ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Certain highly emotional experiences have the potential to produce long-lasting and meaningful changes in personality. Two such experiences are spiritual transformations and experiences of profound beauty. However, little is known about the cognitive appraisals or narrative elements involved in such experiences, how they are similar, and how they differ. In a study of emotion-related narratives, these experiences were found to share many features but also differ in their valence. Experiences of profound beauty are almost always positive, but spiritual transformations are both positive and negative. Moreover, spiritual transformations seem to produce long-lasting change, but experiences of profound beauty, although evocative, do not seem to produce long-lasting change. An emotion approach helps to elucidate two understudied but important emotional experiences. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Content may be subject to copyright.
Comparing Spiritual Transformations and Experiences of
Profound Beauty
Adam B. Cohen
Arizona State University and University of
California, Berkeley
June Gruber
Yale University and University of California,
Berkeley
Dacher Keltner
University of California, Berkeley
Certain highly emotional experiences have the potential to produce long-lasting and
meaningful changes in personality. Two such experiences are spiritual transformations
and experiences of profound beauty. However, little is known about the cognitive
appraisals or narrative elements involved in such experiences, how they are similar, and
how they differ. In a study of emotion-related narratives, these experiences were found
to share many features but also differ in their valence. Experiences of profound beauty
are almost always positive, but spiritual transformations are both positive and negative.
Moreover, spiritual transformations seem to produce long-lasting change, but experi-
ences of profound beauty, although evocative, do not seem to produce long-lasting
change. An emotion approach helps to elucidate two understudied but important
emotional experiences.
Keywords: religion, spirituality, conversion, beauty, awe
In recent years, the study of spirituality has
become an emergent focus in psychological sci-
ence. Psychologists have focused on many posi-
tive aspects of spirituality and have recognized the
importance of people’s spiritual identities and the
relation of religious and spiritual variables to a
variety of physical and mental health outcomes
(Hill & Pargament, 2003; Koenig, McCullough,
& Larson, 2001; MacDonald, 2000). Hill and Par-
gament (2003) have defined spirituality as a
search for the sacred (see also Zinnbauer, Parga-
ment, & Scott, 1999). Walker and Pitts (1998)
asked people to describe spiritual people using a
prototype approach, and such people are seen as
devout (e.g., they believe in a higher power and
are likely to pray), committed, meditative, con-
tented, trustworthy, and caring. Underwood and
Teresi (2002) developed a scale to measure daily
spiritual experiences and claimed that daily spiri-
tual experiences include (among others) feelings
of connection to all life, joy that lifts people out of
daily concerns, strength, comfort, being spiritually
touched by the beauty of creation, gratitude, and
acceptance of other people. LaPierre (1994) ar-
gued that spirituality involves several dimensions,
including (1) a search for meaning in life, (2) an
encounter with the transcendent, (3) a sense of
community, (4) a search for ultimate truth or high-
est value, (5) a respect and appreciation for the
mystery of creation, and (6) a personal transfor-
mation. All of these aspects seem, on the face of it,
to be positively valenced.
Adam B. Cohen, Department of Psychology, Arizona
State University, and Institute of Personality and Social
Research, University of California, Berkeley; June Gruber,
Department of Psychology, Yale University, and Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley;
Dacher Keltner, Institute of Personality and Social Research
and Department of Psychology, University of California,
Berkeley.
This work has been funded by the Spiritual Transforma-
tion Scientific Research Program, sponsored by the Metan-
exus Institute on Religion and Science, with the generous
support of the John Templeton Foundation. Adam B. Cohen
also gratefully acknowledges the support of a Templeton
Advanced Research program grant, also from Metanexus.
The views expressed do not necessarily represent those of
Metanexus or Templeton. We thank Alexandra Baksa, Priy-
anka Dhar, and Ingrid Hernandez for help with data entry
and coding.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Adam B. Cohen, Department of Psychology,
Arizona State University, P.O. Box 871104, Tempe, AZ
85287-1104. E-mail: adamcohen@asu.edu
Psychology of Religion and Spirituality © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 2, No. 3, 127–135 1941-1022/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019126
127
In the psychology of religion, there has long
been an interest in studying spiritually transfor-
mative experiences. As is so often the case in
psychology, we found our inspiration for the
present study in the classic writings of William
James. James (1902/1997) devoted two chapters
of his classic book, Varieties of Religious Ex-
perience, to conversion, which is surely one of
the most important subtypes of spiritual expe-
riences. James wrote extensively about how
these experiences can result in feelings and
changes including the loss of all worry, cer-
tainty of salvation (among Christians), the per-
ception of knowing truths not known before, a
sense of newness, and cleanness in world (see
also Starbuck, 1897).
In addition to the aforementioned positive
aspects of religious conversions, James (1902/
1997) noted that these experiences may involve
a “sense of incompleteness and imperfection;
brooding; depression; morbid introspection;
sense of sin; anxiety about the hereafter; distress
over doubts, and the like” (p. 167). It would
make an important contribution to the psycho-
logical study of religion and spirituality to re-
mind the field of the theory that spiritual expe-
riences do not consist solely of positive aspects
but can be highly frightening, arousing, and
disillusioning. Hill (2002), inspired by the writ-
ings of James, outlined how spiritual transfor-
mation or growth can result from positive and
negative emotion (e.g., fear, unmet needs for
meaning).
In this study, we borrowed from methodolog-
ical tools used in emotion research to document
the emotional characteristics of spiritual trans-
formations, compared with experiences of pro-
found beauty. We chose to contrast spiritual
transformations with experiences of profound
beauty for two reasons. First, experiences of
profound beauty are one of the elements of the
Daily Spiritual Experience Scale (Underwood
& Teresi, 2002), which is theoretically an influ-
ential view of spiritual experiences and a com-
monly used measure. Second, we hypothesized
that it is likely that experiences of profound
beauty would be more uniformly positive and
would give us the chance to investigate whether
spiritually transformative experiences are also
likely to be positive or whether James’s analysis
was correct in pointing to the negative aspects.
This is an interesting comparison, because one
could theorize that experiences of profound
beauty could have many of the same conse-
quences, such as increased other-focus, and
cognitive accommodation (increased under-
standing of self and world; Keltner & Haidt,
2003).
Experiences of Profound Beauty
Beautiful stimuli have been theorized to con-
tain both complexity and order, to encourage
making sense of the visual scene, and to shift
levels of physiological arousal (Berlyne, 1971).
These characteristics—most notably, complex-
ity and the demand to find new meaning—are
central to awe experiences (Keltner & Haidt,
2003). Visual stimuli, such as landscapes, often
elicit feelings of beauty and awe, and prefer-
ences for certain landscape features have been
theorized to have an evolutionary basis (Kaplan
& Kaplan, 1989; Orians & Heerwagen, 1992).
