ArticlePDF Available

The Halo Effect: Evidence for Unconscious Alteration of Judgments

Authors:

Abstract

Staged 2 different videotaped interviews with the same individual—a college instructor who spoke English with a European accent. In one of the interviews the instructor was warm and friendly, in the other, cold and distant. 118 undergraduates were asked to evaluate the instructor. Ss who saw the warm instructor rated his appearance, mannerisms, and accent as appealing, whereas those who saw the cold instructor rated these attributes as irritating. Results indicate that global evaluations of a person can induce altered evaluations of the person's attributes, even when there is sufficient information to allow for independent assessments of them. Furthermore, Ss were unaware of this influence of global evaluations on ratings of attributes. In fact, Ss who saw the cold instructor actually believed that the direction of influence was opposite to the true direction. They reported that their dislike of the instructor had no effect on their rating of his attributes but that their dislike of his attributes had lowered their global evaluations of him. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal of Feisonality and Social Psychology
1977, Vol 35, No. 4, 250-256
The Halo Effect: Evidence for Unconscious
Alteration of Judgments
Richard E. Nisbett and Timothy DeCamp Wilson
University of Michigan
Two different videotaped interviews were staged with the same individual—a
college instructor who spoke English with a European accent. In one of the
interviews the instructor was warm and friendly, in the other, cold and distant.
The subjects who saw the warm instructor rated his appearance, mannerisms,
and accent as appealing, whereas those who saw the cold instructor rated these
attributes as irritating These results indicate that global evaluations of a person
can induce altered evaluations of the person's attributes, even when there is
sufficient information to allow for independent assessments of them. Further-
more, the subjects were unaware of this influence of global evaluations on
ratings of attributes. In fact, the subjects who saw the cold instructor actually
believed that the direction of influence was opposite to the true direction. They
reported that their dislike of the instructor had no effect on their ratings of
his attributes but that their dislike of his attributes had lowered their global
evaluations of him.
Although the halo effect is one of the oldest
and most widely known of psychological phe-
nomena, surprisingly little is known about its
nature. The halo effect is generally defined as
the influence of a global evaluation on evalua-
tions of individual attributes of a person, but
this definition is imprecise with respect to the
strength and character of the influence. At
one extreme, the halo effect might be due
simply to an extrapolation from a general
impression to unknown attributes. Global
evaluations might color presumptions about
specific traits or influence interpretation of
the meaning or affective value of ambiguous
trait information. Thus, if we like a person, we
often assume that those attributes of the per-
son about which we know little are also favor-
able.
(Politicians often seem to capitalize on
this tendency by appearing warm and friendly
but saying little about the issues.) Such a
phenomenon could best be described as a de-
The present research was supported by National
Science Foundation Grant BNS75-23191.
We are indebted to Eugene Borgida and Hersz
Zukier for their assistance and advice and to Lee
Ross for criticism of an early draft of the manuscript.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Richard E
Nisbett, Research Center for Group Dynamics, Insti-
tute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor, Michigan 48106.
duction from an implicit personality theory
holding that "nice people tend to have nice
attributes and less nice people have less nice
attributes." Similarly, the global evaluation
may alter the interpretation of the meaning
or evaluation of ambiguous attributes. Thus,
if one is told that a warm and friendly person
is impetuous, a quite different set of behaviors
come to mind from those that occur when one
is told that the impetuous person is angry
and hostile. The evaluation of the attribute
impetuous may differ in the two cases, but
the difference could be a mere interpretive
one arising from the ambiguity inherent in the
term impetuous.
It is possible, however, that the halo effect
is a much more powerful phenomenon. Global
evaluations may be capable of altering percep-
tions of even relatively unambiguous stimuli,
about which the individual has sufficient in-
formation to render a confident judgment. For
example, a person's appearance may be per-
ceived as more attractive if we like the person
than if we do not. If so, then the halo effect
involves much more than influence on pre-
sumptions about attributes or interpretations
of the meaning of attributes, but extends to
alteration of judgments about attributes for
which we generally assume we are capable of
rendering independent assessments.
250
HALO EFFECT251
The literature to date on the halo effect
does not provide a clear picture of either the
strength or the nature of the phenomenon. Al-
though correlational studies are plentiful, they
do not serve to demonstrate even that the
phenomenon exists. In any array of trait rat-
ings,
correlations tend to be positive, that is,
affectively consistent, but it is always possible
that the correlations are based on veridical
social perception. For example, people with
more pleasant personalities may actually be
better looking on the average than people with
less pleasant personalities. Alternatively, peo-
ple's criteria for likability and good looks may
differ in such a way as to produce positive
correlations in the absence of any influence of
one judgment or the other. For example, a
college football player and a drama major
may have different criteria for both likability
and good looks in other males. The football
player may prize a hail-fellow-well-met style
and may regard brawn as essential to mascu-
line good looks. The drama major may favor
verbal wit and a lean, ascetic appearance. If
an open personal style and brawniness are
associated and if a witty style and lean, as-
cetic looks are associated, then both the foot-
ball player and the drama major might show
positive correlations between their judgments
of likability and looks, even though they give
high scores on both dimensions to utterly dif-
ferent people and even though neither judg-
ment has in any way influenced the other.
