ArticlePublisher preview available

Metacognition and Problem Solving: A Process-Oriented Approach

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Four studies were conducted to demonstrate that the positive effects of verbalization on solution transfer found in previous studies were not due to verbalization per se but to the metacognitive processing involved in the effort required to produce explanation for solution behaviors. In Experiments 1, 2, and 3, a distinction was made between process-oriented, problem-oriented, and simple "think aloud" verbalizations. The process-oriented (metacognitive) solvers performed significantly better than nonprocess control groups on both training and transfer tasks. Experiment 4 further demonstrated this effect by showing that process-oriented participants consistently form more sophisticated problem representations and develop more complex strategies. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1995,
Vol. 21, No.
1,205-223
Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
Metacognition and Problem Solving: A Process-Oriented Approach
Bernadette Berardi-Coletta
University of Illinois at Chicago
Roger L. Dominowski
University of Illinois at Chicago
Linda S. Buyer
Governor's State University
Elizabeth R. Rellinger
University of Notre Dame
Four studies were conducted to demonstrate that the positive effects of verbalization on solution
transfer found in previous studies were not due to verbalization per se but to the metacognitive
processing involved in the effort required to produce explanations for solution behaviors. In
Experiments 1,2, and
3,
a distinction was made between process-oriented, problem-oriented, and
simple "think aloud" verbalizations. The process-oriented (metacognitive) solvers performed
significantly better than nonprocess control groups
on
both training and transfer
tasks.
Experiment
4 further demonstrated this effect by showing that process-oriented participants consistently form
more sophisticated problem representations and develop more complex strategies.
We observe, anecdotally, in our ordinary encounters with
colleagues, peers, and acquaintances that individuals engaged
in a difficult or complex problem solving task often talk aloud
while trying to find the solution. Many of us have the sense
that, somehow, it
is
helpful to talk through a problem and that,
somehow, this aids the process of finding the solution. Al-
though we are not sure specifically why it helps or what the
mechanism is that provides
us
with such assistance, the urge to
think out loud is almost irresistible.
So,
what happens when
we
engage in such an act? What is it
that is so helpful? Much of the problem-solving research that
includes verbalization as part of the paradigm has confirmed
that, indeed, some kind of thinking aloud is beneficial both
during practice/learning trials and on transfer of learning to
other tasks (Ahlum-Heath & DiVesta, 1986; Berry, 1983;
Berry & Broadbent, 1984,1987; Chi, Bassok, Lewis, Reimann,
& Glaser,
1989;
Gagne & Smith, 1962; Stinessen,
1985;
Wilder
& Harvey,
1971).
Generally, verbalization instructions in these
problem-solving studies require stating reasons for solution
moves, choices, trials, and so forth. An exception was the Chi
et al. (1989) study that simply asked participants to "think
aloud." Chi et al. observed that when asked to simply think
aloud without further instructions, good problem solvers were
more likely to make statements defined as self-explanations
and self-monitoring than were poor problem
solvers.
Thus, the
relationship between verbalization or, more specifically, stat-
ing reasons and problem-solving performance seems to be a
robust, positive one.
Explanations of the effect are varied. Gagne and Smith
(1962) were the first to posit an explanation. They explained
the beneficial effects of verbalizing on solutions to the Tower-
of-Hanoi problem by stating that requiring students to verbal-
Bemadette Berardi-Coletta and Roger L. Dominowski, Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago; Linda S. Buyer,
Department of Psychology, Governor's State University; Elizabeth R.
Rellinger, Department of
Psychology,
University of Notre Dame.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Bernadette Berardi-Coletta, 180 North Michigan Avenue, #1120,
Chicago, Illinois
60601.
ize makes them "stop and think." Similarly, Stinessen (1985)
proposes that verbalization "slows one down," and Ahlum-
Heath and Divesta (1986) suggest that a control mechanism is
induced. Berry (1983) and Berry and Broadbent (1984) further
specify that verbalization keeps attention focused on salient
features of the problem and forces concentration on critical
task components. Berry and Broadbent (1987) also propose
that the timing of explanations (i.e., participant vs. experi-
menter control of presentation of explanations) is important
because "temporary
cognitive
units"
must be activated at the time
they are needed, if lasting domain knowledge is to be enhanced.
Additionally, Wilder and Harvey (1971) conclude that perfor-
mance is
linked
to
'Verbal
mediation"
whether
overt
or covert.
These explanations all suggest that verbalization focuses
attention on and enhances problem-domain knowledge. That
is,
verbalization slows one down or makes one stop and think
more carefully about salient problem features and critical task
components. This is consistent with Stein, Way, Benningfield,
and Hedgecough (1986) who concluded that unless critical
problem features are made explicitly salient, transfer will not
occur between similar but contextually different problems.
