ArticlePublisher preview available

Effect of choice on paired-associate learning

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Conducted 3 paired-associate learning experiments with a total of 120 male and female undergraduates. Under some conditions, a-b learning was facilitated when ss chose responses to be learned on a subsequent a-b list. Ss who chose their a-b responses and were subsequently forced to learn a competing set of material (a-c) showed a relatively greater disruption of learning than ss who did not have the opportunity to choose either a-b or a-c. All of these effects required that when ss chose their responses, the choosing occurred in the presence of their respective stimuli. Simply choosing responses in the absence of the stimuli produced performance which was not different from that resulting when ss were denied the opportunity to choose their responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal
ol
Experimental Psychology
1971,
Vol.
91, No. 1,
47-53
EFFECT
OF
CHOICE
ON
PAIRED-ASSOCIATE
LEARNING1
LAWRENCE
C.
PERLMUTER
2
RICHARD
A.
MONTY
Bawdoin
College
United
States
Army
Human
Engineering
Laboratories,
Aberdeen
Proving
Ground, Maryland
AND
GREGORY
A.
KIMBLE
University
of
Colorado
One
experiment revealed that
under
some
conditions,
A-B
learning
is
facilitated
when
5s
choose responses
to be
learned
on a
subsequent
A-B
list.
There
is
also
some
evidence
that
Ss who
choose
their
A-B
responses
and are
subsequently
forced
to
learn
a
competing
set of
material
(A-C)
show
a
rela-
tively
greater
disruption
of
learning
than
5s who did not
have
the
opportunity
to
choose
either
A-B or
A-C.
All of
these
effects
require
that
when
5s
choose
their
responses, this choosing occurs
in the
presence
of
their respective stimuli.
Simply
choosing responses
in the
absence
of the
stimuli
produces
performance
which
is not
different
from
that
resulting
when
5s are
denied
the
opportunity
to
choose
their
responses.
The
effect
of
giving
5s
the
opportunity
to
choose
the
materials they
wish
to
learn
in
a
paired-associates (PA) paradigm
is a
complex, relatively unexplored area.
Nu-
merous
questions arise
in
this context, only
a few of
which
will
be
examined here.
First,
it
seems intuitively reasonable
to
assume
that
if 5s are
permitted
to
choose
the
response
items
to be
learned
in an A-B
list,
their
performance should
be
superior
to
those
5s who
have been similarly exposed
to
alternative
materials
but
required
to
learn
"forced"
S-R
pairs
(i.e.,
S-R
pairs
not of
their
own
choosing), either
because
of
the
opportunity
for
5 to
form
associations
particularly
suited
to him or as a
result
of
a
general enhancement
of the
motivational
state
of the
organism.
While
no
unambiguous
tests
have been
made
of
this
question,
Postman's
(1968)
1
This
research
was
supported
in
part
by
Grant
MH
15315
from
the
National
Institute
of
Mental
Health
to the
senior
author
and by
Grant
8-9-1502
75-
4005(010)
from
the
Office
of
Education
to the
third
author. Reproduction
for any
purpose
of the
United
States
Government
is
permitted.
Experi-
ment
I was
conducted
while
the
senior
author
served
as
visiting
professor
at the
Behavioral
Re-
search
Laboratory
of the
Human
Engineering
Laboratories. Special thanks
to
Susan
Luek
for
running
5s of
Exp.
I and to
Margie Rosenberger
for
running
5s of
Exp.
II and
III. Thanks also
to
Frank
Keefe
for his
assistnce
in
data
reduction.
2
Requests
for
reprints
should
be
sent
to
Lawrence
C.
Perlmuter,
Department
of
Psychology,
Bowdoin
College,
Brunswick, Maine
04011.
review
of
related work suggests
the
evi-
dence
to
date seems
to be
contrary
to
this
intuitive
reasoning.
For
example, Under-
wood,
Ham,
and
Ekstrand (1962) revealed
that
learning
of
preferred
and
nonpreferred
materials proceeded
at the
same
rate.
In
a
more direct
test,
Brown
and
Read
(1970)
found
no
difference between
free
(choice)
and
yoked (forced)
5s
with
either
total
trials
or
total
errors
to a
learning
criterion.
However,
the
free
5s
were allowed
to
select
and
change their pairings throughout
the
learning
of the
list
while
the
yoked
5s
learned only
the two final
lists
selected
by
the
free
5s.
The
main purpose
of the
present
study,
then,
is to
directly
assess
the
effect
of
choos-
ing
the A-B
responses prior
to
learning
the
A-B
list.