To demonstrate that global evaluations al-
ter evaluations of particular attributes re-
quires an experimental approach. There have
been two sorts of experimental studies in this
area. One type employs adjectives rather than
persons as the stimuli (e.g., Anderson, 1974;
Asch, 1946). This research supports the gen-
eralization that the evaluative rating given to
a trait adjective is a function of the total
adjective set in which it is embedded. In a
sense, then, the global evaluation alters the
evaluation of a particular attribute. But this
work suffices only to show that the total ad-
jective set may influence the meaning or eval-
uation of a particular adjective whose meaning
or value is somewhat ambiguous when con-
sidered in isolation, as in the example above
of the interpretation of the adjective im-
Petuous.
Another type of experimental study has
employed actual persons as the stimuli
(Clif-
ford & Walster, 1973; Dion, 1972; Dion,
Berscheid, & Walster, 1972; Miller, 1970),
but these studies suffice to show only the weak
"altered presumption" version of the halo ef-
fect because they provide extremely impover-
ished information about the attributes to be
rated. For example, Miller (1970) gave photo-
graphs of attractive and unattractive target
persons to subjects and asked them to rate the
personalities of the target persons. Attractive
people were ascribed more favorable traits
than unattractive people, but this merely
constitutes a demonstration that people's im-
plicit personality theories include the assump-
tion that physically attractive people have
more attractive personalities. A convincing
demonstration of the strong version of the
halo effect phenomenon would require giving
subjects sufficient information on the attri-
butes to be rated to allow for an independent
assessment of the attributes.
One study in the above tradition went sub-
stantially further. Landy and Sigall (1974)
showed that evaluations of an essay given by
male college students were substantially
higher when the alleged writer of the essay
was an attractive woman than when the al-
leged writer was an unattractive woman. The
effect was very pronounced, especially when
the essay was of relatively poor quality. This
study supports the stronger interpretation of
the halo phenomenon because, unlike the
attributes in the above studies, the subjects
had information about the woman's essay that
was entirely sufficient to allow for an inde-
pendent assessment of its quality. The Landy
and Sigall study, however, was not precisely
a study of the halo effect because it was a
single attribute rather than a global evalua-
tion that was manipulated and a person's
product rather than an attribute that was
measured.
The present experiment was designed to
address an additional issue—the question of
people's awareness of the halo effect. Nisbett
and Wilson (1977) have recently argued that
people have little awareness of the nature or
even the existence of the cognitive processes
that mediate judgments, inferences, and the
production of complex social behavior. The
252RICHARD E NISBETT AND TIMOTHY
DECAMP
WILSON
halo effect would appear particularly likely
to be such a subterranean, unrecognized
process. If people were capable of rendering
independent assessments of attributes, they
would surely do so. The halo effect, particu-
larly if it is the strong, distorting influence
postulated by some theorists, would appear
to depend upon lack of awareness for its
existence. On the other hand, it is not incon-
ceivable that subjects might actually be able
to report correctly on its operation if they
were directly questioned about it (e.g., Oh
yes,
now that you mention it, maybe part of
the reason I liked his looks was because I
liked his personality). The present experiment
directly assessed people's reports about the
nature and direction of the influence of global
evaluations on evaluations of attributes.
Method
Overview
College student subjects were asked to evaluate a
psychology instructor, whom they viewed in a video-
taped interview, on several dimensions The subjects
were told that the investigators were studying the
possibility that ratings of an instructor presented in
such a brief fashion might resemble ratings by stu-
dents who had taken an entire course with the in-
structor The subjects were shown one of two differ-
ent interviews with the same instructor, a native
French-speaking Belgian who spoke English with a
fairly pronounced accent. In one interview, the in-
structor presented himself as a likable teacher, re-
spectful of his students' intelligence and motives,
flexible in his approach to teaching, and enthusiastic
about his subject matter In the other interview, he
appeared to be quite unlikable, cold and distrustful
toward his students, rigid and doctrinaire in his
teaching style. After viewing the videotaped inter-
view, the subjects rated the instructor's likability,
as well as the attractiveness of his physical appear-
ance,
his mannerisms, and his accent In order to
explore the question of subject awareness, some sub-
jects were then asked whether the instructor's likabil-
ity had affected their ratings of his appearance, man-
nerisms, and accent. Others were asked the reverse
question, namely, whether their evaluation of his
appearance, mannerisms, or accent had affected their
ratings of his likability. It was anticipated that the
subjects would rate the instructor as having a more
attractive physical appearance, more attractive man-
nerisms, and a more attractive accent when he was
likable than when he was unlikable It was also an-
ticipated that the subjects would be unaware of these
effects on their judgments and would deny that their
global evaluation of the instructor had affected their
other ratings.
Procedure
The subjects were 118 University of Michigan stu-
dents enrolled in introductory psychology; 62 were
males and 56 were females. The results for the two
sexes were entirely similar and therefore were com-
bined for presentation. The subjects participated in
the experiment in groups of 6-17. Sessions were con-
ducted in such a way as to insure that no overt
communication between subjects took place. (But in
any case, conclusions are not affected when the group
rather than the individual was used as the unit of
analysis.) On arrival, the subjects were ushered into
a well-appointed conference room with chairs oriented
toward a 19-inch (.48-meter) television monitor.
