Content uploaded by Mathias Clasen
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Mathias Clasen on Jun 27, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
Monsters Evolve: A Biocultural Approach to Horror Stories
Mathias Clasen
Aarhus University
Horror fiction is a thriving industry. Many consumers pay hard-earned money to be scared witless by
films, books, and computer games. The well-told horror story can affect even the most obstinate skeptic.
How and why does horror fiction work? Why are people so fascinated with monsters? Why do horror
stories generally travel well across cultural borders, if all they do is encode salient culturally contingent
anxieties, as some horror scholars have claimed? I argue that an evolutionary perspective is useful in
explaining the appeal of horror, but also that this perspective cannot stand alone. An exhaustive,
vertically integrated theory of horror fiction incorporates the cultural dimension. I make the case for a
biocultural approach, one that recognizes evolutionary underpinnings and cultural variation.
Keywords: horror fiction, monsters, fear, biocultural analysis, evolutionary psychology
Our fear of monsters in the night probably has its origins far back in
the evolution of our primate ancestors, whose tribes were pruned by
horrors whose shadows continue to elicit our monkey screams in dark
theaters. (Shepard, 1996, p. 29)
The popularity of horror fiction is puzzling. Why would anyone
willingly expose himself or herself to such aversive, outlandish
nonsense; what is the appeal of supernatural monsters and moan-
ing ghosts? I argue that a biocultural approach, one that integrates
research into evolved psychology with attention to sociohistorical
circumstance, is uniquely equipped to answer these questions. By
taking into account evolved cognitive architecture, a biocultural
approach offsets the tendency toward cultural determinism inher-
ent in the widespread socioconstructivist approach to horror (e.g.,
Kendrick, 1992; Skal, 2001). Human cognition has been fine-tuned
by natural selection to deal with dangers in the environment, and
horror fiction depends crucially on this mental machinery.
Horror fiction is a multimillion-dollar industry. For the past
decade and a half, horror films have enjoyed a market share of
about 5% (not counting the closely related, sometimes overlapping
genres of thriller and suspense films; The Numbers—Top-
Grossing Genres, 2011). The horror film Paranormal Activity
(2007), produced for a measly $15,000, has grossed close to
$200,000,000 worldwide (Paranormal Activity, 2011), despite its
thin storyline and dubious premise of an angry demon from Hell.
In literature, Stephen King and his fellow horror mongers continue
to thrill audiences around the planet with stories of the grotesque
and macabre. Video games manifestly designed to maximally
spook players, such as Silent Hill, are a thriving enterprise.
The fact of the matter is that creepy stories of ghosts, monsters,
crazed killers, and otherworldly hauntings continue to draw flocks
of thrill-seekers to the comfortable half-darkness of cinemas and
the shelves labeled “horror” in bookstores and libraries worldwide.
The well-told horror story can send a shudder down the spine of
even the most obstinate rationalist. Why is it that educated, en-
lightened persons are susceptible to being frightened by fictional
tales about supernatural monsters, and why are many people at-
tracted to such stories? Why does commercial horror fiction travel
so well across cultural borders? Apparently, even the glare of
scientific progress and general enlightenment are insufficient to
kill off the supernatural menaces that stalk us in the stories we
consume.
Something about vampires, zombies, ghosts, werewolves, and
chainsaw-wielding maniacs makes them uniquely interesting to
many people. But the ubiquity of such monsters in fiction certainly
cannot be a reflection of reality—let alone the reality of 21st
century citizens in industrial countries. The likelihood of being
assaulted by a homicidal maniac in a hockey mask is only mar-
ginally greater than the likelihood of being assaulted by a thirsty
vampire or a hungry zombie. I argue that horror stories do not
reflect empirical reality but rather the psychology of our species.
The sustained generation and consumption of horror fiction over
space and time suggest that a species-typical cognitive architecture
for dealing with danger is brought into play by such stories. In this
article, I provide a brief sketch of cognitive architecture designed
for danger management and analyze the correspondence between
this mental machinery and the structure of horror fiction and horror
monsters.
Natural-Born Scaredy-Pants
Before we turn to the current profusion of monsters in popular
entertainment, we need to take a look at our past. The Homo line
diverged from that of our closest relatives, the chimpanzees and
bonobos, about 5 to 7 million years ago, and a long process of
natural selection further whittled away at our species and gave rise
I am immensely indebted to Joe Carroll for his eagle-eyed editorial
assistance and unstinting support. I also would like to warmly thank Daniel
Sznycer for giving feedback on a draft of this article; my colleagues at
Interdisciplinary Evolutionary Studies, Aarhus University, for valuable
discussions and support; and Leda Cosmides, John Tooby, and the other
good people at the Center for Evolutionary Psychology, University of
California, Santa Barbara, for providing a stimulating writing environment.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mathias
Clasen, Department of English, Aarhus University, DK-8000 Aarhus C,
Denmark. E-mail: engmc@hum.au.dk
Review of General Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association
2012, Vol. 16, No. 2, 222–229 1089-2680/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027918
222
to anatomically modern humans some 200,000 years ago (Wade,
2007). It means that we all have human nature in common, that is,
a set of genetically transmitted traits and dispositions common to
all members of the species Homo sapiens sapiens. All organisms
evolve in an adaptive relationship with their environments. Organ-
isms that are better suited to their physical, ecological, or social
environments leave more offspring, increasing their genetic rep-
resentation in future generations. All living humans are thus de-
scended from a very long line of well-adapted organisms, none of
whom failed at the exacting tasks of surviving and reproducing.
