ArticlePublisher preview available

Maintenance of knowledge: Questions about memory we forgot to ask

American Psychological Association
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Used 2 methods—successive relearning and cross-sectional adjustment—to examine processes involved in the maintenance of knowledge. Successive relearning is illustrated in a laboratory study with 90 undergraduates; English–Spanish word pairs were periodically relearned, and the intersession interval was varied from a few seconds to 30 days. It was found that indefinite access to the acquired information remained highly probable if the retraining interval did not exceed the access interval. Cross-sectional adjustment is a nonlaboratory method designed to investigate the acquisition and maintenance of complex knowledge systems under ecologically realistic conditions. The method is illustrated in a study with 275 university students and 576 alumni in which maintenance of knowledge of a city was found to be related to the frequency, recency, duration, and distribution of visits during a 46-yr retention interval. Statistical and interpretive procedures for each method are detailed. (9 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
of
Experimental
Psychology:
General
1979,
Vol. 108,
No. 3,
296-308
Maintenance
of
Knowledge:
Questions About Memory
We
Forgot
to Ask
Harry
P.
Bahrick
Ohio
Wesleyan
University
SUMMARY
Memory
research
has
contributed little toward understanding acquisition, main-
tenance,
or
loss
of
complex knowledge systems. This
is so
because such systems
are
acquired
and
maintained over long time periods
that
cannot
be
accommodated
by
traditional research methods. Acquisition
of
semipermanent knowledge typically
involves repeated
exposure
to
information, with
losses
of
information during
in-
tervals between exposures. Continued maintenance
of
knowledge depends
on pe-
riodic
access.
Two
methods
are
described
to
investigate these processes.
The
method
of
successive relearning
is a
laboratory method illustrated
in a
study
in
which English-Spanish word pairs
are
periodically relearned
and the
effect
of
varying
the
intersession interval
from
a few
seconds
to 30
days
is
estab-
lished.
It is
shown
that
indefinite
access
to the
acquired
information
remains highly
probable
if the
retraining interval does
not
exceed
the
access interval.
If the
access
interval
is
much longer than
the
retraining interval, much
of the
acquired informa-
tion becomes inaccessible during
the
access interval.
The
method
of
cross-sectional adjustment
is a
nonlaboratory method designed
to
investigate
the
acquisition
and
maintenance
of
complex knowledge systems
under ecologically realistic conditions. This method
can be
used
if a
large
number
of
individuals
acquired
the
same information,
if the
time
of
acquisition dates back
from
a few
months
to
many years,
and if
both
the
original level
of
knowledge
and
the
degree
of
rehearsal during
the
retention interval
can be
estimated with
ac-
ceptable reliability.
The
method
is
illustrated
in a
study
in
which maintenance
of
knowledge
of a
city
is
related
to the
frequency,
recency,
duration,
and
distribution
of
visits during
a
46-yr. retention interval.
Subjects
are
assigned
to
time
groups
based
on how
long
ago
they
acquired
the
information;
average
retention performance
for
various time groups yields
an un-
adjusted
retention
curve.
Such
curves
must
be
corrected
for
differences
among
the
groups
in the
degree
of
original learning
and in the
amount
of
rehearsal.
These
adjustments
are
made
on the
basis
of
multiple
regression
equations
that
reflect
correlations
of
rehearsal variables
and
retention performance.
It is
shown that
the
adjustment
process
can
correct
for
major
inequalities
among
groups.
The
multiple
regression equations
are
used
further
to
obtain estimates
of the
amount
of re-
hearsal
necessary
to
maintain
the
original
level
of
knowledge.
Copyright 1979
by the
American
Psychological
Association,
Inc.
0096-344S/79/0803-0296$00.7S
296
... Um dem Vergessen und den damit verbundenen abträglichen Konsequenzen für den Erwerb von Wissen entgegenzuwirken, sollten Übungsmaßnahmen zur Konsolidierung des Wissens und somit zur Förderung des Erhalts des Wissens ergriffen werden. So hat der Gedächtnisforscher Harry Bahrick schon vor mehr als vier Jahrzehnten darauf hingewiesen, dass der Erhalt unserer wichtigsten Ressource, das Wissen, explizite Übungsmaßnahmen erfordere und dass Lernen im Grunde immer als Zyklus aus Wissenserwerb, Vergessen und Wiedererwerb des Wissens verstanden werden sollte (Bahrick 1979). Auch in weiteren etablierten Theorien des Erwerbs kognitiver Fertigkeiten (z. ...