The subjects were told that the psychology depart-
ment had recently conducted a large-scale evaluation
of teachers of introductory psychology:
At the end of the term teachers were evaluated by
their students on a number of dimensions, ranging
from traditional questions of teaching effectiveness
to questions about matters such as physical ap-
pearance and mannerisms We are interested in
finding out whether evaluations of teachers who
are seen in brief videotaped interviews at all re-
semble the evaluations of these teachers by students
who were exposed to them for a whole semester.
We will ask you to look at these videotapes and
then rate the teachers on some of the same dimen-
sions employed in the original study.
The subjects were then shown two videotaped in-
terviews All subjects were first shown a 7-minute
filler interview with a bright and pleasant instructor
who answered such questions as, What is your gen-
eral educational philosophy? Do you encourage stu-
dent discussion? How do you handle evaluations of
student work? Then half the subjects saw the Bel-
gian instructor answer the same questions in his
warm guise and half saw him answer the questions
in his cold guise Each of these interviews also lasted
about 7 minutes.
The warmness or coldness of the instructor was ap-
parent in his answers to virtually all the questions
For example, in his warm guise the instructor an-
swered the question about student discussion by say-
ing that, yes, he encouraged discussion and that he
was stimulated by the give and take it provided and
felt that it enhanced student interest in the lecture
material. In his cold guise he answered the question
by saying that he didn't allow much discussion be-
cause "there's a time to be a student and a time to
be a professor" and he wouldn't be the professor if
he didn't know more than they did. In his warm
guise the instructor answered the question about
evaluation of student work by saying that he gave
paper assignments that the students seemed to like
and profit from and gave exams that were a mixture
of objective questions and thought questions. In his
cold guise he said he gave weekly multiple-choice
quizzes because otherwise "y°u can't trust them to
do the reading."
HALO EFFECT253
Dependent Measures
After seeing each interview, all subjects were asked,
"How much do you think you would like this
teacher?,"
and were asked to rate him on an 8-point
scale ranging from "like extremely" to "dislike ex-
tremely." The questionnaire then read: "Listed below
are some characteristics of the teacher you saw.
Please indicate how you think you would feel about
each of the characteristics if you were to take a
course from the teacher" The characteristics in-
cluded physical appearance, mannerisms, and for the
Belgian instructor, accent. The subjects rated each
characteristic on the scale below:
extremely appealing
very appealing
somewhat appealing
slightly appealing
slightly irritating
somewhat irritating
very irritating
extremely irritating
In addition, 34 subjects were told that the investi-
gators were interested in knowing "how much, if at
all, your liking for the teacher influenced the ratings
you just made." The subjects indicated their answers
on scales of the following type:
My liking for the teacher caused me to rate his
physical appearance:
much higher
higher
slightly higher
had no effect
slightly lower
lower
much lower
Another 56 subjects were asked to indicate "how
much, if at all, the characteristics you just rated in-
fluenced your liking of the teacher." The subjects
indicated their answers on scales of the following
type:
Physical appearance made me like him:
much more
more
slightly more
had no effect
slightly less
less
much less
After responding to the questionnaire, the subjects
were quizzed intensively about their reactions to the
videotapes and to the questionnaire items, and then
were debriefed.
Results
Effect
of
Global
Evaluation
on
Evaluations of Attributes
The manipulation had a very pronounced
effect on liking of the teacher. The t value
contrasting the warm condition mean of 5.48
with the cold condition mean of 3.18 is 8.62
(df =
1/117,
p <
.00001
).1
Figure 1 presents
ratings of physical appearance, mannerisms,
and accent as a function of the liking manipu-
lation. For ease of presentation, all of the
positive (appealing) ratings are grouped to-
gether and contrasted with the negative (ir-
ritating) ratings. A small number of subjects
checked the point intermediate between ap-
pealing and irritating, and these subjects are
included with the irritating ratings. It may be
seen that the effect of the manipulation on at-
tribute ratings was very pronounced. A sub-
stantial majority of the subjects who saw the
teacher in his warm guise rated his physical
appearance as appealing, whereas a substan-
tial majority of those who saw him in his cold
guise rated his appearance as irritating (t =
4.71,
df - 1/H7, p < .00001). A majority of
warm condition subjects rated the teacher's
mannerisms as appealing, whereas a majority
of cold condition subjects rated bis manner-
isms as irritating (I = 4.06, df -
1/117,
p <
.0001).
About half the warm condition sub-
jects rated the teacher's accent as appealing
80
60
PERCENT
OF
40
SUBJECTS
20
0
80
60
PERCEUT
OF
40
SUBJECTS
20
0
80
60
PERCENT
Of
40
SUBJECTS
20
WARM TEACHER
I
COLD TEACHER
Physical Appearance
Mannerisms
Accent
APPEAUW IRMTHIIC
1
APfOLIK IRRITATIHC
Figure 1. Ratings of Teacher's Physical Appearance,
Mannerisms, and Accent as a Function of Manipu-
lated Warmth versus Coldness.
1 All p values are based on two-tailed tests.
254RICHARD E. NISBETT AND TIMOTHY
DECAMP
WILSON
and half as irritating, whereas the overwhelm-
ing majority of subjects in the cold condition
rated his accent as irritating (* = 3.78, df
1/117,
p< .0002).