Coping with dangers is a major challenge in the evolutionary
process. Today, as in our prehistorical evolutionary past, human
beings are born into the world with a suite of adaptations designed
for managing a wide array of dangers.
Imagine this. You are alone, at night, walking in the woods.
From somewhere, suddenly, come a sound, a rustling, and some-
thing that sounds like growling. These auditory cues, perhaps
implying lethal danger from a predator, generate in you a precau-
tionary neurophysiological reaction. Attention is sharply focused
on the potential threat, all thoughts of the upcoming Sex and the
City marathon or soccer match forgotten. Your pulse climbs, you
start sweating, your mouth dries out, energy is directed to the big
muscles and away from the digestive system: These various re-
sponses are jointly mobilized by the emotion of fear (Tooby &
Cosmides, 2000). Your body prepares for confrontation or flight,
pending further evidence. It could be a false alarm, but given that
reacting to a false alarm is vastly less catastrophic than failing to
react to a lethal threat, responding with a heightened state of
emergency is a safe bargain and hence the baseline response
(Marks & Nesse, 1994).
Now imagine this. You are alone, at night, watching a Japanese
horror movie on cable. From somewhere, suddenly, comes a little
pale girl with long black hair. Similar physiological changes take
place in your body, even though there is no physical danger. But
we scare easily—jumping at shadows is simply the safer bargain—
and even virtual danger scenarios such as horror fiction capitalize
on this tendency.
Psychological Adaptations to Danger
Like all other animals, humans are equipped with evolved
machinery that enables them to deal with possible dangers to their
survival, and emotions are crucial parts of the machinery. In
humans, fear is mediated by highly conserved, ancient circuits in
the brain. In fact, humans and rats respond to sudden danger
in much the same way (LeDoux, 1996, pp. 130–131). Emotions
are functional systems that guide behavior, deeply embedded in
mammalian brains (and perhaps other classes of animals as well).
Fear and anxiety are adaptations to dangerous environments. They
help us stay clear of things that could be harmful to us.
Life in ancestral environments was dangerous. We know from
the archeological record that humans and our hominid ancestors
were frequently prey to other animals and also to fellow humans.
So, for example, we find skulls of hominins with puncture marks
that match perfectly the teeth of large feline predators (Hart &
Sussman, 2008). The vast majority of our species’ evolutionary
history was spent living in small bands of hunter–gatherer societ-
ies, and we know from anthropological studies of modern-day
hunter–gatherer societies that such existence is dangerous. Among
Ache´ foragers in Paraguay, 6% of all deaths are the result of
poisonous snakebites, making this the single most common lethal
accident. Among adult males, snakebites account for 14% of all
deaths, and jaguar attacks for another 8%. Death by the hands of
other humans is even higher than deaths due to animal predation:
External warfare accounts for 36% of deaths among adult males.
Violence generally is responsible for 55% of all Ache´ deaths (Hill
& Furtado, 1996, pp. 162–163).
We would expect this kind of precarious existence to have left
deep grooves in human nature. And indeed, experimental psychol-
ogists have provided a wealth of evidence for the kind of dedicated
threat-detection and handling system that this selective regime
would shape. Humans’ attention is preferentially engaged by evo-
lutionarily recurrent, fear-relevant stimuli. So, for example, test
subjects are faster at detecting an image of a snake among a mass
of flowers than they are at detecting a flower among a mass of
snakes (Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001), the hypothesis being that
the danger from poisonous snakebites exerts a selection pressure
for fast detection of serpents. Summing up previous research, Arne
Öhman (2000) wrote that
responses of fear and anxiety originate in an alarm system shaped by
evolution to protect creatures from impending danger. This system is
biased to discover threat, and it results in a sympathetically dominated
response as a support of potential flight or fight. (p. 587)
The fear response is controlled by the autonomic nervous system
and so is largely impervious to higher order cognitive control, and
the system is biased, that is, hyper-responsive and prone to erring
on the side of caution. Thus, Öhman and Mineka (2001) posit a
dedicated fear module that directs attention toward potential
threats, causes appropriate emotional states, and produces behav-
ioral outputs that are, on average and statistically speaking, adap-
tive (see also Boyer & Bergstrom, 2011; Boyer & Lie´nard, 2006;
Eilam, Izhar, & Mort, 2011; Lie´nard, 2011; Neuberg, Kenrick, &
Schaller, 2011; and Woody & Szechtman, 2011). Humans, like
other animals, are born with preprogrammed instructions for what
kind of dangers might exist in the world, and experience (first-
hand or vicarious) fills in the picture. Being born with an abstract
predator template—for example, a template specifying large ani-
mals with sharp teeth and forward-facing eyes—and an instruction
to pay attention to such predators gets the job done (H. C. Barrett,
2005). Evolved danger-management systems require calibration, a
useful design feature because different biotopes offer somewhat
different threats. In one environment, it may be useful to be on the
lookout for wolves; in another, crocodiles; in yet another, moun-
tain lions.
This substrate of genetically prepared learning gives a “nonran-
dom distribution of fears” (Marks & Nesse, 1994, p. 255). It is
vastly easier to acquire fear of spiders or large animals than to
acquire fear of saturated fats or cars, despite the fact that cardio-
vascular disease and automotive accidents are more likely to kill us
by several orders of magnitude. This distribution is reflected in
horror stories that feature fearsome monsters. Modern-day horror
stories, by flinging us into virtual universes that brim with lurking
dangers and aggressive predators, send us on a journey backward
in time, to the dark days in human phylogeny when the setting sun
signified grave danger and real monsters could very well be
gathering just outside the fragile circle of light cast by the bonfire.