For example, Orians and Heerwagen (1992)
theorized that humans have evolved preferences
for landscapes that embody features of a high-
quality African savanna during the Pleistocene
era, in which the vast majority of human evo-
lution took place. Of interest in this regard,
people who were randomly assigned to live near
trees and grass managed life stress better than
those assigned to live in areas without such
natural features (Kuo, 2001). Natural beauty, it
seems, gives people a sense of understanding
and perspective, consistent with recent claims
about the correlates of awe. In the present study,
we hypothesized that natural scenes would
commonly be reported as eliciting awe-filled
experiences of profound beauty.
Goals of the Present Research
We sought to uncover differences and simi-
larities between two highly emotional experi-
ences with the capacity to produce change,
spiritual transformations, and experiences of
profound beauty. To pursue these aims, we re-
lied on narrative techniques used in the study of
emotion-related appraisal, feeling, and thought
(e.g., Shaver et al., 1987; Smith & Ellsworth,
1985) that have been used to identify the core
meanings of different emotions (e.g., Ellsworth
& Smith, 1988a, 1988b; Roseman, 1991, 2004;
Smith & Ellsworth, 1985) and to differentiate
between subtypes of emotions, such as the self-
conscious emotions of embarrassment and
128 COHEN, GRUBER, AND KELTNER
shame (e.g., Keltner & Buswell, 1996; Miller &
Tangney, 1992).
Also, we used measures of cognitive apprais-
als because of the long tradition of investigating
the appraisal structure of positive emotions
(e.g., Roseman, 1991; Smith & Ellsworth,
1985), such as for love in romantic relationships
(Fitness & Fletcher, 1993) and interest (Silvia,
2005). Appraisal processes powerfully deter-
mine which emotion is experienced in a given
situation (Roseman, 1991, 2004). Because of
the complexity and the uncharted character of
the emotional experiences of spiritual transfor-
mation and profound beauty, we also solicited
open-ended narrative descriptions of the expe-
riences to be able to extract prototypes.
Method
Undergraduate students from the University
of California, Berkeley, were asked to fill out an
anonymous questionnaire packet concerning
one of two experiences: spiritually transforma-
tive experiences or experiences of profound
beauty. We chose an undergraduate population
because of theorizing and empirical work by
James (1902/1997), Starbuck (1897, 1900), and
others, which have pointed to late adolescence–
early adulthood as a time when such experi-
ences are common and can have important, far-
reaching consequences.
Participants were randomly assigned to write
about either a spiritually transformative experi-
ence or an experience of profound beauty. In the
beauty condition, participants (n63) were
instructed as follows:
We are interested in experiences that involve a pro-
found sense of beauty. People describe many different
kinds of such experiences. If you have ever had such an
experience, please tell us about it below. If you have
not ever had such an experience, please answer the
questions below in terms of what you think a typical
experience is like that involves a profound sense of
beauty.
In the spiritual transformation prompt, partic-
ipants (n84) were instructed as follows:
We are interested in experiences that people consider
to be a spiritual transformation. People describe many
different kinds of such experiences. A spiritual trans-
formation may be of the religious variety, it may have
to do with what you consider to be sacred, it may be in
response to something in nature, it may result from
relationship with other people, or art, or many other
things. If you have ever had such an experience, please
tell us about it below. If you have not ever had such an
experience, please answer the questions below in terms
of what you think a typical spiritual transformation is
like.
We provided more examples for the spiritual
transformation prompt (religion, sacred, nature,
relationship, art) because of the highly idiosyn-
cratic and heterogeneous meanings of spirituality
(Zinnbauer et al., 1999), and we wanted partici-
pants to feel free to write about whatever domain
or experience of interest. More participants were
given the spiritual transformation version because
pilot work suggested that these experiences are
less common than beauty experiences.
We present data on the actual experiences of
the participants, not their descriptions of their
views of typical experiences. The dependent
measures included cognitive appraisals of the
experiences (see Table 1; Smith & Ellsworth,
1985); ratings of the involvement of 46 emo-
tions and emotionlike states (see Table 1); and
open-ended narrative descriptions of the causes,
thoughts, feelings, physiology/behavior, and
consequences of the experiences (Fehr & Rus-
sell, 1984; Shaver et al., 1987; see Table 2). We
first solicited the narrative data so that cognitive
appraisal items and emotion ratings would not
influence the open-ended reports.
A detailed coding scheme to categorize the
narrative features of the essays was developed
on the basis of both (a) theoretical consider-
ations and (b) a reading of participants’ essays.
Two trained research assistants who were un-
aware of condition (beauty or spiritual experi-
ence) and hypotheses both coded a randomly
chosen subset of 30 experiences: 15 beauty ex-
periences and 15 spiritual transformations.
Agreement for each narrative code was high
(see Table 2), so we present data from one of
the coders, randomly chosen.
Results
Demographics
For all results, ns differ very slightly because
of skipped questions. In the condition for expe-
riences of profound beauty, 36 women and 16
men provided their gender. Of those reporting
ethnic background (n46), 17 were European
American, 19 were Asian, and 6 were Hispanic,