The results demonstrate that global assess-
ment of a person can powerfully alter evalua-
tions of particular attributes. That is, this is
the case if one is willing to make the assump-
tion that the attributes manifested by the
confederate were in fact constant across ex-
perimental conditions. Although this is a rea-
sonable assumption, it is by no means indis-
putable. It might have been the case, for
example, that the teacher smiled a lot in his
warm guise, thus making his appearance more
appealing and/or frowned a lot in his cold
guise, thus making it less appealing. Similarly,
his mannerisms, both physical and verbal,
might have differed when he was playing the
two roles. It is even conceivable that his ac-
cent might have been less pronounced and
harsh when he was playing the warm role
than when playing the cold role.
In order to demonstrate that the teacher's
physical appearance did not in fact differ
across the two conditions, a follow-up study
was conducted with 34 subjects from the same
introductory psychology pool. These subjects
80
60
Kucat
Of
40
SUBJECTS
20
80
60
PERCENT
or 40
SUBJECTS
20
80
60
PERCENT
OF
40
SUBJECTS
20
WARM
TEACHER
|
COLO TEACHER
Physical
Appearonc*
DECREASED
»0
INOEASED
RATH*
EFFECT
MTK
DECREASED
NO
INCREASED
RATUK EFFECT RATHX
60
PERCENT
40
Of
SUBJECTS
20
PERCENT
OF
40
SUBJECTS
20
60
PERCEUT
OF
40
SUBJECTS
20
WARM
TEACHER
|
COLD
TEACHER
Physical
Appearance
DECREASED
M
MOUSED
RATNK EFFECT
ROM
DECEASED
M
INCREASED
RATHK EFFECT BATIK
Figure 2. Reported Effect of Teacher Likability on
Ratings of Physical Appearance, Mannerisms, and
Accent.
Figure 3. Reported Effect of Reactions to Physical
Appearance, Mannerisms, and Accent on Ratings of
Likability of Teacher.
were shown the interviews minus the audio
portions (purportedly for a study of nonverbal
communication) and were then asked to rate
the physical appearance of the teacher, as well
as his mannerisms. The subjects shown the
warm version rated the teacher's physical ap-
pearance and mannerisms only trivially higher
than the subjects shown the cold version (for
both, t < 1). This indicates that the physical
appearance and nonverbal mannerisms of the
teacher did not in fact differ across conditions.
Thus,
it can be assumed that the ratings of
physical appearance for the original subjects
differed solely because of the global affective
difference induced by the audio portion of the
interview.
Awareness of the Influence of
Global Evaluation
The subjects were apparently unaware of
the nature of the influence of global evalua-
tion on their ratings. Thirty-four of the sub-
jects were asked if their liking for the teacher
had affected their ratings of his attributes.
Figure 2 presents these subjects' answers to
this question. For ease of presentation, the
data are divided into three categories: (a)
decreased rating (representing 3 scale points),
(b) no effect (representing 1 scale point), and
(c) increased rating (representing 3 scale
points). It may be seen that a majority of
HALO EFFECT255
subjects, in both the warm and cold condi-
tions,
believed that their overall liking for
the teacher had not influenced their ratings.
Moreover, for all ratings in both conditions,
as many subjects felt that their overall evalu-
ation had increased their ratings as believed
it had decreased their ratings. None of the
distributions differs significantly from the
zero,
no effect point (all six one-sample ts
not significant).
Fifty-six subjects were asked if their evalu-
ations of the teacher's appearance, manner-
isms,
or accent had affected their liking of the
teacher. Figure 3 presents the results. It may
be seen that the subjects who saw the warm
teacher were divided on the question of
whether the ratings of particular attributes
affected liking. Only for the physical appear-
ance question did the majority of subjects
express the opinion that appearance had had
no effect on liking. For the other two ques-
tions,
approximately equal numbers of sub-
jects believed that the ratings had increased,
decreased, or had no effect on liking. None of
the three distributions differs significantly
from a mean of zero.
For the subjects who saw the cold teacher,
the results were quite different. On balance,
the subjects believed that their evaluations of
each of the three attributes had decreased
their liking for the teacher. The mean of all
three distributions differs from zero (t for
physical appearance = 3.28, p < .01; t for
mannerisms = 2.80, p < .01; / for accent =
2.74, p < .01).
Thus,
the subjects appear to have gotten
matters precisely backwards. Their liking for
the teacher was manipulated, and this affected
their ratings of particular attributes. Yet the
subjects did not acknowledge this effect, and
the subjects who saw the cold teacher actually
reported the opposite effect. These subjects
reported that their evaluations of the teacher's
attributes lowered their liking for him, al-
though they denied that their liking for the
teacher had affected their ratings of his attri-
butes.
(Logically, of course, it is possible that
the subjects who saw the cold teacher are
partly right. It is conceivable that the
teacher's appearance, mannerisms, and accent
were all intrinsically unattractive. In that
event, the subjects who saw the cold teacher
might have been accurate when they reported
that their liking for the teacher was decreased
because of their evaluations of these attri-
butes.
If that were the case, then the subjects
in the warm condition erred in believing that
the teacher's appearance, mannerisms, and
accent were neutral and had had no effect on
their liking of him.)
Conversations with the subjects following
the experiment served to reinforce the implica-
tions of Figures 2 and 3. The subjects were
asked if they were quite sure that their global
evaluations had not influenced their ratings of
the teacher's attributes. Most subjects per-
sisted in their denial—some warily, perhaps
because the}' suspected the experimenter knew
something they didn't, and some heatedly.
