ArticlePublisher preview available

Effects of Changes in Relative Cue Strength on Context-Dependent Recognition

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Theoretical analyses and empirical studies address the issue of how context-dependent recognition is affected by changes in the relative strength of retrieval cues. Analyses of global memory models based on K. Murnane and M. P. Phelps' (1994) general context model showed that if context strength is held constant, context effects are predicted to either increase or remain unchanged when item strength increases. In contrast, the outshining hypothesis (S. M. Smith, 1988, 1994) predicts that context effects will decrease as item strength increases. Three studies are reported in which item strength was manipulated with spaced repetitions, study time, or a levels-of-processing manipulation. The results support the general context model. Implications for the outshining hypothesis and for global memory models are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1995,
Vol. 21, No.
1,158-172
Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0278-7393/95/S3.00
Effects of Changes in Relative Cue Strength
on Context-Dependent Recognition
Kevin Murnane and Matthew P. Phelps
Pennsylvania State University
Theoretical analyses and empirical studies address the issue of how context-dependent recognition
is affected by changes in the relative strength of retrieval cues. Analyses of global memory models
based on K. Murnane and M. P. Phelps' (1994) general context model showed that if context
strength is held constant, context effects are predicted to either increase or remain unchanged
when item strength increases. In contrast, the outshining hypothesis (S. M. Smith, 1988, 1994)
predicts that context effects will decrease as item strength increases. Three studies are reported in
which item strength was manipulated with spaced repetitions, study time, or a levels-of-processing
manipulation. The results support the general context model. Implications for the outshining
hypothesis and for global memory models are discussed.
Almost all theories of memory propose that memory perfor-
mance is sensitive to the relative memory strength of the
information in the cues used to probe memory during a
retrieval task. In this article, we use Murnane and Phelps'
(1994) general context model to explore the effects of changes
in relative cue strength within global activation systems of
recognition. Several widely cited studies have failed to find an
effect on recognition of changes in context between learning
and test. A prominent explanation of this failure holds that
context dependence is not always observed in recognition
because the difference in the relative memory strengths of the
item and context information in the typical recognition test cue
masks the effects of changes in context between learning and
test. We demonstrate the limited conditions under which
global activation theories predict this masking effect and
present three studies in which no evidence of masking was
found. We also show that current versions of specific global
activation theories are capable of predicting the effects of
changes in relative cue strength on context-dependent recogni-
tion that were observed in the studies.
A fundamental problem that any theory of memory must
solve is how people are able to access or retrieve an individual
episode from among the many, often similar, episodes that are
stored in memory. Many current theories seek to solve this
problem with mechanisms that link the probability of success-
ful retrieval of an episode to the degree to which the informa-
tion in the episode matches, or is similar to, the information in
the retrieval cue (Tulving,
1983;
Tulving & Thompson, 1973).
In many theories, (mis)matched context information is seen as
Kevin Murnane and Matthew P. Phelps, Department of Psychology,
Pennsylvania State University.
This research was supported in part by Grant SBR-9319549 from the
National Science Foundation and a research initiation grant from
Pennsylvania State University. We thank Douglas Hintzman and
Steven M. Smith for their helpful comments on a draft of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Kevin Murnane, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State Uni-
versity, 519 Moore Building, University Park, Pennsylvania 16802.
Electronic mail may be sent via Internet to kxm20@psuvm.psu.edu.
especially important in the retrieval of individual episodes.
Context information
is
usually thought of as information that is
present in the processing environment, either at encoding or at
retrieval, and that
is
peripheral to or incidental to the cognitive
task being performed. For example, suppose a reader were
trying to retrieve the contents of this article several days after
having read it. The content of the article is considered to be
item or target information. Context information that was
encoded along with the item information might include details
about the room in which the paper was read (environmental
context), relevant or irrelevant information retrieved from
memory when reading the paper (semantic context), and
information concerning the mood (emotional context) or
physiological condition (physiological context) while reading
the paper. Because the unique episode of reading the paper is
stored in memory as an amalgam of item and context informa-
tion, the probability of successful retrieval of the sought-after
item information from the episode can be enhanced by
including the appropriate context information in the retrieval
cue.