223
MONSTERS EVOLVE: A BIOCULTURAL APPROACH
Horror Stories Trigger Evolved Danger-Management
Systems
All normally developed humans are equipped with cognitive
machinery dedicated to threat detection and handling, machinery
that has been assembled and fine-tuned incrementally by a long
process of natural selection. We have evidence to suggest that
horror fiction runs on the same machinery. For example, research-
ers studying emotion have found horror films very effective for
ecologically valid emotion elicitation in laboratory experiments on
fear. In particular, researchers have found clips from The Shining
(1980) and Silence of the Lambs (1991) useful as fear-eliciting
stimuli (Gross & Levenson, 1995; Rottenberg, Ray, & Gross,
2007). We can draw two conclusions from this research: Horror
movies cause predictable, reliable emotional states across subjects
(people do not interpret what they watch arbitrarily), and horror
films engender actual emotions of fear and anxiety, not simulacra
or perversions of these emotions.
Evidence is also forthcoming from an intriguing study of a
woman with neurological impairment (Feinstein, Adolphs, Dama-
sio, & Tranel, 2011a). This patient, famous in neurological liter-
ature as SM, suffers from focal bilateral amygdala lesions. She is,
in other words, fearless because of localized brain damage. In a
recent experiment, a team of researchers subjected the woman to a
variety of fear-inducing situations. They took her to a pet shop and
exposed her to snakes and tarantulas, they dragged her on a tour of
a haunted house, and they had her watch clips from a number of
well-known horror films. In the pet store, SM was “spontaneously
drawn to the snake terrariums.” She asked repeatedly if she could
touch or hold even large, dangerous snakes, and “also attempted to
touch a tarantula, but had to be stopped because of the high risk of
being bitten” (Feinstein et al., 2011a, pp. 34–35). When they
visited Waverly Hills Sanatorium, a commercially run “haunted
attraction” in Kentucky, SM showed no fear but reported “a high
level of excitement and enthusiasm” (p. 35):
The hidden monsters attempted to scare SM numerous times, but to no
avail. She reacted to the monsters by smiling, laughing, or trying to
talk to them. In contrast, their scare tactics typically elicited loud
screams of fright from the other members of the group. More than
showing a lack of fear, SM exhibited an unusual inclination to
approach and touch the monsters. Ironically, SM scared one of the
monsters when she poked it in the head because she was “curious” as
to what it would feel like. (Feinstein et al., 2011a, p. 35)
The team exposed SM to clips of varying length from a number
of horror films, including The Ring (2002), The Shining (1980),
The Blair Witch Project (1999), Halloween (1978), Silence of the
Lambs (1991), and Se7en (1995; Feinstein, Adolphs, Damasio, &
Tranel, 2011b, p. 9), all of which induced high levels of fear in
healthy control subjects. As the authors note,
SM exhibited no fear responses and reported experiencing little to no
fear across the entire battery of fear-inducing films. Nonetheless, she
found the fear films to be exciting and entertaining, and in one case,
she inquired about the name of the movie so she could rent it from the
video store later that day. (Feinstein et al., 2011a, p. 35)
It is striking that patient SM not only lacked fear but also
displayed high interest in fearful situations and objects. As the
research team argued, “fear-inducing stimuli are still capable of
eliciting changes in attention and arousal through structures other
than the amygdala” (Feinstein et al., 2011a, p. 37). Horror mon-
sters are not only terrifying, they are captivating.
Monsters Real and Imagined
“What scares me is what scares you. We’re all afraid of the same
things. That’s why horror is such a powerful genre,” according to
veteran horror filmmaker John Carpenter (as cited in McCarty &
McLaughlin, 2006). His observation has the ring of common
sense, and is in fact borne out by research on the topic. People do
tend to fear similar things simply because we are similarly con-
structed. A long process of evolution gave us minds that are on the
lookout for certain kinds of dangers in the environment, dangers
that are sometimes hopelessly atavistic. The same process gave us
endlessly creative imaginations, and these two capacities together
give rise to a multitude of fictional monsters.
The primary function of a fictional monster is to be salient. It
can fulfill that function by being dangerous because humans are
hard-wired to pay attention to dangerous agents, but the monster
becomes even more interesting by being unnatural. Cognitive
research on religion has produced evidence that counterintuitive
agents, particularly minimally counterintuitive agents such as
ghosts and bleeding statues, are more salient, easier to remember,
and more likely to be faithfully transmitted than ordinary or
completely bizarre agents (Boyer, 2001; J. L. Barrett, 2004;
Norenzayan, Atran, Faulkner, & Schaller, 2006; see Grodal, 2009,
on the application of this finding to supernatural film).
Indeed, monsters appear in stories, myths, and artwork all over
the world. As David Gilmore (2003) has documented through
extensive anthropological research, “people everywhere and at all
times have been haunted by ogres, cannibal giants, metamorphs,
werewolves, vampires, and so on” (p. ix). Likewise, according to
Stephen Asma (2009), the “monster archetype seems to appear in
every culture’s artwork” (p. 282). Psychology goes a long way
toward explaining our fascination with made-up monsters and the
underlying structure of monsters as unnatural dangerous agents.