One young woman said, in a somewhat ex-
asperated tone, "Before he even began talk-
ing, I made my judgment about his appear-
ance and then I stuck with it." The
conversations gave reason to suspect, in fact,
that some of the subjects in Figure 2 who re-
ported that their liking for the teacher had
influenced their ratings did not actually be-
lieve this. One of the subjects who had indi-
cated this said, on being probed about his
answer: "Actually, I just turned the question
around I disliked his accent a little, so that
made me dislike him more."
Discussion
The present results support the strong in-
terpretation of the halo effect phenomenon.
They indicate that global evaluations alter
evaluations of attributes about which the in-
dividual has information fully sufficient to
allow for an independent assessment. These
results, it should be noted, are consistent with
the very earliest theorizing about the phe-
nomenon. Thorndike (1920), who gave the
phenomenon its name, clearly believed that
it represented far more than an effect on
presumptions about or interpretations of the
evaluative meaning of attributes, but rather
that it represented a fundamental inability
to resist the affective influence of global
evaluation on evaluation of specific attri-
butes.
Subsequent theorists have tended to
share this view, though in the absence of
strong evidence to support it.
256RICHARD E NISBETT AND TIMOTHY
DECAMP
WILSON
The layman has probably never doubted
the existence of an inability to separate evalu-
ations of attributes from global evaluations,
at least at the affective extremes (and so long
as he was referring to the foibles of others).
The strong interpretation of the halo effect is
embedded in such sayings as "love is blind"
and "a face only a mother could love." The
present study, however, suggests that global
evaluations may have a pronounced impact on
evaluations of specific attributes even when
the global evaluations in question are less ex-
treme than love and hate.
The present results, and those of Landy and
Sigall (1974), also provide prima facie evi-
dence that people lack awareness of the in-
fluence of one evaluation on another. Indeed,
it would appear that the altered judgments
require the absence of awareness. The sub-
jects in the present study would surely have
been disconcerted to know that they were in-
capable of rendering an independent evalua-
tion of such characteristics as physical appear-
ance,
mannerisms, and accent. They would
have been disconcerted, that is, if they could
have been persuaded that they had been in-
fluenced by their global evaluation. The sub-
jects were quite unfazed by the debriefing,
probably because each subject exempted him-
self from the accusation that he had been
prone to such an influence. The subjects in
the Landy and Sigall (1974) study would un-
doubtedly have been even more horrified if
they could have been convinced that they had
upgraded their evaluations of the essay of an
attractive woman or downgraded their evalua-
tions of the essay of a plain woman.
But the prima facie case is not the only
one that can be made. The subjects in the
present study were directly quizzed about
their beliefs concerning the nature and direc-
tion of influence. They overwhelmingly re-
ported that their ratings of particular attri-
butes had not been affected by their global
evaluations. The subjects who saw the cold
instructor actually believed that the influence
ran in the opposite direction—that their dis-
like of the teacher's attributes prompted their
unfavorable global evaluation.
When considering the establishment of
rules concerning blind review, conflict of in-
terest, nepotism, and the like, it would there-
fore seem advisable to consider more than the
possibility that some individuals in the system
may be venal and corrupt. The protestations
of even the most virtuous and disinterested
participants that they are capable of inde-
pendent judgments should be considered sus-
pect.
References
Anderson, N. H. Information integration theory: A
brief survey. In D H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson,
R. D. Luce, & P Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary
developments
in
mathematical
psychology
(Vol. 2).
San Francisco: Freeman, 1974.
Asch, S. E. Forming impressions of personality. Jour-
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1946, 41,
258-290.
Clifford, M. M., & Walster, E. The effect of physical
attractiveness on teacher expectations. Sociology of
Education, 1973, 46, 248-258.
Dion, K K Physical attractiveness and evaluation of
children's transgressions Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1972, 24, 207-213
Dion, K. K., Berscheid, E., & Walster E. What is
beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and So-
cial Psychology 1972, 24, 285-290
Landy, D., & Sigall, H. Beauty is talent: Task evalu-
ation as a function of the performer's physical at-
tractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 1974, 29, 299-304.
Miller, A G. Role of physical attractiveness in im-
pression formation. Psychonomic Science, 1970, IP,
241-242
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we
can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.
Psychological Review, 1977, 7, 231-259.
Thorndike, E. L A constant error in psychological
ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1920, 4,
25-29.
Received August 30, 1976
... The moderating role of country image can be explained by the halo effect. The halo effect refers to a phenomenon in which the salient characteristics of an object affect the evaluation of other characteristics of the object [68]. The halo effect also plays an important role in regard to explaining consumer behavior. ...
... Country image is an individual's total of all the informational, descriptive, and inferential beliefs about a particular country [62,63]. The study proposed the moderating role of country image, which is grounded in the halo effect theory [68,70]. The result revealed that H8c and H8g were statistically supported. ...