Will
5s who
choose responses
to be
learned
on A-B and who are
subsequently
forced
to
learn
an A-C
list perform
as
well
on
A-C as
those
who
choose neither
A-B
nor
A-C,
but
rather
are
forced
to
learn both
sets
of
materials?
If A-B
bonds
are
stronger
in the
choice condition,
it
seems
reasonable
to
predict
they
might produce
greater interference
later.
This
is the
second major question examined
in the
present article.
Third,
if 5s are
permitted
to
choose
their
responses
for the A-B
list,
but
choose
in
the
absence
of the
stimulus, will
the
pre-
47
... Similarly, psychological researchers have long suggested the positive influence of choice on behavioural performances such as learning and memory tasks (Iyengar & Lepper, 1999;Perlmuter, Monty & Kimble, 1971;Setogawa, Mizuhiki, Matsumoto, Akizawa & Shidara, 2014). In a recent study, Setogawa et al. (2014) suggested self-choice was critical in that when a participant chooses a task rather than being instructed to complete it, that task's value might be enhanced. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although patient-preferred live music (PPLM) is a frequently utilised receptive music therapy intervention, a neurological rationale for this treatment does not yet exist. The current paper reviews existing literature and proposes several potential neurologic rationales for PPLM as a receptive music therapy intervention for neurotypical adult patient populations. Additionally, the authors discuss gaps in the current research and make suggestions for further inquiries. The term ‘patient-preferred live music’ is parsed into four separate components: music, familiarity/preference, choice/autonomy, and live performance. The authors searched relevant neuroscience and music therapy literature to find research concerning each of these components. Results indicated extensive neuroscience research regarding the brain’s neurologic response to music, mostly pertaining to the reward system and the process of dopamine release. Additionally, the authors found evidence to suggest that exposure to familiar stimuli and the act of making a choice may both be neurologically reinforcing. Research regarding the mirror neuron system may be a vital entry point from which to begin investigating the live and social aspects of PPLM. Further music-specific and neuroscience research is required to confirm these hypotheses. While various researchers have investigated individual components of PPLM, there is a lack of basic music therapy and neuroscience research regarding the paradigm as a whole. Further investigation is warranted.
... The most unexpected of these findings, however, were the effects of control on the accuracy of responding. Although Monty and Perlmuter (1975) reported enhanced accuracy in a paired-associates learning task by humans, it is somewhat surprising that comparable effects were obtained with relatively simple task demands such as pursuit tracking and matching-to-sample. Yet the performance values obtained in these tasks in the SEL condition were substantially better than performance in the NSEL condition. ...
Article
Full-text available
This investigation was designed to determine whether perceived control effects found in humans extend to rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) tested in a video-task format, using a computer-generated menu program, SELECT. Choosing one of the options in SELECT resulted in presentation of 5 trials of a corresponding task and subsequent return to the menu. In Experiments 1–3, the animals exhibited stable, meaningful response patterns in this task (i.e., they made choices). In Experiment 4, performance on tasks that were selected by the animals significantly exceeded performance on identical tasks when assigned by the experimenter under comparable conditions (e.g., time of day, order, variety). The reliable and significant advantage for performance on selected tasks, typically found in humans, suggests that rhesus monkeys were able to perceive the availability of choices.
... In addition to being an important antecedent of feelings of autonomy, subsequent motivational and performance benefi ts of choice may lie in its ability to support the perception or experience of having control (Langer, 1975;Rotter, 1966;Taylor, 1989) and the need for competence (Cordova & Lepper, 1996;Henry, 1994;Henry & Sniezek, 1993;Katz & Assor, 2007;Monty & Perlmuter, 1987;Patall et al., 2008;Perlmuter & Monty, 1977;Perlmuter, Scharff , Karsh, & Monty, 1980;Tafarodi, Milne & Smith, 1999;Tafarodi, Mehranvar, Panton, & Milne, 2002). Based on a series of studies showing that choice of either stimulus or response words in a paired-associates memory task led to enhanced performance outcomes (Monty & Perlmuter, 1975;Monty, Rosenberger, & Perlmuter, 1973;Perlmuter, Monty, & Kimble, 1971), Perlmuter and Monty (1977) argued that the performance benefi ts of choice are a result of increased motivation in the form of enhanced perceived control and subsequent increased arousal and cognitive engagement with the task (Monty & Perlmuter, 1987;Perlmuter et al., 1980). Similarly, Henry (1994;Henry & Sniezek, 1993) argued that the benefi ts of choice lie in its ability to increase perceived control and subsequent self-effi cacy. ...
