ArticlePublisher preview available

Information processing, situation specificity, and the generality of risk-taking behavior

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Constructed 2 structurally similar risk-taking tasks to evaluate intertask consistency of individual differences. Only the mode of response differed between tasks. A total of 92 college students served as Ss. In 1 task, Ss chose their preferred bet within each of a number of pairs of bets. In the other, Ss set selling prices for these same bets. A measure of S's preference for "long-shot" gambles was obtained from each response. Reliable individual differences were found for each measure. However, the intermeasure correlation was relatively low considering the high degree of similarity between tasks. It is argued that the 2 response modes triggered different methods of processing information about probabilities and payoffs in a way that perturbed individual differences and reduced intertask consistency. Information-processing considerations may be one important component of the situation specificity prevalent in risk-taking behavior. Results suggest that high correlations are unlikely between risk-taking measures in structurally different settings or between risk-taking and other behaviors. (24 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
of
Personality
and
Social Psychology
1972,
Vol.
22, No. 1,
128-134
INFORMATION
PROCESSING,
SITUATION
SPECIFICITY,
AND
THE
GENERALITY
OF
RISK-TAKING
BEHAVIOR1
PAUL SLOVIC
2
Oregon
Research
Institute
An
effort
was
made
to
construct
two
structurally similar risk-taking tasks
in
order
to
evaluate
intertask consistency
of
individual differences. Only
the
mode
of
response
differed
between tasks.
In one
task, subjects chose their preferred
bet
within each
of a
number
of
pairs
of
bets.
In the
other, they
set
selling
prices
for
these same bets.
A
measure
of the
subject's preference
for
"long-shot"
gambles
was
obtained
from
each
response. Reliable individual
differences
were
found
for
each measure. However,
the
intermeasure correlation
was
relatively
low
considering
the
high degree
of
similarity
between
tasks.
It is
argued
that
the two
response modes triggered
different
methods
of
processing information
about probabilities
and
payoffs
in a way
that perturbed individual
differences
and
reduced
intertask
consistency. Information-processing considerations
may
be one
important component
of the
situation
specificity
prevalent
in risk-taking
behavior.
These results imply that high correlations
are
unlikely between risk-
taking measures
in
structurally
different
settings
or
between risk taking
and
other
behaviors.
Kogan
and
Wallach
(1967)
posed
a
ques-
tion
fundamental
to our
understanding
of
risk-taking behavior: "How important
are
individual
consistencies relative
to
situational
constraints when
it
comes
to
predicting some-
one's
inclination
for
risk
or
conservatism?
[p.
208]."
Slovic
(1962)
found
little correlation
among several
different
kinds
of
risk-taking
measures administered
to the
same subjects.
This
result
implied
that
most
of the
variation
among individuals
was
situationally deter-
mined,
and it
casts
doubt upon
the
existence
of
a
general
trait
of
risk-taking propensity.
Since then, this failure
to find
transsituational
generality
for
individual differences
in
risk
taking
has
been replicated
in
studies
by
Bfichacek
(1968), Flanders (1970), Good-
1
This research
was
supported
by the
Personnel
and
Training Research Programs, Psychological Sci-
ences
Division,
Office
of
Naval Research, under con-
tract N00014-68-C-0431, Contract Authority
NR-
153-311,
and by
Grants
MH
12972
and MH
1S414
from
the
National
Institute
of
Mental Health.
The
author
is
indebted
to
Douglas
MacPhillamy
and
Barbara
Madigan
for
assistance
in
conducting
this
experiment
and
analyzing
the
results
and to
Robyn Dawes, Lewis Goldberg, Sarah Lichtenstein,
Douglas
MacPhillamy, Leonard Rorer,
and
Bernard
Weiner
for
their
comments
on the
manuscript.
2
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
Paul
Slovic,
Oregon Research Institute, P.O.
Box
3196,
Eugene, Oregon 97403.
man
(1970),
Greene (1963, 1964), Higbee
(1971),
Johnson (1963), Weinstein (1969),
and
Weinstein
and
Martin
(1969).
Some
investigators, such
as
Cohen
and
Christianson (1970), have reacted
to
this
negative experimental evidence with disbe-
lief,
arguing
that
it flies in the
face
of
per-
sonal experience that
attests
to the
coherence
of
individual behavior. Others have criticized
these studies
in
more
specific
ways
in an at-
tempt
to
explain
why
they failed
to find the
expected levels
of
generality.