Article
Full-text available
The objective of this study is to identify the role of Chinese tourists’ shopping values in the formation of their outcome variables. This study more specifically proposes that two dimensions of shopping values, which include utilitarian and hedonic shopping values, positively affect brand prestige. In addition, it was hypothesized that brand prestige helps in regard to enhancing well-being perceptions and brand preference. It was also proposed that well-being perceptions and brand preference have a positive influence on word-of-mouth. This study also hypothesized the moderating role of country image in the proposed model. The survey responses were collected from 634 Chinese duty-free tourists. The data analysis results showed that both utilitarian and hedonic shopping values significantly affect brand prestige. Moreover, brand prestige was found to be a significant determinant of well-being perceptions, and well-being perceptions positively affect brand preference. Brand preference is a critical factor affecting word-of-mouth. Lastly, country image played a moderating role in the relationship between brand prestige and well-being perceptions and brand preference and word-of-mouth.
... Groups who were located approximately in the "admiration" quadrant of the SCM, such as physicians and college students, were more defined by their social groups than by their genders or use of ADV, as none of these two factors had any influence on their ratings. A certain halo effect of their group membership can be assumed [106]. Further, we argue that physicians are expected to use modern technology in their practice on a regular basis and college students are possibly perceived to be tech-savvy (either by participants who are college students or by the largely middle-aged participants); thus, the alleged use of ADV had no effect. ...
Article
Full-text available
Innovations like autonomous delivery vehicles (ADV) have the potential to transform last-mile delivery to make it more sustainable and human-centered. Yet only if these technologies are socially acceptable can they live up to their potential. Using the Stereotype Content Model (SCM), we assessed how different social groups were perceived when they used autonomous delivery vehicles. Based on the two dimensions of the SCM, warmth and competence, we found combinations of group stereotypes and use of ADV that impact social acceptability, and we identified systematic effects of gender and vehicle usage on social acceptability. Our results highlight the importance of social perception for the acceptance of autonomous vehicles and the relevance of the intersections of gender, vehicle usage, and social group for an accurate and comprehensive evaluation of the social acceptability of autonomous delivery vehicles.
... Therefore, when a person is served by an employee with a visible disability, the potential negative attitude or affective reactions toward PWDs could bias their perceptions. This could be explained as the halo effect, which refers to the attitude of an independent attribute being affected by another attribute cognitively [47]. Meanwhile, a study on employers from the hospitality industry has found that companies share related concerns about people with a disability being unable to do the required work, or the need for extra unspecified accommodation expenses, as set by the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA); although, such accommodations are not exceptionally costly [48]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigates the effect of hiring people with a disability in the service staff on customers’ perspectives in Jordan, with the moderating role of disability type. The quantitative research method was adopted to obtain the necessary data for the research; the study sample was convenient and consisted of 765 participants who are social media users belonging to different age groups. The data were collected using an online questionnaire distributed through various social media platforms. Findings exhibited that the antecedent variable positively affects the consequent variables (quality, brand image, and loyalty), while there was no significant effect on purchase intention or stereotyping. Further, hiring people with a disability had the strongest impact on perceived brand image. The findings from this research could eventually furnish assistance to existing and future entrepreneurs to build sustainable enterprises while keeping pace with a nation becoming socially responsible.
... Each one of these approaches shows both advantages and disadvantages. First, auto-informed methods, such as questionnaires or interviews, for which participants are directly asked to report their status are the only way to access the individual's subjective perception are limited by the individual's own ability to introspect, since many psychological processes can occur unconsciously or with low levels of consciousness (Nisbett and Wilson 1977). Moreover, in certain cases it is possible that cognitive biases (such as social desirability bias) interfere with the reports, making the information not entirely reliable. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Esports gaming is an area in which videogame players need to cooperate and compete with each other, influencing their cognitive load, processing, stress, and as well as social skills. Here it is unknown to which extent traditional videogame play (with desktop setting) can affect physiology of players' autonomic nervous system. For such, we propose a study where we have measured distinct electrodermal and cardiac activity metrics over competitive players during several League of Legends gameplay sessions. We mainly found that game performance (whether winning or losing the game) affects significantly to both EDA and ECG, where losing players to have a higher stress levels, and reversely with winning players. We also found that crucial specific ingame events such as "Killing", "Dying" or "Destroying Turret" significatively increases electrodermal and cardiac activity over players more than other less-relevant events such as "Placing Wards" or "Destroying Turret Plates". By analyzing activity on player roles we found trends of distinct activity on these measurements, potential for exploration in future studies.
... Each one of these approaches shows both advantages and disadvantages. First, auto-informed methods, such as questionnaires or interviews, for which participants are directly asked to report their status are the only way to access the individual's subjective perception are limited by the individual's own ability to introspect, since many psychological processes can occur unconsciously or with low levels of consciousness (Nisbett and Wilson 1977). Moreover, in certain cases it is possible that cognitive biases (such as social desirability bias) interfere with the reports, making the information not entirely reliable. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Esports gaming is an area in which videogame players need to cooperate and compete with each other, influencing their cognitive load, processing, stress, and as well as social skills. Here it is unknown to which extent traditional videogame play (with desktop setting) can affect physiology of players' autonomic nervous system. For such, we propose a study where we have measured distinct electrodermal and cardiac activity metrics over competitive players during several League of Legends gameplay sessions. We mainly found that game performance (whether winning or losing the game) affects significantly to both EDA and ECG, where losing players to have a higher stress levels, and reversely with winning players. We also found that crucial specific ingame events such as "Killing", "Dying" or "Destroying Turret" significatively increases electrodermal and cardiac activity over players more than other less-relevant events such as "Placing Wards" or "Destroying Turret Plates". By analyzing activity on player roles we found trends of distinct activity on these measurements, potential for exploration in future studies.