Chapter
Motivation is that which moves us to action. Human motivation is thus a complex issue, as people are moved to action by both their evolved natures and by myriad familial, social, and cultural influences. The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation aims to capture the current state-of-the-art in this fast developing field. The book includes theoretical overviews from some of the best-known thinkers in this area, including articles on Social Learning Theory, Control Theory, Self-determination Theory, Terror Management Theory, and the Promotion and Prevention perspective. Topical articles appear on phenomena such as ego-depletion, flow, curiosity, implicit motives, and personal interests. A section specifically highlights goal research, including chapters on goal regulation, achievement goals, the dynamics of choice, unconscious goals and process versus outcome focus. Still other articles focus on evolutionary and biological underpinnings of motivation, including articles on cardiovascular dynamics, mood, and neuropsychology. Finally, articles bring motivation down to earth in reviewing its impact within relationships, and in applied areas such as psychotherapy, work, education, sport, and physical activity.
... Prior research has shown that when participants make choices about when and what to learn, memory for the chosen information is often enhanced, leading to a "choice effect" DuBrow et al., 2019;Gureckis & Markant, 2012;Rotem-Turchinski et al., 2019). For example, letting participants select cues or targets during paired-associate learning can improve cued recall (e.g., Monty & Permuter 1975;Perlmuter et al., 1971; see also Watanabe & Soraci 2004) as does honoring participants' choices about what to restudy (Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006). Additionally, allowing participants to make decisions regarding aspects of learning such as presentation order or duration can benefit memory Murty et al., 2015Murty et al., , 2019Voss et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Predicting what we will remember and forget is crucial for daily functioning. We were interested in whether evaluating something as likely to be remembered or forgotten leads to enhanced memory for both forms of information relative to information that was not judged for memorability. We presented participants with lists of words to remember for a later test and on each list, participants were asked to identify some words that they were confident that they would remember and some words that they believed that they were most likely to forget on the test. Relative to words not given a prediction, memory was enhanced for words participants selected as likely to be remembered but also for words participants indicated were most likely to be forgotten. We also examined whether requiring participants to engage in self-cued directed forgetting by selecting a subset of words to be remembered or forgotten produced a memory advantage for these words. Results again demonstrated enhanced memory for selected words regardless of whether they were designated as to-be-remembered or to-be-forgotten. Thus, we demonstrate a reactivity type of effect such that when participants are asked to select certain items, this process can enhance memory regardless of the reason for selecting the item, potentially arising as a result of these words becoming more distinct and/or receiving additional processing. As such, the present results are consistent with the richness of encoding and metacognition modifying attention to cues accounts of reactivity.
... While several studies have begun to assess the influence of volition, whether or not volition affects memory performance cannot be determined from these studies because too few participants chose to deceive and were excluded (Romeo et al., 2019), because volitional deception was not compared with an instructed control group (Li et al., 2022), or because volitional liars were combined with instructed liars in final analyses (Hudson et al., 2019). In the education domain, memory is enhanced when individuals can exert control over their learning environment, including decisions regarding what information is encoded (Perlmuter et al., 1971). Selection may be strategic such that individuals choose items to receive further study that are easier to remember, which can benefit them on a later memory test (Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
General Audience Summary It is common for liars to provide statements on multiple occasions, such as during a criminal investigation. Therefore, it is important to understand not only what people think about their ability to remember their lies (and truths) over time but also what effect lying has on a person’s memory for what they experienced. Research on the relationship between lying and memory has also often failed to account for individual choice in when to lie and tell the truth. The present study was conducted to address these issues. Participants performed a series of simple actions with objects and then either lied or told the truth about actions either by denying that they had performed the action, or by describing how they performed the action. In Experiment 1, people repeatedly lied or told the truth for some actions. In Experiment 2, some people were allowed to choose which actions they would lie or tell the truth about. We found that people think they will remember the truth better, and telling the truth was associated with better memory performance for whether actions were performed and how they had been talked about previously, regardless of whether people could choose when to lie and tell the truth. People also tended to overestimate their ability to remember when they had denied performing an action. These findings suggest that individuals who choose to lie by denying may increase the odds of their statements being inconsistent across multiple statements, and that motivation to lie might be more important to the relationship between lying and memory than choosing when to lie.
Article
Full-text available
The directed forgetting paradigm has long been used to test whether humans can voluntarily choose to forget learned information. However, to date, nearly all directed forgetting paradigms have involved a forced-choice paradigm, in which the participants are instructed about which learned information they should forget. While studies have repeatedly shown that this directed forgetting does lead to a decreased ability to later remember the information, it is still unclear whether these effects would be present if participants were allowed to, of their own accord, choose which information they wanted to forget. In two experiments here, we introduce a free-choice variety of the item method directed forgetting paradigm and show that directed forgetting effects are robust, both for instructed and voluntary forgetting. We discuss the implications of our findings for notions of voluntary forgetting and for the self-choice effect in memory.
