ArticlePublisher preview available

Seeing Virtues in Faults: Negativity and the Transformation of Interpersonal Narratives in Close Relationships

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

It is proposed that individuals develop story-like representations of their romantic partners that quell feelings of doubt engendered by their partners' faults. In Study 1, dating individuals were induced to depict their partners as rarely initiating disagreements over joint interests. Such conflict avoidance was then turned into a fault. In scaled questionnaires and open-ended narratives, low-conflict individuals then constructed images of conflict-engaging partners. These results suggest that storytelling depends on considerable flexibility in construal as low-conflict Ss possessed little evidence of conflict in their relationships. Study 2 further examined the construal processes underlying people's ability to transform the meaning of negativity in their stories (e.g., seeing virtues in faults). Paradoxically, positive representations of a partner may exist, not in spite of a partner's faults, but because of these imperfections. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1993.
Vol. 65. No. 4. 707-722
Copyright 1993 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/93/S3.00
Seeing Virtues in
Faults:
Negativity and the Transformation of Interpersonal
Narratives in Close Relationships
Sandra L. Murray and John G. Holmes
It is proposed that individuals develop story-like representations of their romantic partners that
quell feelings of doubt engendered by their partners' faults. In Study 1, dating individuals were
induced
to
depict their partners
as
rarely initiating disagreements over joint
interests.
Such conflict
avoidance was then turned into a fault. In scaled questionnaires and open-ended narratives, low-
conflict individuals then constructed images of conflict-engaging partners. These results suggest
that storytelling depends on considerable flexibility in construal as low-conflict
Ss
possessed little
evidence of conflict in their
relationships.
Study
2
further examined the construal processes under-
lying people's ability to transform the meaning of negativity in their stories
(e.g.,
seeing virtues in
faults).
Paradoxically, positive representations of
a
partner may exist—not in spite of a partner's
faults—but because of these imperfections.
Coming to terms with the reality of a less-than-perfect
partner is perhaps the greatest challenge in the more serious
stages of a relationship's development. Faults in an intimate
may engender doubts about whether the partner really is the
"right" person as well as underline the considerable risks posed
by interdependence. Yet, in the face of such threats to their
convictions, partners may continue to experience strong feel-
ings
of hope for their relationship's ultimate
success.
This
juxta-
position of hope and uncertainty enhances individuals' desire
to "quell the babble of competing inner
voices"
underlying feel-
ings of doubt (Jones
&
Gerard, 1967, p. 181).
We believe that individuals construct stories about their
partners to diminish feelings of doubt, thereby affirming and
protecting their positive convictions. Such convictions are pro-
spective in nature, reflecting an individual's confidence that his
or her partner really is the right person and can be counted on
to be caring and responsive across time and situations (Holmes
Sandra L. Murray and John G. Holmes, Department of Psychology,
University of
Waterloo,
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
We would like to thank Dale Griffin, Mike Ross, and Mark Zanna
for their insightful comments on earlier versions of thisarticle.
We
also
benefited tremendously from the critiques and comments provided by
Roy Baumeister and three anonymous
reviewers.
We
are
also
indebted
to Alisa Lennox, Julie Perks, Stephen Taylor, and Lee Westmaus for
their assistance in conducting the research. This article was prepared
with the support of a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Coun-
cil of Canada
(SSHRC)
Doctoral Fellowship
to
Sandra
L.
Murray and
a
SSHRC research grant to John G. Holmes.
Portions of this article were presented at the Canadian Psychologi-
cal Association annual convention
in
Calgary,
Alberta, in June
1991;
at
the Society for Experimental Social Psychology annual meeting in Co-
lumbus, Ohio, in October
1991;
and at the International Society for the
Study of Personal Relationships convention in Orono, Maine, in July
1992.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
to
San-
dra
L.
Murray, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Wa-
terloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1. Electronic mail may be sent to
smurray@watarts.uwaterloo.ca.
& Rempel, 1989). We propose that individuals protect these
convictions by weaving cogent stories that depict potential
faults or imperfections in their partners in the best possible
light.
As we
explore, the potential for considerable flexibility in
the construal of apparent negativity may be an integral feature
of
this
defensive storytelling process
(e.g.,
Gergen, Hepburn, &
Fisher, 1986). For instance, an individual might reconcile the
threat posed by a partner's stubbornness during conflicts by
interpreting it as a sign of integrity, rather than egocentrism.