... Since the seminal halo effect study by Nisbett and Wilson (1977) showing that one's overall evaluation of a person leads to positive assessments about otherwise unknown attributes, the concept has expanded. The halo effect, a term coined by Thorndike (1920), showed an overestimation of favorable qualities of higher-ups in one's profession-a consistent error that was like a halo and demonstrated a frequent lack of objective criteria in evaluating personal qualities. ...
Article
Countries in the Middle East go to considerable lengths using mass media to try to maintain or improve their images among the U.S. public. The same countries often engage in negative media campaigns in the U.S. against each other, attempting to bring down public support for regional rivals. This study examined diplomatic evaluations of five Middle East countries—rating Saudi Arabia, Israel, Palestine, Qatar, and UAE as an ally, neutral, or enemy of the U.S.—among a large, representative sample of U.S. adults (N=2,059), and assessed measures of news use, social media use, political partisanship, and demographic variables as predictors of the evaluations. News use and other media use predicted little variance in diplomatic ratings; the strongest predictors of positive ratings of a given country were having rated one or more of the other countries positively also—what we term a “regional halo effect.” A key implication of this study is that attempting to harm public perceptions of a regional rival may be self-defeating for a given country, as negative attitudes toward the former country are associated with poor attitudes toward the latter.
Article
Full-text available
More organizations use AI in the hiring process than ever before, yet the perceived ethicality of such processes seems to be mixed. With such variation in our views of AI in hiring, we need to understand how these perceptions impact the organizations that use it. In two studies, we investigate how ethical perceptions of using AI in hiring are related to perceptions of organizational attractiveness and innovativeness. Our findings indicate that ethical perceptions of using AI in hiring are positively related to perceptions of organizational attractiveness, both directly and indirectly via perceptions of organizational innovativeness, with variations depending on the type of hiring method used. For instance, we find that individuals who consider it ethical for organizations to use AI in ways often considered to be intrusive to privacy, such as analyzing social media content for traits and characteristics, view such organizations as both more innovative and attractive. Our findings trigger a timely discussion about the critical role of ethical perceptions of AI in hiring on organizational attractiveness and innovativeness.
Article
Research has established that social factors are integral to trauma recovery. Yet, there is relatively little data on the association between social interactions from different supports and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms. Moreover, few studies have measured these factors from multiple informants. This paper examined social interactions from different sources (i.e., negative and positive reactions from a chosen close other [CO], family/friends, and general non‐COs) and their association with PTSD symptoms using multi‐informant reports of the social constructs (i.e., from the individual exposed to trauma [TI] and their CO). Participants (N = 104 dyads) were recruited from an urban center within 6 months of the TIs’ exposure to a traumatic event. TIs were assessed using the Clinician‐Administered PTSD Scale. TI self‐report, t(97) = 2.58, p = .012, and CO collateral report of disapproval from family/friends, t(97) = 2.14, p = .035, and TI self‐report of general disapproval, t(97) = 4.91, p < .001, emerged as significant predictors of PTSD symptoms when compared with other social constructs. Interventions that target family members’ and friends’ reactions to trauma survivors and societal discourse around trauma and reactions to trauma survivors are recommended. Clinical interventions that buffer against TIs’ experiences of disapproval and guide COs on the provision of supportive responses are discussed.
Article
Celebrity is a form of policy influence that can occur under distinctive circumstances. This paper draws on the regulatory/policy capture literature to develop a model of celebrity capture that explains how interest groups can affect policy in the absence of economic clout or constituency mobilization. We posit that the likelihood of celebrity capture increases when several factors align: (1) a context open to change; (2) reduced oversight in decisionmaking processes; (3) organizations that have credibility and a halo effect due to their celebrity status; and (4) an uncoordinated sector with weak intermediary organizations. The analysis applies process tracing to account for the success of one celebrity‐founded and celebrity‐led organization, WE Charity, in shaping the design and being awarded sole‐source implementation of the CAD $543 million Canada Student Service Grant (CSSG) program during COVID‐19. The CSSG, which proposed to pay up to 100,000 students to “volunteer” in nonprofits over the course of a summer, quickly failed and became a public ethical scandal.
Article
Full-text available
Black young adults use social media frequently and they are especially active on Twitter. Black young adults are exposed to both racially-affirming positive content and also racially-discriminatory negative content on Twitter. Correlational investigations have suggested that exposure to such content has psychological consequences, yet our understanding of acute responses to race-related Twitter messages remains hampered by the lack of a social media stimuli set that can be employed in an experimental setting. Therefore, we developed and tested an initial stimuli set of Tweets with different valences (i.e., positive, negative) and content (i.e., race-related, non-race-related) that can be used for testing experimental hypotheses about physiological, emotional, and behavioral changes associated with exposure to Twitter content in subsequent research. In two studies with Black college-attending young adult Twitter users (N = 43; Mage = 19.26) and a national sample of Black young adult Twitter users (N = 290, Mage = 25.85), we developed and tested the internal consistency, construct, convergent, ecological, and criterion-related validity of the stimuli set. The final Tweet Stimuli Set for Content about Black People (TSS-CBP) includes 16 stimuli grouped into four conditions (positive non-race-related, positive race-related, negative non-race-related, and negative race-related).