Horror monsters are usually supercharged predators with counter-
intuitive traits, well designed to capture and hold our attention.
They are tailored to have a specific effect on the human mind, and
the reason they succeed is that there are regularities in human
cognitive architecture that make sense only in the light of our
evolutionary history. Nonetheless, a cultural component is neces-
sary to understand particular monsters and the historical develop-
ment of the horror genre.
Cultural Ecologies of Monsters
Like all other imaginative constructs, monsters come into being
in a complex relation between psychological machinery, environ-
mental conditions, and cultural narratives: They grow in the soil of
adapted minds and are fertilized by cultural ecology. Four exam-
ples will help clarify this principle: werewolves, zombies, vam-
pires, and ghosts.
The Werewolf
Although the global entertainment industry has transmitted
werewolves to the farthest corners of the planet, the creature began
224 CLASEN
life as a tweaked version of a local predatory animal, the wolf.
Asma (2009) noted, “in the imaginative construction of a fright-
ening beast, a folk culture will frequently embellish the local
predators rather than compose a completely novel monster” (p.
126). Thus, we find different shape-shifters in different ecologies:
a were-tiger in India and other Asian regions, a were-bear in North
America, a were-leopard in Africa, a were-boar in Greece and
Turkey, a were-crocodile in Indonesia and Africa, and so on (Rose,
2000, pp. 389–390).
Different as these shape-shifters are on the surface, they are
products of the same universal psychological processes producing
output with local environmental and cultural inputs. Psychological
adaptations that make humans interested in dangerous organisms,
as well as mechanisms that make counterintuitive agents particu-
larly salient to humans, coupled with a knowledge or impression of
local predators and a memory bank of monster stories, work
together to elaborate on the culturally transmitted idea of a shape-
shifter and to make the were-animal a successful cultural figure.
The various ethnic were-animals are variations on a basic theme,
constrained by human cognition, and the very idea of a were-
animal is a spectacular embodiment of the commonsensical obser-
vation that human nature is fraught with conflicting forces, some
of them dark and bestial. The werewolf is efficient both as a
metaphor for the “beast within man” and as a literal, tweaked
predator reminiscent of the kind of monsters that stalked our
ancestors.
The Zombie
The zombie packs a double whammy in its dual assault as a
physically dangerous agent that is riddled with pathogens. It wants
to eat you, and it is extremely infectious. Furthermore, the zombie
is a counterintuitive and thus salient idea: It is a reanimated human
corpse and a “person” without a mind, or at least with severely
impaired cognitive functioning (and thus the opposite of a ghost, a
mind without a body). From about 1925, the representation of
zombies in English-language books increased by more than 500%,
and by 2000 it had increased by 3,250% (see Figure 1). The most
important turning point was George A. Romero’s film Night of the
Living Dead (1968), a disturbing and commercially successful film
that pushed the stumbling zombie into the spotlight of mass
entertainment.
Why did the zombie become so popular so quickly and in this
particular era? Some commentators have isolated cultural anxieties
that zombies are somehow supposed to engage with or symbolize.
For example, David J. Skal (2001) reads Dawn of the Dead (1978)
as a “razor-edged social satire: [it] peopled a shopping mall with
flesh-eating zombies, an indelible image of consumerism gone
mad” (p. 309). Night of the Living Dead has been interpreted as an
imaginative transfiguration of cold war anxiety (Pulliam, 2007).
Stephen King (2010) sees the 2004 remake of Dawn of the Dead
as a trigger of post-9/11 terrorism anxiety. There is some truth to
this widespread constructivist approach, but it is not sufficient to
explain why the zombie became a popular horror monster. The
constructivist perspective is too narrow in its exclusion of biology
and evolved cognition. In the case of zombies, these horror mon-
sters do gain much of their power and salience as metaphors for
sociocultural anxieties, but this power works in tandem with (and
secondarily to) the literal, predatory, and disease-salient presence
of zombies (Clasen, 2010a). The zombie was gradually introduced
into pop culture during the 20th century and rose swiftly in
popularity and visibility following the commercial success of
Romero’s groundbreaking film (Pulliam, 2007). It could never
have achieved this level of cultural success if it had not connected
squarely with adaptive dispositions to fear lethal attack and infec-
tious agents.
The Vampire
The vampire is an ancient figure found in cultures throughout
the world (Carter, 2007). It is enjoying an immense boom in
popular culture today (see Figure 1). A vampire is a reanimated
Figure 1. Ngram (Michel et al., 2010). The corpus “English” (containing digitized versions of millions of
English-language books) has been searched for occurrences of the terms vampire,zombie, and werewolf to give
a rough estimate of the relative popularity of these monsters. At the millennium, the vampire leads vastly, with
the zombie a distant second and the werewolf an honorable third.
225
MONSTERS EVOLVE: A BIOCULTURAL APPROACH
corpse whose sole sustenance is the blood of living humans. The
vampire has changed and multiplied enormously over the past 300
years, so that today’s teen idol vampires from such stories as L. J.
Smith’s The Vampire Diaries series and Stephenie Meyer’s Twi-
light saga are barely recognizable as descendants of the nocturnal
revenants that preyed on rural Eastern European populations in the
18th century (Vanderbeke, 2010). Metaphorically speaking, we
have a vampire genotype that is expressed as different phenotypes
in different ecologies and modified by cultural selection over time.
Different vampire phenotypes require different explanations.