... Bei noch längeren Abständen nahm die Lernwirksamkeit wieder ab, vermutlich weil bereits zu viele Informationen aus der ersten Lerngelegenheit vergessen wurden, als die Wiederholung anstand. Eine Untersuchung von Bahrick (1979) zum Vokabellernen (Spanisch-Englisch) legt ebenfalls nahe, dass für das langfristige Behalten längere Lernintervalle sinnvoll sein können. Hier zeigte sich bei einem Lerntest nach 1 Monat und auch noch nach 8 Jahren (Bahrick und Phelphs 1987) eine Überlegenheit eines Lernintervalls von 30 Tagen gegenüber einem Lernintervall von nur 1 Tag. ...
... Die theoretischen Annahmen, warum sich sukzessives Wiederlernen förderlich auf die Konsolidierung von Wissen auswirken sollte, sind weitgehend identisch zu den theoretischen Annahmen zu den Wirkungen der Einzelkomponenten Abrufübung und verteiltes Üben, die oben bereits ausgeführt wurden. Diese Annahmen sollen an dieser Stelle nicht wiederholt werden, sondern stattdessen das Bemerkenswerte an der Idee von sukzessivem Wiederlernen beleuchtet werden, die auf Bahrick (1979) zurückgeht. So ging Bahrick bei der Konzeption von sukzessivem Wiederlernen, im Einklang mit empirischen Befunden zu Vergessensraten (s. ...
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung Dem Üben und der Konsolidierung von Wissen zum Schutz vor Vergessen wird in der Unterrichtsforschung bislang vergleichsweise wenig Aufmerksamkeit geschenkt. Wir beschreiben die Rolle des Übens und seine Funktionen beim Lernen und stellen drei konsolidierende Übungsformen vor, deren Potenzial für das nachhaltige Lernen theoretisch gut begründet und empirisch belegt ist: Abrufübung, verteiltes Üben und sukzessives Wiederlernen. Wir diskutieren jeweils den Stand der Forschung, Erklärungsansätze und offene Fragen. Zudem diskutieren wir, inwiefern die Integration von verschachteltem Üben eine sinnvolle Weiterentwicklung darstellen könnte. Allgemeine Desiderate für zukünftige Forschung beziehen sich auf die Klärung der Rolle des Übens im Rahmen des bedeutungshaltigen, sinnvoll eingebetteten Lernens und die Frage, wie sich Übungsmethoden mit generativen Lernaktivitäten und dem selbstregulierten Lernen verbinden lassen.
... No supervision is required. Therefore, using this tool allows for selfregulated and evidence-based learning strategies such as distributed learning (e.g., Bahrick, 1979;Cepeda et al., 2006), self-testing (e.g., Roediger & Karpicke, 2006;Schwieren et al., 2017), or successive relearning (e.g., Rawson et al., 2013). No user data is stored. ...
... Thus, the testing effect arises with considerable delay after learning. Similarly, positive learning outcomes of distributed learning usually arise after longer periods of time (Bahrick, 1979). Therefore, several effects on learning and motivation cannot be demonstrated/experienced within a single seminar session. ...
Article
Full-text available
This article describes the implementation of a digital case-exploration tool and its use in teacher education. The cases were constructed according to empirically based psychological theories and represent school-related scenarios in which the students could choose among options for action. After selecting an action, its psychological consequences are presented in text or video including theoretical explanations how and why the selected action would lead to the presented consequences. We present first quasi-experimental evaluation data demonstrating the tool's usefulness regarding learning outcomes, students’ active participation, and motivation.
... Therefore, students must correct errors in their understanding before the final test. Similar research on successive relearning has revealed that engaging in retrieval practice across several spaced sessions until one achieves a criterion (e.g., three correct retrievals of each item) can lead to significant, durable memory improvements (Bahrick, 1979;Rawson & Dunlosky, 2011, 2022Rawson et al., 2018), including on applicationbased questions (Badali & Greve, 2023). Participants in these studies were incentivized to complete the quizzes through payment. ...