Article
Full-text available
60 male undergraduates read an essay supposedly written by a female college freshman. They then evaluated the quality of the essay and the ability of its writer on several dimensions. By means of a photo attached to the essay, 20 Ss were led to believe that the writer was physically attractive and 20 that she was unattractive. The remaining Ss read the essay without any information about the writer's appearance. 30 Ss read a version of the essay that was well written while the other Ss read a version that was poorly written. Significant main effects for essay quality and writer attractiveness were predicted and obtained. Ss who read the good essay evaluated the writer and her work more favorably that Ss who read the poor essay. Ss also evaluated the writer and her work most favorably when she was attractive, least when she was unattractive, and intermediately when her appearance was unknown. The impact of the writer's attractiveness on the evaluation of her and her work was most pronounced when the "objective" quality of her work was relatively poor. (15 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Reviews evidence which suggests that there may be little or no direct introspective access to higher order cognitive processes. Ss are sometimes (a) unaware of the existence of a stimulus that importantly influenced a response, (b) unaware of the existence of the response, and (c) unaware that the stimulus has affected the response. It is proposed that when people attempt to report on their cognitive processes, that is, on the processes mediating the effects of a stimulus on a response, they do not do so on the basis of any true introspection. Instead, their reports are based on a priori, implicit causal theories, or judgments about the extent to which a particular stimulus is a plausible cause of a given response. This suggests that though people may not be able to observe directly their cognitive processes, they will sometimes be able to report accurately about them. Accurate reports will occur when influential stimuli are salient and are plausible causes of the responses they produce, and will not occur when stimuli are not salient or are not plausible causes. (86 ref)
Article
Evidence is reviewed which suggests that there may be little or no direct introspective access to higher order cognitive processes. Subjects are sometimes (a) unaware of the existence of a stimulus that importantly influenced a response, (b) unaware of the existence of the response, and (c) unaware that the stimulus has affected the response. It is proposed that when people attempt to report on their cognitive processes, that is, on the processes mediating the effects of a stimulus on a response, they do not do so on the basis of any true introspection. Instead, their reports are based on a priori, implicit causal theories, or judgments about the extent to which a particular stimulus is a plausible cause of a given response. This suggests that though people may not be able to observe directly their cognitive processes, they will sometimes be able to report accurately about them. Accurate reports will occur when influential stimuli are salient and are plausible causes of the responses they produce, and will not occur when stimuli are not salient or are not plausible causes.
Article
Rosenthal and Jacobson found that a teacher's expectations about a child's behavior strongly influence his actual behavior. Generally, teachers form their first impressions of children, and thus develop their expectations for them, from two sources of information--the children's school record and their physical appearance. In this experiment, teachers were given objective information, presumably about a child's scholastic and social potential, accompanied by a photograph of an attractive or an unattractive boy or girl. It was found that the child's attractiveness was significantly associated with the teacher's expectations about how intelligent the child was, how interested in education his parents were, how far he was likely to progress in school, and how popular he would be with his peers.
Article
Examined whether physically attractive stimulus persons, both male and female, are (a) assumed to possess more socially desirable personality traits than physically unattractive stimulus persons, and (b) expected to lead better lives (e.g., be more competent husbands and wives and more successful occupationally) than unattractive stimulus persons. Sex of Subject * Sex of Stimulus Person interactions along these dimensions also were investigated. Results with 30 male and 30 female undergraduates indicate a "what is beautiful is good" stereotype along the physical attractiveness dimension with no Sex of Judge * Sex of Stimulus interaction. Implications of such a stereotype on self-concept development and the course of social interaction are discussed.
Article
Gave 360 male and 360 female undergraduate judges photographs, previously scaled as high, moderate, or low in physical attractiveness, and asked Ss to record their impressions of the stimulus persons on an adjective checklist. Results showed high attractiveness to be associated with positive traits, the reverse holding for low attractiveness. Data are consistent with the hypothesis that, in a 1st-impression situation, a person's level of attractiveness may evoke in a perceiver a consistent set of expectancies by a process of trait inference. This kind of process accords well with previous research relating physical attractiveness to interpersonal processes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Preliminary evidence indicates that effects of a physical attractiveness stereotype may be present at an early childhood developmental level. Several of the mediating processes that may be responsible for these effects presuppose that adults display differential treatment toward attractive and unattractive children in circumstances in which their behavior is identical. The present study used a situation integral to the socialization process, that in which the child has committed a transgression and the socializing adult must evaluate the child's behavior. 243 female undergraduates rated 7-yr-olds reading descriptions of the act and viewing a photograph of the child involved. Within a 2 * 2 * 2 * 2 design (Attractiveness of Child*Severity of Transgression*Sex of Child*Type of Transgression), support was found for the hypotheses that (a) the severe transgression of an attractive child is less likely to be seen as reflecting an enduring disposition toward antisocial behavior than that of an unattractive child and (b) the transgression itself tends to be evaluated less negatively when commited by an attractive child. No differences in intensity of advocated punishment were found. These and additional findings are discussed.
Information integration theory: A brief survey
  • N H Anderson
Anderson, N. H. Information integration theory: A brief survey. In D H. Krantz, R. C. Atkinson, R. D. Luce, & P Suppes (Eds.), Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology (Vol. 2).