Such storytelling preserves the integrity of
individuals'
narra-
tives,
thereby promoting
a sense
of felt security in the face of the
considerable risks posed by interdependence.
The structure of relationship-affirming narratives may
change subtly over time
as
partners' positive and negative quali-
ties become more or less salient. Early on in romantic relation-
ships,
individuals typically experience strong positive feelings
as they
attend almost exclusively
to
their partners'
positive
quali-
ties (Holmes
&
Boon,
1990;
Weiss, 1980). Self-presentation, in-
teraction across restricted, positive domains, and intimates' de-
sire not to perceive negative qualities (e.g., Brehm, 1988) likely
all combine to create somewhat simple-minded, idealized
narratives (Holmes & Rempel, 1989). As a result, individuals'
initial sense of security in their relationships rests largely on the
pull of positivity (cf. Brickman, 1987).
Yet,
as
interdependence increases, individuals begin interact-
ing across broader, more conflictual domains, and the poten-
tial for partners to exhibit negative behaviors increases (Le-
vinger, 1983). As the first, most directly affirming response to
apparent negativity, individuals may simply deny that their
partners' seemingly negative behavior reflects any underlying
disposition or attribute. For example, individuals may avoid
attributing negative traits to their partners
by
tagging behaviors
suggestive of such traits to specific, unstable features of the
situation
(e.g.,
Bradbury
&
Fincham,
1990;
Holtzworth-Munroe
& Jacobson, 1985). However, as instances of
negative
behavior
across disparate situations accumulate, dispositional attribu-
tions become increasingly difficult to avoid. Somewhat para-
doxically though, individuals may become increasingly moti-
707
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Indeed, people are strongly motivated to view their partner (Murray et al., 1996a) and their relationship (Rusbult et al., 2000) in an overly positive light. But even the most satisfying relationships involve at least some unpleasant experiences, such as conflicts (Braiker & Kelley, 1979) and rejections (Murray et al., 2006), leading people to engage-sometimes even unintentionally-in processes of motivated reasoning that allow them to ignore or reject such negativity (see Lemay & Clark, 2015 (Lemay & Melville, 2014;Murray & Holmes, 1994), or even turn relational flaws into virtues (Murray & Holmes, 1993) and distort other romantic realities without realizing they do so (Cole et al., 2016), all in ways to maintain positive relationship evaluations. ...
... And high divorce rates combined with pervasive declines in relationship satisfaction suggest that the negativity that people are able to encapsulate or defuse through motivated reasoning does not simply disappear. Rather, as Murray and Holmes (1993) speculated years ago, it may leave people vulnerable to a possible resurfacing of such negativity in future encounters with the partner. ...
Article
Satisfying romantic relationships offer numerous social and health benefits, making it critical to understand the trajectory of relationship satisfaction. In recent years, research has begun to examine the role of automatic processes in relationship contexts. In particular, a growing number of studies have incorporated indirect (implicit) measures developed by socio‐cognitive researchers to capture people's automatic partner attitudes —the spontaneous affective reactions toward their partner that people may at times be unable to access or unwilling to self‐report in more direct (explicit) measures like questionnaires. In this paper, we review work illustrating how automatic partner attitudes can help explain, predict, and promote both the functioning and the well‐being of romantic couples. That is, we integrate theoretical perspectives on interdependence, attachment, and attitudes to discuss empirical evidence relative to why automatic partner attitudes differ from self‐reported judgments of relationship satisfaction, how such attitudes form in the context of real‐world relationship experiences, and under which conditions they affect dyadic interactions in everyday life. Further, we identify important questions that remain unanswered and provide recommendations that may benefit future work on couples and beyond.
... Explicit reports, however, often reveal surprisingly little about future trajectories of relationship satisfaction (Joel et al., 2020;Lavner & Bradbury, 2012;McNulty et al., 2013). One reason why self-report measures may miss signals of strength or weakness in a relationship is that they are vulnerable to several limitations, including partners' lack of accurate self-knowledge, limited memory capacity, and motivated misperceptions of their relationship (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960;Endo et al., 2000;Fletcher & Kerr, 2010;Fowers et al., 2001;Murray, 1999;Murray & Holmes, 1993;Rusbult et al., 2000). Indeed, romantic relationships are a domain in which individuals hold especially strong perceptual biases (McNulty & Karney, 2001), which may make them unwilling or unable to acknowledge what they spontaneously think and feel toward their partner (Hicks et al., 2021). ...