Drawing on archeology, forensic pathology, folklore, and history,
Paul Barber (1988) convincingly argues that vampires in the early
18th century were really a product of a prescientific misunder-
standing of biochemical processes. Because nobody had come up
with a germ theory of disease, disease was understood in terms of
agency, often intentional intervention by supernatural agents, not
in terms of invisible microorganisms. (Similarly, the werewolf
may have served as an explanation for pathological outbreaks of
homicidal violence in the dark ages before professional psychiatry
and the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders.) Some modern authors have moved the vampire
figure from the uncanny world of demonic forces and supernatural
causation to a naturalistic universe of communicable disease, for
example, Richard Matheson in his 1954 novel I Am Legend (Cla-
sen, 2010b), thus mirroring the cumulative success of natural
science as an explanatory system; Bram Stoker’s Van Helsing
studies forgotten books of the occult; Matheson’s Robert Neville
looks at vampire cells in his microscope. The kind of hypersexu-
alized vampires that have recently become popular arguably reflect
human female mating strategies, rather than evolutionarily recur-
rent threats embodied in predatory monsters: These slightly dan-
gerous, highly attractive, and pathologically devoted male vam-
pires seem to condense “cads” and “dads” into one (see Johnson,
2011, on Twilight). Such stories are predominantly about mate
choice rather than escaping a dangerous predator. Mate prefer-
ences rest on a universal, biological substrate, but they are mod-
ulated by cultural norms. It is obviously important to understand
the kind of culture that gives rise to for example Stephenie Mey-
er’s odes to unbridled materialism, impossibly attractive people,
and idealized romantic love, which could not have arisen in the
culture of Eastern Europe in the 18th century. They are at home in
contemporary America.
Vampires can be powerful, potent metaphors, but first and
foremost they are salient, counterintuitive, usually dangerous mon-
sters, and their ubiquity in popular culture reflects our species’
preoccupation with dangerous agents in the environment.
The Ghost
The ghost is, basically, a disembodied mind, a counterintuitive
agent that commands attention and historically has been useful for
explaining odd events (missing objects, the impregnation of Mary
in the absence of mortal male gametes, the sound you hear from
the kitchen just before you fall asleep). Changing theological
attitudes and scientific accomplishments alter the significance of
ghosts (Finucane, 1984). Nonetheless, all ghosts spring from the
same cognitive tendencies: overattributing agency to inexplicable
events and an intuitive dualism, that is, our innate tendency to view
humans as consisting of material as well as spiritual selves
(“souls”; Bloom, 2004; Boyer, 2001). Supposing that a person’s
immaterial spirit persists after the body dies requires no great leap
of faith.
To understand a specific ghost story, we need to understand both
the mental machinery that produces and entertains ghosts and the
cultural context in which the ghost floats around. For example,
the way that Henry James describes his ghosts in The Turn of the
Screw (1898) is heavily influenced by late-Victorian “psychical”
discourse about spirits and hauntings and also by contemporary
“spectral” technologies such as photography and x-rays (Grove,
1997). James’s ghosts threaten with an unspecified moral debase-
ment that must be understood in relation to Victorian mores and
norms of propriety. Nonetheless, James’s story can still send a
shiver down the spine of a reader far removed from fin de sie`cle
Britain.
In the 1979 horror film The Amityville Horror, the evil spirits
are significant in terms of financial, rather than spiritual, ruin. A
family moves into a haunted house, which is soon being ripped
apart by ghosts. The film came out in the wake of the economic
recession of the mid-1970s, and as Stephen King (1983) recollects,
“I found myself wondering not if [the protagonists] would get out
alive but if they had adequate homeowner’s insurance” (p. 143).
King’s observation rings true. The film probably tapped into
widespread economic anxiety and used ghosts as a metaphor for
the sort of apparently impenetrable, malicious, and ultimately
unreasonable forces that can ruin you financially. Nonetheless, the
ghosts are more than metaphors: They are plain creepy in their
literal reality-violating, vengeful aspect. Moreover, the fear of
financial ruin is obviously dependent on local socioeconomic
parameters and was particularly salient in this age of recession, but
it rests on a biological substrate. Insofar as material resources are a
proximate means to achieve the ultimate goal of reproductive
success (Cronk, 1991), it should come as no surprise that people
generally attempt to avoid going broke. Moreover, although the
culturally contingent subtext of The Amityville Horror gave it an
edge to contemporary viewers, it can still frighten audiences that
are not particularly concerned with impending bankruptcy. That is
because malicious, dangerous ghosts are frightening cross-
culturally, to Stone Age cave dwellers and the citizens of modern
nation-states alike.
Monsters vary, but not endlessly and not arbitrarily. They are
variations on evolutionarily constrained themes. Different ecolo-
gies provide different configurations of imaginative spaces in
which monsters grow. These ecologies, whether broadly cultural
or psychologically individual, are in turn circumscribed by human
nature. This is something that the successful horror writer, director,
or programmer intuitively knows: He or she is like a locksmith,
forging a monstrous key that fits in the cognitive lock of an
organism that is shaped by universal nature and further calibrated
by local culture.
Functions of Fictional Fear
Horror fiction capitalizes on cognitive and physiological ma-
chinery that is a product of natural selection. Some horror fictions
rely on powerful fear or even terror responses (and few horror film
directors are loath to throw in a startling scare or two), and others
are more dependent on low-key anxiety-provoking storylines and
monsters. Common to the outright horrifying and the more subtly
226 CLASEN
disturbing horror stories is that they evoke strong emotion and
have us raptly probing the fictional landscape for danger.