One
criticism
is
that
they have failed
to
include
a
proper
sampling
of
risk-taking behaviors. However,
the
studies cited above have employed
a re-
markably thorough
assortment
of
risk-taking
tasks
in
problem-solving, athletic, social, voca-
tional,
and
pure gambling situations. Only
those
tasks
highly similar
in
structure
and
involving
the
same sorts
of
payoffs
(e.g.,
all
financial, all
social,
etc.)
have
shown
any
generality (e.g., Slakter, 1969), and,
as
simi-
larity decreases, these intertask correlations
rapidly decrease (Kogan
&
Wallach,
1964;
Weinstein, 1969).
Researchers
such
as
Flanders
(1970),
Ko-
gan and
Wallach (1964),
and
Slovic (1964)
have hypothesized
that
more generality would
exist
among
tasks
which provided real, rather
than hypothetical,
payoffs.
Higbee
(1971)
has
proposed
just
the
opposite—that
imagined
or
128
... Die Skala zur Erhebung des individuellen Widerstands gegenüber Veränderungen (WgV) konnte durch breit angelegte Korrelationsstudien weiter validiert werden. Signifikante Zusammenhänge bestanden zu Persönlichkeitseigenschaften wie: Ambiguitätstoleranz 10 (Budner, 1962), Sensationssuche (Zuckerman & Link, 1994), Dogmatismus (Rokeach,1960), Risikoaversion (Slovic, 1972) und Offenheit für Erfahrungen (Digman, 1990;Oreg et al., 2008). Personen, für welche eine hohe Persönlichkeitsakzentuierung bezüglich Dogmatismus und Neurotizismus oder wenig Offenheit für Erfahrung ermittelt werden konnte, wiesen eher einen höheren Widerstand gegenüber Änderungen auf (Oreg, 2003). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
To cope with the challenges of climate change, proactive changes in our habits of consumption are inevitable. Food consumption goes along with a large environmental impact, thus the food we buy yields substantial ecological saving potentials. Sustainable nutrition necessitates a certain willingness to change on side of the consumer, therefore the consumer shares a certain amount of responsibility regarding the market equilibrium. The Resistance to Change (RTC) scale makes a person's refusal towards changes measurable but has yet not been used in the context of nutrition. This thesis examines to what extent the individual resistance to changes correlates with aspects of sustainable nutrition. Regarding operationalization, this study seeks to develop a suitable questionnaire based on theoretical background and optimizes a german version of the RTC scale. To answer the research question, statements of 118 german-speaking adults, collected by an online survey, are statistically analysed with the aid of correlation analysis. Results show, that some aspects of sustainable nutrition-as consumption of animal products-yield weak coherences with RTC. In contrast, there is no relation with organic foods. Moreover, with small effect size, there is a significant negative correlation of the packaging variable and resistance variable observable. Thus, it is assumed that with pronounced RTC, there is less heed to eco-friendly packaging. Also, participants consume less animal but more ecological products, when they stated to be mindful of eco-friendly packaging. The results highlight the assumptions of previous research, that individual-related attributes are of importance regarding sustainable behaviour. For further research, the examination of a potential correlation of environmentally relevant moral values and RTC seems promising. Moreover, the question how far awareness for sustainable diets, paired with willingness to behaviour change, has impact on the individual eating habits, remains auspicious.
... Byrnes et al., 1999;Hanoch et al., 2006;Slovic, 1972;Weber et al., 2002), recent empirical economic studies have challenged this to suggest that individuals' risk preferences are domain-specific (Beauchamp et al., 2017;Dohmen et al., 2011). They may be altered by large exogenous shocks, such as natural disasters (Eckel et al., 2009), financial crises (Sahm, 2012) and traumatic events from conflicts (e.g., Bozzoli et al., 2011;Jakiela & Ozier, 2019;Voors et al., 2012) etc. ...
Article
Full-text available
A popular uprising in 2014, led to a revolution that overthrew the sitting President of Burkina Faso. We investigate if individuals' risk attitudes changed due to this revolution. We examine this impact by the main determinants of risk attitudes: gender, age and level of education. The analysis is based on unique panel survey data, allowing us to track the changes in the risk attitudes of the same individuals before, during and after the revolution. Our results suggest that individuals become risk averse during the revolution but return back to their pre‐revolution risk attitudes, with a slight increase in their risk attitudes, after the revolution is over.