... Indeed, existing literature on couples has often suggested that IPEs may be more sensitive indicators of positive and negative experiences in the relationship than are EPEs (see Hicks & McNulty, 2019). A central idea underpinning this perspective is that people's explicit evaluations of their romantic partners are often heavily shaped by motivated biases aimed at perceiving the partner in a positive light (see Fletcher & Kerr, 2010;Gagné & Lydon, 2004;Murray, 1999), sometimes even in the face of partner's flaws or misbehavior (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990;Hall & Taylor, 1976;Murray & Holmes, 1993). A few studies support this notion. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evidence suggesting that implicit partner evaluations (IPEs), but not explicit evaluations (EPEs), can predict later changes in satisfaction and relationship status has led researchers to postulate that IPEs must be especially sensitive to relational reward and costs. However, supporting evidence for this assumption remains scarce, and very little is known regarding how IPEs versus EPEs actually update in everyday life. Two studies (one in-lab dyadic interaction study, N = 255, and one 14-day dyadic diary study, N = 348) investigated updating in IPEs and EPEs in the context of real-life relationship experiences. Study 1 revealed that the level of positive and negative experiences that a couple encountered while discussing a divergence of interests in their relationship predicted pre-to-post changes in EPEs, but not in IPEs. Study 2 revealed that IPEs showed less sensitivity to everyday relationship experiences across multiple metrics over the course of 14 days. Specifically, compared with EPEs, IPEs fluctuated less at the within- (vs. between-) person level, showed less-abrupt changes from day-to-day, and had a substantially weaker relationship with same-day positive and negative experiences. Rather than covarying with same-day experiences, IPEs appeared sensitive to relationship experiences aggregated across multiple prior days as well as to highly diagnostic relationship experiences, such as breakup. Consistent with recent advances in social-cognitive research, these findings support a modified account of IPE sensitivity, according to which IPEs show only gradual shifts under everyday circumstances, but more-dramatic shifts under highly diagnostic circumstances. Implications of these findings for close relationships and implicit social cognition research are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
... Concerns, ambivalence, fear, and doubts regarding the relationship are common during the early stages of a romantic relationship or in time of conflicts (Brickman, 1987), and they depend on spontaneous characteristics of the couple's relationship as consequences of changes in interdependence (Thompson & Holmes, 1996). After all, one of the most difficult challenges in developing a long-term relationship is to accept that the partner is less than perfect (Murray & Holmes, 1993). ...
Article
Relationship Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder (ROCD) is considered a clinical variant of obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) in which obsessions may be focused on the relationship itself (relationship-centered ROCD [RC-ROCD]), or on the partner's perceived flaws (partner-focused ROCD [PF-ROCD]). Despite the growing interest in ROCD, not much is known regarding contextual, cognitive, and personality determinants of the symptoms. Using multiple regression analysis, we investigated results of an online survey administered to 164 healthy participants, assessing fear of guilt, RC- and PF-ROCD symptoms, and narcissistic and paranoic personality traits. Results showed the significant effects of fear of guilt and relationship duration on both ROCD subtypes. Narcissism was found to predict PF-ROCD only, and effects of paranoia were found to predict RC-ROCD only, providing possible support for differential diagnoses. We discuss influence mechanisms. Taken together, these results offer a valuable source of knowledge in approaching the assessment of and intervention for ROCD as well as sexual dysfunction.
... Similarly, evaluations may be initially unstable because they are influenced by the (somewhat random) flow of early conversations and events; friends and acquaintances should have had more opportunities to assess the fit between stable features of the perceiver and the target. Furthermore, people often feel motivated to defend established relationships against the sense that partners are less-than-ideal (Gagne & Lydon, 2004;Murray & Holmes, 1993). But, they should be more willing to deliberate carefully about whether partners are a good match as they consider whether to spend time with one potential partner rather than another before making any official commitments. ...