The match between context information at learning and at
retrieval has been identified as an important factor determin-
ing retrieval of a specific learning episode (Gillund & Shiffrin,
1984;
Humphreys, Bain, & Pike, 1989), and mismatched
context information at learning and at retrieval has been
identified as an important cause of forgetting (Bjork &
Richardson-Klavehn, 1989; Mensink & Raaijmakers, 1988,
1989;
Raaijmakers
&
Shiffrin,
1981;
Smith, 1988).
The idea that context information plays an important role in
the retrieval of individual episodes leads to a straightforward
prediction: Testing memory in an environmental context that is
different from the learning context should have a negative
effect on performance. Studies that have tested memory with
either free or cued recall have generally verified this prediction
(although see Fernandez & Glenberg, 1985, for a notable
exception). However, previous studies that have explored
context-dependent recognition have produced conflicting re-
sults (see Smith, 1988, for a review of both the recognition and
recall literature). Testing in an environmental context that is
different from the learning context has affected recognition
performance in a number of studies (Geiselman
&
Bjork, 1980;
158
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Although there is some evidence for this account-for example, the finding of large context effects with free recall, intermediate effects with cued recall, and small or absent effects with recognition (e.g., Godden & Baddeley, 1980;Smith, Glenberg, & Bjork, 1978)-there is also contradictory evidence. For example, Murnane and Phelps (1995) parametrically increased trace strength by increasing the number of times an item was presented at study. From an outshining perspective, as item strength increases, then the detrimental effect of changed context should decrease, as context information becomes an increasingly redundant source of retrieval information. ...
... Rather than diminishing effects of context being due to redundancy of context information as cue strength increases, they are due to decreasing reliance on recollective processes in memory performance with such increases in cue strength. Another implication of this is that outshining, as a description rather than a mechanism, should occur only with increases in cue strength, and not with increases in trace strength (Dougal & Rotello, 1999;Murnane & Phelps, 1995). Specifically, increasing trace strength by repeated presentations of study items would not exhibit outshining, because, if anything, those repeated presentations are likely to lead to an increase in the extent to which recognition may be accompanied by recollection by increasing the opportunity for elaborative processing at study (Gardiner et al., 1994;Macken & Hampson, 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
Evidence for effects of changed environmental context on recognition has been equivocal. Using 3 experiments, the author investigated the role of environmental context from a dual-processing approach. Experiment 1 showed that testing word recognition in a novel context led to a reliable decrement but only for recognition accompanied by conscious recollection, with familiarity-based recognition judgments being unaffected. This was replicated in Experiment 2 using stimuli that were novel to the participants (nonwords). Experiment 3 showed that the decrement in recollection also occurred when the changed-context condition involved presenting items in a different but familiar context. The results suggest that effects of environmental context will only be found when recognition is accompanied by conscious recollection and that this effect is due to a specific item–context association.
... The results of the present experiment are in line with the predictions of the cuing-withcontext account of context reinstatement effects in recognition. They demonstrate that while context reinstatement does affect recognition processes -an effect that is often difficult to obtain when only recognition performance is considered (Hockley, 2008;Murnane & Phelps, 1993;1994;1995;Smith et al., 1978) -this effect is larger when contexts are individually paired with single study items than when contexts are shared across multiple study items. Such a role of context load is consistent with context being used as an independent cue in recognition tests (Macken, 2002;Rutherford, 2004), revealing the role of the cue-overload principle (Watkins & Watkins, 1975): ...