with the remainder being from other ethnic
groups. In the spiritual experiences condi-
129SPIRITUALITY AND BEAUTY
Table 1
Cognitive Appraisals and Emotion Ratings of Spiritual and Beauty Experiences
Cognitive appraisal and emotion rating
Beauty Spiritual
tCohen’s dMSDMSD
Cognitive appraisals (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985)
Pleasant 4.6 0.8 4.0 1.3 2.8
ⴱⴱ
0.59
Enjoyable 4.6 1.0 3.8 1.3 3.3
ⴱⴱⴱ
0.71
Consider further 4.0 0.2 4.0 1.3 0.4 0.00
Devote attention to 2.2 1.3 2.5 1.5 1.1 0.22
Circumstances beyond anyone’s control 3.4 1.5 3.5 1.4 0.2 0.07
Had the ability to influence what was
happening 2.4 1.5 2.9 1.4 1.7
0.34
Someone other than yourself was controlling
what was happening 2.5 1.6 3.0 1.5 1.4 0.32
Understand 3.7 1.2 3.6 1.2 0.2 0.08
Uncertain 1.8 1.1 2.3 1.1 2.2
0.45
Predict what was going to happen 2.9 1.4 2.3 1.3 2.2
0.44
Problems that had to be solved 1.9 1.2 2.7 1.5 2.8
ⴱⴱ
0.61
Obstacles 1.9 1.2 .8 1.4 3.3
ⴱⴱⴱ
0.86
Fair 3.5 1.3 3.4 1.5 0.6 0.07
Cheated or wronged 1.3 0.9 1.7 1.3 1.9
0.37
Responsible did you feel 2.4 1.4 3.3 1.5 2.8
ⴱⴱ
0.63
Someone or something other than yourself
was responsible 3.2 1.5 3.4 1.5 0.8 0.13
Exert yourself 1.7 1.1 2.8 1.5 4.1
ⴱⴱⴱ
0.87
Effort 2.4 1.3 3.1 1.4 2.5
ⴱⴱ
0.52
Emotion or emotionlike feature
Amusement 3.2 1.5 2.4 1.4 2.5
0.55
Anger 1.4 1.0 1.8 1.3 1.9
0.36
Animation 2.4 1.4 2.0 1.4 1.3 0.29
Arousal 3.7 1.2 3.4 1.5 0.9 0.23
Awe 4.5 1.0 3.9 1.4 2.6
ⴱⴱ
0.51
Beauty 4.8 0.7 3.3 1.7 5.9
ⴱⴱⴱ
1.27
Compassion 3.4 1.5 3.5 1.6 0.2 0.06
Confusion 1.6 1.1 2.7 1.3 4.4
ⴱⴱⴱ
0.93
Connection to others 3.3 1.5 3.6 1.5 0.8 0.20
Contempt 1.4 1.0 1.8 1.4 1.2 0.34
Contemplative 3.6 1.3 3.5 1.6 0.4 0.07
Contentment 4.2 1.1 3.2 1.6 3.3
ⴱⴱⴱ
0.76
Desire 3.0 1.4 3.2 1.6 0.5 0.14
Disappointment 1.4 1.0 1.8 1.3 1.5 0.36
Disgust 1.2 0.8 1.6 1.3 1.5 0.39
Embarrassment 1.5 1.1 1.8 1.3 1.1 0.25
Excitement 3.5 1.4 3.3 1.5 0.7 0.14
Fear 1.6 1.2 2.3 1.5 2.5
0.53
Goals and values 2.6 1.4 3.5 1.5 2.9
ⴱⴱ
0.63
Guilt 1.2 0.6 1.9 1.4 3.1
ⴱⴱ
0.71
Happiness 4.2 1.1 3.7 1.5 1.7
0.39
Hope 3.8 1.2 3.8 1.4 0.1 0.00
In control 2.5 1.4 3.0 1.4 1.7
0.36
Inspiration 4.5 0.8 3.9 1.4 2.6
ⴱⴱ
0.56
Interest 4.2 1.2 3.5 1.5 2.2
0.53
Jealousy 1.4 1.1 1.2 0.8 0.9 0.20
Lively 3.3 1.4 2.9 1.6 1.2 0.27
Love 3.6 1.5 3.6 1.5 0.1 0.00
Out of control 1.8 1.3 2.2 1.3 1.3 0.31
Pain 1.2 0.8 1.9 1.4 3.0
ⴱⴱ
0.65
Relaxation 3.7 1.3 2.9 1.6 2.8
ⴱⴱ
0.56
130 COHEN, GRUBER, AND KELTNER
tion, 29 women and 13 men provided their
gender. Of participants in this condition, 11
reported their identity as European Ameri-
can, 21 as Asian, and 8 as Hispanic or Latino/a.
There were no significant differences between
the spiritual and beauty conditions in propor-
tions of men versus women,
2
(1, N
94) 0.00, ns; proportion of Hispanic or
Latino/a identity (yes vs. no),
2
(1, N
93) 1.14, p.29; or proportion of White
versus Asian identity,
2
(1, N68) 1.15,
p.28.
Cognitive Appraisals
Participants rated both spiritual transforma-
tions and beauty experiences as highly pleasant
and enjoyable and wanted to consider them
further. Beauty experiences were rated more
pleasant, enjoyable, and predictable. Partici-
pants felt more responsible for beauty experi-
ences. Spiritual transformations were rated as
involving more uncertainty, more obstacles to
be overcome, and more problems that need to
be solved; participants appraised spiritual trans-
formations as requiring more exertion and ef-
fort. These findings clearly suggest, as devel-
oped in the early work of James (1902/1997)
and Starbuck (1897/1900), that spiritual and
conversion experiences often involve nega-
tively valenced feelings and that the current
emphasis on the predominantly positive valence
of spirituality may misrepresent this facet of
spiritual experience.
Gender and ethnicity effects on spiritual
and beauty experiences. Men (coded as 1;
women coded as 0) were significantly more
likely to rate that they had the ability to influ-
ence what was happening (r.29, n92, p
.01), whereas women were more inclined to
endorse the notion that someone other than the
participant was controlling what was happening
(r⫽⫺.22, n93, p.05), as well as
someone other than the self being responsible
(r⫽⫺22, n92, p.05). These correlations
are interesting but should be seen as preliminary
because of the large number of correlations.
However, we also note that the pattern of cor-
relations for gender differences tells a consistent
story, with men feeling more in control and
responsible and women feeling less in control
and responsible.
For ethnicity (White vs. Asian), correlations
were small and no correlation was significant.
Emotion ratings. Beauty experiences were
rated as involving significantly more amuse-
ment, awe, beauty, contentment, inspiration, in-
terest, and relaxation than spiritual experiences.
Spiritual transformations were rated as involv-
ing significantly more confusion, fear, pain, re-
Table 1 (continued)
Cognitive appraisal and emotion rating
Beauty Spiritual
tCohen’s dMSDMSD
Religion 2.3 1.6 3.2 1.8 2.4
0.54
Relief 2.4 1.4 3.0 1.6 1.9
0.41
Sadness 1.6 1.1 2.4 1.5 2.9
ⴱⴱ
0.63
Self-esteem 2.7 1.5 2.6 1.5 0.5 0.07
Sense of purpose 3.2 1.4 3.6 1.5 1.3 0.28
Sexual arousal 1.4 1.1 1.1 0.6 1.6 0.32
Spirituality 3.3 1.5 3.9 1.4 1.9
0.41
Skepticism 1.4 0.9 2.1 1.4 3.0
ⴱⴱ
0.63
Sympathy 1.8 1.2 2.1 1.2 1.1 0.25
Surprise 2.8 1.5 2.8 1.6 0.2 0.00
Tension 1.5 0.9 1.8 1.3 1.3 0.28
Thoughtful 3.7 1.3 3.8 1.3 0.2 0.08
Worry 1.4 1.0 2.0 1.4 2.4
0.51
Way you deal with stress 1.6 1.1 2.1 1.5 2.0
0.39
Virtue 2.9 1.3 2.8 1.6 0.2 0.07
Note. Emotion ratings ranged from 1 (not at all)to5(very much). tindicates the significance of the mean difference in
ratings between beauty and spiritual experiences.
p.10.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001. ns range from 40 to 41 for spiritual experiences and from 50 to 52 for
beauty experiences.