Article
The present work assessed a quizzing method that combines the benefits of retrieval practice and feedback, whereby learners must continue taking quizzes until they achieve a perfect score with feedback provided (i.e., mastery quizzing). Across four experiments ( n = 952; age 18–76, M = 37.10, SD = 11.61; 50% female, 48% male, 2% other gender or did not identify), we explored whether such quizzing produces better retention than more traditional forms of quizzing. Participants were adults recruited online who were not screened for content knowledge of the materials. In Experiment 1, participants read explanations of psychological concepts frequently taught in introductory psychology classes. They completed one, two, or three quizzes without feedback or engaged in mastery quizzing. Performance on a final test was better in the mastery quiz condition relative to the comparison quiz conditions. Further experiments showed the mastery quizzing advantage was maintained when comparison quiz conditions also included feedback (Experiment 2) and when conditions encouraged attention to feedback (Experiment 3). Mastery quizzing benefits persisted when retention intervals were increased to 30 minutes and 2 days (Experiment 4). Thus, mastery quizzing may be a simple method of encouraging more durable learning than traditional approaches to quizzing.
... In the former case, the HPK participant is relearning an answer, not engaging in entirely new learning. Since Ebbinghaus's savings measure, the field has recognized that it is easier to relearn information than it is to learn it initially (e.g., Bahrick, 1979Bahrick, , 1984Ebbinghaus, 1913). Likewise, participants with partial knowledge about a question will need less new learning to acquire the missing information and answer it correctly on the posttest compared to those with little or no relevant knowledge. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is commonly claimed that higher domain knowledge enhances new learning—the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. However, a recent meta-analysis (Simonsmeier et al., 2022) has challenged this idea, finding no overall relationship between prior knowledge and new learning across hundreds of highly variable effect sizes. The authors note that this variability and lack of randomized controlled experiments preclude broad claims regarding the influence of prior knowledge on learning. The present study (conducted in 2020) provides an experimental assessment of the causal effect of prior domain knowledge on new learning. Participants were randomly assigned to receive training in one of two academic domains over 3 days before learning new information about topics in both domains for a later test. Training was specific to three of four topics within that domain, allowing the untrained topic in the trained domain to act as a measure of new learning in that domain. New learning, measured as final test performance or knowledge gains, did not differ between the high and low domain knowledge conditions. Experimentally induced prior domain knowledge did not affect new learning.
... [26] There is evidence suggesting that longer intervals between repetitions bene t memory consolidation. [27] In any case, massed learning was proven less effective that any type of spaced repetition. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Students preparing for the medical school admission exam are vulnerable to learning difficulties because of the abrupt transition after high school. Spaced repetition is a recognized method for improving long-term memory retention. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effectiveness of spaced repetition in students preparing for the medical school entrance exam. Methods All students preparing for the medical school entrance exam in the year 2023 at the University of Rouen, France were invited to participate in a final individual self-questionnaire. The survey included questions about their learning technique, whether they used spaced repetition or not, and questions about their lifestyle. Factors associated with success in the examinations were assessed. Results We collected 523 responses (84.6%), 134 (25.6%) students achieved success in the medical school admission examinations while 389 (74.4%) did not. In the success group, 60 (44.8%) employed spaced repetition and 79 (20.3%) in the failure group. The use of spaced repetition was significantly associated with success in examinations in univariate analysis (p < 0.001) and after multivariate adjustments. Other variables such as sport practice and longer sleep duration were also associated with success. Conclusions Organized study habits, including spaced repetition during medical school preparation, seem to boost exam success. Additionally, lifestyle and varied learning techniques may aid students' academic achievements.