Gick and Holyoak
(1980)
and Catrambone and Holyoak (1989)
reached similar
conclusions.
They all found that unless partici-
pants were told that two problems were analogous to one
another, participants did not make the connection and were
unable to use information from one problem to solve the other.
Clearly, then, it is necessary, at some level, for critical
features of a problem to be attended to, remembered, and
applied appropriately. Verbalization seems to enhance this
component of
the
problem-solving
process.
But the question
of
how
verbalization accomplishes this
is
not fully answered. One
important variable that has not been closely examined is the
type of verbalization required and/or produced. Participants
in all but one of the above-mentioned studies were required to
state a reason or tell why they made a particular solution
attempt, trial, or
move.
Dominowski (1990) has proposed that
the demand that one explain oneself invokes executive pro-
cesses such as monitoring, planning, and attention to problem
features and that this yields "more efficient performance."
205
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Meta-cognition enhances cognitive task performance, a key trait in high-performing individuals across learning, problemsolving, and engineering design [17][18][19][20]. Researchers use metacognition training to improve cognitive performance such as learning ability [21,22], math problem-solving [23][24][25][26] and engineering design [20,27]. ...
... All evaluations were graded on a numerical scale from 0 to 20, with 10 as the minimum passing grade. Additionally, grades were assigned according to the equivalent ECTS grading scale (from E to A): E-Sufficient (10-11), D-Satisfactory (12-13), C-Good (14-15), B-Very good (16)(17), A-Excellent (18)(19)(20). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This research-to-practice full paper introduces a novel graduate course, Human Errors in Software Development (HESD) based on a metacognition framework. Software defects pose a significant threat to the reliability and safety of computer systems, incurring trillions of dollars in costs globally. Addressing and rectifying defects in programs present a formidable challenge for Computer Science (CS) students. Given the critical role of cognition in software development, there is a pressing need for a course designed to train students to address human errors in various cognitive activities of software development. To our knowledge, there is currently no semester-long nor quarter-long university course offering comprehensive training to students on handling human errors in software development. HESD equips students with a profound understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underlying human errors in software development. It aims to enhance students' awareness and cognitive abilities to proactively prevent human errors in software development and, consequently, reduce defects in programs they develop. HESD comprises three stages: Stage I provides students with explicit knowledge of human errors in software development; Stage II fosters students' awareness and regulation abilities to effectively address human errors during software development; Stage III encourages students to apply acquired knowledge and skills in diverse contexts. The newly designed course was delivered to master's students at a large public university over a 14-week semester. Nine students in Software Engineering were enrolled in the course. Comprehensive surveys were used to evaluate the course's attractiveness and usefulness to students. The average satisfaction score was 4.2 (Min = 3, Max = 5, SD = 0.8) in a five-point Likert scale (where 1 means "very dissatisfied" and 5 means "very satisfied"), signifying that the students were quite satisfied with the course. A survey designed for assessing human error knowledge, awareness and regulation ability was filled out by the students before the course and at the end of the course. Results showed that the course has significantly enhanced students' Human Error Knowledge in software development by 153%, improved Human Error Awareness in software development by 62%, and elevated students' Human Error Regulation by 63%. The students found the course to be highly interesting, practical, and valuable.
... Briefly, metacognition can be described as cognition about cognition (Flavell, 1976). Studies have shown that this metacognitive awareness leads to better decision-making (Argyris, 1991;Berardi-Coletta, Buyer, Dominowski, & Rellinger, 1995;Swanson, 1990). Metacognition is thus considered a key concept in the problem-solving process (Celiker, 2015;Hartman, 1998). ...
... Problem solving and learning have been shown to improve in students who are taught to monitor and reflect on their understanding of math problems (Z. Mevarech & Fridkin, 2006; Z. R. Mevarech & Kramarski, 2003;Schurter, 2002), and in people who are asked evaluative questions about the solving process (Berardi-Coletta et al., 1995;Delclos & Harrington, 1991;Lan et al., 1998). Also, in simple perceptual decision tasks, reporting decision confidence has been shown to improve response accuracy (Aguilar-Lleyda et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Self-monitoring seems to be crucial for regulatory behavior, but it is not clear how it influences performance in simple cognitive tasks. Some studies suggest that increased monitoring improves metacognitive regulation and enhances performance, while others suggest it impairs learning, problem solving, or perceptual processes. We investigated whether the requirement to report confidence in perceptual decisions affects metacognitive regulation and response accuracy. Participants performed a visual discrimination task in which they provided two responses: initial and final. Depending on the condition, participants reported their confidence (either together with or following the initial decision), performed an additional task, or were asked to observe a blank screen between two responses. We expected that reporting decision confidence would induce efficient regulatory activity, which would benefit final accuracy. In three experiments, we did not find evidence that rating confidence improves regulatory processing or performance in perceptual tasks. Rather, when confidence ratings were retrospective, the final response improvement was smaller compared to the condition with no additional task, and changes of mind were less frequent and less corrective. Confidence ratings given jointly with the initial response generally decreased accuracy. The results suggest that deliberate monitoring might put additional strain on cognitive resources and impair lower-order task processing.