131SPIRITUALITY AND BEAUTY
Table 2
Features of Narratives About Beauty Experiences and Spiritual Transformations
Variable % Agreement % Beauty % Spiritual
2
Effect size phi
Causes
Nature 93.3 54.9 6.8 24.84
ⴱⴱⴱ
.51
Alone 83.3 11.8 13.6 0.08 .03
Religious event 76.7 0 34.1 20.65
ⴱⴱⴱ
.47
Travel 86.7 13.7 13.6 0.00 .00
Death/injury 96.7 2.0 15.9 5.96
.25
Thoughts
Connection to others 93.3 5.9 20.5 4.55
.22
Connection to God 96.7 2.0 29.5 14.30
ⴱⴱⴱ
.39
Small 90.0 7.8 9.1 0.05 .02
Appreciation 73.3 31.4 9.1 7.06
ⴱⴱ
.27
Understanding of religion 93.3 0 4.5 2.37 .16
Understanding of self 80.0 2.0 25.0 11.36
ⴱⴱⴱ
.35
Understanding of the world 76.7 2.0 15.9 5.96
.25
Understanding of others 93.3 0 2.3 1.17 .11
Purpose 90.0 2.0 13.6 4.72
.22
Negative thoughts about religion 96.7 0 2.3 1.17 .11
Positive thoughts about others 90.0 2.0 4.5 0.52 .07
Positive thoughts about self 96.7 11.8 4.5 1.60 .13
Positive thoughts about world 86.7 7.8 0 3.60
.20
Feelings
Happiness 80.0 35.3 27.3 0.70 .09
Beauty 80.0 47.1 0 27.71
ⴱⴱⴱ
.54
Awe 86.7 41.2 15.9 7.26
ⴱⴱ
.28
Calm 83.3 29.4 22.7 0.54 .08
Gratitude 90.0 7.8 11.4 0.34 .06
Relief 96.7 0 9.1 4.84
.23
Excitement 96.7 15.7 9.1 0.93 .10
Confusion 100.0 0 4.5 2.37 .16
Sadness 80.0 3.9 18.2 5.10
.23
Behaviors/physiology
Crying 96.7 11.8 25.0 2.82
.17
Smiling/laughing 96.7 31.4 15.9 3.08
.18
Heart racing 100.0 15.7 6.8 1.81 .14
Eyes wide 90.0 0 4.5 2.37 .16
Stillness 100.0 17.6 0 8.58
ⴱⴱ
.30
Exclamations (e.g., Wow!) 96.7 17.6 4.5 3.96
.20
Silence 83.3 45.1 50.0 0.23 .05
Changes
No change 86.7 17.6 9.1 1.46 .12
Changed understanding of self 76.7 15.7 54.5 15.97
ⴱⴱⴱ
.41
Changed understanding of world 80.0 13.7 29.5 3.56
.19
Religious strengthening 90.0 0 27.3 15.92
ⴱⴱⴱ
.41
Calm 96.7 2.0 2.3 0.01 .01
Sense of control 96.7 0 0 0 .00
Sense of purpose 93.3 0 6.8 3.59
.19
Connection to God 90.0 0 20.5 11.52
ⴱⴱⴱ
.35
Appreciation 93.3 29.4 9.1 6.10
ⴱⴱ
.25
Sense of beauty in the world 83.3 25.5 0 12.99
ⴱⴱⴱ
.37
Positive emotions about religion 90.0 0 11.4 6.12
ⴱⴱ
.25
Note. Agreement refers to the percent agreement for 30 randomly chosen narratives (15 beauty and 15 spiritual) between
two coders who were unaware of condition and hypotheses.
2
(1, N95) refers to the significance of the difference in
proportions between beauty and spiritual experiences.
p.10.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001. For phi statistics, we neglected the arbitrary sign of the coefficient.
132 COHEN, GRUBER, AND KELTNER
ligion, sadness, skepticism, (marginally more)
spirituality, skepticism, worry, and involving
the way the participant deals with stress. Again,
this dovetails nicely with early work by James
and Starbuck on the negatively valenced facets
of spiritual experience and conversion.
Compared with men, women (coded as 0;
men coded as 1) rated experiences as involving
more connection to others (r⫽⫺.34, n93,
p.001) and marginally more inspiration
(r⫽⫺.19, n93, p.10). Whites (coded as
1; Asians coded as 0) rated experiences as in-
volving more beauty (r.25, n67, p.05),
interest (r.26, n67, p.05), and skep-
ticism (r.28, n66, p.05), whereas
Asians rated experiences as involving more re-
lief (r⫽⫺.29, n66, p.05). Again, given
the large number of correlations that we did not
correct for multiple tests, these results should be
viewed as preliminary.
Narrative Data
Table 2 reports results of narrative analyses.
Causes. Religious events and death more
frequently caused spiritual experiences. Nature
was much more frequently the source of beauty
experiences.
Thoughts. Connection to others, connec-
tion to God, understanding the self, understand-
ing the world, and sense of purpose were more
common in spiritual experiences. Appreciation
and positive thoughts about the world were
more common in beauty experiences. Feeling
small, understanding religion, negative thoughts
about religion, and positive thoughts about oth-
ers did not more frequently typify the spiritual
and beauty experiences.
Feelings. Happiness and feeling calm were
common in both kinds of experiences. Grati-
tude, excitement, and confusion were also not
different. Both relief and sadness were more
common in spiritual transformation. Beauty and
awe were more common in beauty experiences.
Physiology and behavior. Silence was a
common behavior in both experiences. Crying
was marginally more common in spiritual
transformation, whereas smiling/laughing
was marginally more common in beauty ex-
periences. Stillness and exclamatory remarks
(e.g., Wow!) were more common in beauty ex-
periences. Heart racing was somewhat more
common in beauty experiences but not signifi-
cantly different.
Changes. James (1902/1997) and Starbuck
(1897/1902, 1900) claimed that conversions
commonly result in a feeling of knowing truths
not known before, perception of newness, and
ecstatic happiness. Paloutzian, Richardson, and
Rambo (1999) reviewed the effects of religious
conversion experiences and concluded that such
experiences appear to have little effect on basic
personality structure but do have big effects on
goals, feelings, attitudes, behaviors, identity,
and life meaning. In our data, changed under-
standing of the self and of the world, religious
strengthening, positive feelings about religion,
and connection to God were all significantly
more common in spiritual transformation, and a
changed sense of purpose was marginally more
common to spiritual experiences. Increased ap-
preciation and seeing more beauty in the world
were significantly more common in beauty ex-
periences. No change seemed more common in
beauty experience, but the difference was not
significant.
Although, as noted earlier, spiritual experi-
ences involved significantly more negative
emotions (e.g., confusion and worry), neither
spiritual transformation nor beauty experiences
commonly involved negative consequences. For
example, for less than 3% of the time did either
experience resulted in negative emotions about the
self, the world, or others, and a lasting feeling of
disappointment was also rare. As James (1902/
1997) noted, these experiences can be difficult and
anxiety provoking, but they appear to resolve
themselves in positive ways—making people feel
free from sin and the world having an appearance
of newness (seePaloutzian et al., 1999, for a more
recent review of evidence on the effects of con-
version experiences).