If horror stories by “artificially” activating adaptations for dan-
ger management invoke negative emotion in their audiences, why
do some people actively seek out such experiences? We know that
certain demographic populations are more likely than others to
find horror stories attractive, particularly as pertains to horror
films. Adolescent males top the list (Hoffner & Levine, 2005;
Weaver & Tamborini, 1996). Many horror films feature disgusting
contents (decaying monsters, bodily mutilations, and so on). Fe-
males have been shown to be higher in disgust sensitivity than
males, probably as a defensive function of immunosuppression
during parts of the reproductive cycle (Lie´nard, 2011, pp. 1069 –
1070). Women might thus be less likely to enjoy the more disgust-
dependent horror films, like some slasher films and works in the
“gorenography” subgenre. Research has shown that sensation-
seeking personality traits are correlated with a preference for
horror films (reviewed in Hoffner & Levine, 2005), and adolescent
males display the highest tendency to engage in dangerous or
quasi-dangerous behavior (Arnett, 1996). Pierre Lie´nard (2011)
notes that males are more likely “to be in intense competition with
same-sex conspecifics” and to form coalitions and strong male–
male bonds, particularly in adolescence and young adulthood (p.
1071). Perhaps horror films provide a stage for almost ritualistic
male–male challenges: Like rites of passage, horror films may
serve as a kind of intragroup challenge and also reinforce group
bonds. The affirmation of such bonds may be what the ordeal of
sitting through a horror film produces: not just “I made it,” but
“We made it—together.”
One study found that respondents reported enjoying a horror
film more if an opposite sex coviewer reacts in a “gender-
appropriate” way (i.e., fearlessness or mastery of fear for boys,
fearfulness or distress for girls). In this study, subjects were
exposed to a clip from Friday the 13th Part 3 (1982). Male
respondents in the company of visibly distressed females claimed
to enjoy the clip about twice as much as those in the company of
fearless female peers (Zillmann & Weaver, 1996). Likewise, girls
enjoyed watching a film in the company of a scared boy consid-
erably less than in the company of a fearless one. Moreover,
respondents rated coviewers as more attractive when their coview-
ers reacted gender-appropriately to the clip. Perhaps a misattribu-
tion of arousal partly accounts for this finding (cf. Dutton & Aron,
1974); the arousal elicited by the film clip is misinterpreted as
arousal caused by the coviewer, which then leads to an inflated
attractiveness rating as a result of rationalization and is exacer-
bated when the coviewer reacts in the desired gender-appropriate
way: “Something has gotten me hot under the collar. Maybe it’s
the person screaming/coping admirably next to me. That person
must be very desirable.” This phenomenon has become known as
the snuggle theory of horror: that horror films can provide a setting
for amorous adolescents to react in socially sanctioned, gender-
specific ways and cuddle up in the semidarkness of the cinema.
We need to go deeper than gender roles and socialization
theories to find the appeal of horror. We have already seen how the
human danger-management system is an adaptation that requires
calibration to local environments. The need for calibration is a
solution to the problem that human genes do not “know” exactly
which dangers their host will encounter during its life span, al-
though they will have a pretty good idea. A child growing up in
Sub-Saharan Africa faces certain dangers that are different from
those faced by an Inuit child and so needs to learn about them. The
key, from the viewpoint of natural selection, is to make such
learning pleasurable. Hence, finding pleasure in vicarious learning
about dangerous agents and situations is adaptive.
Steen and Owens (2001) argue that human chase play specifi-
cally is adaptive in that it helps to mature a child’s predator
detection and evasion skills. Natural selection has made play
behavior self-rewarding, simply because play is adaptive: We, and
all other mammalian infants in whom it has been looked for,
exhibit a remarkable tendency to spontaneously engage in play and
exploratory behavior. These apparently nonfunctional behaviors
actually develop skills that are likely to become critically impor-
tant later in life (S
ˇpinka, Newberry, & Bekoff, 2001). If one meets
a hungry predator for the first time, it is desirable to have a store
of surrogate experience with predator evasion to draw from rather
than proceed by trial-and-error. Thus, like pretend play, fiction is
a kind of mental simulation (Oatley, 1999) that gives us surrogate
experience risk-free and at low cost (Carroll, 1999, 2011; Tooby &
Cosmides, 2001). Scary stories about dangerous monsters illustrate
this principle well (Sugiyama, 2006).
The fascination with monsters that many people (especially
children) feel is probably the result of an adaptive tendency to pay
attention to such dangerous agents and to learn about their behav-
ior vicariously, what H. Clark Barrett called the “Jurassic Park
hypothesis” (2005, p. 218). In this view, the modern horror story
is a kind of supernormal stimulus, a hypertrophied variation on
chase play: It is a technology that enlists all manner of monstrous
agents to tap into an adaptive motivational system for learning
about danger and to calibrate our responses to danger. As Kim
Newman (2011) put it, “the central thesis of horror in film and
literature is that the world is a more frightening place than is
generally assumed” (p. 5). Consuming horror fiction could thus be
adaptive. It has all the benefits of learning about danger and one’s
own response to danger, but without the risk of actual harm. One
reviewer (Wong, 2000), looking back at Jaws (1975), noted how in
the wake of this film, a “mundane event like going for a swim at
the beach just wasn’t the same again . . . just like many people
stopped taking showers after seeing Psycho.” To cease showering
is probably not very adaptive, unless one finds alternative means of
personal hygiene, whereas showing a greater degree of vigilance
while bathing in the sea—even if great white sharks very rarely
attack people—could be a sensible strategy.