Article
Full-text available
Context in which events are embedded is often hypothesized to serve as an independent cue for retrieval. This means that any effects of context need to obey two basic principles of cue-dependent memory: Memory retrieval should be augmented when, first, encoding context is reinstated and, second, this context uniquely specifies individual items stored in memory. Both of these regularities are well supported for recall tests, but they remain contentious in recognition tests. Here, in three experiments, we assess whether unique and nonunique contexts affect memory processes when reinstated during recognition. However, rather than focusing on measures of recognition performance, we looked at confidence judgments collected during recognition that should be particularly sensitive to recollective effects resulting from context cuing. Experiments 1 and 2, using old/new and forced-choice recognition tests, respectively, documented positive effects of context reinstatement on confidence in correct recognition identifications, but only for contexts uniquely associated with individual items. These effects emerged even when there were no reliable context effects in recognition performance measures. Experiment 3 showed the same effect of context reinstatement, moderated by context load, when spontaneous recognition of a previous study episode occurred during restudy. These results demonstrate the role of context as an independent retrieval cue both in deliberate and spontaneous recognition.
... The usage of such scenes to manipulate exposure is in line with behavioral and neuroimaging studies which have shown that it is likely to induce similar responses as traumatic events (e.g., Coan & Allen, 2007;Glauser & Scherer, 2008;Grandjean, Sander, & Scherer, 2008;Hartikainen, Ogawa, & Knight, 2000;Okon-Singer et al., 2014;Vuilleumier, 2005). Furthermore, the nature of the training aimed to reflect a core characteristic that differentiates firefighters and CSI police in their job requirements, based on the different type of skills that are associated with each of these professions: focusing on context vs details in the scene (see Murnane & Phelps, 1993, 1995Rutherford, 2004 for similar manipulations). We postulated that in line with previous studies, individuals would demonstrate an equal capacity to learn and retain positive and negative stimulus-outcome associations, independent of exposure. ...
Thesis
Previous research shows an inconsistent relationship between trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms; while some individuals develop symptoms following trauma, others show resilience. This study proposes an innovative predictor and moderator of PTSD symptoms, namely, flexibility–that is, the ability to flexibly and efficiently alternate between different behavioral responses based on situational demands. Five studies were conducted to address critical gaps on this subject. Study 1 compared the predictive value of different types of flexibility on PTSD symptom development following trauma in college students. It is the first study to show a stronger predictive relationship between cognitive flexibility– the ability to recognize and implement possible responses to a situation– as compared to regulatory flexibility–the ability to modulate emotional experience across situations– and PTSD symptoms. Study 2 tested the effect of the exposure to traumatic stimuli on cognitive flexibility performance in college students. The findings reveal reductions in a specific domain of cognitive flexibility for those who were exposed to traumatic versus neutral stimuli, providing a proof of concept to the assumed link between trauma exposure and impairments in cognitive flexibility. Study 3 examined the associations between trauma, cognitive flexibility performance in neutral and traumatic situations, and PTSD symptoms. The study demonstrates a link between impaired cognitive flexibility in the context of traumatic content and PTSD symptoms in traumatized firefighters, while innovatively investigating cognitive flexibility in the context of neutral and traumatic content. Study 4 explored the interactive effect of traumatic exposure and impaired cognitive flexibility on the tendency to develop either depression or PTSD symptoms. It extends previous investigations by including depression as a secondary trauma-outcome in traumatized college students. The study reveals that distinct impairments of cognitive flexibility interact with trauma exposure to predict differences in symptom development (depression versus PTSD). Study 5 examined whether impairments in cognitive flexibility moderate the relationship between early adversity and PTSD symptoms following additional adult trauma. The study highlights the interactive role of early adversity and impaired cognitive flexibility in predicting PTSD symptoms in firefighters with additional exposure to trauma in adulthood, generalizing the possible benefit of improved cognitive flexibility for individuals experiencing trauma at different life periods. This thesis demonstrates that – across multiple contexts – impaired flexibility is a vulnerability factor for PTSD symptoms, accounting for differences in trauma responses. These consistent findings suggest that improving individuals’ flexibility may promote their mental health following trauma.