... DRTC is a relatively new construct in the change literature, yet it has become one of the more commonly studied person factors in the past decade (Fugate, 2013). DRTC "is related to, yet distinct from, other personality traits such as intolerance for ambiguity… risk aversion (Slovic, 1972), and openness to experience (Digman, 1990)" (Oreg et al., 2008, 936). This conceptualization departs from historical notions in which employee resistance is a dependent variable or reaction to change. ...
Article
Full-text available
This field study of a merger examines the antecedent factors and processes that explain two different forms of employee support for change-compliance and championing. Our overarching goal is to understand why some employees comply and others champion change efforts. We examine the combined effects of context and person factors on both positive and negative employee reactions to change, and then investigate the differential effects of these reactions on employee support for change. Results support our hypotheses and show that change management support (context factor) negatively predicts threat appraisals and positively predicts challenge appraisals. Both compliance and championing are positively predicted by challenge appraisals, and threat appraisals are negatively related to championing. Analyses also reveal that the positive relationship between change management support and challenge appraisal is stronger when dispositional resistance to change (person factor) is high. Moderated-mediation analyses suggest employees' compliance and championing for change are differentially affected by management actions, their own dispositional resistance, and that these effects are mediated through positive and negative appraisals.
... Risk aversion. Risk aversion was measured using the scale developed by Cable and Judge (1994), based on original scales developed by Slovic (1972) and Gomez-Mejia and Balkin (1989). Example items include, "I am not willing to take risks when choosing a job or a company to work for" and "I view risk of a job as a situation to be avoided at all costs." ...
Article
Full-text available
In a departure from the organizational development literature, this study hypothesized that managerial responses to organizational change are influenced by 7 dispositional traits (locus of control, generalized self-efficacy, self-esteem, positive affectivity, openness to experience, tolerance for ambiguity, and risk aversion). Data were collected from 6 organizations (N = 514) to test the hypotheses. The 7 traits were reduced to 2 factors: Positive Self-Concept and Risk Tolerance. Both of these trait factors significantly predicted self-reports and independent assessments of coping with change. Results also indicated that coping with organizational change was related to extrinsic (salary, job level, plateauing, job performance) and intrinsic (organizational commitment, job satisfaction) career outcomes and that coping mediated roughly half of the relationships between the dispositional factors and these career outcomes.
... Currently, available information corresponds to information that fulfills consumer needs, while additional information refers to information beyond consumer needs (i.e., supplementary information). Based on our conceptualization, when informed consumers transition from online search to offline evaluation, they focus their attention on a limited number of target-related and determinant attributes (Büttner et al., 2014;Chung et al., 2022) and base their decisions on salient current information (Frederick et al., 2009;Kahneman & Frederick, 2002;Slovic, 1972). Therefore, the type of information provided by a salesperson influences consumers' responses. ...
Article
Full-text available
Retailers hire salespeople in physical stores to boost revenue by influencing consumer decision‐making. However, the Internet provides consumers with a variety of online product information before they enter the physical store, and this rich online information reduces their willingness to interact with offline salespeople. Using regulatory focus theory, we explore why well‐informed consumers avoid sales interactions and explore strategies for salespeople to mitigate this negative effect. Across three studies, we show that high consumer informedness induces a heightened prevention focus that causes consumers to avoid sales interactions to control the decision‐making process by themselves (Studies 1 and 2). This effect can be alleviated when salespeople provide consumers with currently available information rather than additional information (Study 3). This research contributes to the literature on consumer informedness, regulatory focus theory, and research on promotional information types and provides guidance for retailers' sales interactions.
... Economists typically model risk through the curvature of a decision-makers utility function (Arrow, 1965;Pratt, 1964) and rely on incentivized behavioral measures to elicit revealed preferences (see, e.g., Charness et al., 2013). By contrast, psychologists commonly treat risk as a multi-faceted psychometric construct (see, e.g., Slovic, 1972;Fischhoff et al., 1978;Slovic et al., 1982) governed by intuitive judgments-risk perceptionsand tend to rely more on the stated-preference tradition. As such, many contributions to the economics literature treat risk preferences as the only determinant of risk-taking behavior and ignore risk perceptions; on the contrary, many contributions to the psychology literature take no account of preferences and treat perceptions as the driving force behind risky choices. ...