General Discussion
An emergent emphasis in the study of emo-
tion is on the positive emotions (Fredrickson,
2001) and emotions that enable the individual to
transcend self-interest— emotions including
gratitude (McCullough, Emmons, & Tsang,
2002; McCullough, Kimeldorf, & Cohen,
2008), love (Gonzaga, Keltner, Londahl, &
Smith, 2001), and awe (Keltner & Haidt, 2003).
In the present research, we examined two kinds
of emotional experiences that have the potential
133SPIRITUALITY AND BEAUTY
to change people in both positive and negative
directions, and perhaps in prosocial directions.
Narrative data capturing the antecedents and
phenomenologies of spiritual and beauty expe-
riences revealed overlapping themes as well as
compelling distinctions. As one might expect,
several of the defining features of these two
kinds of experiences were similar, including
high levels of awe. This is consistent with
claims that at the core of awe are two dimen-
sions of meaning—vastness and the need to
shift knowledge structures (Keltner & Haidt,
2003). Spiritual transformation and beauty ex-
periences were both highly positive in valence;
there was an emphasis on circumstances more
so than on the self being in control and an
emphasis on understanding. These similarities
are all the more impressive in light of the fact that
the elicitors of spiritual experiences and beauty—
religious events and nature, respectively—
differed. It is also important to note the prepon-
derance of similarities in spiritual and beauty
experiences across gender and across our two
ethnic groups.
At the same time, however, we observed nu-
merous and systematic differences in these ex-
periences. Spiritual transformation experiences
were defined by greater uncertainty, obstacles,
problems, and turmoil. Their emotional content
involved more negative affects, such as guilt,
pain, sadness, and worry. Also, in terms of
change-related consequences, participants were
much more likely to report gaining understand-
ing into the self, the world, and a sense of
purpose as a result of spiritual experiences.
These findings make an important point with
respect to the study of spiritual experiences.
Modern theorizing about spirituality has fo-
cused mostly on positive aspects of spirituality
such as contentedness and gratitude, to the rel-
ative exclusion of negatively valenced emotions
(Hill, 2002). Nevertheless, as far back as James
(1902/1997), theorists have highlighted the neg-
ative emotions that can comprise spiritual trans-
formations. Our findings suggest that emotions
such as anxiety, sadness, and pain are central to
religious and spiritual experiences. Of interest, our
data also suggest that these experiences result in
positive changes, consistent with previous theoriz-
ing about awe experiences and accommodation
(Keltner & Haidt, 2003), and awe was a com-
monly cited emotion by our participants.
This work makes contributions to our understand-
ings of two important emotional experiences—
spiritual transformations and experiences of pro-
found beauty. Our data have suggested a common
core in appraisal, narrative structure, and self-rated
emotions, as well as some important differences
between these types of experiences.
References
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology.
New York: Meredith.
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988a). From
appraisal to emotion: Differences among unpleas-
ant feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 271–
302. doi:10.1007/BF00993115
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988b). Shades of
joy: Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant
emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 301–331. doi:
10.1080/02699938808412702
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. (1984). Concept of emotion
viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464 –
486. doi:10.1037/0096 –3445.113.3.464
Fitness, J., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). Love, hate,
anger, and jealousy in close relationships: A pro-
totype and cognitive appraisal analysis. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 942–958.
doi:10.1037/0022–3514.65.5.942
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emo-
tions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-
build theory of positive emotions. American
Psychologist, 56, 218 –226. doi:10.1037/0003-
066X.56.3.218
Gonzaga, G. C., Keltner, D., Londahl, E. A., &
Smith, M. D. (2001). Love and the commitment
problem in romantic relations and friendship.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
247–262. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.81.2.247
Hill, P. C. (2002). Spiritual transformation: Forming
the habitual center of personal energy. Research in
the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 13, 87–108.
Retrieved from http://www.brill.nl/default.aspx?
partid210&pid10503
Hill, P. C., & Pargament, K. I. (2003). Advances in
the conceptualization and measurement of religion
and spirituality: Implications for physical and
mental health research. American Psycholo-
gist, 58, 64 –74. doi:10.1037/1941–1022.S.1.3
James, W. (1997). Varieties of religious experience.
New York: Touchstone. [Original work published
1902]
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of
nature: A psychological perspective. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1996). Evidence for
the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and
134 COHEN, GRUBER, AND KELTNER
guilt: A study of recalled antecedents and facial
expressions of emotion. Cognition and Emo-
tion, 10, 155–171. doi:10.1080/026999396380312
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe:
A moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cog-
nition and Emotion, 17, 297–314. doi:10.1080/
02699930302297
Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B.
(2001). Handbook of religion and health. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Kuo, F. E. (2001). Coping with poverty: Impacts of
environment and attention in the inner city. Envi-
ronment and Behavior, 33, 5–34. doi:10.1177/
00139160121972846
LaPierre, L. L. (1994). A model for describing spir-
ituality. Journal of Religion and Health, 33, 153–
161. Retrieved from http://www.springer.com/
publichealth/journal/10943
MacDonald, D. A. (2000). Spirituality: Description,
measurement, and relation to the five-factor model
of personality. Journal of Personality, 68, 153–
198. doi:10.1111/1467– 6494.00094
McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J.-A.
(2002). The grateful disposition: A conceptual and
empirical topography. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 82, 112–127. doi:10.1037/
0022–3514.82.1.112
McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., & Cohen,
A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism? The
social causes, social effects, and social evolution
of gratitude. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 17, 281–285. doi:10.1111/j.1467– 8721
.2008.00590.x
Miller, R. S., & Tangney, J. P. (1994). Differentiating
embarrassment and shame. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology, 13, 273–287. Retrieved from
http://www.guilford.com/cgi-bin/cartscript.cgi?p.
pr/jnsc.htm&dirperiodicals/per_psych&cart_id
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved
responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cos-
mides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind:
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of
culture (pp. 555–579). London: Oxford University
Press.
Paloutzian, R. F., Richardson, J. T., & Rambo, L. R.
(1999). Religious conversion and personality
change. Journal of Personality, 67, 1047–1079.
doi:10.1111/1467– 6494.00082
Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of
discrete emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 161–
200. doi:10.1080/02699939108411034
Roseman, I. J. (2004). Appraisals, rather than un-
pleasantness or muscle movements, are the pri-
mary determinants of specific emotions. Emo-
tion, 4, 145–150. doi:10.1037/1528 –3542.4.2.145
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C.
(1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration
of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086. doi:
10.1037/0022–3514.52.6.1061
Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring
the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89 –
102. doi:10.1037/1528 –3542.5.1.89
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of
cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 48, 813– 838. doi:
10.1037/0022–3514.48.4.813
Starbuck, E. D. (1897). A study of conversion. Amer-
ican Journal of Psychology, 8, 268 –308. Retrieved
from http://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/
ajp.html
Starbuck, E. D. (1900). Psychology of religion: An
empirical study of the growth of religious con-
sciousness. London: Walter Scott.