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of the current study was to examine the influence of scent-based context reinstatement on eyewitness identification and recall accuracy. Participants (N = 184) attended two sessions. In the first session, participants viewed a video of a staged, non-violent theft and were asked to recall details of the crime and perpetrator. Participants returned for the second session one week later where they again completed the recall form and a photographic lineup task. In each session, participants were randomly exposed to either a vanilla scent or no scent. It was hypothesized that a match in scent at both sessions would elicit the greatest identification accuracy. A trend was found for a higher correct identification rate and a higher correct rejection rate when both sessions matched in scent, versus mismatched. Scent match did not influence the number of descriptors or proportion of accurate descriptors recalled. Overall, the current findings suggest that scent cues may have a small impact on eyewitness accuracy. Implications and future directions are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction. Two lines of research stand out in the field of context-dependent memory research: context-dependent reproduction and context-dependent recognition. Different environmental contexts influence the productivity of involuntary memory using free playback and recognition techniques. Our study aims to establish the influence of external contexts of involuntary memorization of lexical items on their subsequent recognition performance. Methods. The procedure varied the states of two variables: global context (room view) and local context (background color and localization of the word on the screen). In the first phase, 107 subjects performed a sensorimotor task that involved the identification of a target stimulus with a recognizable characteristic (the letter ‘a’ as part of a word). In the second stage, where contextual conditions varied, subjects were required to recognize previously presented words in a series of new distractor words. The recognition accuracy, the reaction time, and the response confidence were evaluated. Results. It was found that the empirical markers "recognition accuracy" and "reaction time" are uninformative in assessing mnemic productivity. The analysis of the indicator "degree of confidence" showed that regardless of the retention / change of contexts, correct answers are given with greater confidence. Repetition of the local context in the situation of verbal stimuli recognition leads to a decrease in response time and an increase in confidence for words with the recognition feature that was relevant to the goal of the activity during memorization. Changing the global context does not induce a similar effect. Discussion. The main factors on which the recognition of lexical units during their unintentional memorization depends are the local context and the "key feature" (E. Tulving) with which the response was associated during encoding. The study of the role of motor context in mnemic activity may become a perspective in the study of the phenomenology of context-dependent memory.
Article
Full-text available
In recognition, context effects often manifest as higher hit and false-alarm rates to probes tested in an old context compared with probes tested in a new context; sometimes, this concordant effect is accompanied by a discrimination advantage. According to the cue-overload account of context effects (Rutherford, 2004), context acts like any other cue, and thus context effects should be larger with lighter context loads. Conversely, the Item, Associated Context, and Ensemble (ICE) account (Murnane et al., 1999) attributes context effects to two factors: subjects erroneously attributing context familiarity to the probe, and the formation of ensembles (mnemonic combinations of item and context). Context familiarity increases as exposure at study increases, and thus ICE predicts larger effects of context as context load increases. Relatedly, ICE predicts larger effects of context as context meaningfulness increases, as meaningful contexts are more likely to be bound to the target in an ensemble. In Experiments 1 and 2, rather than manipulate context load during the study phase, we relied on subjects' preexperimental context exposure to manipulate context load. Subjects studied words superimposed on photographs of their university campus or another university campus. At test, targets and distractors were evenly divided between study and novel contexts and between familiar and unfamiliar contexts. In Experiment 3, we manipulated context familiarity within the experimental session. Results supported ICE, suggesting that context does not act as a retrieval cue in recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Murdock (1974, Human Memory: Theory and Data, Lawrence Erlbaum) distinguished between the encoding and retrieval of item information (the representation of individual events) and associative information (the representation of relations between separate events). Mandler (1980, Psychological Review, 87, 252-271) proposed that recognition decisions could be based on the sense of familiarity engendered by the stimulus or on the retrieval of conceptual, semantic, and contextual information about the target. These two distinctions have motivated a considerable amount of research over the past 40 years and have provided much of the bases for our current understanding of recognition memory. Selective aspects of this research are reviewed to show how theories of recognition memory have developed to embody these two dichotomies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Environmental context is thought to influence recognition memory in relatively few cases, and it is an empirical matter to determine the conditions under which a positive effect occurs. Amateur divers learnt visually presented word lists in two environments: in air on a small boat and underwater at a depth of 20 m. Recognition memory was examined by a yes-no procedure in either the environment of learning or the alternative environment. Environmental context produced small but statistically reliable effects on recognition memory. Words learnt underwater were best recalled there and vice versa.