Article
Full-text available
In an online experiment with a sample of 4,287 managers from small- and medium-sized enterprises in Denmark, we present participants with scenario-dependent outcomes of a hypothetical investment prospect and elicit their perception of risk and their perception of the investment’s attractiveness (as a proxy for investment preferences). The experimental data is merged with a set of background variables on the company from the Danish registry which allows controlling for firm-specific effects. We find that risk perception is driven by the likelihood and the return associated with the worst-case scenario as well as the size of the required investment. Furthermore, we provide evidence that managers’ perception of the project’s attractiveness is significantly associated with their individual-level risk preferences and the interaction effect between risk preferences and risk perception. This implies that not only the characteristics of the different scenarios but also individuals’ risk preferences play an important role when assessing the attractiveness of a business opportunity.
Article
Full-text available
Although prior work has characterized creativity as a primarily agentic endeavor, we diverge from this perspective and argue for agentic and communal pathways to creativity that offer unique advantages to each gender. We draw from social role theory to predict that risk-taking and empathic tendencies—as agentic and communal mechanisms, respectively—help explain how gender influences creativity. We also identify contextual moderators that can strengthen the communal pathway—predicting a more positive relationship between empathic tendency and creativity as well as a stronger indirect effect via empathic tendency when the tasks demand perspective-taking and when usefulness is explicitly incorporated in creativity assessment. With a meta-analysis of 753 independent samples (265,762 individuals), we find support for a communal pathway (i.e., women are creative via empathic tendency) and for an agentic pathway (i.e., men are creative via risk-taking tendency). We also find that the communal pathway is stronger when usefulness is explicitly incorporated in creativity assessment. Task demands for perspective-taking did not show a moderating effect. Taken together, our findings provide a more balanced account of the gender–creativity relationship, demonstrate why men and women differ in creativity and when women can leverage the communal mechanism to enhance creativity, and inform theory and practice towards a more gender-equitable workplace.
Article
The 1‐in‐X numerical format (e.g., 1 in 200) has been found to increase subjective probability evaluations and behavioral intentions in hypothetical scenarios compared with the N‐in‐NX format (e.g., 5 in 1000). However, it remains unclear whether this format can also bias choices between truly incentivized options. In four online studies ( N = 1039), participants were presented with a small endowment (i.e., 1£) and an actual choice between two options: a sure loss of a part of such endowment and a lottery with the chance to lose the entire endowment, presented using either the 1‐in‐X or the N‐in‐NX format. In Studies 1–3, where the two options were equivalent in expected monetary value (EV) and the lottery was described with varying degrees of concreteness, participants preferred the lottery option to a lesser extent when the chance of losing the endowment was presented using the 1‐in‐X format compared with the N‐in‐NX format. The same effect was replicated in Study 4a when the lottery option had a higher EV than the sure loss, showing that the 1‐in‐X effect can also lead individuals to deviate from maximizing EV. However, the effect vanished in Study 4b when the difference in EV between the two options increased. Implications for risk communication and a possible interpretation of the results are discussed accordingly.
Chapter
Psychologists have been observing and interpreting economic behaviour for at least fifty years, and the last decade, in particular, has seen an escalated interest in the interface between psychology and economics. The Cambridge Handbook of Psychology and Economic Behaviour is a valuable reference resource dedicated to improving our understanding of the economic mind and economic behaviour. Employing empirical methods – including laboratory experiments, field experiments, observations, questionnaires and interviews – the Handbook covers aspects of theory and method, financial and consumer behaviour, the environment and biological perspectives. With contributions from distinguished scholars from a variety of countries and backgrounds, the Handbook is an important step forward in the improvement of communications between the disciplines of psychology and economics. It will appeal to academic researchers and graduates in economic psychology and behavioural economics.