Underwood, L. G., & Teresi, J. A. (2002). The Daily
Spiritual Experience Scale: Development, theoretical
description, reliability, exploratory factor analysis,
and preliminary construct validity using health-
related data. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 24,
220 –233. doi:10.1207/S15324796ABM2401_04
Walker, L. J., & Pitts, R. C. (1998). Naturalistic
conceptions of moral maturity. Developmental
Psychology, 34, 403– 419. doi:10.1037/0012–
1649.34.3.403
Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K. I., & Scott, A. B.
(1999). The emerging meanings of religiousness
and spirituality: Problems and prospects. Journal
of Personality, 67, 889 –919. doi:10.1111/1467–
6494.00077
Received April 1, 2009
Revision received December 29, 2009
Accepted January 19, 2010
135SPIRITUALITY AND BEAUTY
... Schellekens (2007, p. 109) believes that the sublime can lead us to contact "what amounts to the source and possibility of our morality." Cohen et al. (2008) pointed out that this unconscious experience of profound beauty "emphasis on circumstances more so than on the self being in control and an emphasis on understanding." In short, as long as everyone returns to their own "ideal humanity," the creation process of sublime beauty has infinite possibilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
In Chinese thought, the concept of the “sublime” is closely intertwined with “beauty,” the two concepts merging to form a sublime beauty that seeks the continuous development of universal life amid change. This paper explores the multifaceted relationship between the Chinese artistic mind and sublime beauty through three dimensions of the research model: potential, processes, and goals. In terms of potential, Chinese artistic thought places full trust in the inherent potential of an individual’s ideal humanity, emphasizes the awakening of the sublime beauty within each person through specific art forms, and cultivates different perspectives to understand the connection between the mind and the sublime beauty of our world. In terms of process, Chinese artistic thought is characterized by the focused sincerity of the creative mindset; creative thinking that balances the “degree” of rational expression of emotions and the “ qi ” cultivated through daily spiritual practice; and the cognition of the sublime, which combines the gradual enlightenment gained from daily experiences of embodied cognition with the sudden enlightenment triggered by creative talent. In terms of artistic goals, ideal humanity in pursuit of sublime beauty is divided into three states: the “self state” that manifests the style of the individual subject, the “no-self state” that transcends the limitations of the self, and the “empty state” that surpasses even the cognitive notion of “transcending the self.” The threefold relationship between ideal humanity and sublime beauty in Chinese art can provide contemporary psychology with research model and unique perspectives on being aware of one’s self-potential, understanding their emotions and other aspects of the mind, and constructing multiple value goals.
... In situations of fear, fear inducing stimuli can make awe perceived as a threat or feeling of powerlessness (Gordon et al., 2017). According to research nature is one of the most powerful elicitors of awe (Cohen et al., 2010) accompanied by spiritual and religious experiences, and viewing Earth from the space known as the "overview effect". Spirituality has been found to be positively correlated with self-transcendent emotions (Van Cappellen et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Spiritual intelligence, a core component of spirituality, is conceptually related to awe, especially in the domain of transcendence. Awe is a self-transcendent emotion that involves the perception of vastness and a subsequent need for accommodation. Awe has been found to have several benefits for the individual self as well as social groups, making its experience of particular interest. This research investigated the association between spiritual intelligence and situational awe in 200 Pakistani university students between the ages of 18 and 25. A correlational, cross-sectional survey was carried out using The Spiritual Intelligence Self-Report Inventory (SISRI 24) and the Situational Awe Scale to test whether the variables were correlated and if age and gender influenced the experience of awe. The results revealed a significant positive association between spiritual intelligence and the experience of awe, indicating that individuals with higher levels of spiritual intelligence tended to experience awe to a greater extent. Additionally, a gender difference was observed, with women reporting higher levels of awe compared to men. No support was found for an age-based variation in situational awe. These findings contribute to the existing body of literature on spiritual intelligence, spirituality, and awe, particularly within the context of Pakistan, and have broad implications for various subfields within psychology.
... Although awe is a prevalent characteristic of the Overview Effect and can explain some of the positive changes that take place, it cannot account for them all (Yaden et al., 2016). For example, Cohen et al. (2010) found that spiritual and self-transcendent experiences are more likely to lead to longer-lasting changes than awe alone. Accordingly, Vaillant (2008) argues that people tend to search for symbolic value in personal experiences and that "particularly intense" experiences can motivate further exploration of how they fit into one's current life narrative (assimilation), which can lead to an update of pre-existing "schemas" (accommodation). ...
Article
Full-text available
Climate scientists increasingly agree that human behavior significantly contributes to global warming and biodiversity decline. Recent research emphasizes the importance of human-nature connectedness as a reliable predictor of psychological wellbeing and increased engagement in pro-environmental behavior. While evidence supports a positive correlation between human-nature connectedness and pro-environmental behavior, establishing causation remains elusive. Nevertheless, exploring this link is crucial, given its potential to enhance pro-environmental behavior. Armed with this understanding, stakeholders can design and implement successful sustainability interventions that promote wellbeing on individual and collective levels. One psychological phenomenon believed to have a strong effect on human-nature connectedness and pro-environmental behavior is “The Overview Effect,” a term used to describe the shift in awareness some astronauts experience when viewing Earth from outside its atmosphere. This pilot study explored whether a 180-degree virtual reality Overview Effect experience created by EarthscapeVR ® influences human-nature connectedness and whether a correlation exists between participants’ average human-nature connectedness scores and openness to experience scores. 60 student participants took part in the study. The results showed significant increases on human-nature connectedness ( p < 0.0021) in the experimental condition compared to the control group ( p = 0.97), with no correlation ( r = 0.137) between participants’ average human-nature connectedness scores and openness to experience scores. While these results are not conclusive and further research is necessary, the initial findings support translating the Overview Effect into virtual reality to promote human-nature connectedness in people.
... Students' emotional engagement can be linked directly to their transformative shifts as it constitutes an embodied experience. For instance, Cohen et al. (2010) show that being in awe is common in spiritual transformation processes. Keltner and Haidt (2003, p. 312) argue that experiences that lead to being in awe can have impactful effects on one's personal life and "may be one of the fastest and most powerful methods of personal change and growth". ...