As Asma (2009) wrote, monsters “probably appear and reappear
in our stories and in our artwork because they help us (and helped
our ancestors) navigate the dangers of our environment” (pp.
283–284). Asma’s functional thesis fits snugly in the biocultural
framework outlined here. Pointing out that the monster is a uni-
versal figure, he plausibly suggests that “stories about monster
threats and heroic conquests provide us with a ritualized, rehears-
able simulation of reality, a virtual way to represent the forces of
nature, the threats from other animals, and the dangers of human
social interaction” (pp. 282–283). Sometimes horror stories can be
traumatic (cf. Cantor & Oliver, 1996), sometimes the pleasure of
watching a horror movie is subordinate or even incidental to the
pleasure of getting together with friends, and sometimes the neg-
ative emotion evoked by a horror story is a nuisance for the reader
interested in other qualities offered by the story. Presumably, some
readers enjoy Stephen King’s descriptions of human interactions
227
MONSTERS EVOLVE: A BIOCULTURAL APPROACH
and mental machinations, but wish that he would turn it down a
notch on the scary stuff. At any rate, horror stories are ubiquitous
in popular culture because they are effective at what they do. And
what they do is provide an imaginative space in which thrilling,
uncanny encounters with all manner of monsters can take place. If
ancestral environments had not been dangerous, if we were all
perfectly fearless, we would have no horror stories today: no
bloodsucking vampires, no scary ghosts, no howling werewolves.
Evolution, Cognition, Culture:
An Integrated Approach
There is no good epistemological reason for ignoring evolution
and cognition in the study of culture. Horror fiction is a good
example: Scholars of horror fiction have everything to gain from
adopting a biocultural perspective, one that integrates attention to
historical circumstance with knowledge of human evolutionary
history and evolved cognitive architecture. Such an integrated
approach has the potential of adding analytic richness and boosting
explanatory power in the study of horror fiction. It avoids the
pitfall of unnecessary monocausal reductionism, both in construc-
tivist and evolutionary approaches: An attempt to explain horror
fiction by appealing only to biological hard-wiring is as futile as
appealing only to cultural contingency. Only a fully integrated
biocultural approach is up to the task of making sense of the
monsters that prowl our storyscapes.
References
Arnett, J. J. (1996). Sensation seeking, aggressiveness, and adolescent
reckless behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 693–702.
doi:10.1016/0191-8869(96)00027-X
Asma, S. T. (2009). On monsters: An unnatural history of our worst fears.
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Barber, P. (1988). Vampires, burial, and death: Folklore and reality. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Barrett, H. C. (2005). Adaptations to predators and prey. In D. M. Buss
(Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 200–223). New York,
NY: Wiley.
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? Walnut Creek,
CA: Altamira Press.
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development
explains what makes us human. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious
thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2011). Threat-detection in child development:
An evolutionary perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
35, 1034–1041. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.010
Boyer, P., & Lie´nard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution
systems and action parsing in development, pathological and cultural
rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 595–613. doi:10.1017/
S0140525X06009332
Cantor, J., & Oliver, M. B. (1996). Developmental differences in responses
to horror. In J. B. Weaver & R. Tamborini (Eds.), Horror films: Re-
search on audience preference and reactions (pp. 63–80). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Carroll, J. (1999). The deep structure of literary representations. Evolution
and Human Behavior, 20, 159–173. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)
00004-5
Carroll, J. (2011). Reading human nature: Literary Darwinism in theory
and practice. New York, NY: SUNY University Press.
Carter, M. L. (2007). The vampire. In S. T. Joshi (Ed.), Icons of horror and
the supernatural (pp. 619–652). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Clasen, M. (2010a). The anatomy of the zombie: A bio-psychological look
at the undead other. Otherness: Essays and Studies, 1, 1–23.
Clasen, M. (2010b). Vampire apocalypse: A biocultural critique of Richard
Matheson’s I Am Legend. Philosophy and Literature, 34, 313–328.
doi:10.1353/phl.2010.0005
Cronk, L. (1991). Wealth, status, and reproductive success among the
Mukogodo of Kenya. American Anthropologist, 93, 345–360. doi:
10.1525/aa.1991.93.2.02a00040
Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual
attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 30, 510–517. doi:10.1037/h0037031
Eilam, D., Izhar, R., & Mort, J. (2011). Threat detection: Behavioral
practices in animals and humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Re-
views, 35, 999–1006. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.002
Feinstein, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., & Tranel, D. (2011a). The human
amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Current Biology, 21,
34–38. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042
Feinstein, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., & Tranel, D. (2011b). The human
amygdala and the induction and experience of fear [Supplemental in-
formation]. Current Biology, 21, 1–11. Retrieved from http://
download.cell.com/current-biology/mmcs/journals/0960–9822/
PIIS0960982210015083.mmc1.pdf. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042
Finucane, R. C. (1984). Appearances of the dead: A cultural history of
ghosts. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books.
Gilmore, D. D. (2003). Monsters: Evil beings, mythical beasts, and all
manner of imaginary terrors. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsyl-
vania Press.