Article
Full-text available
4 experiments with a total of 42 male and 48 female undergraduates and 48 female high school students, investigated the consistency of cooperative or competitive behavior of Ss in 2 different experimental situations. Ss who had behaved either extremely competitively or extremely cooperatively in a Game of Chicken or a Prisoner's Dilemma game were tested in 1 of 2 conditions: (a) a mixed-motive task where payoff was obtained through a series of steps which the other player could block (the Paddle Game), or (b) a picture-interpretation task involving story writing and discussion in dyads. In the Paddle Game, most Ss cooperated, but previous high competitors waited less often for their turn, were usually paid 1st, and in 1 experiment, earned more money. In the picture-interpretation task, few behavioral differences were found between cooperators and competitors, but pairs of competitors were in greater agreement in their story interpretations and portrayed more cynicism about interpersonal relationships than did others. Female cooperator pairs showed high agreement and low cynicism. This suggests that competitors may share certain views, different from cooperators, about interpersonal relationships. However, the findings do not justify broad generalizations from behavior in experimental games to other interpersonal situations. (26 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Examined D. Pruitt's (see pa, vol. 43:17351) finding on the choice-dilemmas questionnaire (cdq) that ss tend to make cautious decisions, but to view their decisions as being risky. He labeled this a "walter mitty" effect. In study i with 28 male and 28 female undergraduates, a possible methodological source of error was investigated. The walter mitty effect was replicated. Study ii with 120 male ss investigated a slightly different kind of "walter mitty-ness" the tendency to anticipate being riskier than one sees himself as having been after the fact. 2-man teams (assigned on the basis of similar cdq scores) participated in a complex decision-making task. Analysis of their behavior indicated that walter mitty-ness may be expressed in actual risk-taking behavior as well as hypothetical risk taking. It is suggested that the cdq (and other measures of hypothetical risk taking) might be viewed as a measure of a personality trait of walter mitty-ness. (16 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This paper outlines three ways of looking at decision processes, and relates them to problems of research management. A classification of decisions is suggested, based on three dimensions of complexity. On each dimension, innovative decisions are normally more complex than operating decisions, but since the latter are more common, there is a danger that the former will be inadequately treated, as shown in an example. These dangers may be mitigated, first, by regarding complex decision‐making as an explicitly cyclical process, and, second, by recognizing the significance of reference standards. Four types of reference standard—historical, external, planning, and imaginative—are noted, and brief consideration is given to their origins and to their influence on the perception not only of problems but also of solutions. It is argued that the range and quality of the reference standards used are likely to be significant factors in the success of research management.
Article
This study was addressed to the question of whether “need for novelty” is a unitary dimension. Six instruments—four attitude inventories and two performance tasks—designed to measure originality, change, sensation-seeking, stimulus-seeking, change-seeking, and cognitive innovation were administered to college Ss. Resulting intercorrelations indicated moderate overlap among attitudinal measures, some overlap between attitude inventories and a performance task with primarily conceptual demands, and no overlap between attitude measures and a performance task with primarily motor demands. Implications for theory and research are discussed.
Article
Intercorrelations were computed among measures of chance risk and interpersonal risk-taking for 120 undergraduate Ss. While low, correlations were in the hypothesized direction, with 6 of 20 exceeding the .05 level. The findings suggest some generality of willingness to take risks over both the material and interpersonal areas.
Article
INVESTIGATED CONSISTENCY IN THE AMOUNT OF INFORMATION SOUGHT PRIOR TO DECISION MAKING ON AN OPTIONAL STOPPING AND ON A DECISION LATENCY TASK. THE HYPOTHESIS THAT INDIVIDUALS WHO SCORE HIGH ON BOTH THE BYRNE REPRESSION-SENSITIZATION (R-S) SCALE AND ON THE RORSCHACH INDEX OF REPRESSIVE STYLE (RIRS) ("SENSITIZERS") DEMAND MORE INFORMATION PRIOR TO DECISION THAN INDIVIDUALS WHO SCORE LOW ON BOTH SCALES ("REPRESSORS") WAS ALSO TESTED. 4 GROUPS OF 20 SS, WHO HAD SCORED HIGH-HIGH (HH), HIGH-LOW (HL), LOW-HIGH (LH), AND LOW-LOW (LL) ON THE 2 TESTS, WERE USED. RESULTS WERE: (1) DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE HIGH AND LOW R-S AND THE HIGH AND LOW RIRS GROUPS ON THE 2 DECISION TASKS DID NOT ATTAIN STATISTICAL SIGNIFICANCE; (2) THE INTERACTION OF R-S * RIRS WAS SIGNIFICANT FOR BOTH TASKS WITH HH AND LL SS HAVING SHORTER DECISION LATENCIES AND MALE HH SS REQUIRING MORE OBSERVATIONS PRIOR TO DECISION; AND (3) THE HH AND LL GROUPS SHOWED GREATER CONSISTENCY ACROSS DECISION TASKS THAN THE LH AND HL GROUPS. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
6 groups of 8th graders received language and mathematics aptitude and achievement test items. Embedded in each of the 4 tests were 10 nonsense items. Risk-taking, examinee awareness of a penalty for guessing, was found to be general to risk behavior on 4 legitimate and 4 nonsense item sets. Risk-taking, however, tended to be inversely correlated with true aptitude or achievement. A factor analysis of the intercorrelations revealed 2 factors: risk and aptitude achievement. Ss low in risk-taking raised their scores when forced to respond to all items in a test. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)