material for others to learn. Their learning of a nonchosen (forced) set of materials was then examined and compared with that of subjects given no opportunity to choose at all. It was found that the choice-for-self condition led to better performance than either the choice-for-other condition or the nochoice (forced) condition. However, when an additional group of subjects was given the opportunity to choose for others and their personal involvement was augmented by emphasizing their willingness to choose, their performance was enhanced relative to subjects offered no choice. The results were discussed in terms of motivational benefits stemming from perceived control.
Article
Full-text available
Conducted 3 paired-associate learning experiments with a total of 120 male and female undergraduates. Under some conditions, a-b learning was facilitated when ss chose responses to be learned on a subsequent a-b list. Ss who chose their a-b responses and were subsequently forced to learn a competing set of material (a-c) showed a relatively greater disruption of learning than ss who did not have the opportunity to choose either a-b or a-c. All of these effects required that when ss chose their responses, the choosing occurred in the presence of their respective stimuli. Simply choosing responses in the absence of the stimuli produced performance which was not different from that resulting when ss were denied the opportunity to choose their responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
The method of Glaze was used to scale 320 words and paralogs for meaningfulness. One hundred Ss provided data from which three such measures were derived. Employing the most conventional of these measures (percentage of Ss responding in less than 2.5 sec) to select the items to be learned, a validating study demonstrated the usual relationship between association value and speed of learning. Other investigations have employed the materials successfully for purposes of control when the main interest of the experiment was in some other problem.
A multipurpose analysis of variance FORTRAN IV computer program
  • D H Butler
  • A S Kamlet
  • R A Monty
BUTLER, D. H., KAMLET, A. S., & MONTY, R. A. A multipurpose analysis of variance FORTRAN IV computer program. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1969, 2, 301-319.