Article
Full-text available
The sustainability crisis is rooted in Western paradigms of separation. To overcome the challenges we face, Western-educated people, like us, must question our deeply internalized ontological assumptions and welcome alternative relational perspectives. Grasping a relational cosmovision, not only cognitively but embodying it physically and emotionally, can enable personal and social transformations that address the root causes of sustainability problems. We explore how relational forms of immersion in Kichwa and Waorani communities, through transformative and place-based learning experiences, may foster the embodiment of relationality. Students from a Western research institution participated in a two-week study-abroad program on Indigenous sustainability solutions in the Ecuadorian Amazon. Participant observation and interviews revealed how students built stronger and more relational connections to nature and questioned the universality of Western ontological assumptions. These transformative shifts were fostered through experiences of embodying relationality facilitated through opportunities of relating, reflecting, and embracing offered by the setting and the program’s activities. Findings showcase how transformative experiences of embodying relationality can contribute to ontological shifts strengthening the relation with and to nature and constitute immersive sustainability solutions. We conclude with a dynamic model of the process through which Western-educated people trained in experiencing themselves as separate individuals realize and embody their embeddedness in the net of relationships with all beings.
... Profound aesthetic experiences like those described by Tinbergen and Collins can evoke a sense of what psychologists call "awe", an emotional response that arises from exposure to overwhelming stimuli that exceed one's existing mental structure (Keltner and Haidt 2003). Experiences of awe may be evoked by unforgettable encounters with nature (Cohen et al. 2010) or intensely moving musicals (Gabrielsson 2011) but are also frequently tied to religion and spirituality (Keltner and Haidt 2003;Silvia et al. 2015). Some work suggests experiences of awe incline people to seek further spiritual experiences (Van Cappellen and Saroglou 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social scientific research challenges stereotypes of scientists as irreligious, on the one hand, and lacking aesthetic sensitivity, on the other. Yet, while some research suggests connections between these domains, the question remains as to whether and how scientists themselves connect their religion or spirituality with their aesthetic experiences in science. Drawing on interviews with 71 biologists and physicists in India, Italy, the United Kingdom, and the United States, we find three distinct logics by which scientists connect these experiences, which we call “evoking”, “grounding”, and “defining”. We also find some scientists assume a modernist logic on which religion or spirituality and science are seen as separate to explain why they do not experience their religion or spirituality and aesthetic experiences as connected. Our findings enhance our understanding of how personal beliefs can shape and be shaped by professional experiences and suggest opportunities for dialogue between scientists and communities of faith centering aesthetic experience.
... It is known that while the experience of profound beauty has the potential to transform personality, it does not itself appear to induce long-term change. 57 These considerations should be confirmed in panel or experimental studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The ability to integrate beauty (AIB) is the ability to inner transformation including thinking about oneself, perceived phenomena, or the world through exposure to an aesthetic object (or phenomenon). Previous research indicates that the AIB is positively related to aesthetic experience. Still, it is unclear whether spirituality can mediate the relationship between the two variables. Spirituality is understood as an experience of transcendence that relates to the unseen and is “larger than human”. The aim of the study was to analyze the relationship between emotional and cognitive experiences related to the reception of art (as the most representative form of beauty) and the ability to connect with spirituality and aesthetic experiences. Methods The online survey included a sample of N = 195 adults (74% female) between the ages of 18 and 54. The Spirituality Scale (SD-36), the Aesthetic Experience Questionnaire (AEQ) and the Ability to Integrate Beauty Scale (AIBS) were used to test hypotheses. Results The analysis revealed a statistically significant, moderate relationship between the ability to integrate beauty and both the total aesthetic experience score and the spirituality scale score. The results support the hypothesis that there is a relationship between aesthetic experience in art and spirituality. The study also confirmed the mediating effect of spirituality on the relationship between aesthetic experience and aesthetic intelligence. Conclusion Individuals with a higher level of spiritual development tend to have a greater ability to integrate beauty and have more intense aesthetic experiences, which in turn may increase their aesthetic intelligence. The results suggest that a deepened spirituality contributes to a greater ability to integrate beauty.
Article
This article investigates the transformative impacts of regular nature-based adventure activity engagement and its long-lasting effects on eudaimonic well-being (EWB), specifically mental health. Although extant research highlights a wide range of well-being and mental health benefits from participation in such pursuits, less is known about experienced outdoor adventure enthusiasts for whom adventure is a fundamental and transformational part of their lives. The study builds on an existing conceptual framework that synthesizes pertinent research concepts on nature-based activity engagement and subjective well-being benefits. It presents key findings from 40 semi-structured in-depth online interviews with respondents from the UK, Germany, and Serbia. Interview data were collated and analyzed using a thematic framework approach. The findings highlight the importance of outdoor adventure activity engagement for respondents’ mental and physical health and long-term well-being. Regular activity participation can be transformational in reducing feelings of ill-being and enhancing EWB. It can improve self-efficacy and identity development and promote the fulfilment of psychological needs, facilitated by key transformational catalyzers. Continually entering a liminal state, experiencing emotions, and overcoming challenges and risks during engagement are crucial to “successful” long-lasting transformation. Further research should continue to explore adventure’s transformational and EWB benefits to develop long-term data.
Article
The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between non‐theistic parental sanctification and well‐being among parents, and whether parents’ primary images of God (authoritative, benevolent, critical, and distant) modified this relationship among American parents. Drawing on nationally representative data from the 2014 Baylor Religion Survey ( N = 1078), our cross‐sectional results suggest that non‐theistic sanctification was linked to greater overall happiness and satisfaction in the parental role. We also found consistent evidence that the relationship between non‐theistic parental sanctification and greater happiness and parental satisfaction was stronger for individuals with either an authoritative or benevolent God image, and weaker among those with parents with a distant God image. The study of sanctification in conjunction with images of the divine offers one fruitful approach to understanding both the benefits and risks of integrating religion/spirituality into daily life.
Article
Purpose In today’s world, there’s a growing recognition of the importance of cultural heritage. Mobile games have emerged as a powerful medium for the dissemination of culture. However, there’s a noticeable gap in the existing research concerning the cultural experiences within games and how they contribute to the development of cultural identity. This study addresses this gap by developing a scale to evaluate cultural experiences and constructing a theoretical model of cultural identity. By focusing on the popular mobile game “王者荣耀” (King of Glory), we highlight its rich representation of intangible cultural heritage elements and its influence on players' cultural identity. By doing so, it seeks to provide valuable insights and guidance for the development and design of mobile games. Design/methodology/approach To achieve its objectives, this study employs a combination of user interviews, factor analysis and structural equation modeling. These methods are used to identify four key dimensions of cultural experience and explore the intricate relationships between various influencing factors and the formation of cultural identity. Findings The concept of cultural experience within games encompasses four distinct facets: emotional connection, design aesthetics, perceived playfulness and knowledge acquisition. Aesthetic appreciation notably influences emotional connection and the overall enjoyment of the game. Furthermore, emotional connection plays a pivotal role in facilitating users' acquisition of knowledge. Additionally, users' cultural heritage values positively impact emotional connection, aesthetic appreciation, emotional attachment and, ultimately, their cultural identity. Within game-based cultural experiences, emotional connection and knowledge acquisition emerge as central elements that significantly influence users' sense of cultural identity. Originality/value The originality of this study lies in its pioneering construction of a comprehensive framework for assessing the impact of cultural experiences within mobile games on cultural identity. By examining “王者荣耀,” this study bridges the gap between intangible cultural heritage and interactive digital media, making an original and significant contribution to both fields.