Article
Full-text available
There are massive literatures on initial attraction and established relationships. But few studies capture early relationship development: the interstitial period in which people experience rising and falling romantic interest for partners who could—but often do not—become sexual or dating partners. In this study, 208 single participants reported on 1,065 potential romantic partners across 7,179 data points over 7 months. In stage 1, we used random forests (a type of machine learning) to estimate how well different classes of variables (e.g., individual differences vs. target-specific constructs) predicted participants’ romantic interest in these potential partners. We also tested (and found only modest support for) the perceiver × target moderation account of compatibility: the meta-theoretical perspective that some types of perceivers experience greater romantic interest for some types of targets. In stage 2, we used multilevel modeling to depict predictors retained by the random-forests models; robust (positive) main effects emerged for many variables, including sociosexuality, sex drive, perceptions of the partner’s positive attributes (e.g., attractive and exciting), attachment features (e.g., proximity seeking), and perceived interest. Finally, we found no support for ideal partner preference-matching effects on romantic interest. The discussion highlights the need for new models to explain the origin of romantic compatibility.
... 24,25 In terms of positive framing, those lower in attachment anxiety may experience an easier post-deployment transition, driven by an ability to frame conf icts in a way that minimizes, or buf ers, the negative impact on a relationship, given that conf icts are perceived as less threatening. 23,40 T is study also identif ed one mechanism to explain why attachment anxiety is related to lower-quality postdeployment partner interactions: namely, beliefs that original expectations about the reunion have not been met. 19,22 T ose higher in attachment anxiety might experience more unpleasant partner interactions as a result of overly optimistic beliefs about their partner and their relationship post-deployment, tied to needs for reassurance and validation and reliance on partners for ...
Article
LAY SUMMARY The goal of this study was to understand whether spousal attachment is related to the quality of post-deployment interactions and issues of conflict reported by spouses of Canadian Armed Forces (CAF) members during a post-deployment reunion. A total of 104 spouses of CAF Regular Force personnel who had recently been reunited with their partners after a deployment were surveyed. Results showed that both attachment anxiety (fear of rejection and abandonment) and attachment avoidance (discomfort with emotional intimacy and closeness) were related to lower-quality post-deployment interactions and the reported frequency of particular conflict issues. Specifically, attachment anxiety was related to more reports of unmet emotional needs, difficulties re-establishing intimacy, finances, and being less likely to describe the conflict in positive terms. The relationship between attachment anxiety and the quality of post-deployment interactions was explained by feeling as though one’s original expectations of the reunion were not met.
Article
Although negotiators need to build and sustain high‐quality relationships, relatively little attention has been given to how they might accomplish this. Negotiation researchers have focused largely on the role of trust and trust violations in dealmaking, neglecting the insights that relationship science can offer. In this article, I integrate research from the close relationships and marriage therapy literatures with negotiation research. Based on this integration, I identify three common themes—relationship history, shared meaning, and relationship work—and draw out nine lessons for repair following actions that disrupt a relationship between negotiators. Drawing on these three themes and their associated lessons, I conclude by identifying four guiding principles for developing strong positive relationships. The first principle highlights the importance of acting within a critical window. Using the opening moments of a negotiation to establish positive emotional tone and cognitive interdependence builds a constructive foundation. Reappraising temporary impasses as transformational opportunities and resetting positive affect immediately after a temporary impasse stabilizes relationships and returns them to a positive trajectory. The second principle highlights the role of pauses. Stepping back from the moment of a temporary impasse enables negotiators to neutralize negative emotions, creating the psychological space to positively reappraise the impasse as a transformational opportunity. The third principle draws attention to the benefits of creating a positive skew within relationships, that is, increasing the salience of positive moments in a relationship's history. Finally, the fourth principle draws upon the concept of positivity resonance to highlight the benefits of synchronicity: the alignment of cognitions and emotions in a way that establishes a shared perspective and a shared response to temporary impasses.
Article
Love has been ignored in group analysis for too long. This article describes different forms of love and attachment love styles. Techniques to assess and work with love are introduced. The social unconscious of love and lovesickness must be made conscious to prevent it from becoming a self-fulfilling prophecy. Technologies impact on dating, relationships and love. Groups can help patients address feelings and behaviour arising from these. Love can be curative provided psychotherapists allow loving feelings to emerge in therapy and work with them. Frequently, being in love or unrequited love results in irrational, destructive behaviour, sorrow and disenfranchised grief. Why love can be disappointing is discussed. Since lovesickness may be diagnosed as depression, using the term lovesickness for emotions and behaviour arising from love related difficulties is proposed. This reduces stigma, because lovesickness is normal.