Article
Full-text available
This study demonstrated that environmental-context(EC)-dependent memory can occur with recognition memory. In Experiment 1, five lists were presented, each in a different room, and recognition memory for all five lists was tested in one of the five rooms. On a yes-no recognition test, recognition was reliably affected by EC. In Experiment 2, two lists were studied, each in a different room, and memory for both lists was tested in one of the two rooms, using either free-recall, cued-recall, or recognition testing. Reliable effects of the test room were found only on the recognition test, which contained a randomized mix of the two lists. The results of both experiments show that contrary to the prevailing belief, EC-dependent recognition does occur in the laboratory.
Article
Full-text available
Although the environmental context effect typically refers to superior performance in the context of original learning, this result is not consistently found across all experimental paradigms or instructional sets. Thus far, a precise statement of the underlying mechanisms and boundaries of the context effect has been absent from the literature, with the research having focused primarily on the robustness of the effect. The current exposition of a theoretical model and interpretation of the environmental context effect not only provides an explanation of the same-context testing superiority, but also provides the necessary parameters allowing for the prediction of situations in which same-context testing should be superior. Furthermore, the model has implications for complex learning situations which have not been tested in the research literature. The current model is grounded in the principles of classical conditioning. Thus, it is assumed that environmental context stimuli function in the same way that classically conditioned stimuli do and, as such, have effects on the various phenomena and processes associated with classical conditioning (e.g, acquisition, transfer, extinction and spontaneous recovery, and latent inhibition). Furthermore, the saliency of such stimuli should be subject to boundaries analogous to those affecting other conditioned stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
The present study helped resolve the apparent conflict between many laboratory list-learning studies, which have not found environmental context-dependent recognition memory, and staged field studies (e.g. Malpass and Devine, 1981), whose results with ‘guided memory’ techniques suggest that eyewitness face recognition should depend upon environmental context reinstatement. It was found in two different experiments that, relative to testing in a new place, returning participants to the environment where a live staged event had occurred improved performance on identification of a confederate's face (i.e., hit rate). Although physical reinstatement improved identification performance in Experiment 1, mental reinstatement instructions to subjects tested in a new environment did not improve identification performance over an uninstructed group. The environmental reinstatement effect did not interact with test delay or confederate. In Experiment 2 it was found that environmental reinstatement improved accuracy (hit rate and foil identification rate) when the correct target was present in the test line-up, and that false identifications were not significantly affected by contextual manipulations when the correct target was absent from the line-up. The results provide an empirical basis for the hypothesis that returning to the scene of an event improves eyewitness face recognition.
Article
Full-text available
The multiple-trace simulation model, MINERVA 2, was applied to a number of phenomena found in experiments on relative and absolute judgments of frequency, and forced-choice and yes-no recognition memory. How the basic model deals with effects of repetition, forgetting, list length, orientation task, selective retrieval, and similarity and how a slightly modified version accounts for effects of contextual variability on frequency judgments were shown. Two new experiments on similarity and recognition memory were presented, together with appropriate simulations; attempts to modify the model to deal with additional phenomena were also described. Questions related to the representation of frequency are addressed, and the model is evaluated and compared with related models of frequency judgments and recognition memory.