Article
Full-text available
Empirical studies have identified significant links between religion and spirituality and health. The reasons for these associations, however, are unclear. Typically, religion and spirituality have been measured by global indices (e.g., frequency of church attendance, self-rated religiousness and spirituality) that do not specify how or why religion and spirituality affect health. The authors highlight recent advances in the delineation of religion and spirituality concepts and measures theoretically and functionally connected to health. They also point to areas for growth in religion and spirituality conceptualization and measurement. Through measures of religion and spirituality more conceptually related to physical and mental health (e.g., closeness to God, religious orientation and motivation, religious support, religious struggle), psychologists are discovering more about the distinctive contributions of religiousness and spirituality to health and well-being.
Article
Full-text available
Recent work on natural categories suggests a framework for conceptualizing people's knowledge about emotions. Categories of natural objects or events, including emotions, are formed as a result of repeated experiences and become organized around prototypes (Rosch, 1978); the interrelated set of emotion categories becomes organized within an abstract-to-concrete hierarchy. At the basic level of the emotion hierarchy one finds the handful of concepts (love, joy, anger, sadness, fear, and perhaps, surprise) most useful for making everyday distinctions among emotions, and these overlap substantially with the examples mentioned most readily when people are asked to name emotions (Fehr & Russell, 1984), with the emotions children learn to name first (Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982), and with what theorists have called basic or primary emotions. This article reports two studies, one exploring the hierarchical organization of emotion concepts and one specifying the prototypes, or scripts, of five basic emotions, and it shows how the prototype approach might be used in the future to investigate the processing of information about emotional events, cross-cultural differences in emotion concepts, and the development of emotion knowledge.
Article
Full-text available
There has long been interest in describing emotional experience in terms of underlying dimensions, but traditionally only two dimensions, pleasantness and arousal, have been reliably found. The reasons for these findings are reviewed, and integrating this review with two recent theories of emotions (Roseman, 1984; Scherer, 1982), we propose eight cognitive appraisal dimensions to differentiate emotional experience. In an investigation of this model, subjects recalled past experiences associated with each of 15 emotions, and rated them along the proposed dimensions. Six orthogonal dimensions, pleasantness, anticipated effort, certainty, attentional activity, self-other responsibility/control, and situational control, were recovered, and the emotions varied systematically along each of these dimensions, indicating a strong relation between the appraisal of one’s circumstances and one’s emotional state. The patterns of appraisal for the different emotions, and the role of each of the dimensions in differentiating emotional experience are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In four studies, the authors examined the correlates of the disposition toward gratitude. Study 1 revealed that self-ratings and observer ratings of the grateful disposition are associated with positive affect and well-being prosocial behaviors and traits, and religiousness/spirituality. Study 2 replicated these findings in a large nonstudent sample. Study 3 yielded similar results to Studies 1 and 2 and provided evidence that gratitude is negatively associated with envy and materialistic attitudes. Study 4 yielded evidence that these associations persist after controlling for Extraversion/positive affectivity, Neuroticism/negative affectivity, and Agreeableness. The development of the Gratitude Questionnaire, a unidimensional measure with good psychometric properties, is also described.
Article
Full-text available
On the basis of the proposition that love promotes commitment, the authors predicted that love would motivate approach, have a distinct signal, and correlate with commitment-enhancing processes when relationships are threatened. The authors studied romantic partners and adolescent opposite-sex friends during interactions that elicited love and threatened the bond. As expected, the experience of love correlated with approach-related states (desire, sympathy). Providing evidence for a nonverbal display of love, four affiliation cues (head nods, Duchenne smiles, gesticulation, forward leans) correlated with self-reports and partner estimates of love. Finally, the experience and display of love correlated with commitment-enhancing processes (e.g.. constructive conflict resolution, perceived trust) when the relationship was threatened. Discussion focused on love, positive emotion, and relationships.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has indicated strong relations between people's appraisals of their circumstances and their emotional states. The present study examined these relations across a range of unpleasant situations in which subjects experienced complex emotional blends. Subjects recalled unpleasant experiences from their pasts that were associated with particular appraisals and described their appraisals and emotions during these experiences. Situations defined by particular appraisals along the human agency or situational control dimensions were reliably associated with different levels of anger, sadness, and guilt, as predicted. However, predicted differences in emotion were not observed for situations selected for appraisals along the certainty or attention dimensions. Most subjects reported experiencing blends of two or more emotions, and correlation/regression analyses indicated that even in the context of these blends, patterns of appraisal similar to those observed previously (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, 1987) characterized the experience of the individual emotions. The regressions further indicated that appraisals along some dimensions were more important to the experience of particular emotions than were appraisals along other dimensions. These central appraisals are compared with the adaptive functions their associated emotions are believed to serve, and the implications of these findings are discussed. Peer Reviewed http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/45362/1/11031_2004_Article_BF00993115.pdf
Article
Full-text available
Following proposals regarding the criteria for differentiating emotions, the current investigation examined whether the antecedents and facial expressions of embarrassment, shame, and guilt are distinct. In Study 1, participants wrote down events that had caused them to feel embarrassment, shame, and guilt. Coding of these events revealed that embarrassment was associated with transgressions of conventions that govern public interactions, shame with the failure to meet important personal standards, and guilt with actions that harm others or violate duties. Study 2 determined whether these three emotions are distinct in another domain of emotion-namely, facial expression. Observers were presented with slides of 14 different facial expressions, including those of embarrassment, shame, and candidates of guilt (self-contempt, sympathy, and pain). Observers accurately identified the expressions of embarrassment and shame, but did not reliably label any expression as guilt.
Article
This article examines traditional and modern psychological characterizations of religiousness and spirituality. Three ways in which religiousness and spirituality are polarized by contemporary theorists are examined: organized religion versus personal spirituality; substantive religion versus functional spirituality; and negative religiousness versus positive spirituality. An alternative approach to understanding religiousness and spirituality is presented that integrates rather than polarizes these constructs, and sets boundaries to the discipline while acknowledging the diversity of religious and spiritual expressions. Directions for future investigations of these two constructs are presented.
Article
This book, the first of its kind, reviews and discusses the full range of research on religion and a variety of mental and physical health outcomes. Based on this research, the authors build theoretical models illustrating the various behavioural, psychological, and physiological pathways by which religion might affect health. They also review research that has explored the impact of religious affiliation, belief, and practice one use of health services and compliance with medical treatment. Finally, they discuss the implications of these findings, examine a number of possible clinical applications, and make recommendations for future research in this area