Grodal, T. K. (2009). Embodied visions: Evolution, emotion, culture, and
film. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Gross, J., & Levenson, R. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cogni-
tion & Emotion, 9, 87–108. doi:10.1080/02699939508408966
Grove, A. W. (1997). Röntgen’s ghosts: Photography, X-rays, and the
Victorian imagination. Literature and Medicine, 16, 141–173. doi:
10.1353/lm.1997.0016
Hart, D., & Sussman, R. W. (2008). Man the hunted: Primates, predators,
and human evolution (Expanded ed.). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Hill, K., & Furtado, A. M. (1996). Ache´ life history: The ecology and
demography of a foraging people. New York, NY: de Gruyter.
Hoffner, C. A., & Levine, K. J. (2005). Enjoyment of mediated fright and
violence: A meta-analysis. Media Psychology, 7, 207–237. doi:10.1207/
S1532785XMEP0702_5
Johnson, J. A. (2011). Vampires are real: An evolutionary view of the
Twilight saga. The Evolutionary Review: Art, Science, Culture, 2, 113–
118.
Kendrick, W. (1992). The thrill of fear. New York, NY: Grove Weidenfeld.
King, S. (1983). Danse macabre. New York, NY: Berkley Books.
King, S. (2010). What’s scary: A forenote to the 2010 edition. In S. King,
Danse macabre (pp. 11–37). New York, NY: Pocket Books.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of
emotional life. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Lie´nard, P. (2011). Life stages and risk-avoidance: Status- and context-
sensitivity in precaution systems. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Re-
views, 35, 1067–1074. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.007
Marks, I. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and fitness: An evolutionary
analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 247–261.
doi:10.1016/0162-3095(94)90002-7
McCarty, M., & McLaughlin, M. (2006). John Carpenter looks back at
Halloween on its 25th anniversary. Science Fiction Weekly, 9. Retrieved
from http://www.scifi.com/sfw/issue339/interview.html
Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Google
Books Team, . . . Aiden, E. L. (2011, January 14). Quantitative analysis
228 CLASEN
of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331, 176–182.
doi:10.1126/science.1199644
Neuberg, S., Kenrick, D., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat manage-
ment systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1042–1051. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev
.2010.08.011
Newman, K. (2011). Nightmare movies: Horror on screen since the 1960s
(Rev. ed.). London, England: Bloomsbury.
Norenzayan, A., Atran, A., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2006). Memory
and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive nar-
ratives. Cognitive Science, 30, 531–553. doi:10.1207/
s15516709cog0000_68
The Numbers. (2011). Top-grossing genres 1995 to 2011. Retrieved from
http://www.the-numbers.com/market/Genres/
Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as
cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology, 3,
101–107. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.3.2.101
Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical
perspectives. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
emotions (2nd ed., pp. 573–593). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention:
Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 130, 466–478. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness:
Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological
Review, 108, 483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483
Paranormal Activity. (2011). Box office data, movie news, cast informa-
tion. Retrieved from http://www.the-numbers.com/movies/2009/
PNACT.php
Pulliam, J. (2007). The zombie. In S. T. Joshi (Ed.), Icons of horror and the
supernatural (pp. 723–753). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
Rose, C. (2000). Giants, monsters, and dragons: An encyclopedia of
folklore, legend, and myth. New York, NY: Norton.
Rottenberg, J., Ray, R., & Gross, J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films.
In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicitation
and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Shepard, P. (1996). The others: How animals made us human. Washington,
DC: Island Press.
Skal, D. J. (2001). The monster show: A cultural history of horror (Rev.
ed.). New York, NY: Faber & Faber.
S
ˇpinka, M., Newberry, R. C., & Bekoff, M. (2001). Mammalian play:
Training for the unexpected. Quarterly Review of Biology, 76, 141–168.
Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/2664002. doi:10.1086/393866
Steen, F. F., & Owen, S. A. (2001). Evolution’s pedagogy: An adaptation-
ist model of pretense and entertainment. Journal of Cognition and
Culture, 1, 289–321. doi:10.1163/156853701753678305
Sugiyama, M. S. (2006). Lions and tigers and bears: Predators as a folklore
universal. In H. Friedrich, F. Jannidis, U. Klein, K. Mellmann, S.
Metzger, & M. Willems (Eds.), Anthropology and social history: Heu-
ristics in the study of literature (pp. 319–331). Paderborn, Germany:
Mentis.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2000). Evolutionary psychology and the
emotions. In N. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
emotions (2nd ed., pp. 91–115). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds?
Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction and the arts. Sub-
Stance, 30, 6–27. doi:10.1353/sub.2001.0017
Vanderbeke, D. (2010). The vampire strikes back: On the history of a
nightwalker. Fastitocalon, 1, 3–19.
Wade, N. (2007). Before the dawn: Recovering the lost history of our
ancestors. London, England: Duckworth.
Weaver, J. B., & Tamborini, J. (Eds.). (1996). Horror films: Current
research on audience preferences and reactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wong, J. (2000). Review of film Jaws (1975). Retrieved from http://
www.moviemutterings.com/reviews/j/jaws.htm
Woody, E., & Szechtman, H. (2011). Adaptation to potential threat: The
evolution, neurobiology, and psychopathology of the security motivation
system. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1019–1033. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.003
Zillmann, D., & Weaver, J. B. (1996). Gender socialization theory of
reactions to horror. In J. B. Weaver & R. Tamborini (Eds.), Horror
films: Research on audience preference and reactions (pp. 81–101).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Received February 8, 2012
Revision received February 8, 2012
Accepted February 22, 2012 䡲
229
MONSTERS EVOLVE: A BIOCULTURAL APPROACH