Chapter
This volume demonstrates how readers can become more effective parents, teachers, students, coaches, managers, or work supervisors, while also gaining practical skills to enhance their self-motivation, communication skills, and intervention acumen. The first eight chapters explain evidence-based principles from applied behavioral science (ABS) that can be used to improve the human dynamics of any situation involving behavior. Fundamentals from humanism are integrated strategically to show how an ABS intervention can be more acceptable, influential, and sustainable. The following twelve chapters detail the deployment of ABS interventions to optimize performance in a wide variety of fields, including occupational and transportation safety, quantity and quality of organizational work behavior, healthcare, athletic coaching, parenting, pre-school and college education, environmental sustainability, and the control of obesity and alcohol abuse. Applied Psychology provides a thorough review of the latest research in relation to these domains and explores issues for future investigation.
Chapter
This volume demonstrates how readers can become more effective parents, teachers, students, coaches, managers, or work supervisors, while also gaining practical skills to enhance their self-motivation, communication skills, and intervention acumen. The first eight chapters explain evidence-based principles from applied behavioral science (ABS) that can be used to improve the human dynamics of any situation involving behavior. Fundamentals from humanism are integrated strategically to show how an ABS intervention can be more acceptable, influential, and sustainable. The following twelve chapters detail the deployment of ABS interventions to optimize performance in a wide variety of fields, including occupational and transportation safety, quantity and quality of organizational work behavior, healthcare, athletic coaching, parenting, pre-school and college education, environmental sustainability, and the control of obesity and alcohol abuse. Applied Psychology provides a thorough review of the latest research in relation to these domains and explores issues for future investigation.
Chapter
This volume demonstrates how readers can become more effective parents, teachers, students, coaches, managers, or work supervisors, while also gaining practical skills to enhance their self-motivation, communication skills, and intervention acumen. The first eight chapters explain evidence-based principles from applied behavioral science (ABS) that can be used to improve the human dynamics of any situation involving behavior. Fundamentals from humanism are integrated strategically to show how an ABS intervention can be more acceptable, influential, and sustainable. The following twelve chapters detail the deployment of ABS interventions to optimize performance in a wide variety of fields, including occupational and transportation safety, quantity and quality of organizational work behavior, healthcare, athletic coaching, parenting, pre-school and college education, environmental sustainability, and the control of obesity and alcohol abuse. Applied Psychology provides a thorough review of the latest research in relation to these domains and explores issues for future investigation.
Article
Full-text available
It is argued that the traditional distinction between verbal learning and retention is arbitrary and that these processes cannot be differentiated. Although traditional operations have been used to distinguish between the 2 constructs, a whole host of more recent operations have served to break down the distinction which was traditionally used. As a result, even the arbitrary distinction between learning and retention which was once utilized is now no longer appropriate. It is suggested the term memory be substituted for learning and retention and that the use of this construct would provide several advantages. (french summary) (23 ref.) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Word-color or trigram-color compound stimuli were used with numbers as responses in paired-associate learning. This was followed by a transfer test in which one or the other components alone was presented as the stimulus. Color showed greater transfer than the trigram stimulus apparently due to its higher meaningfulness. Words produced greater transfer than colors possibly due to a bias toward dealing with verbal material rather than greater meaningfulness. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2006 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Transfer in A-B, A-D and A-B, A-Br paradigms was compared under conditions where stimulus-response pairings were (a) determined randomly by E; (b) constructed freely by S on the basis of associative organization; or (c) yoked-control pairings. In free pairings, each S paired items on each trial during learning without knowledge of pairing from previous trials. Negative transfer and unlearning of first-list associations was found when the pairings were random. Under free- and yoked-pairing conditions, however, there were shifts to positive transfer, and no unlearning occurred. It was concluded that negative transfer and forgetting are not necessary consequences within A-D and A-Br paradigms when Ss impose organization on the list.
Continued word association scaling of stimulus meaningfulness (m) and common response probabilities (p) for standardized verbal items
  • B G Andreas
ANDREAS, B. G. Continued word association scaling of stimulus meaningfulness (m) and common response probabilities (p) for standardized verbal items. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, April 1967.
A multi-purpose analysis of variance FORTRAN IV computer program
  • D H Butler
  • A S Kamlet
  • R A Monty
BUTLER, D. H., KAMLET, A. S., & MONTY, R. A. A multi-purpose analysis of variance FORTRAN IV computer program. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1969, 2, 301-319.
Association and performance in the analysis of verbal learning
  • L Postman
POSTMAN, L. Association and performance in the analysis of verbal learning. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.), Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968.