... According to Afflerbach et al. (2017), the metacognitive regulation of reading is expressed as reading strategies, deliberate, goal-directed attempts to control and modify the reader's effort to decode text, understand words, and construct text meanings. Prior studies indicate that metacognitive regulation, the abilities learners employ to oversee their cognitions, correlates positively with proficient problem-solving (Berardi-Coletta et al., 1995), transfer (Lin & Lehman, 1999), and self-regulated learning (Zepeda et al., 2015). However, metacognitive regulation has a different number of constructs and different levels of description. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the effects of metacognitive awareness on the reading comprehension skills of Grade 7 students at Odiongan National High School, Odiongan, Romblon, addressing a crucial gap in existing literature. With reading comprehension skills being fundamental to academic success, understanding the role of metacognitive awareness becomes imperative. Grounded in Flavell's Metacognition Theory, this research aims to investigate the level of metacognitive awareness and its effects on reading comprehension skills with a descriptive-causal approach. Methodologically, a quantitative approach was used, utilizing reading comprehension assessments alongside a modified Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) adapted from Schraw & Dennison (1994). Using a four-point Likert-type rating scale questionnaire, students rated their metacognitive awareness, divided into Metacognitive Knowledge (MK) and Metacognitive Regulation (MR). Metacognitive knowledge encompasses declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge, while Metacognitive Regulation involves planning, monitoring, and evaluating. Using stratified random sampling, 198 respondents were selected out of 401 Grade 7 students, preceded by a pilot study with 20 respondents to refine research instruments and assess the validity and reliability. Furthermore, data were collected, processed, and analyzed using a statistical tool called Multiple Linear Regression. The results show that metacognitive awareness is positively correlated with the reading comprehension skills of Grade 7 students. This suggests the pivotal role of metacognitive awareness in enhancing reading comprehension skills.
... Several studies have confirmed the crucial role of metacognition in problem-solving skills and general intelligence [7]. For instance, Berardi-Coletta et al. [8] found that people with higher metacognition exhibited significantly better problem-solving strategies in training and task transfer compared to those with lower abilities. Rickey and Stacey [9] also suggested that the enhancement of metacognition improved problem-solving abilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Metacognition is essential in supporting students’ academic success, problem-solving abilities, and overall achievement. Therefore, this study aimed to provide a detailed description of metacognitive activities of students when engaging in contextual mathematical problem-solving. The participants comprised 11th-grade students from SMA Negeri 3 Jayapura, Papua Province, Indonesia. Descriptive and qualitative methods were adopted, and the data were collected using various test instruments and interview guidelines. Subsequently, the data were analyzed through the phases of reduction, presentation, as well as conclusions, and triangulation methods were used to ensure robustness and reliability. The results showed that metacognitive awareness occurred when students considered previous knowledge and experiences in solving contextual mathematical problem. Metacognitive evaluation activities occurred when relevant methods and steps were assessed, while metacognitive regulation manifested when contemplating the actions required to solve problem. This showed that students used various metacognitive activities, namely awareness, evaluation, and regulation, to address contextual mathematical problem.