Article
Research on the social consequences of narcissism points to an intriguing paradox: narcissists are socially aversive and destructive to healthy interpersonal relationships; yet, narcissists also have an ability to be socially magnetic and attractive. This raises the question we seek to answer in this paper: Are narcissists socially accepted by coworkers in the workplace, and if so, when? Drawing on the social‐constructionist perspective and power‐dependence theory, we propose that others’ dependency on narcissists plays a critical role in determining narcissists’ social acceptance in the workplace. Results from two time‐lagged independent studies suggest that narcissists with a high level of expertise status experience less ostracism than non‐narcissists, particularly in a group with high group goal interdependence; by contrast, narcissists who are perceived to lack expertise status experience greater ostracism than non‐narcissists, particularly in a group with low group goal interdependence. In Study 2, in addition to ostracism, we also examined social inclusion and popularity of narcissists, and we found that narcissists with high expertise status are more likely to be social included and to become popular, particularly in a group with a high level of group goal interdependence.
Article
We draw on the sociological theories of the “liability of newness” and the “liability of adolescence” to generate new insights into relationship evolution. First, we show how a new relationship during its “honeymoon” exhibits a unique constellation of two conditions, namely information asymmetry and forbearance. Next, we explain how a relationship evolves along two processes that involve passive learning and decay, respectively. In themselves, these processes will move a relationship towards a long-term “transactional” state and possibly termination, but the processes can also be actively shaped using governance mechanisms of various kinds. The latter, however, requires a nuanced account of types of governance mechanisms and the particular conditions they are intended to induce. We consider how the general mechanisms of (1) incentives and (2) information sharing can be deployed in standardized or customized fashions, respectively. Next, we suggest how different manifestation of governance mechanisms impact a relationship’s underlying evolutionary processes and evolved relationship states. In general, our framework represents a new perspective on relationship evolution; one that involves the purposeful management of initial conditions and their related evolutionary processes.
Article
Full-text available
26 couples (mean age 20.3 yrs) planning marriage participated in the initial stage of the study, and 9 of the 21 couples remaining intact during the study completed data (including the Marital Relationship Inventory) at all 3 follow-up points. A correlation coefficient of .59 was obtained, indicating that the more positively premarital couples had rated their communication, the more satisfied they were with their relationship 5½ yrs later. Findings are consistent with the social learning model of marriage hypothesis that communication deficits precede the development of marital distress. (4 ref)
Chapter
As survey researchers, we must often rely on the introspection of strangers. If we wish to know a person’s opinion on some complex political issue, we have little recourse but to ask him or her. We can take pains to present the questions in as simple, comprehensible, and unbiased a manner as possible; in the end, however, the difficult task of remembering, organizing, and synthesizing disparate information into a single coherent attitude, and then mapping that attitude onto the available response alternatives, must fall to the respondent alone. Research by Converse (1970) and others suggests that this task may often be too much for respondents: They may end up responding with transitory and inconsistent “nonattitudes” that provide little information about their true beliefs.
Chapter
This chapter examines social psychological implications of human subjectivity—implications of the fact, and perhaps more importantly the insight, that people are governed not by the passive reception and recognition of some invariant objective reality, but by their own subjective representations and constructions of the events that unfold around them. The history of the subjective-objective distinction, first in some traditional theoretical and methodological concerns of social psychology, and then human motivation are discussed in this chapter. Social cognition, a research area that has held center stage in the field for most of the past two decades is expalined. The particular focus will be the problem of situational construal and its contribution to the difficulties of predicting social actions and making inferences or attributions about social actors. Construal processes are variable and uncertain, and they contribute heavily to the variability and unpredictability of a wide range of social responses. The second and less familiar thesis, social perceivers, fail to recognize, or at least fail to make adequate inferential allowance for, these “vagaries” of construal. People characteristically make attributions and other social judgments, and decisions predicated on a kind of naive realism. The process of subjective construal is fundamental to psychological inquiry at all levels of analysis.