Article
Incorporating curriculum to effectively help veterinary students learn how to provide accessible quality care to all pet owners is needed. The primary aims of this study are to explore how a 2-week rotation at a veterinary medical service-learning clinic (Wisconsin Companion Animal Resources, Education, and Social Services [WisCARES]) improves (a) comfort in working with clients from diverse race and low socioeconomic (SOCI) backgrounds and (2) confidence in leading cases, communication skills, and providing a spectrum of care options. Students were surveyed at five time points pre-rotation: mid-week 1, mid-week 2, end of rotation, and 1-month post. A total of 115 survey series were at least partially completed. Of the 97 responses that include background information, 68 (70%) students reported having “no to a few weeks” of experience working with diverse or low SOCI populations. When comparing themselves to before starting the rotation, student responses indicated increased comfort (mean = 4.54, standard deviation [SD] = 0.54) and compassion (mean = 4.42, SD = 0.78) working with low-income or homeless populations, more comfort interacting with members of different race or ethnicity groups (mean = 4.21, SD = 0.82), and more appreciation for the human-animal bond (mean = 4.42, median = 5). Students also reported that spending time at WisCARES positively impacted their confidence in a clinical setting, managing and communicating about financial decisions, and approaching cases creatively. Giving students an opportunity to lead cases with clients from diverse races and low SOCI backgrounds can enhance levels of comfort with practice and improve confidence.
Article
Full-text available
Some neural representations gradually change across multiple timescales. Here we argue that modeling this “drift” could help explain the spacing effect (the long-term benefit of distributed learning), whereby differences between stored and current temporal context activity patterns produce greater error-driven learning. We trained a neurobiologically realistic model of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus to learn paired associates alongside temporal context vectors that drifted between learning episodes and/or before final retention intervals. In line with spacing effects, greater drift led to better model recall after longer retention intervals. Dissecting model mechanisms revealed that greater drift increased error-driven learning, strengthened weights in slower drifting temporal context neurons (temporal abstraction), and improved direct cue–target associations (decontextualization). Intriguingly, these results suggest that decontextualization—generally ascribed only to the neocortex—can occur within the hippocampus itself. Altogether, our findings provide a mechanistic formalization for established learning concepts such as spacing effects and errors during learning.
Article
Full-text available
SS WERE REQUIRED TO SELECT PREVIOUSLY EXPOSED PICTURES OF COMMON OBJECTS FROM AMONG SERIES OF ALTERNATIVE PICTURES GRADED IN SIMILARITY IN THE PROTOTYPES. RESPONSE FREQUENCIES WERE PLOTTED IN THE FORM OF GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS, AND SUCH GRADIENTS WERE OBTAINED FOLLOWING 4 STAGES OF TRAINING AND 3 RETENTION INTERVALS. IN PART II, SS WERE TRAINED BY EXPOSING THE SAME PROTOTYPE STIMULI, BUT RECOGNITION TESTS CONSISTED OF ALTERNATIVES AT 1 OF 3 HOMOGENEOUS LEVELS OF SIMILARITY TO THE PROTOTYPES. LEARNING CURVES BASED UPON THE 3 TYPES OF TESTS DIFFER MARKEDLY IN SLOPE, REFLECTING THE DIFFERENTIAL SENSITIVITY OF VARIOUS DICHOTOMOUS TESTS TO THE CHANGES IN THE DISCRIMINABILITY FUNCTION. IT WAS SHOWN THAT THE SLOPE OF EACH CURVE COULD BE PREDICTED ACCURATELY FROM THE GRADIENTS OBTAINED IN PART I. THUS, GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS WERE SHOWN TO BE SENSITIVE, PARSIMONIOUS REPRESENTATIONS OF THE RECOGNITION LEARNING PROCESS. (16 REF.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Tested 392 high school graduates for memory of names and portraits of classmates selected from yearbooks. The retention interval since graduation varied from 2 wks to 57 yrs. Performance was adjusted by multiple regression procedures to control the effects of various conditions on original learning and rehearsal of the material. Results show that identification and matching of names and faces remained approximately 90% correct for at least 15 yrs, even for members of very large classes. Free recall was independent of class size and recognition memory, and declined with negative acceleration by 60% during 48 yrs. This very slow forgetting process is attributed to distribution of practice and overlearning effects far more extreme than those observed in laboratory research. The decline of free recall is associated with the forgetting of the nature of the interpersonal relationship which serves as a retrieval mediator. (33 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Thesis
http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/null/1/6614561.pdf
Article
Peer Reviewed http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/32209/1/0000268.pdf
Handbook of experimental psychology
  • C I Hovland
Hovland, C. I. Human learning and retention. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951.
Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague
  • A Julliand
  • E Rodriguez
Julliand, A. & Rodriguez, E. Frequency dictionary of Spanish words. The Hague, Holland: Mouten, 1964.