... They argue that the manager's metacognitive lens helps PIP systems evolve into a ''learning-oriented problem-solving framework'' [3] (p. 135). Indeed, both metacognitive processes and PIP systems employ similar functional mechanisms such as (i) process focus, (ii) learning-by-doing approach, and (iii) continuous improvement through knowledge creation [3], [20], [21], [22]. Although the existing PIP literature has mainly focused on how firms acquire knowledge through their internal process-improvement techniques, there is still an insufficient understanding of how organizations create knowledge from external sources, such as suppliers and buyers. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores how organizations can capture and create knowledge in supply chain relationships. In particular, this study investigated the impact of metacognitive capabilities on knowledge creation in supply chain networks. The research model was empirically verified using sample data from 151 U.S. corporations. Empirical evidence showed that organizations with strong metacognitive capabilities tend to more effectively externalize tacit knowledge gained from supply chain activities into explicit knowledge outcomes such as product and process innovations. These results suggest that organizations can better handle the complex challenges of product and process innovation when leveraging metacognitive capabilities. The practical and theoretical implications of the findings and future research agendas are discussed in depth.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this research is to compare the effectiveness of metacognitive therapy, solution-focused therapy and endurance exercises on the problem-solving styles of female students with social anxiety disorder. The design of this research is a quasi-experimental type of pre-test-post-test with a unequal control group and a one-month follow-up. The statistical population was female students of the second level of high school in the three educational districts of Kermanshah in the academic year 2023-2024. From this population, 60 students with social anxiety were selected by screening and according to the criteria for entering the study. They were randomly assigned in 4 groups of 15 people (three experimental and one control). The research tools included social anxiety questionnaires (Kanor, 2000) and problem-solving styles (Dzurila et al., 2002). Data were analyzed with SPSS23 software and Analysis of variance test and Benferoni's post hoc test. The findings showed that metacognitive therapy, solution-focused therapy, and endurance exercises increased effective problem-solving styles and decreased ineffective problem-solving styles in people with social anxiety (P≤0.05) and among the three treatments, Metacognition therapy in efficient problem-solving styles and metacognition and solution-oriented treatment in ineffective problem-solving styles were more effective than endurance exercises (P≤0.05). Based on these findings, it can be said that all three treatment models can be used in the Iranian clinical sample. Article Info:
Article
Full-text available
Five experiments investigated transfer from multiple analogs to a superficially dissimilar target problem. When subjects explicitly compared the analogs and then immediately attempted to solve the target problem in the context of a single experiment, transfer was obtained with significant frequency even without a hint that the analogs and target were related. Prehint transfer was sharply reduced or eliminated when the source analogs and the target were presented in different contexts, even when the transfer test was immediate. However, prehint transfer was enhanced, even after a context shift and a week-long delay between reading the source analogs and solving the problem, when the following conditions were met: The target problem was reworded slightly to emphasize a structural feature that it shared with the analogs; three rather than two source analogs were provided; and detailed, schema-oriented questions were used to help subjects focus on the problem-relevant aspects of the stories. Although spontaneous transfer between small numbers of dissimilar analogs is difficult to obtain, it can be achieved by manipulations that foster abstraction of a problem schema from the training examples. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In three experiments, we investigated the conditions under which relevant knowledge is spontaneously transferred to problem-solving tasks. Subjects were presented with key concepts that could help them solve problems presented at a later time in the experiment. The key concepts were embedded in statements that had surface structures similar or dissimilar to those of the problems and that emphasized relevant or irrelevant properties of the key concepts for the problemsolving task (contextual relevance). The results indicated that the spontaneous transfer of clue information to subsequent problem-solving tasks is affected by the contextual relevance and the surface-structure similarity of the clue statements to the problems. The findings also suggested that the surface-structure similarity and the contextual relevance of clue statements differentially affect the accessibility and appropriate application of key concepts in problem-solving tasks. The implications of these results for understanding the transfer of knowledge in problem-solving tasks are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The effect of controlled verbalization on learning to solve complex problems was investigated. Fifty participants individually solved the six-disk Tower of Hanoi problem as a criterion task, following one of the five treatments represented in a 2 × 2 factorial design with an appended control group. One factor was the presence or absence of a practice series which required participants to provide verbal rationales for their moves. The other was the presence or absence of verbalization on the six-disk criterion task. The control participants performed the practice tasks and the criterion task without verbalization. Although practice tended to be more effective than no practice for improving performance, its strongest effect occurred when it was coupled with controlled verbalization. Controlled verbalization during the criterion task facilitated performance, but only for subjects who received no prior practice. It was concluded that verbalization was most helpful during the initial flexible stages of learning to solve problems before the skill had become organized. The discussion indicated that performance is facilitated by the quality and timing of the use of verbalizations rather than by the mere activity of verbalizing.
Article
Subjects first attempted to complete familiar phrases with either minimal or strong cues to solutions. If solution generation failed, answers were provided. One-week retention was better following successful generation than either unsuccessful generation or mere reading of answers. Retention was also better if success had been achieved with minimal cues. These findings are interpreted as suggesting that representation in semantic memory as a gestalt or functional unit determines the emergence of the generation effect and that cognitive effort is a factor in the strength of the generation effect.
Article
Three experiments examine the effects of two different forms of explanation on assisted performance on a complex search task. The task involves determining which of a set of factories is responsible for polluting a river by testing the river for the presence or absence of various pollutants. All subjects receive computer suggestions as to which pollutants they should test for. In addition, some subjects receive an explanation of the principle according to which the advice program works. Two forms of explanation are compared. Experiment 1 shows that subjects who are allowed to ask “why” each computer recommendation is made perform significantly better than those who are provided with a block text of explanation at the start of each trial. Experiment 2 shows that the latter type of explanation is not completely ineffective, however. Subjects who are required to verbalize following the single explanation perform significantly better than do subjects who are required to verbalize but who have not received any form of explanation. Finally, Experiment 3 shows that subjects who receive the multiple “why” explanations or the explanation/verbalization combination maintain a superior performance level on subsequent unaided trials.
Article
Three experiments explore the relationship between performance on a cognitive task and the explicit or reportable knowledge associated with that performance (assessed here by written post-task questionnaire). They examine how this relationship is affected by task experience, verbal instruction and concurrent verbalization. It is shown that practice significantly improves ability to control semi-complex computer-implemented systems but has no effect on the ability to answer related questions. In contrast, verbal instruction significantly improves ability to answer questions but has no effect on control performance. Verbal instruction combined with concurrent verbalization does lead to a significant improvement in control scores. Verbalization alone, however, has no effect on task performance or question answering.
Article
This study explored the effects of overt and covert verbalization instructions on problem solving in high school subjects. A series of three-circle problems were administered to groups instructed to either (1) say a reason for each move they made (overt verbalization), (2) think of a reason for each move as if they were going to say it (covert verbalization), or (3) work the problems silently (control). Consistent with previous findings, subjects instructed to overtly verbalize were superior to control subjects on a transfer task requiring no overt verbalization. No significant differences, however, were observed between overt and covert verbalizers. This finding was interpreted to indicate that, in adults, covert verbalization can be as effective in mediating problem solving behavior as talking aloud. It was suggested that this may not hold true for children, however. (For related document, see PS 005 424.) (Author)
Article
The present paper analyzes the self-generated explanations (from talk-aloud protocols) that “Good” and “Poor” students produce while studying worked-out examples of mechanics problems, and their subsequent reliance on examples during problem solving. We find that “Good” students learn with understanding: They generate many explanations which refine and expand the conditions for the action parts of the example solutions, and relate these actions to principles in the text. These self-explanations are guided by accurate monitoring of their own understanding and misunderstanding. Such learning results in example-independent knowledge and in a better understanding of the principles presented in the text. “Poor” students do not generate sufficient self-explanations, monitor their learning inaccurately, and subsequently rely heavily on examples. We then discuss the role of self-explanations in facilitating problem solving, as well as the adequacy of current AI models of explanation-based learning to account for these psychological findings.
Article
The experiments examine the influence of metacognitive experience on the transfer of logical processes in a problem solving setting. Subjects were presented with two versions of Wason's (1966) selection task. Although they were able to perform successfully on the concrete tasks (following a minimal explanation of the correct solution on an initial trial), the majority were not able to transfer a successful method to the abstract tasks. Verbalization during, or following, the concrete tasks produced substantial transfer effects however. It is suggested that verbalization may lead to an increased awareness of past behaviour, particularly of those aspects necessary for successful solution.
Article
The use of an analogy from a semantically distant domain to guide the problemsolving process was investigated. The representation of analogy in memory and processes involved in the use of analogies were discussed theoretically and explored in five experiments. In Experiment I oral protocols were used to examine the processes involved in solving a problem by analogy. In all experiments subjects who first read a story about a military problem and its solution tended to generate analogous solutions to a medical problem (Duncker's "radiation problem"), provided they were given a hint to use the story to help solve the problem. Transfer frequency was reduced when the problem presented in the military story was substantially disanalogous to the radiation problem, even though the solution illustrated in the story corresponded to an effective radiation solution (Experiment II). Subjects in Experiment III tended to generate analogous solutions to the radiation problem after providing their own solutions to the military problem. Subjects were able to retrieve the story from memory and use it to generate an analogous solution, even when the critical story had been memorized in the context of two distractor stories (Experiment IV). However, when no hint to consider the story was given, frequency of analogous solutions decreased markedly. This decrease in transfer occurred when the story analogy was presented in a recall task along with distractor stories (Experiment IV), when it was presented alone, and when it was presented in between two attempts to solve the problem (Experiment V). Component processes and strategic variations in analogical problem solving were discussed. Issues related to noticing analogies and accessing them in memory were also examined, as was the relationship of analogical reasoning to other cognitive tasks. Peer Reviewed http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/23210/1/0000139.pdf